DCM: Advanced topics Klaas Enno Stephan Neural population activity 0.4 0.3 Laboratory for Social & Neural Systems Research Institute for Empirical Research in Economics University of Zurich 0.2 u2 0.1 0 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100 0.6 u1 0.4 x3 0.2 0 0.3 0.2 0.1 0 x1 x2 3 fMRI signal change (%) 2 1 Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging Institute of Neurology University College London 0 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100 4 3 m n dx A ui B (i ) x j D ( j ) x Cu dt i 1 j 1 2 1 0 -1 3 2 1 0 Methods & models for fMRI data analysis in Neuroeconomics, April 2010 Overview • Bayesian model selection (BMS) • Nonlinear DCM for fMRI • Integrating tractography and DCM Model comparison and selection Given competing hypotheses on structure & functional mechanisms of a system, which model is the best? Which model represents the best balance between model fit and model complexity? For which model m does p(y|m) become maximal? Pitt & Miyung (2002) TICS Bayesian model selection (BMS) Model evidence: Gharamani, 2004 log p( y | , m) KLq , p | m KLq , p | y, m p(y|m) p( y | m) p( y | , m) p( | m) d y all possible datasets accounts for both accuracy and complexity of the model allows for inference about structure (generalisability) of the model Various approximations, e.g.: - negative free energy, AIC, BIC Model comparison via Bayes factor: p( y | m1 ) BF p ( y | m2 ) Penny et al. 2004, NeuroImage Stephan et al. 2007, NeuroImage Approximations to the model evidence in DCM Logarithm is a monotonic function Maximizing log model evidence = Maximizing model evidence Log model evidence = balance between fit and complexity log p( y | m ) accuracy ( m ) complexity( m ) log p( y | , m) complexity( m ) No. of parameters In SPM2 & SPM5, interface offers 2 approximations: Akaike Information Criterion: Bayesian Information Criterion: AIC log p( y | , m) p p BIC log p ( y | , m ) log N 2 AIC favours more complex models, BIC favours simpler models. No. of data points Penny et al. 2004, NeuroImage Bayes factors To compare two models, we can just compare their log evidences. But: the log evidence is just some number – not very intuitive! A more intuitive interpretation of model comparisons is made possible by Bayes factors: positive value, [0;[ p( y | m1 ) B12 p( y | m2 ) Kass & Raftery classification: Kass & Raftery 1995, J. Am. Stat. Assoc. B12 p(m1|y) Evidence 1 to 3 50-75% weak 3 to 20 75-95% positive 20 to 150 95-99% strong 150 99% Very strong The negative free energy approximation • Under Gaussian assumptions about the posterior (Laplace approximation), the negative free energy F is a lower bound on the log model evidence: log p( y | m) log p( y | , m) KLq , p | m KLq , p | y, m F KLq , p | y, m F log p( y | m) KLq , p | y, m The complexity term in F • In contrast to AIC & BIC, the complexity term of the negative free energy F accounts for parameter interdependencies. KLq( ), p( | m) 1 1 1 T ln C ln C | y | y C1 | y 2 2 2 • The complexity term of F is higher – the more independent the prior parameters ( effective DFs) – the more dependent the posterior parameters – the more the posterior mean deviates from the prior mean • NB: SPM8 only uses F for model selection ! BMS in SPM8: an example attention M1 stim M1 M2 M3 M4 M3 stim PPC V1 attention V1 V5 PPC M2 M2 better than M1 BF 2966 F = 7.995 PPC attention stim V1 V5 M3 better than M2 BF 12 F = 2.450 V5 M4 attention PPC M4 better than M3 BF 23 F = 3.144 stim V1 V5 Fixed effects BMS at group level Group Bayes factor (GBF) for 1...K subjects: GBFij BF (k ) ij k Average Bayes factor (ABF): ABFij K BF (k ) ij k Problems: - blind with regard to group heterogeneity - sensitive to outliers Random effects BMS for group studies r ~ Dir (r ; ) mk ~ p(mk | p) mk ~ p(mk | p) mk ~ p(mk | p) m1 ~ Mult (m;1, r ) y1 ~ p( y1 | m1 ) y1 ~ p( y1 | m1 ) y2 ~ p( y2 | m2 ) y1 ~ p( y1 | m1 ) Dirichlet parameters = “occurrences” of models in the population Dirichlet distribution of model probabilities Multinomial distribution of model labels Model inversion by Variational Bayes (VB) Measured data y Stephan et al. 2009, NeuroImage LD m2 MOG FG LD|LVF MOG FG LD|RVF MOG LD|LVF RVF stim. LD Subjects -30 -25 -20 LD LG LG LVF stim. RVF LD|RVF stim. m2 -35 -15 MOG FG LD LG LG FG m1 LVF stim. m1 -10 -5 Log model evidence differences 0 5 Stephan et al. 2009, NeuroImage p(r >0.5 | y) = 0.997 1 5 4.5 4 pr1 r2 99.7% m2 3.5 m1 p(r 1|y) 3 2.5 2 1.5 r1 84.3% r2 15.7% 0.5 0 0 1 11.8 2 2.2 1 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 r 0.6 1 0.7 0.8 0.9 1 Validation of VB estimates by sampling 30 1 11.8 Sampling Approach Variational Bayes 25 F 20 15 10 5 0 2 4 6 1 8 10 12 14 Simulation study: sampling subjects from a heterogenous population • Population where 70% of all subjects' data are generated by model m1 and 30% by model m2 LD|LVF m1 MOG FG LD LD LG LG RVF LD|RVF stim. • Random sampling of subjects from this population and generating synthetic data with observation noise Stephan et al. 2009, NeuroImage LVF stim. LD m2 MOG • Fitting both m1 and m2 to all data sets and performing BMS MOG FG FG MOG FG LD|RVF LD|LVF LG LG RVF stim. LD LVF stim. 18 true values: 1=220.7=15.4 2=220.3=6.6 16 14 12 mean estimates: 1=15.4, 2=6.6 10 8 1 true values: r1 = 0.7, r2=0.3 0.9 0.8 mean estimates: r1 = 0.7, r2=0.3 0.7 0.6 <r> 0.5 0.4 6 0.3 4 0.2 2 0.1 0 m1 m2 1 0 m1 m2 700 0.9 true values: 1 = 1, 2=0 0.8 0.7 mean estimates: 1 = 0.89, 2=0.11 0.6 0.5 600 500 400 300 0.4 0.3 200 0.2 100 0.1 0 0 m1 m2 log GBF12 nonlinear Model space partitioning: Nonlinear hemodynamic models vs. linear ones log GBF 80 Summed log evidence (rel. to RBML) FFX linear 60 40 20 p(r >0.5 | y) = 0.986 1 0 5 CBMN CBMN(ε) RBMN RBMN(ε) CBML CBML(ε) RBML RBML(ε) RFX 12 4.5 10 4 m2 m1 alpha 8 3.5 p r1 r2 98.6% 6 3 p(r 1|y) 4 2 2.5 2 0 CBMN CBMN(ε) RBMN RBMN(ε) CBML CBML(ε) RBML RBML(ε) 1.5 1 16 14 12 m1 m2 4 8 1* k r2 26.5% r1 73.5% 0.5 0 0 10 alpha Model space partitioning 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 r k 1 0.6 0.7 0.8 0.9 1 1 6 4 8 2* k 2 k 5 0 nonlinear models linear models Stephan et al. 2009, NeuroImage Overview • Bayesian model selection (BMS) • Nonlinear DCM for fMRI • Integrating tractography and DCM Dynamic causal modelling (DCM) Hemodynamic forward model: neural activityBOLD (nonlinear) Electric/magnetic forward model: neural activityEEG MEG LFP (linear) Neural state equation: x F ( x, u, ) fMRI Neural model: 1 state variable per region bilinear state equation no propagation delays ERPs Neural model: 8 state variables per region nonlinear state equation propagation delays inputs y y BOLD y activity x2(t) neuronal states t Neural state equation intrinsic connectivity modulation of connectivity direct inputs Stephan & Friston (2007), Handbook of Brain Connectivity hemodynamic model x integration modulatory input u2(t) t λ activity x3(t) activity x1(t) driving input u1(t) y x ( A u j B( j ) ) x Cu x x x u j x A B( j) x C u bilinear DCM non-linear DCM modulation driving input driving input modulation Two-dimensional Taylor series (around x0=0, u0=0): dx 2 f x2 f f 2 f f ( x, u ) f ( x0 ,0) ... x u ux ... 2 dt x 2 x u xu Bilinear state equation: m dx A ui B ( i ) x Cu dt i 1 Nonlinear state equation: m n dx (i ) ( j) A ui B x j D x Cu dt i 1 j 1 Neural population activity 0.4 0.3 0.2 u2 0.1 0 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100 0.6 u1 0.4 x3 0.2 0 0.3 0.2 0.1 0 x1 x2 3 fMRI signal change (%) 2 1 0 Nonlinear dynamic causal model (DCM): 4 m n dx (i ) ( j) A ui B x j D x Cu dt i 1 j 1 2 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100 3 1 0 -1 3 2 1 Stephan et al. 2008, NeuroImage 0 Nonlinear DCM: Attention to motion Stimuli + Task Previous bilinear DCM Attention Photic .52 (98%) .37 (90%) .42 (100%) Büchel & Friston (1997) 250 radially moving dots (4.7 °/s) Conditions: F – fixation only A – motion + attention (“detect changes”) N – motion without attention S – stationary dots V1 Motion SPC .56 (99%) .69 (100%) .47 (100%) .82 (100%) .65 (100%) IFG V5 Friston et al. (2003) Friston et al. (2003): attention modulates backward connections IFG→SPC and SPC→V5. Q: Is a nonlinear mechanism (gain control) a better explanation of the data? attention modulation of back- M1 PPC ward or forward connection? stim additional driving effect of attention on PPC? M3 stim M2 M2 better than M1 BF = 2966 V1 attention PPC Stephan et al. 2008, NeuroImage V1 V5 BF = 12 M3 better than M2 V1 V5 M4 bilinear or nonlinear modulation of forward connection? attention stim V5 PPC attention PPC BF = 23 M4 better than M3 stim V1 V5 attention MAP = 1.25 0.10 0.8 0.7 PPC 0.6 0.26 0.5 0.39 1.25 stim 0.26 V1 0.13 0.46 0.50 V5 0.4 0.3 0.2 0.1 0 -2 motion Stephan et al. 2008, NeuroImage -1 0 1 2 3 4 p( DVPPC 5,V 1 0 | y ) 99.1% 5 motion & attention static motion & no attention dots V1 V5 PPC observed fitted Learning of dynamic audio-visual associations 1 Conditioning Stimulus CS1 Target Stimulus CS2 0.8 or p(face) or CS 0 Response TS 200 400 600 Time (ms) 800 0.6 0.4 0.2 2000 ± 650 0 0 200 400 600 trial den Ouden et al. 2010, J. Neurosci . 800 1000 Hierarchical Bayesian learning model pk 1 k volatility vt-1 pvt 1 | vt , k ~ N vt , exp( k ) vt prt 1 | rt , vt ~ Dir rt , exp( vt ) probabilistic association rt rt+1 observed events ut ut+1 1 prediction: p rt , vt , K u1:t 1 t rt 1 , vt 1 p vt vt 1 , K p rt 1 , vt 1 , K u1:t 1 drt 1dvt 1 update: p rt , vt , K u1:t 0.2 p r , v , K u p u r dr dv dK t 1:t 1 Behrens et al. 2007, Nat. Neurosci. 0.6 0.4 p rt , vt , K u1:t 1 p ut rt t p(F) p r 0.8 t t t t 0 400 440 480 520 Trial 560 600 Comparison of different learning models 1 Reaction times 450 True Bayes Vol HMM fixed HMM learn RW 0.8 p(F) RT (ms) 440 430 420 0.6 0.4 410 0.2 400 390 0.1 0.3 0.5 0.7 0.9 0 400 440 480 True probabilities Rescorla-Wagner Hidden Markov models (2 variants) den Ouden et al. 2010, J. Neurosci . 0.7 560 600 Bayesian model selection: 0.6 Exceedance prob. Alternative learning models: 520 Trial p(outcome) hierarchical Bayesian learner performs best 0.5 0.4 0.3 0.2 0.1 0 Categorical model Bayesian learner HMM (fixed) HMM (learn) RescorlaWagner Stimulus-independent prediction error Putamen Premotor cortex p < 0.05 (cluster-level wholebrain corrected) 0 -0.5 0 -0.5 -1 -1.5 -2 BOLD resp. (a.u.) BOLD resp. (a.u.) p < 0.05 (SVC) -1 -1.5 p(F) p(H) den Ouden et al. 2010, J. Neurosci . -2 p(F) p(H) Prediction error (PE) activity in the putamen PE during reinforcement learning O'Doherty et al. 2004, Science PE during incidental sensory learning den Ouden et al. 2009, Cerebral Cortex According to the FEP (and other learning theories): synaptic plasticity during learning = PE dependent changes in connectivity Prediction error gates visuo-motor connections • Modulation of visuo-motor connections by striatal PE activity • Influence of visual areas on premotor cortex: – stronger for surprising stimuli – weaker for expected stimuli den Ouden et al. 2010, J. Neurosci . p(H) p(F) PUT d = 0.011 0.004 d = 0.010 0.003 p = 0.010 PPA PMd p = 0.017 FFA Overview • Bayesian model selection (BMS) • Nonlinear DCM for fMRI • Integrating tractography and DCM Diffusion-weighted imaging Parker & Alexander, 2005, Phil. Trans. B Probabilistic tractography: Kaden et al. 2007, NeuroImage • computes local fibre orientation density by spherical deconvolution of the diffusion-weighted signal • estimates the spatial probability distribution of connectivity from given seed regions • anatomical connectivity = proportion of fibre pathways originating in a specific source region that intersect a target region • If the area or volume of the source region approaches a point, this measure reduces to method by Behrens et al. (2003) 1.6 Integration of tractography and DCM 1.4 1.2 1 R1 R2 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 0 -2 -1 0 1 2 low probability of anatomical connection small prior variance of effective connectivity parameter 1.6 1.4 1.2 1 R1 R2 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 0 Stephan, Tittgemeyer et al. 2009, NeuroImage -2 -1 0 1 high probability of anatomical connection large prior variance of effective connectivity parameter 2 LD|LVF FG (x3) probabilistic tractography FG (x4) LD DCM structure FG left * 24 1.50 102 24 43.6% LG (x2) LG right LG left 12* 1.17 102 12 34.2% LD|RVF RVF stim. FG right 13* 5.37 103 13 15.7% LD LG (x1) * 34 2.23 103 34 6.5% BVF stim. LVF stim. 6.5% v 0.0384 2 1.8 15.7% 1.6 connectionspecific priors for coupling parameters v 0.1070 1.4 1.2 1 0.8 0.6 34.2% 43.6% v 0.5268 v 0.7746 0.4 0.2 0 -3 -2 -1 0 1 2 3 anatomical connectivity Connection-specific prior variance as a function of anatomical connection probability ij 0 1 0 exp( ij ) m 1: a=-32,b=-32 m 2: a=-16,b=-32 m 3: a=-16,b=-28 m 4: a=-12,b=-32 m 5: a=-12,b=-28 m 6: a=-12,b=-24 m 7: a=-12,b=-20 m 8: a=-8,b=-32 m 9: a=-8,b=-28 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 m 10: a=-8,b=-24 m 11: a=-8,b=-20 m 12: a=-8,b=-16 m 13: a=-8,b=-12 m 14: a=-4,b=-32 m 15: a=-4,b=-28 m 16: a=-4,b=-24 m 17: a=-4,b=-20 m 18: a=-4,b=-16 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0 • 64 different mappings by systematic search across hyperparameters and 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 m 19: a=-4,b=-12 m 20: a=-4,b=-8 m 21: a=-4,b=-4 m 22: a=-4,b=0 m 23: a=-4,b=4 m 24: a=0,b=-32 m 25: a=0,b=-28 m 26: a=0,b=-24 m 27: a=0,b=-20 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 m 28: a=0,b=-16 m 29: a=0,b=-12 m 30: a=0,b=-8 m 31: a=0,b=-4 m 32: a=0,b=0 m 33: a=0,b=4 m 34: a=0,b=8 m 35: a=0,b=12 m 36: a=0,b=16 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 m 37: a=0,b=20 m 38: a=0,b=24 m 39: a=0,b=28 m 40: a=0,b=32 m 41: a=4,b=-32 m 42: a=4,b=0 m 43: a=4,b=4 m 44: a=4,b=8 m 45: a=4,b=12 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 0.5 • yields anatomically informed (intuitive and counterintuitive) and uninformed priors 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 m 46: a=4,b=16 m 47: a=4,b=20 m 48: a=4,b=24 m 49: a=4,b=28 m 50: a=4,b=32 m 51: a=8,b=12 m 52: a=8,b=16 m 53: a=8,b=20 m 54: a=8,b=24 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 m 55: a=8,b=28 m 56: a=8,b=32 m 57: a=12,b=20 m 58: a=12,b=24 m 59: a=12,b=28 m 60: a=12,b=32 m 61: a=16,b=28 m 62: a=16,b=32 m 63 & m 64 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 0 0 0.5 1 0 0 0.5 1 0 0 0.5 1 0 0 0.5 1 0 0 0.5 1 0 0 0.5 1 0 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 0 0.5 1 0 0 0.5 1 log group Bayes factor 600 400 200 log group Bayes factor 0 0 10 20 30 model 40 50 60 0 10 20 30 model 40 50 60 10 20 30 model 40 50 60 700 695 690 685 680 post. model prob. 0.6 0.5 0.4 0.3 0.2 0.1 0 0 m 1: a=-32,b=-32 m 2: a=-16,b=-32 m 3: a=-16,b=-28 m 4: a=-12,b=-32 m 5: a=-12,b=-28 m 6: a=-12,b=-24 m 7: a=-12,b=-20 m 8: a=-8,b=-32 m 9: a=-8,b=-28 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 m 10: a=-8,b=-24 m 11: a=-8,b=-20 m 12: a=-8,b=-16 m 13: a=-8,b=-12 m 14: a=-4,b=-32 m 15: a=-4,b=-28 m 16: a=-4,b=-24 m 17: a=-4,b=-20 m 18: a=-4,b=-16 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 m 19: a=-4,b=-12 m 20: a=-4,b=-8 m 21: a=-4,b=-4 m 22: a=-4,b=0 m 23: a=-4,b=4 m 24: a=0,b=-32 m 25: a=0,b=-28 m 26: a=0,b=-24 m 27: a=0,b=-20 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 m 28: a=0,b=-16 m 29: a=0,b=-12 m 30: a=0,b=-8 m 31: a=0,b=-4 m 32: a=0,b=0 m 33: a=0,b=4 m 34: a=0,b=8 m 35: a=0,b=12 m 36: a=0,b=16 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 m 37: a=0,b=20 m 38: a=0,b=24 m 39: a=0,b=28 m 40: a=0,b=32 m 41: a=4,b=-32 m 42: a=4,b=0 m 43: a=4,b=4 m 44: a=4,b=8 m 45: a=4,b=12 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 m 46: a=4,b=16 m 47: a=4,b=20 m 48: a=4,b=24 m 49: a=4,b=28 m 50: a=4,b=32 m 51: a=8,b=12 m 52: a=8,b=16 m 53: a=8,b=20 m 54: a=8,b=24 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 1 m 55: a=8,b=28 m 56: a=8,b=32 m 57: a=12,b=20 m 58: a=12,b=24 m 59: a=12,b=28 m 60: a=12,b=32 m 61: a=16,b=28 m 62: a=16,b=32 m 63 & m 64 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 0.5 0 0.5 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 0 0.5 Stephan, Tittgemeyer et al. 2009, NeuroImage 1 0 0.5 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 0 0.5 1 0 0.5 0 0.5 1 0 0 0.5 1 Further reading: Methods papers on DCM for fMRI – part 1 • Chumbley JR, Friston KJ, Fearn T, Kiebel SJ (2007) A Metropolis-Hastings algorithm for dynamic causal models. Neuroimage 38:478-487. • Daunizeau J, David, O, Stephan KE (2010) Dynamic Causal Modelling: A critical review of the biophysical and statistical foundations. NeuroImage, in press. • Friston KJ, Harrison L, Penny W (2003) Dynamic causal modelling. Neuroimage 19:1273-1302. • Kasess CH, Stephan KE, Weissenbacher A, Pezawas L, Moser E, Windischberger C (2010) Multi-Subject Analyses with Dynamic Causal Modeling. NeuroImage 49:30653074. • Kiebel SJ, Kloppel S, Weiskopf N, Friston KJ (2007) Dynamic causal modeling: a generative model of slice timing in fMRI. Neuroimage 34:1487-1496. • Marreiros AC, Kiebel SJ, Friston KJ (2008) Dynamic causal modelling for fMRI: a twostate model. Neuroimage 39:269-278. • Penny WD, Stephan KE, Mechelli A, Friston KJ (2004a) Comparing dynamic causal models. Neuroimage 22:1157-1172. • Penny WD, Stephan KE, Mechelli A, Friston KJ (2004b) Modelling functional integration: a comparison of structural equation and dynamic causal models. Neuroimage 23 Suppl 1:S264-274. Further reading: Methods papers on DCM for fMRI – part 2 • Stephan KE, Harrison LM, Penny WD, Friston KJ (2004) Biophysical models of fMRI responses. Curr Opin Neurobiol 14:629-635. • Stephan KE, Weiskopf N, Drysdale PM, Robinson PA, Friston KJ (2007) Comparing hemodynamic models with DCM. Neuroimage 38:387-401. • Stephan KE, Harrison LM, Kiebel SJ, David O, Penny WD, Friston KJ (2007) Dynamic causal models of neural system dynamics: current state and future extensions. J Biosci 32:129-144. • Stephan KE, Weiskopf N, Drysdale PM, Robinson PA, Friston KJ (2007) Comparing hemodynamic models with DCM. Neuroimage 38:387-401. • Stephan KE, Kasper L, Harrison LM, Daunizeau J, den Ouden HE, Breakspear M, Friston KJ (2008) Nonlinear dynamic causal models for fMRI. Neuroimage 42:649662. • Stephan KE, Penny WD, Daunizeau J, Moran RJ, Friston KJ (2009) Bayesian model selection for group studies. Neuroimage 46:1004-1017. • Stephan KE, Tittgemeyer M, Knösche TR, Moran RJ, Friston KJ (2009) Tractographybased priors for dynamic causal models. Neuroimage 47: 1628-1638. • Stephan KE, Penny WD, Moran RJ, den Ouden HEM, Daunizeau J, Friston KJ (2010) Ten simple rules for Dynamic Causal Modelling. NeuroImage 49: 3099-3109. Dynamic causal modelling (DCM) Hemodynamic forward model: neural activityBOLD (nonlinear) Electric/magnetic forward model: neural activityEEG MEG LFP (linear) Neural state equation: x F ( x, u, ) fMRI Neural model: 1 state variable per region bilinear state equation no propagation delays ERPs Neural model: 8 state variables per region nonlinear state equation propagation delays inputs Take-home messages • Bayesian model selection (BMS): generic approach to selecting an optimal model from a set of competing models • random effects BMS for group studies: posterior model probabilities and exceedance probabilities • nonlinear DCM: enables one to investigate synaptic gating processes via activity-dependent changes in connection strengths • DCM & tractography: probabilities of anatomical connections can be used to inform the prior variance of DCM coupling parameters • DCM implementations do not only exist for fMRI data, but also for electrophysiological data Thank you