International Baccalaureate Diploma Program Psychology Extended Essay The role of biological factors in mediating interpersonal attraction Research Question: To what extent do biological factors play a role in influencing attraction? Word count: 3999 Candidate code: jrv461 1 Table of Contents Introduc)on ............................................................................................................................ 3 Dopamine and a3rac)on ........................................................................................................ 5 Testosterone and a3rac)on .................................................................................................... 8 Oxytocin and a3rac)on ........................................................................................................ 11 Cogni)on, culture, and a3rac)on ......................................................................................... 14 Discussion ............................................................................................................................. 18 Conclusion ............................................................................................................................ 20 Works Cited .......................................................................................................................... 22 2 Introduc)on Human attraction is a complex and multidimensional social behavior that continues to be a part of ongoing research and investigation. This paper defines attraction via the definition stated in the American Psychological Association Dictionary of Psychology, which defines attraction as “the interest in and liking of one individual by another, or the mutual interest and liking between two or more individuals. Interpersonal attraction may be based on shared experiences or characteristics, physical appearance, internal motivation (e.g., for affiliation), or some combination of these. Also called interpersonal attraction.” (American Psychological Association). Currently, some biological factors of attraction are well-researched with a multitude of supporting studies such as Wedekind et al (Wedekind et al.) and Walum et al (Walum et al.). Some of those factors and additional theories that could be applied in an attempt to explain attraction are included in the three fields of psychology – the biological, cognitive, and sociocultural fields. The biological approach to psychology, also alternatively referred to as biopsychology is the psychological perspective that places heavy emphasis on comprehending human behavior and cognitive processes through the explanations of the influence of various biological factors that stem from the physiological mechanisms of our bodies, such as neurotransmitters or hormones. Neurotransmitters are chemical substances synthesized within the neuronal cell body and bind to the receptors on the postsynaptic neuron to catalyze reactions. Following these functions, neurotransmitters are used as mediators to transmit signals from one neuron to another or to a 3 target cell within our brains. Additionally, Neurotransmitters help control our functions such as thoughts and feelings that are relevant to attraction (Cleveland Clinic). Hormones on the other hand are also chemical messengers. Except they travel within the body’s bloodstream to organs instead of between synapses like neurotransmitters. They act slower over time with a longer duration and affect our bodily functions such as our moods and sexual function. The advancement of medical technology over the past decade has greatly increased its accessibility and subsequently paved the way for more research in the biological field to investigate interpersonal attraction. Thus, this essay will focus on examining modern studies in an attempt to reevaluate how neurotransmitters and hormones affect the formation of attraction. Hence, the research question of this essay is: To what extent do biological factors play a role in influencing attraction? 4 Dopamine and a1rac)on Dopamine is a neurotransmitter that affects our brains and provides us with a sense of pleasure, satisfaction, and motivation when released. This neurotransmitter is produced in the substantia nigra, hypothalamus, and the ventral tegmental area within our brains (Juárez Olguín et al.). The ventral tegmental area is also known for regulating reward consumption, learning, memory, and similar aspects by facilitating the release of dopamine toward downstream regions (Cai and Tong). Fisher et al (Fisher et al.) conducted a study on neurotransmitters, specifically investigating the relationship between the release of dopamine and the mediation of attraction by using functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI). Fisher et al was a quasi-experiment using a sample of 10 women and 7 men with a mean age of 21 who were intensely in love within the range of 1 to 17 months. Each participant was semi-structurally interviewed to determine the intensity and duration of love. They were also required to complete a self-reported Likert scale questionnaire called the “Passionate Love Scale” (PLS) to measure the characteristics that are commonly attributed to being in romantic love, which is developed through the preceding formation of attraction (Lovering). For example, some of the questions included in the PLS are “I would feel deep despair if ___ left me”, and “I would rather be with ___ than anyone else”. The Likert Scale ranges from numbers 1-9, with 1 denoting “not true”, and 9 denoting “definitely true” (Hatfield and Sprecher). The procedure consisted of repeating a standardized and timed process of participants viewing images of their neutral acquaintances and beloved partners, which were identified and 5 selected for the experiment based on a preliminary inquiry. For further context, photographs were chosen as the independent variable stimuli based on a previous study by Mashek et al, which identified visual photographic stimuli to be the most effective in evoking intense feelings of romantic love compared to other stimuli such as touch, or voice (Mashek et al 2000). The photographs were cropped to show the head only, and the image quality of all the photographs was controlled. Additionally, a numerical countback distraction task was implemented to mitigate the carryover effects from viewing their partner and then the acquaintances. The number is selected randomly, and the participants are required to count back in increments of 7. Fisher et al’s procedure repeated a 120-second 4-step process for a total of 6 times, lasting 12 minutes for each participant. The first 30 seconds involved the participant viewing an image of their beloved partner. Then, the next 40 seconds required the completion of the numerical countback distraction task and in the following 30 seconds, the participant viewed a photo of the neutral acquaintance, with the last 20 seconds consisting of the countback distraction task again. The first image viewed by each participant was either their partner or neutral acquaintance, thus, the experimental process was also counterbalanced across participants accordingly. During the procedure, the participant is scanned using the fMRI to examine the activation of brain regions. The findings according to the fMRI showed that the right ventral tegmental area of the brain had significant activation and increased responses while viewing the photograph of the participant’s beloved partner compared to the distraction task and neutral acquaintance. Furthermore, participant scores from the PLS were used to triangulate the activation of areas 6 surrounding the ventral tegmental area. Higher PLS scores were correlated with greater activation within regions surrounding the ventral tegmental area and the caudate nucleus, which is another structure responsible for emotion and romantic interactions (Driscoll et al.). Hence, the findings suggest that dopamine may play a role in mediating attraction to a loved one because there is a correlational relationship supported by scientific evidence between the activation of dopamine-producing areas and seeing the romantic partner. Although the fMRI utilized in Fisher et al’s study only measures regional activation and does not directly measure levels of dopamine, it does provide a more scientific perspective compared to the other studies. Additionally, the findings by Fisher et al are supported by prior findings by Bartels and Zeki (Bartels and Zeki) that utilized a similar procedure but used samples that were already engaged in long-term relationships, and their study also found cortical activation near regions like the caudate nucleus, which are almost identical to Fisher et al. Likewise, Zald et al (Zald) concluded that presentations of monetary rewards also induced the release of dopamine within the caudate nucleus, where Fisher et al’s study found activation. The combination of these two supporting studies strengthens the presence and role of dopamine in mediating attraction. 7 Testosterone and a1rac)on Testosterone is the major sex hormone within males and a crucial component in the development and growth of masculine features. This hormone is produced within the gonads of men and the ovaries of women, and functions in regulating the libido of both sexes (LeWine). Ronay and Von Hippel (Ronay and Hippel) conducted a study to determine whether or not young men would take greater risks in the presence of an attractive female compared to the presence of a male researcher, and whether or not the testosterone hormone would play a role in such behavior. The experiment recruited a total of 96 young Australian skateboarders at a skating park with a mean age of 21.58 years for a naturalistic and highly ecologically valid setting. 43 skaters were allocated to the male-researcher condition and the remaining 53 were assigned to the female-researcher condition. The experimental procedure first required all the skaters to identify an easy skateboarding trick that they could mostly successfully complete, and a difficult skateboarding trick that they could only successfully complete about 50% of the time. Next, the participants needed to attempt both the easy and difficult tricks 10 times each for a total of 20 attempts while being video recorded by a male researcher. After a short break, depending on which condition the skaters were allocated to, they had to attempt both levels of tricks 10 times each again in front of either the same male researcher or an attractive 18-year-old female researcher who was blind to the study. The attractiveness of the female researcher was determined beforehand via Likert Scale ratings. 20 men who were similar in age to the skaters viewed individual photographs of female researchers and rated their appearance on a scale of 1 (very unattractive) to 7 (very attractive), 8 and the highest-rated female researcher was selected for this experiment. During the second round of attempting skateboard tricks, the skateboarding trick attempts were coded for and split into 3 categories – success, crash landing, and aborted attempt by two raters. Consequently, higher levels of aborted attempts are indicative of low risk-taking, and lower levels would indicate the opposite. Finally, after each round of skateboarding attempts, researchers took saliva samples from the participants to measure the levels of testosterone. Similarly, the heart rate of the participants was monitored by wearing a smartwatch, and measurements were taken before and after both rounds of skateboarding. Additionally, all tests were conducted under strict time periods ranging from 2-6 pm to control for diurnal variations that may cause fluctuations of testosterone concentration within the sample. The findings concluded that participants under the attractive female researcher condition took significantly more risks than the participants under the male researcher. During the second round of skateboarding, when the conditions were differentiated, the participants who performed under the female researcher took more risks, denoted by the drastically decreased number of aborted attempts compared to the same statistic in the first round, and also the second round of aborted attempts for those under the male researcher group. Likewise, testosterone levels measured in the saliva samples of participants were also significantly higher in the female researcher condition with an average of 295.95pmol/L compared to the male researcher condition, which only contained an average of 212.88pmol/L of testosterone. However, heart rates did not differ noticeably between the two groups. Hence, the findings of Ronay and Von Hippel’s study support the argument that hormones (in this case testosterone) may also play a role in mediating interpersonal attraction, due to the 9 correlation between performing under an attractive female researcher, and increased levels of testosterone. Additionally, skateboarders may have taken higher risks to display higher qualities of sexual selection to advertise themselves as a better partner (Wilke et al.). 10 Oxytocin and a1rac)on Oxytocin is a hormone with similar functions to dopamine. Oxytocin is produced in the hypothalamus and released into our bloodstream by the pituitary gland. Oxytocin is also regarded as a love hormone because it fosters positive and pleasurable feelings and is produced when we are sexually aroused or have fallen in love. Additionally, oxytocin also promotes positive behaviors such as relaxation, trust (Watson), bonding, and attachments during social interactions (Bosch and Young). Based on the effects of oxytocin, Theodoridou et al (Theodoridou et al.) conducted a double-blind placebo-controlled study to determine whether or not oxytocin will increase the perceived facial trustworthiness and attractiveness of unseen individuals. The sample consisted of 96 British students with a mean age of 21 and an equal number of males and females. The participants were randomly allocated to either the experimental condition, where oxytocin was administered intranasally, or the placebo condition which used a placebo nasal spray without oxytocin. The procedure consisted of rating faces electronically included in either the trustworthiness stimuli (30 images of faces with neutral expressions) or the attractiveness stimuli (48 neutral faces). Each of the two sets of stimuli contained an equal number of male and female images, and facial attractiveness was always rated before facial trustworthiness. Additionally, the participants were also required to rate the faces for their mood to account for the influence on perception either immediately before or after the procedure based on three criteria – Positive affect, wakefulness, and calmness. The mood ratings ranged from 1-6, with 1 denoting 11 “definitely not”, and 6 denoting “extremely”. Higher scores in mood ratings mean the images signify higher levels of the 3 criteria mentioned before. Firstly, each participant manually administered an intranasal dose of either oxytocin or the placebo depending on their condition, then they were required to wait for 25 minutes for their bodies to absorb the oxytocin before beginning the experiment. 47 participants completed the trustworthiness and attractiveness ratings first after 25 minutes of intranasal administration, and 49 participants completed the mood ratings first, then the trustworthiness and attractiveness ratings second after 45 minutes of administration. During the rating process, participants were presented with the question “How attractive is this face (1-7)” under the face on the image. The ratings ranged from the numbers 1-7, 1 denoting unattractive, and 7 denoting attractive, and the same method was also applied to ratings for trustworthiness. The ratings were inserted electronically using a keyboard, and the duration of the experiment was dependent on the participant’s own pace without time constraints. The findings concluded that participants within the intranasal oxytocin group consistently rated the same set of faces as being more trustworthy and attractive compared to the placebo group. Additionally, statistical measurements displayed no significant differences in terms of ratings when factoring in the participant’s sex, and order effects were also not present despite one group rating for trustworthiness and attractiveness after 45 minutes of administration. Also, mood ratings differ between groups and did not affect the ratings for trustworthiness and attractiveness. Thus, based on the findings of Theodoridou et al, the presence of the oxytocin hormone was correlated with the elevated perception of trustworthiness and attractiveness in both sexes. 12 Furthermore, when this study is examined in combination with (Riem et al.), which concluded that oxytocin administration increases the presence of prosocial behaviors, Theodoridou et al’s study suggests oxytocin as a facilitating factor for attraction, especially when physical attractiveness is the strongest predictor for attraction in both sexes according to (Luo and Zhang). Moreover, the trusting behaviors exhibited by participants after the administration of intranasal oxytocin are supported by the findings of Kosfeld et al (Kosfeld et al.) and Baumgartner et al (Baumgartner et al.), where the use of oxytocin also increased trust among participants in a game involving real monetary stakes. Although attraction is not directly measured in Theodoridou et al, findings by Singh et al (Singh et al.) were able to identify trust as a highly prioritized component within attraction and the development of relationships, which helps to partially triangulate the findings by Theodoridou et al. 13 Cogni)on, culture, and a1rac)on A different approach to attraction will discuss studies and theories related to the sociocultural and cognitive fields of psychology. Cognitive psychology is a field that studies human mental processes and aims to explain human behavior through human cognition such as decision-making, systems of thinking, logic, and perception (Wagemans). On the other hand, sociocultural psychology is another branch of psychology that emphasizes the influence of human social and cultural factors such as cultural and societal norms, human interactions, and surrounding environmental elements on our behavior, and the formation of personal or group behavior and cognition (Waude). Hence, cognitive, and sociocultural studies aim to provide additional insight into the influence of interpersonal attraction rather than directly opposing the biological approach. A cognitive psychology model for attraction will be used to provide an additional aspect to the formation of attraction. The similarity-attraction model (encyclopedia) is a cognitive model stating that people tend to be attracted to others who are similar to themselves. Some of the important similarities listed in the similarity attraction model include but are not limited to our values, attitudes, preferences, and other personal traits. Furthermore, similarities attract because lesser differences result in fewer disagreements and strengthen social reinforcement. These social mechanisms draw parallels to social identity theory as well, which contains processes such as social categorization where individuals mentally identify and categorize themselves into social groups based on similar traits (Mcleod). Additionally, similarities between people bring about familiarity and comfort, which can help enhance positive feelings and facilitate social bonding (Nicholson). 14 Markey and Markey (Markey and Markey) conducted a study in relation to the similarityattraction model aiming to measure the extent to which interpersonal similarities influence partner selection. A sample of 169 romantic-partner-seeking American undergraduate students with a mean age of 19 was selected for this experiment. The procedure consisted of splitting the participants into different groups consisting of 26 individuals each, and they were instructed to complete one questionnaire at a time following its listed order. The first was the Interpersonal Adjective Scale (IAS), which is a 64-item adjective questionnaire used for each participant to rate their own personality, such as being “arrogantcalculating” or “warm-agreeable” (Wiggins). The second step involves the participants completing unrelated questionnaires as a distraction task to better conceal the aim of the study. The last step involved the sample completing an altered version of the first IAS. This time, each participant was told to answer the 64-item adjectives in a manner that best described their ideal romantic partner, and it did not matter whether or not their ideal romantic partner was a real or fictional person. The findings concluded that based on the same IAS, the adjectives used to describe each participant’s ideal romantic partner were significantly correlated (p<0.01) with what they used to describe themselves. The results of Markey & Markey’s study are coherent with and support the similarity attraction model. Just like how the model states that we are more likely to be attracted to those who are similar to us, Markey & and Markey’s participants also used similar adjectives to describe their own personality and the personality of their ideal romantic partners. 15 On the other hand, Thomas et al (Thomas et al.) carried out a sociocultural-based experiment by studying the cross-cultural mate preferences between men and women in the East and West. A total of 2587 participants were sampled and recorded from seven academic institutions spanning 5 different countries - Australia, Norway, Malaysia, Singapore, and the United Kingdom. The procedure consisted of splitting the top 10 countries represented by participants within the sample into two different groups for cross-cultural comparison. The first group was labeled the “Western” group and contained European or European-influenced countries like Australia, Norway, the United States of America, and New Zealand. The “Eastern” group on the other hand was composed of Eastern countries like Malaysia, Singapore, and China. Next, each participant was given “mate dollars” to allocate to 8 traits - kindness, good financial prospects, humor, creativity, chastity, physical attractiveness, and religiosity to create three long-term hypothetical partners. This allocation process was repeated 3 times each using low ($16), medium ($32), and high ($48) mate dollar conditions. The results concluded that the two culture groups differed in almost every single trait while allocating using the low-budget condition. For example, men and women in the Eastern and Western groups both prioritized kindness and physical attractiveness. However, both sexes in the Eastern group prioritized good financial prospects and religiosity compared to both sexes prioritizing humor in the Western group. These findings are strongly indicative of cultural differences in mate preference between Western and Eastern countries. In combination with the cognitive psychology similarity-attraction model, these results provide additional external factors to attraction that are not purely biological. 16 The findings by Fiore and Donath (Fiore and Donath) support both Markey and Markey and Thomas et al’s study. Fiore and Donath’s study on dating sites concluded that users showed a preference for people who were highly similar in multiple categories, which supports the cognitive similarity-attraction model. Furthermore, Fiore and Donath also found that some individuals prioritized certain categories over others, which draws parallel to the findings of Thomas et al. Additionally, Thomas et al’s study is further supported by Li et al (Li et al.), who found that mate preferences can predict actual mate choices and also the formation of interpersonal attraction. 17 Discussion An issue with the biological approach is its reductionist and atomistic nature because it only examines specific psychological aspects through the sole lens of physiological influences, some of which are out of our control, and research in this field often ignores external influences that are not biological, such as human cognition and environmental factors that may also affect our behavior. The same could be said about all the respective studies used in this essay to examine the roles of neurotransmitters and hormones in influencing interpersonal attraction. Another problem that persists throughout these studies is the use of questionnaires to provide an operational definition of love because it cannot be measured directly. Questionnaires are dependent on self-reported data, which are vulnerable to interpretation bias and demand characteristics such as the social desirability effect. Additionally, a few weaknesses that permeate many studies within the biological field are its correlational nature, high artificiality, and low ecological validity. For example, the effects of dopamine in Fisher et al, and testosterone in Ronay and Von Hippel are not independent variables that are manipulated. These two studies manipulate external variables such as humans to test the presence of a cerebral activation or hormone to draw deterministic conclusions based on correlational relationships that are not causal. Furthermore, the laboratory conditions and environment within Fisher et al and Theodoridou et al are well-controlled with high internal validity, but low ecological validity and mundane realism. It is very difficult to transfer the image rating-based process in Fisher et al and Theodoridou et al into real-life contexts to measure levels of attraction because physical interactions such as touch and communication with the partner are entirely omitted within both studies while such factors already play a role in basic communication (Wrench et al.). 18 Conversely, Ronay and Von Hippel’s naturalistic study helps reinforce the biological argument of attraction due to high ecological validity, where the settings of the experimental context and behaviors are applicable to real life due to lesser laboratory constraints compared to Fisher et al and Theodoridou et al while still controlling for important factors such as experimenter attractiveness and time of experimentation. Naturalistic conditions and high ecological validity are important aspects that should be implemented in all three psychological fields to attraction because it is incredibly difficult to single out a specific independent variable such as neurotransmitters or cognitive patterns even under situations with extremely high internal validity. Hence, the naturalistic conditions of a study aid in partially mimicking contextual factors within the real world, making the findings of a study less artificial and more transferable and applicable to daily life. For example, Thomas et al’s study has high ecological validity because the constrained budget method is reflective of real-life mate choice where tradeoffs for certain characteristics are considered during an overall evaluation of a person and mate selection. 19 Conclusion Based on the 5 studies described and evaluated above, the extent to which biological factors play a role in interpersonal attraction still requires additional research due to the lack of definitive evidence from neurotransmitters, hormones, and even the broader research of genetics. Although biopsychology studies were better controlled and used relatively more scientific methods compared to cognitive and sociocultural studies, biological factors alone cannot provide a determinative explanation for human attraction due to their complex and multidimensional nature. Furthermore, biopsychology’s use of scientific methodology is not enough to justify a greater relevance than sociocultural or cognitive psychology because neither of the three fields has disproved the other using empirical research. In conclusion, the effects of dopamine, testosterone, and oxytocin investigated by Fisher et al, Ronay and Von Hippel, and Theodoridou et al, and the similarity-attraction model and the culture-dependent preference for partner characteristics explored by Markey and Markey and Thomas et al seem to work in unison to facilitate interpersonal attraction. Future investigation into this field could aim to establish a causal effect between the behavior of attraction and an independent variable to reduce the presence of bidirectional ambiguity and correlational explanations and utilize more natural settings/conditions to increase ecological validity and the applicability of findings to real-life situations, because from this essay, it seems that the formation of attraction is a combination of all three cognitive, sociocultural, and biological fields. But as of now, the presence of cognitive and sociocultural factors in psychology implies that we still have partial free will over whom we love and associate with, and human relationships are not determined by a system of polypeptides and amino acids. 20 21 Works Cited American Psychological Association. “Cognitive Psychology Explores Our Mental Processes.” Apa.org, Jan. 2014, www.apa.org/education-career/guide/subfields/brain-science. Bartels, Andreas, and Semir Zeki. “The Neural Basis of Romantic Love.” NeuroReport, vol. 11, no. 17, 27 Nov. 2000, p. 3829, journals.lww.com/neuroreport/Fulltext/2000/11270/The_neural_basis_of_romantic_love.46.aspx -laysource=BBC. Accessed 11 Nov. 2023. Baumgartner, Thomas, et al. “Oxytocin Shapes the Neural Circuitry of Trust and Trust Adaptation in Humans.” Neuron, vol. 58, no. 4, May 2008, pp. 639–650, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2008.04.009. Bosch, Oliver J., and Larry J. Young. “Oxytocin and Social Relationships: From Attachment to Bond Disruption.” Behavioral Pharmacology of Neuropeptides: Oxytocin, 2017, pp. 97–117, https://doi.org/10.1007/7854_2017_10. Accessed 20 Jan. 2021. Cai, Jing, and Qingchun Tong. “Anatomy and Function of Ventral Tegmental Area Glutamate Neurons.” Frontiers in Neural Circuits, vol. 16, no. 867053, 20 May 2022, https://doi.org/10.3389/fncir.2022.867053. Cleveland Clinic. “Neurotransmitters: What They Are, Functions & Types.” Cleveland Clinic, 14 Mar. 2022, my.clevelandclinic.org/health/articles/22513-neurotransmitters. Driscoll, Margaret E., et al. “Neuroanatomy, Nucleus Caudate.” PubMed, StatPearls Publishing, 2021, www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK557407/#:~:text=The%20caudate%20nucleus%20functions% 20not. 22 encyclopedia. “Similarity/Attraction Theory | Encyclopedia.com.” Encyclopedia.com, 2000, www.encyclopedia.com/social-sciences/applied-and-social-sciencesmagazines/similarityattraction-theory. Fiore, Andrew T., and Judith S. Donath. “Homophily in Online Dating.” CHI ’05 Extended Abstracts on Human Factors in Computing Systems - CHI ’05, Apr. 2005, https://doi.org/10.1145/1056808.1056919. Fisher, Helen, et al. “Romantic Love: An FMRI Study of a Neural Mechanism for Mate Choice.” The Journal of Comparative Neurology, vol. 493, no. 1, 2005, pp. 58–62, https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.20772. Hatfield, Elaine, and Susan Sprecher. The Passionate Love Scale. 1986. Juárez Olguín, Hugo, et al. “The Role of Dopamine and Its Dysfunction as a Consequence of Oxidative Stress.” Oxidative Medicine and Cellular Longevity, vol. 2016, no. 1, 2016, pp. 1–13, https://doi.org/10.1155/2016/9730467. Kosfeld, Michael, et al. “Oxytocin Increases Trust in Humans.” Nature, vol. 435, no. 7042, June 2005, pp. 673–676, https://doi.org/10.1038/nature03701. LeWine, Howard E. “Testosterone — What It Does and Doesn’t Do.” Harvard Health, 16 July 2015, www.health.harvard.edu/staying-healthy/testosterone--what-it-does-and-doesnt-do. Li, Norman P., et al. “Mate Preferences Do Predict Attraction and Choices in the Early Stages of Mate Selection.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, vol. 105, no. 5, Nov. 2013, pp. 757– 776, https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033777. Lovering, Nancy. “The Psychology of Love: Theories and Facts.” Psych Central, 20 Sept. 2022, psychcentral.com/relationships/the-psychology-of-love. 23 Luo, Shanhong, and Guangjian Zhang. “What Leads to Romantic Attraction: Similarity, Reciprocity, Security, or Beauty? Evidence from a Speed-Dating Study.” Journal of Personality, vol. 77, no. 4, Aug. 2009, pp. 933–964, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2009.00570.x. Markey, Patrick M., and Charlotte N. Markey. “Romantic Ideals, Romantic Obtainment, and Relationship Experiences.” Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, vol. 24, no. 4, Aug. 2007, pp. 517–533, https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407507079241. Mashek, D., Aron, A., & Fisher, H. (2000). Identifying, evoking, and measuring intense feelings of romantic love. Representative Research in Social Psychology, 24, 48–55. Mcleod, Saul. “Social Identity Theory - an Overview | ScienceDirect Topics.” Www.sciencedirect.com, 5 Oct. 2023, www.sciencedirect.com/topics/social-sciences/social-identity-theory. Nicholson, Jeremy. “Does Similarity Lead to Attraction and Compatibility? | Psychology Today.” Www.psychologytoday.com, 26 Sept. 2017, www.psychologytoday.com/intl/blog/the-attractiondoctor/201709/does-similarity-lead-attraction-and-compatibility. Riem, Madelon M.E., et al. “Does Intranasal Oxytocin Promote Prosocial Behavior to an Excluded Fellow Player? A Randomized-Controlled Trial with Cyberball.” Psychoneuroendocrinology, vol. 38, no. 8, Aug. 2013, pp. 1418–1425, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.12.023. Accessed 20 May 2020. Ronay, Richard, and William von Hippel. “The Presence of an Attractive Woman Elevates Testosterone and Physical Risk Taking in Young Men.” Social Psychological and Personality Science, vol. 1, no. 1, Jan. 2010, pp. 57–64, https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550609352807. Singh, Ramadhar, et al. “On the Importance of Trust in Interpersonal Attraction from Attitude Similarity.” Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, vol. 32, no. 6, 23 Mar. 2015, pp. 829– 850, https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407515576993. 24 Theodoridou, Angeliki, et al. “Oxytocin and Social Perception: Oxytocin Increases Perceived Facial Trustworthiness and Attractiveness.” Hormones and Behavior, vol. 56, no. 1, June 2009, pp. 128–132, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2009.03.019. Thomas, Andrew, et al. Mate Preference Priorities in the East and West: A Cross-Cultural Mate Preference Priorities in the East and West: A Cross-Cultural Test of the Mate Preference Priority Model Test of the Mate Preference Priority Model Citation Citation. 2019. Wagemans, Johan. “Cognitive Psychology.” ScienceDirect, Elsevier, 1 Jan. 2005, www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/abs/pii/B0123693985004801. Walum, H., et al. “Genetic Variation in the Vasopressin Receptor 1a Gene (AVPR1A) Associates with Pair-Bonding Behavior in Humans.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, vol. 105, no. 37, 2 Sept. 2008, pp. 14153–14156, https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0803081105. Accessed 12 Apr. 2020. Watson, Stephanie. “Oxytocin: The Love Hormone.” Harvard Health, 2021, www.health.harvard.edu/mind-and-mood/oxytocin-the-love-hormone. Waude, Adam. “Cultural Influences and the Sociocultural Approach.” Psychologistworld.com, Psychologist World, 3 Feb. 2016, www.psychologistworld.com/cognitive/learning/socioculturalapproach-vygotsky-zone-proximal-development. Wedekind, Claus, et al. “MHC-Dependent Mate Preferences in Humans.” Proceedings: Biological Sciences, vol. 260, no. 1359, 1995, pp. 245–249, www.jstor.org/stable/pdf/50182.pdf?refreqid=fastlydefault%3A52fba826fbbce39021478c7dea1bc075&ab_segments=0%2Fbasic_search_gsv2%2Fc ontrol&origin=&initiator=&acceptTC=1. Accessed 17 Oct. 2023. 25 Wiggins, Jerry S. “APA PsycNet.” Psycnet.apa.org, 1979, psycnet.apa.org/record/1980-23392-001. Accessed 11 Nov. 2023. Wilke, Andreas, et al. “Is Risk Taking Used as a Cue in Mate Choice?” Evolutionary Psychology, vol. 4, no. 1, Jan. 2006, p. 147470490600400, https://doi.org/10.1177/147470490600400130. Wrench, Jason S., et al. “Chapter 5: Nonverbal Communication.” Milnepublishing.geneseo.edu, milnepublishing.geneseo.edu/interpersonalcommunication/chapter/5/#:~:text=These%20categori es%20include%20haptics%20(touch. Accessed 15 Nov. 2023. Zald, D. H. “Dopamine Transmission in the Human Striatum during Monetary Reward Tasks.” Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 24, no. 17, 28 Apr. 2004, pp. 4105–4112, www.jneurosci.org/content/24/17/4105, https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.4643-03.2004. 26