See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/263916935 Elaboration Likelihood Model: The cognitive route to advertising effect Article in International Journal of Management and Social Sciences · January 2006 CITATIONS READS 0 1,167 All content following this page was uploaded by Esha Patnaik on 28 December 2020. The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file. Elaboration Likelihood Model: The Cognitive Route to Advertising Effect Esha Patnaik VOLUME 1, NUMBER 1 INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF THE INTERDISCIPLINARY SOCIAL SCIENCES http://www.SocialSciences-Journal.com First published in 2006 in Melbourne, Australia by Common Ground Publishing Pty Ltd www.CommonGroundPublishing.com. © 2006 (this paper), the author(s) © 2006 (selection and editorial matter) Common Ground Authors are responsible for the accuracy of citations, quotations, diagrams, tables and maps. All rights reserved. Apart from fair use for the purposes of study, research, criticism or review as permitted under the Copyright Act (Australia), no part of this work may be reproduced without written permission from the publisher. For permissions and other inquiries, please contact <cg-support@commongroundpublishing.com>. ISSN: 1833-1882 Publisher Site: http://www.SocialSciences-Journal.com The INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF THE INTERDISCIPLINARY SOCIAL SCIENCES is a peer refereed journal. Full papers submitted for publication are refereed by Associate Editors through anonymous referee processes. Typeset in Common Ground Markup Language using CGCreator multichannel typesetting system http://www.CommonGroundSoftware.com. Elaboration Likelihood Model: The Cognitive Route to Advertising Effect Esha Patnaik, Indian Institute of Management, Ahmedabad, India Abstract: The paper examines the use of the Elaboration Likelihood Model in the field of advertising. A review of prior research in the field considers the scope for application of the model to advertising. Two approaches based on this model classical conditioning and cognitive response theory – that are used in developing ads are elaborated upon. The need for further research to bring out a clear relationship between some of these principles and the effectiveness of advertising is also discussed. Keywords: Advertising, Psychology, Elaboration Likelihood Model, Classical Conditioning, Cognitive Response Model Introduction HE EFFECTIVENESS OF an advertisement (advertisement and ad are used interchangeably in this paper) is measured in terms of its ability to create, sustain, and/or increase the market of the product advertised. Central to this function is the persuasiveness of the ad to develop a positive attitude in the audience towards the advertised product. Thus, understanding of how information in ads is received and interpreted by the audience becomes crucial to effective advertising. Within this psychological framework, the Elaboration Likelihood Model (henceforth ELM) of communication processing provides a referral point for understanding how information from the environment is processed cognitively. Discussing ELM in the advertising context, this paper reviews prior research on the application of ELM in advertising. Based on this understanding, two cognitive approaches – the cognitive response model and classical conditioning – are elaborated upon for their role in effective advertising. T Elaboration Likelihood Model ELM (Petty, Cacioppo and Schumann, 1983) is a theory of communication processing (Hennessey and Anderson, 1990) that explains the process of persuasion and attitude formation (Jones, 2005). The degree to which a piece of information will be processed depends on the elaboration likelihood of the information. A widely studied model in persuasion research, ELM outlines conditions under which persuasion occurs through regulation of the messages and the message environment available to the individual (Karson and Korgaonkar, 2001) and the individual factors that govern information processing. In an advertising context, an ad functions to highlight a product or service. The probability of the ad being noticed and drawing the attention of the audience will determine to what extent the message will be processed or elaborated upon. This again is contingent upon the individual’s ability (Petty et al., 1983), motivation (Sen, 2003), and opportunity to elaborate (Cho, 1999) on the message. Elaboration will also depend on the context and knowledge of the ad and product background. Thus, based on the degree of processing and nature of cues present in the ad, the model proposes two cognitive routes to persuasion - the central route and the peripheral route. These two routes are based on the nature of cognitive processes that occur at the time of persuasion. The central cognitive process focuses on analysis of product-related information or logical arguments. Peripheral cognition relies more on peripheral cues such as environmental and affective or emotional cues in the persuasion context rather than on message content (Hennessey and Anderson, 1990). The more the likelihood of elaboration of message details, greater the chances of central processing gaining importance over peripheral routes (Whittler and Spira, 2002; Petty et al., 1983). Thus, message-based persuasion works via the central route while noncontent cues such as details in the message environment become more prominent in peripheral processing (Darley and Smith, 1993). The central route is an active and conscious cognitive process to evaluate a persuasive argument (Whittler and Spira, 2002). The individual extensively processes relevant information in the message (Jones, 2005) and persuasion occurs only when the message information integrates with existing belief structures (Agostinelli and Grube, 2002). When there are too many messages in the environment, or the INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF THE INTERDISCIPLINARY SOCIAL SCIENCES, VOLUME 1, NUMBER 1, 2006 http://www.SocialSciences-Journal.com, ISSN 1833-1882 © Common Ground, Esha Patnaik, All Rights Reserved, Permissions: cg-support@commongroundpublishing.com 134 INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF THE INTERDISCIPLINARY SOCIAL SCIENCES, VOLUME 1 scope for cognitive elaboration (Hausknecht and Moore, 1986) or message-related thinking is limited (Karson and Korgaonkar, 2001) people are unable or not motivated to (Haugtvedt, Petty, and Cacioppo, 1992) carefully and thoroughly analyse every message that they come across. They then use the peripheral route to channel information. In this route to persuasion the emphasis is not so much on reasoning, contents and merits of the message as on simple associations with situational factors, such as the affective component of the ad (Agostinelli and Grube, 2002). For example, advertisers explore the ‘feelgood’ factor by introducing some component in the ad that generates the feeling of ‘I am worth it’ in the potential customer. Ads for high-end or life-style products may employ such a technique (Sen, 2003) by playing upon the emotion of the customer, rather than offering a rationale as to how that product or service actually adds to or enhances the standard of living. Involvement plays a crucial moderating role in determining the elaboration component and route to persuasion (Karson and Korgaonkar, 2001; Cole, Ettenson, Reinke, and Schrader, 1990). Involvement refers to the association with or interest that an individual has in the ad or the product. It has three properties – intensity, direction and persistence (Andrews, Durvasula and Akhter, 1990): the degree of involvement, whether it is positive or negative, and its endurance, all impact the ad effect by influencing motivation and the ability to process the product qualities (Hennessey and Anderson, 1990). In high involvement an issue or product is evaluated based on its true merit as there is greater exploration of the environment for product-relevant information. Information is then processed via the central route. However, when involvement is low (Swasy and Munch, 1985) or the issue does not involve extensive product-relevant searching, there is more reliance on affective components and information processing is done via the peripheral route. Interestingly, in high involvement there is a greater chance of the message being rejected since it is more likely to come in for more detailed scrutiny (Petty et al., 1983). There is more resistance to the persuasion attempt. Examining the role of strength of message in information processing, Hennessey and Anderson (1990) found that the strength of the argument influenced the degree of positive response of involved individuals than of those who were less involved, while peripheral or affective cues influenced low-involvement individuals more than highinvolvement individuals. Strong arguments or message content can generate negative impact in individuals who have low involvement. The initial concept of involvement as a determinant in the selection of the route to persuasion (Petty et al., 1983) did not differentiate amongst the various dimensions of involvement that come into play. Cole et al. (1990), found different individuals have different involvement functions, such as, situation involvement, issue involvement, advertisement involvement, and product involvement. Thus, high involvement in the ad by itself may not necessarily be an indicator of high product involvement. This may explain why some ads become extremely popular without the effect getting translated into increased interest in the product. An individual’s need for cognition also governs the choice of route for information processing (Inman, McAlister and Hoyer, 1990). High need-forcognition individuals are intrinsically motivated to engage in cognitive activities and hence are more likely to use the central route (Haugtvedt et al., 1992). Low need-for-cognition individuals are more easily affected by promotional components rather than product qualities (Haugtvedt and Petty, 1989) and thus, use the peripheral route to cognition. They use less complex decision rules to decide whether or not to get persuaded by ad messages. High needfor-cognition individuals respond more positively to strong arguments than to weak arguments (Haugtvedt et al., 1992). According to Whittler and Spira (2002), for such individuals, bias for or against the advertising translates into corresponding attitudes towards the product. Looking at environmental factors, situational variables like the message environment and message source or endorser play significant roles in the choice of route to persuasion (Haugtvedt and Petty, 1989; Petty et al., 1983) and determining the message outcome (Whittler and Spira, 2002). When the ad is for a product of low involvement for the consumer, affective components such as the celebrity status of the product endorsers or source characteristics (Whittler and Spira, 2002) are most effective in bringing about an attitude change (Inman et al., 1990). An attractive and/or credible spokesperson may be sufficient to draw a more favourable response from the audience as compared with a less attractive presenter, without logical evaluation of the message argument (Agostinelli and Grube, 2002). Strong message features create high personal relevance for the audience and increase the chances of central processing. Cole et al., (1990) found the effect of an expert source heightened even in situations of high involvement. In cases of moderate involvement, situational factors like source credibility enhance the effect of the ad (Agostinelli and Grube, 2002) by generating the motivation to think and actively explore the ad message (Brinol, Petty and Tormala, 2004). Attitude change through the central route is said to be more enduring and predictive of future purchase ESHA PATNAIK behaviour (Jones, 2005; Darley and Smith, 1993; Miniard, Sirdeshmukh and Innis, 1992; Petty et al., 1983). There is cognition, learning and logical processing, and integration of product-related information with one’s overall belief and cognitive structure. Considering that attitude towards ads is affected by deep-seated belief patterns, it is all the more important for advertisers to pay attention to what to say and how to say it (Darley and Smith, 1993). The product description used in the ad claim is a good indicator of how best to frame the message. Selecting the right attributes to associate with the brand in the ad is crucial in predicting the likely impact on the audience (Darley and Smith, 1993). The relationship or independence of the two routes to persuasion is yet to be clearly determined. The interplay of the two routes has been brought out in several studies. Ads with affective components have demonstrated equal recall value as those with rational information (Gorn, 1982). Examination of the effectiveness of banner ads on the web (Shamdasani, Stanaland and Tan, 2001) revealed that high source credibility reduced counterargument and enhanced the chances of the message being accepted uncritically or with minimum evaluation and bringing about an attitude change. However, this occurred when the website content was congruent with the advertised material. Relevance, rather than just reputation of the website on which the ad is placed, was important in determining the ad effect. Over-emphasis on factual presentation of product details overlooks the influence of situational variables in the persuasion process, including cases of high involvement. Some researchers (Miniard et al., 1992) propound an inverse relationship between the degree of involvement and effect of peripheral cues. In contrast, examining the impact of alcohol counteradvertising, Agostinelli and Grube (2002) discovered that even when the message in counter-advertising was factually correct, heavy drinkers found ads for alcohol more desirable. Whether a cue will act as a central or a peripheral cue or will be rejected altogether depends on a host of factors such as the product, individual, situation, and the ad (Cole et al., 1990). A peripheral cue for one may be a central cue for another. There is no fixed rule to determine which is a central cue and which one is peripheral. The quality of argument can strengthen the ad even in the absence of relevant product information. ELM is not a unidirectional model where the peripheral route is used only when the central route is not applicable or as a secondary solution. In cases of moderate involvement, both central and peripheral routes may be functional (Karson and Korgaonkar, 2001). Examining the apparently mutually exclusive nature of central and peripheral processing, Lord, Lee and Sauer (1995) offer a combined influence hypothesis which holds that attitude towards an ad, and subsequently towards the product, is a function of the combined effects of central and peripheral cues. Exposure to ads is not always by choice. People are generally involuntarily exposed to ads in various media even when they may not be actively seeking information. Introducing affective components or peripheral cues that generate a positive attitude in the potential consumer towards the ad (Gorn, 1982) can effectively translate into positive evaluation of the product/brand advertised. Two psychological principles that closely represent the routes to persuasion propounded by ELM are the cognitive response model and classical conditioning. These have been the subject of much research in the context of advertising. The next two sections discuss the two principles for their role in information processing and determining advertising effectiveness. Cognitive Response Model The central route to persuasion has been effectively represented through the cognitive response model. According to this model, attitude towards an ad impacts attitude towards the brand through development of brand cognitions (Gresham and Shimp, 1985). The model is an attempt to understand how cognitive responses in the form of acquiring and changing attitudes towards persuasive messages take place as a result of information processing by the brain. The concept of ‘cognitive response analysis’ was used (Love and Greenwald, 1978) to explain the viewing of the communication recipient as an active processor in a persuasive communication environment. Attitude is an enduring favourable or unfavourable position on an issue or subject. Cognitive responses and attitudes are highly correlated and influence each other - cognitive response leads to the formation of attitudes (favourable or unfavourable) and attitudes are responsible for how new information is processed. Studies have found that favourable thoughts elicited by a communication correlate positively with attitude change, whereas unfavourable thoughts generated by a communication show a strong negative relationship with persuasion (Hastak and Olson, 1989; Love and Greenwald, 1978). Along with the amount and the direction or valence of thoughts generated in response to an ad, the confidence people have in their thoughts can also influence the ad effect (Brinol et al., 2004). This is apparent in cases of high involvement where people are actively involved in information processing and evaluation. The greater one’s confidence in one’s thoughts, the higher is their impact on attitudes. In cases of high involvement, even source credibility can enhance the effect of 135 136 INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF THE INTERDISCIPLINARY SOCIAL SCIENCES, VOLUME 1 thought confidence through the process of self-validation. Retention of the message is considered an important component in attitude change. Presumably, the more a person retains a message, the more persuasion effect there will be. However, studies show that acceptance, rather than retention, of content governs the effectiveness of persuasive messages. Love and Greenwald (1978) found no necessary relationship between communication retention and persuasion. They emphasized on the message recipient’s understanding and interpretation of the message content and product information or the evaluative content as determinant of what and how much of the message is remembered for later recall. Repetition, an important factor in retention, has also not found much support in research findings for its effectiveness in bringing about a positive attitude change. Maximum attitude change may be expected during the first few exposures to the ad, but then a diminishing effect may set in (Belch, 1982). Repeated exposure in a short span or high exposure conditions can lead to the onset of tedium and effects can range from no positive change in attitude to more negative evaluations, as compared with exposure spread over a longer period. Such findings raise questions about the efficacy of closely spaced repetition of ads during programme telecasts on television, as is a common practice with advertisers. Thus, it is not just memory, but the quality of memory in terms of elaborations, associations and inferences that will govern attitude towards the ad, and consequently, towards the product. Rather than a mechanistic stimulus-response model based on repetition and retention, ad effect is the result of the combined effect of prior associations, learning and cognitive sets regarding the product and the product category (Weilbacher, 2003). Exploring the role of individual factors in attitudes towards ads, Sudman and Schwarz (1989) highlight the role of moods in the development of positive or negative evaluations. An individual is more likely to recollect positive information while in a good mood and negative information when in a bad mood. Translating this into ad effect, if the person is exposed to the ad while in a good mood, the positives of the ad and ad message are more likely to be noticed, while there is likely to be more negative evaluation if the person is in a bad mood. The result is selective memory of ad and product attributes. The implications of such a phenomenon for repetition of ads across a variety of situations need to be examined in greater detail. Does repetition neutralise affective responses, or does it strengthen the initial emotion generated by the ad? Also, some individuals have a consistent tendency to react more strongly than others to emotion-provoking situations (Moore, Harris and Chen, 1994). Such individuals will experience bore- dom faster in the absence of positive emotions, as also be more tolerant of repetition if the ad appeals to their positive emotions. This finding counters the widely used advertising strategy of identifying a target segment as a homogenous group that reacts in a similar fashion to an ad. Consumers make a conscious effort to attend to the advertisement, and search for relevant information before making a selection regarding the product or service (Weilbacher, 2003). They are assumed to be interested in actively thinking and elaborating on incoming information (Petty et al., 1983). While Darley and Smith (1993) consider factual messages to be more effective for central persuasion, they also warn against the generalisation that objective claims are always more effective than subjective elements in the ad. An individual cannot be expected to be constantly processing the multitude of advertising information broadcast through various media. There is likely to be selection / rejection of messages from the environment depending on the degree of involvement. People will be more motivated to put in the cognitive effort required to process information if their involvement is high (Petty et al., 1983). Highlighting the tangible, rather than the intangible, product attributes in the ad increases the likelihood of central processing (Darley and Smith, 1993). At the same time, in a highly competitive scenario where the audience is constantly bombarded with ads for competing products that may not have much differentiating attributes, advertisers have to look beyond “what the brand is at a transactional level, but what it can be made or seem to be in the mind of the consumer” (Weilbacher, 2003). Adding another dimension to this discussion, McCracken (1987) emphasizes on the cultural context in which ads are interpreted. People seek symbolisms or meanings that they can identify within the ad – meanings of what it implies to hold a certain position in life, to belong to a group, and so on. The manner in which the audience relates with the ad, depending on their context, associations and involvement (Cacioppo and Petty, 1989) will determine the ad effect. Also, some prior knowledge about the product teamed with moderate exposure is likely to have the maximum impact in terms of bringing about a positive attitude change. The cognitive associations generated by the ad should be rich enough to engage the audience in further exploration. This will trigger the central route to persuasion, leading to logical evaluation of product information. Classical Conditioning Classical conditioning has been identified as one approach to explain the peripheral route to persuasion (Allen and Madden, 1985). It has been widely studied ESHA PATNAIK in the context of development of attitude towards an ad and, consequently, towards the product. According to this theory offered by Pavlov, a person can be trained to respond (conditioned response) to a previously neutral stimulus (unconditioned stimulus) by pairing it with another relevant stimulus (conditioned stimulus). In his classic experiment, Pavlov trained a dog to salivate to the sound of a bell by pairing the sound with the availability of food in close temporal contiguity (McSweeney and Bierley, 1984; Reed Jr, 1975). The same principle of conditioning is applied in advertising. External stimuli that elicit positive emotions are paired with the product so that the product itself elicits a favourable reaction. When a new product for which people might have neutral feelings is advertised repeatedly during a popular sporting event, it is possible for the product to eventually generate excitement on its own, through repeated pairings with the event. Where the advertised product or service by itself may not have generated an affective response, transference of feelings occurs because of emotional components associated with the paired stimulus. A noted study on the role of classical conditioning in advertising effectiveness was conducted by Gorn (1982). He found that attitudes towards a product may be developed by associating it with other liked (or disliked) stimuli in the commercial. Clubbing an advertised product with liked music in an experimental set-up, he observed that the product was selected a greater number of times than when it was clubbed with music that was not much liked. He concluded that some of the feelings associated with the music got transferred to the product during the pairing. Thus, music, colour, and stimuli with emotional connotations in the ad may evoke corresponding emotions in consumers when paired with the product being advertised. Researchers have obtained varying results regarding the effect of prior familiarity with the unconditioned stimulus on the advertising effect. Allen and Madden (1985) argue that the unconditioned stimulus must alter the subjects’ affective or feeling state to be able to have a positive impact. Positive associations with the unconditioned stimulus produce positive emotions regarding the product. It triggers certain memories and perceptual cues that are transferred to the product being paired with it. Countering the belief that pairing with a liked unconditioned stimulus automatically leads to positive attitudes towards the advertised product (conditioned stimulus), McSweeney and Bierley (1984) consider the novelty factor as instrumental in classical conditioning. Familiarity with the unconditioned stimulus reduces the likelihood of conditioning. Similarly, familiarity and prior association with the product outside the paired situation in the ad will affect the strength of the pairing and reduce the effect of classical conditioning (Gresham and Shimp, 1985) due to latent inhibition (Stuart, Shimp, and Engle, 1987; McSweeney and Bierley, 1984). The use of novel unconditioned and/or conditioned stimulus increases the chances of a positive mental association. For example, if the stimulus being paired with the product is extremely attractive or popular, it might overshadow the product, and not help towards increasing brand awareness among consumers (McSweeney and Bierley, 1984). According to Blair and Shimp (1992), when a product in an ad is paired with music that has prior unpleasant association, the ad effect is less favourable than when the audience has no prior associations and is exposed to the music only as part of the pairing. They suggest a conservative approach of using relatively unfamiliar music as background to negate the effect of prior associations and maximise the impact of classical conditioning. As discussed earlier in this paper, the mood of the subject has an impact on his/her perception of the ad, and this mood can be affected by the nature of stimulus paired with the product. In instances of low involvement, negative, neutral and positive emotive cues exerted a progressively enhancing influence on attitudes towards ads. Using background music as an affective moderator for measuring brand attitude formation, Bozman and Mueling (1994) found that in cases of high involvement, music as a neutral emotive cue was less favourable than as a negative or a positive cue. Compared with this, another study (Englis and Pennell, 1994) showed that meaningful music used in the background interfered in the interpretation of the ad message due to affective associations with the music in the minds of the audience. The use of a favourite music for advertising purposes can also be resented, affecting the individual’s perception of the product. Brooker and Wheatley (1994) demonstrated that ads using music depend more on the affective, rather than the cognitive, component to influence consumers. There is more concentration on the mood generated rather than information exploration and hence, lower information recall. Music with faster tempo creates more cheerful feelings, though that might affect information recall. While Sudman and Schwarz (1989), as discussed in the earlier section, found positive or negative moods to translate into corresponding evaluations of ads, Brooker and Wheatley’s study demonstrates low transference of the effects of a positive mood to positive ad evaluation. They also found music was more effective when used in the introductory part as a lead-in to the product information than when used in the background, since the latter interfered in information processing. The effects of classical conditioning can be expected to be manifested only after several exposures to 137 138 INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF THE INTERDISCIPLINARY SOCIAL SCIENCES, VOLUME 1 the pairing and the effects cannot be expected to last indefinitely even after the pairing has been stopped (McSweeney and Bierley, 1984). Empirical evidence also questions the assumption that repeated exposure to advertising of a product paired with an unconditioned stimulus generates more positive response towards the product (Allen and Madden, 1985). Awareness is another point of contention in explaining classical conditioning in advertising. Proving that an ad effect is the result of classical conditioning is difficult as the audience may not be aware of the likely cause for their ad preference (Gorn, 1982). Self-reports are unlikely to provide evidence in support of classical conditioning. According to Reed Jr (1975), the result of classical conditioning is not due to any rational association between cause and effect but the “subliminal” effect of pairing the advertised product with a desired satisfaction of a need such as love or acceptance. Researchers are divided over the degree of conscious effort involved in classical conditioning. Allen and Janiszewski (1989) did not find conditioning happening without awareness. In classical conditioning the unconditioned stimulus is expected to generate anticipation for the product through repeated pairing, leading the audience to predict and actively seek product information. What is also assumed to be classical conditioning may be the effect of repeated exposure (Gorn, 1982). In the former there is the involvement of affect, so that even when there is repeated exposure the person will not like an ad if there is some negative affect involved. Rossiter and Percy (1980) propose the role of visual imagery as an unconditioned stimulus in ad persuasion by creating positive associations in the mind of the audience. A dual loop of verbal belief and visual imagery creates a classical conditioning situation that generates a positive attitude effect. Classical conditioning also works for negative imagery by creating associations of what to avoid. Gresham and Shimp (1985) found classical conditioning to work better with television ads, which are more emotionally evocative, than with print ads, and for products that are relatively new, as compared with established brands. The pairing up of motion and sound with visual appeal adds to the intensity of the conditioning variable in television ads. Inappropriate pairing can have a detrimental effect on the attitude towards the ad. If the stimulus being paired with the product is not congruent with the product qualities, it can actually turn people away from trying the good or service, or cause them to consider other alternatives. The sequencing of the unconditioned and the conditioned stimuli is to be correctly planned. Under laboratory conditions, forward conditioning (unconditioned stimulus before conditioned stimulus) has been found to be more effective than backward conditioning (Stuart et al, 1987). Highlighting the importance of temporal positioning of conditioned and unconditioned stimuli, McSweeney and Bierley (1984) discuss that while traditional classical conditioning theories emphasize on the prior temporal positioning of the conditioned stimulus before the unconditioned stimulus for maximum effect, developments in the field stress on the predictive nature of the conditioned stimulus and not just its temporal positioning for classical conditioning to occur. The effectiveness of classical conditioning in advertising is still a moot point. While laboratory conditions appear to support its role (McSweeney and Bierley, 1984), not much empirical consumer research is available on whether choice of product can be classically conditioned (Gorn, 1982). Many of the experiments on classical conditioning have been done in the laboratory on non-human subjects. Though the approach offers a plausible explanation of how ad effect occurs, the role of higher order cognition and awareness is not clearly brought out (McSweeney and Bierley, 1984). While the method may be used to grab initial customer attention, the ability of classical conditioning to bring about a favourable perception of the advertised product needs further validation. It also raises another question - is classical conditioning more effective in case of limited availability of information regarding the product, or would similar results be noticed even when subjects had detailed information regarding product qualities and attributes? Logical analysis of product attributes might change one’s perceptions formed on the basis of an affective state of mind. Taking this further, Allen and Madden (1985) stress on the affective rather than the cognitive role of classical conditioning in advertising impact. Examining rival views as to whether classical conditioning involves cognitive or affective components, Kim, Allen and Kardes (1996) find both to have a role to play in the process. Rather than an either-or situation, they complement each other during conditioning. It generates interest and encourages the audience to seek additional product information. To this end, classical conditioning plays a dual role of affectively influencing the individual for engaging in cognitive information-seeking behaviour. The apparent effect of classical conditioning makes it an effective advertising tool (Grossman and Till, 1998), particularly in the absence of more cognitive appeals. Then again, positive or negative associations with the ad may or may not translate into corresponding feelings for the product. In the words of Gresham and Shimp (1985), “what is termed as classical conditioning is open to alternate explanations”. It is debatable if a complex process like choosing a product based on advertising can be so ESHA PATNAIK easily explained by classical conditioning (McSweeney and Bierley, 1984). While experimental evidence supports the possibility, there is need for more real life examples that classical conditioning actually works in positively influencing consumer preference. Scope for Research Many early studies appear to form a simple causeeffect relationship between information processing and formation of attitudes towards ads, and consequently towards the product. The role of other mediating variables has to be examined both in the context of and independent of ELM, to bring out clearly its importance in the field. The paper reviews some of the important studies on the two routes of persuasion. Cultural contexts and social norms are expected to influence the processing of advertising content. Comparative studies across cultures can validate whether these factors influence the processing of advertising content and use of the route to persuasion. The internet is a medium that has vast scope for exploration in the advertising context (Karson and Korgaonkar, 2001; Cho, 1999). Given the dynamic nature of the medium, conventional theories and assumptions might not be sufficient to explain how the viewer responds to online advertising. Even in traditional media like the radio, the impact of central and peripheral cues needs to be examined in greater detail (Darley and Smith, 1993) before drawing any conclusions regarding the medium’s effectiveness in providing objective product information and persuading the audience. References Agostinelli, Gina, & Grube, Joel W. (2002). Alcohol counter-advertising and the media. Alcohol Research and Health, 26(1), 15-21. Allen, Chris T., & Janiszewski, Chris A. (1989). Assessing the role of contingency awareness in attitudinal conditioning with implications for advertising research. Journal of Marketing Research, 26(1), 30-43. Allen, Chris T., & Madden, Thomas J. (1985). A closer look at classical conditioning. Journal of Consumer Research, 12(3), 301-315. Andrews, J. Craig, Durvasula, Srinivas, & Akhter, Syed H. (1990). A framework for conceptualizing and measuring the involvement construct in advertising research. Journal of Advertising, 19(4), 27-40. Belch, George E. (1982). The effects of television commercial repetition on cognitive response and message acceptance. Journal of Consumer Research, 9(1), 56-65. Blair, M. Elizabeth, & Shimp, Terence A. (1992). Consequences of an unpleasant experience with music: A second-order negative conditioning perspective. Journal of Advertising, 21(1), 35-41. Bozman, Carl S., & Mueling, Darrel (1994). The directional influence of music backgrounds in television advertising. Journal of Applied Business Research, 10(1), 14-18. Brinol, Pablo, Petty, Richard E., & Tormala, Zakary L. (2004). Self-validation of cognitive responses to advertisements. Journal of Consumer Research, 30(4), 559-573. Brooker, George, & Wheatley, John J. (1994). Music and radio advertising: Effects of tempo and placement. Advances in Consumer Research, 21(1), 286-290. Cacioppo, John T., & Petty, Richard E. (1989). Effects of message repetition on argument processing, recall, and persuasion. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 10(1), 3-12. Cho, Chang-Hoan (1999). How advertising works on the WWW: Modified Elaboration Likelihood Model. Journal of Current Issues and Research in Advertising, 21(1), 33-50. Cole, Catherine, Ettenson, Richard, Reinke, Suzanne, & Schrader, Tracy (1990). The Elaboration Likelihood Model (ELM): Replications, extensions and some conflicting findings. Advances in Consumer Research, 17(1), 231-236. Darley, William K., & Smith, Robert E. (1993). Advertising claim objectivity: Antecedents and effects. Journal of Marketing, 57(4), 100-113. Englis, Basil G., & Pennell, Greta E. (1994). “This note’s for you…:” Negative effects of the commercial use of popular music. Advances in Consumer Research, 21(1), 97. Gorn, Gerald J. (1982). The effects of music in advertising on choice behavior: A classical conditioning approach. Journal of Marketing, 46(1), 94-101. Gresham, Larry G., & Shimp, Terence A. (1985). Attitude toward the advertisement and brand attitudes: A classical conditioning perspective. Journal of Advertising, 14(1), 10-17, 49. Grossman, Randi Priluck, & Till, Brian D. (1998). The persistence of classically conditioned brand attitudes. Journal of Advertising, 27(1), 23-31. Hastak, Manoj, & Olson, Jerry C. (1989). Assessing the role of brand-related cognitive responses as mediators of communication effects on cognitive structure. Journal of Consumer Research, 15(4), 444-456. Haugtvedt, Curtis P., & Petty, Richard E. (1989). Need for cognition and attitude persistence. Advances in Consumer Research, 16(1), 33-36. Haugtvedt, Curtis P., Petty, Richard E., & Cacioppo, John T. (1992). Need for cognition and advertising: Understanding the role of personality variables in consumer behavior. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 1(3), 239-260. Hausknecht, Douglas R., & Moore, Danny L. (1986). The effects of time compressed advertising on brand attitude judgments. Advances in Consumer Research, 13(1), 105-110. 139 140 INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF THE INTERDISCIPLINARY SOCIAL SCIENCES, VOLUME 1 Hennessey, Judith E., & Anderson, Shirley C. (1990). The interaction of peripheral cues and message arguments on cognitive responses to an advertisement. Advances in Consumer Research, 17(1), 237-243. Inman, J. Jeffrey, McAlister, Leigh, & Hoyer, Wayne D. (1990). Promotion signal: Proxy for a price cut? Journal of Consumer Research, 17(1), 74-81. Jones, Joseph M. (2005). Examinations of relatedness between direct consumer premiums and promoted products: Assessing impact in different time periods. Advances in Consumer Research, 32, 482-483. Karson, Eric J., & Korgaonkar, Pradeep K. (2001). An experimental investigation of internet advertising and the elaboration likelihood model. Journal of Current Issues and Research in Advertising, 23(2), 53-72. Kim, John, Allen, Chris T., & Kardes, Frank R. (1996). An investigation of the mediational mechanisms underlying attitudinal conditioning. Journal of Marketing Research, 33(3), 318-328. Lord, Kenneth R., Lee, Myung-Soo, & Sauer, Paul L. (1995). The combined influence hypothesis: Central and peripheral antecedents of attitude toward the ad. Journal of Advertising, 24(1), 73-85. Love, Robert E., & Greenwald, Anthony G. (1978). Cognitive responses to persuasion as mediators of opinion change. Journal of Social Psychology, 104(2), 231-241. McCracken, Grant (1987). Advertising: Meaning or information? Advances in Consumer Research, 14(1), 121-124. McSweeney, Frances K., & Bierley, Calvin (1984). Recent developments in classical conditioning. Journal of Consumer Research, 11(2), 619-631. Miniard, Paul W., Sirdeshmukh, Deepak, & Innis, Daniel E. (1992). Peripheral persuasion and brand choice. Journal of Consumer Research, 19(2), 226-239. Moore, David J., Harris, William D., & Chen, Hong C. (1994). Exploring the role of individual differences in affect intensity on the consumer’s response to advertising appeals. Advances in Consumer Research, 21(1), 181-187. Petty, Richard, E., Cacioppo, John T., & Schumann, David (1983). Central and peripheral routes to advertising effectiveness: The moderating role of involvement. Journal of Consumer Research, 10(2), 135-146. Reed, Jr, Omer Lee (1975). The psychological impact of TV advertising and the need for FTC regulation. American Business Law Journal, 13(2), 171-183. Rossiter, John R., & Percy, Larry (1980). Attitude change through visual imagery in advertising. Journal of Advertising, 9(2), 10-16. Sen, Asha Ganesan (2003). Consumer insights and creativity. IIMB Management Review, 15(3), 124-126. Shamdasani, Prem N., Stanaland, Andrea J. S., & Tan, Juliana (2001). Location, location, location: Insights for advertising placement on the web. Journal of Advertising Research, 41(4), 7-21. Stuart, Elnora W., Shimp, Terence A., & Engle, Randall W. (1987). Classical conditioning of consumer attitudes: Four experiments in an advertising context. Journal of Consumer Research, 14(3), 334-349. Sudman, Seymour, & Schwarz, Norbert (1989). Contributions of cognitive psychology to advertising research. Journal of Advertising Research, 29(3), 43-53. Swasy, John L., & Munch, James M.(1985). Effects on source processing and persuasion. Journal of Consumer Research, 11(4), 877-886. Weilbacher, William M. (2003). How advertising affects consumers. Journal of Advertising Research, 43(2), 230-234. Whittler, Tommy E. & Spira, Joan Scattone (2002). Model’s race: A peripheral cue in advertising messages? Journal of Consumer Psychology, 12(4), 291-301. About the Author Esha Patnaik Masters in Psychology. Taught several courses in business communications, psychology and counselling. Editor of a magazine on psychology and counselling. THE INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF INTERDISCIPLINARY SOCIAL SCIENCES EDITORS Mary Kalantzis, University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign, USA. Bill Cope, University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign, USA. EDITORIAL ADVISORY BOARD Patrick Baert, Cambridge University, Cambridge, UK. Norma Burgess, Syracuse University, Syracuse NY, USA. Vangelis Intzidis, University of the Aegean, Rhodes. Paul James, RMIT University, Melbourne, Australia. José Luis Ortega Martín, Universidad de Granada, Spain. Chryssi Vitsilakis-Soroniatis, University of the Aegean, Rhodes, Greece. Please visit the Journal website at http://www.SocialSciences-Journal.com for further information: - ABOUT the Journal including Scope and Concerns, Editors, Advisory Board, Associate Editors and Journal Profile - FOR AUTHORS including Publishing Policy, Submission Guidelines, Peer Review Process and Publishing Agreement SUBSCRIPTIONS The Journal offers individual and institutional subscriptions. For further information please visit http://iji.cgpublisher.com/subscriptions.html Inquiries can be directed to subscriptions@commongroundpublishing.com INQUIRIES Email: cg-support@commongroundpublishing.com View publication stats