RESEARCH ARTICLE Diametrically Opposed Associations Between Academic Achievement and Social Anxiety Among University Students With and Without Autism Spectrum Disorder Gil Zukerman , Gili Yahav, and Ester Ben-Itzchak Research findings indicate that anxiety, social anxiety in particular, is the most common experience reported by individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) attending postsecondary education. Among students without ASD, higher levels of social anxiety have been postulated to correlate with impaired academic achievement; restriction of one’s social network because of anxiety is thought to lead to reduction of access to resources important for learning such as social/emotional support and collaborative learning. However, despite growing interest in the outcomes of young students with ASD, no research has studied the associations between academic achievement and anxiety among students with ASD. This study examined the association between social anxiety and grade point average (GPA) among university students: 55 diagnosed with ASD, 31 without ASD but high levels of social anxiety, and 25 without ASD and with low levels of social anxiety (controls). GPAs were significantly lower for the ASD group than for the two non-ASD groups. Among students without ASD, a negative correlation between social anxiety and grades was observed whereas the reverse pattern was found for the ASD group, meaning that for students with ASD, higher levels of social anxiety were associated with higher grades. Additionally, in a regression analysis, ASD diagnosis, social anxiety, and the interaction of group × social anxiety significantly predicted GPA. Possible explanations for this finding, as well as implications for interventions among this population of high-functioning students with ASD, are discussed. Autism Res 2019, 12: 1376–1385. © 2019 International Society for Autism Research, Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Lay Summary: This study compared the relationship between levels of social anxiety and grades in students with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and in students without ASD who had either high social anxiety or low social anxiety (controls). Among the group with ASD, higher levels of social anxiety were associated with higher grades, whereas the reverse pattern was found among the other groups. This finding’s implications for interventions among students with ASD are discussed. Keywords: academic; ASD; social anxiety; grade point average Introduction Interest in the outcomes of young adults with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) has increased, fueled by its rising prevalence (1:59 according to the Centers for Disease Control [2014]), the aging of ASD cohorts previously studied as children, and reports of poor outcomes in adults with ASD resulting in social and financial costs to society [reviewed in Steinhausen, Mohr Jensen, & Lauritsen, 2016; Volkmar, Jackson, & Hart, 2017]. Although almost half of those with ASD are considered to have average or above average cognitive ability [i.e., 43.9% with an Intelligence Quotient (IQ) >85, Christensen et al., 2016] and expected to yield “optimal outcomes,” studies of this subpopulation have reported a high frequency of unemployment and lacking social and economic independence [Anderson, Liang, & Lord, 2014; Gotham et al., 2015; Roy, Prox-Vagedes, Ohlmeier, & Dillo, 2015]. As only a minority of such young adults with ASD have a university education [full 4 years of college; 14%, Helles, Wallinius, Gillberg, Gillberg, & Billstedt, 2016; 17.4%, Newman et al., 2011], it appears that, beyond intellectual ability, other factors may significantly influence academic achievement among this population. Postsecondary education is a significant predictor of positive outcomes in the general population, as well as those with ASD [Newman et al., 2011]. For students with ASD, however, the challenges of achieving it are manifold; beyond academic demands, they also include the burden of adjusting to new social environments, poor planning and organizational skills, and difficulties adapting to unfamiliar settings with new routines. All of these factors might disrupt successful functioning in campus life for individuals with ASD [Volkmar et al., 2017]. Gelbar, Smith, and Reichow [2014], who reviewed the literature concerning college students with ASD, reported that the research is scarce, mostly case reports, and with little focus on academic achievement. From the Department of Communication Disorders, School of Health Sciences, Ariel University, Ariel, Israel (G.Z., G.Y., E.B.-I.); Bruckner Center for Research in Autism Spectrum Disorder, Ariel University, Ariel, Israel (E.B.-I.) Received December 9, 2018; accepted for publication May 7, 2019 Address for correspondence and reprints: Gil Zukerman, Department of Communication Disorders, School of Health Sciences, Ariel University, Ariel, Israel. E-mail: gilzu@ariel.ac.il Published online 21 May 2019 in Wiley Online Library (wileyonlinelibrary.com) DOI: 10.1002/aur.2129 © 2019 International Society for Autism Research, Wiley Periodicals, Inc. 1376 Autism Research 12: 1376–1385, 2019 INSAR Studying a sample of 35 college students with ASD, Gelbar, Shefyck, and Reichow [2015] reported a relatively high grade point average (GPA) of 3.27 (on a 4.0 scale), with 80% reporting a GPA above 3.0. Newman et al. [2011], however, reported that students with ASD had a lower likelihood of completing their degree (38.8%) compared to students from the general population (52.4%) and to those with disabilities in general (40.7%). Additionally, among studies describing the feelings of students with ASD, anxiety was most common, followed by loneliness, depression, and feelings of isolation from peers [Gelbar et al., 2014; Jackson, Hart, Brown, & Volkmar, 2018; White, Ollendick, & Bray, 2011]. In a survey study conducted in Australia, students reported an ability to cope with their academic workload but frequent struggling with mental health issues: anxiety (90.2%), depression (70.7%), and loneliness (61%) were reported as “moderate” to “big” concerns [Anderson, Carter, & Stephenson, 2018]; similar findings were reported among American students in another survey study [Jackson et al., 2018]. Anxiety disorders, particularly social anxiety [Maddox & White, 2015; Spain, Sin, Linder, McMahon, & Happé, 2018], are one of the most frequent comorbidities among the ASD population [reviewed in Howlin & Magiati, 2017] even among those who are cognitively able [Gillberg, Helles, Billstedt, & Gillberg, 2016; Lever & Geurts, 2016; Moss, Howlin, Savage, Bolton, & Rutter, 2015]. Social anxiety is characterized by marked fear or anxiety about social situations because of possible negative evaluation or scrutiny by others [DSM-5, American Psychiatric Association (APA), 2013]. The negative evaluation that may arise when attending social interactions (e.g., having a conversation, meeting new people), being observed in daily situations (e.g., eating or drinking), or performing in front of others (e.g., presenting an argument in a meeting, making a speech) [DSM-5, APA, 2013]. Sufferers of social anxiety frequently assume others are highly critical, possess maladaptive negative beliefs regarding their social competence (“I am boring”), and dread acting in a way that will reveal anxiety or unintentionally offend others [Clark & Wells, 1995; Leary, Kowalski, & Campbell, 1988; Niles et al., 2014; Rapee & Heimberg, 1997]. Often, this leads to marked avoidance of social interaction. In some cases, high levels of social anxiety symptoms may result in a diagnosis of social anxiety disorder (SAD), also known as social phobia [Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5), APA, 2013]. Although the high prevalence of anxiety disorders among individuals with ASD has led to studies utilizing cognitive behavioral therapy [CBT; Murphy et al., 2017; Reaven et al., 2018; Wise et al., 2019], most have been conducted with children or adolescents, and have used general CBT protocols not specifically designed for addressing social anxiety. As social anxiety may significantly affect the ability to adapt to everyday independent functioning among students without and with ASD [Kraper, Kenworthy, Popal, Martin, & Wallance, 2017], remedying this gap in adult ASD research is necessary. INSAR Social anxiety has been postulated to influence academic achievement by affecting the availability of social resources that may carry psychological gains (e.g., social and emotional support) as well as instrumental gains (participating in collaborative learning). As larger social networks (i.e., a network of social interactions and personal relationships) are thought to increase available resources [Blansky et al., 2013; Crosnoe, Cavanagh, & Elder, 2003; Goguen, Hiester, & Nordstrom, 2010], social anxiety, characterized by fear and avoidance of social interaction, may lead to lesser social ties, smaller social networks, and reduced academic performance [Russell & Topham, 2012]. Indeed, a longitudinal study of Brook and Willoughby [2015] indicated a significant negative association between levels of social anxiety and students’ academic grades. However, a systematic review of the literature on anxiety disorders and academic outcomes among typically developing adolescents has revealed mixed findings [de Lijster et al., 2018]. To the best of our knowledge, no research thus far has compared academic achievement among adult students with ASD to typically developed students or to students with other anxiety disorders, or examined factors that might affect their academic performance such as mental health disorders that are prevalent in ASD [Howlin & Magiati, 2017]. Moreover, as individuals with ASD are characterized by diminished interest in collaborative activities, social anhedonia, and more restricted social networks [van Asselt-Goverts, Embregts, Hendriks, Wegman, & Teunisse, 2015; Baminger & Kasari, 2000; Chamberlain, Kasari, & Rotheram-Fuller, 2007; Chevallier, Grèzes, Molesworth, Berthoz, & Happé, 2012; Chevallier, Molesworth, & Happé, 2014], their use of social resources to influence learning may be less frequent, resulting in a different relationship between social engagement and academic performance. In addition, significant negative correlations between academic performance and general trait anxiety (a disposition to perceive stressful situations as dangerous or threatening [Endler & Kocovski, 2001]) have been reported [ChamorroPremuzic & Furnham, 2003; Macher et al., 2012; Pintrich & DeGroot, 1990]. Associated with less efficient cognitive strategies and reduced investment of personal resources [Cassady & Johnson, 2002; Mathews, Davies, Westerman, & Stammers, 2000], trait anxiety, and its possible effects on academic performance among college students with ASD were also examined in this study. The current study had two main goals: (a) to compare academic achievement (GPA) in university students with ASD to those without ASD (with low or high levels of social anxiety symptoms); and (b) to evaluate the association between academic achievement, anxiety (social and trait anxiety), and autistic traits. High social anxiety symptoms [Spain et al., 2018] and a 22%–50% prevalence of SAD among individuals with ASD have been reported across studies [Maddox & White, 2015]. To the best of our knowledge, the level of social anxiety among university students with ASD Zukerman et al./Academics and social anxiety in ASD 1377 (defined as “optimal outcome”) has not been studied. To do so, our sample consisted of three groups: students with ASD (ASD group), and students without ASD whose scores on a self-report questionnaire indicated either high levels of social anxiety symptoms beyond the cutoff point for social phobia (SP group) or low levels of social anxiety (control group). We hypothesized that: (a) the ASD and SP groups would exhibit lower academic achievement (GPA) compared to controls; (b) across all groups, autistic trait severity would be negatively correlated with GPA; and (c) among the non-ASD groups, anxiety levels (social anxiety, in particular) would be negatively associated with academic performance (GPA). Methods Participants The study population included 111 first-degree (Bachelor’s) students (13 females; mean age = 24.21, SD = 2.69), studying in a medium size (15,000 students) public university. Fifty-five participants (four females; mean age = 23.56, SD = 2.81) were diagnosed with ASD (ASD group). Thirtyone participants had no ASD diagnosis but a high level of social anxiety (SP group; five females; mean age = 25.06, SD = 2.62). The third group included 25 participants (four females; mean age = 24.56, SD = 2.18) with no ASD diagnosis and a low level of social anxiety. The high male to female ratio in the ASD group reflects the strong male bias in ASD consistently reported in the literature [Werling & Geschwind, 2013a, 2013b]. An inclusion criteria for the study involved being a full-time university student. Inclusion criteria for the ASD group included a diagnosis of ASD from a licensed neurologist or psychiatrist, and a score ≥32 on the autism spectrum quotient (AQ) [Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, Skinner, Martin, & Clubley, 2001]. This score was associated with clinically significant levels of autistic traits, and is used as a screening tool for autism spectrum conditions among the general population [Baron-Cohen et al., 2001]. All participants in the ASD group had met the university’s entry requirements and had interviewed with the university’s “integration program” that provides support services to students with ASD. Only candidates demonstrating potential to adjust to on-campus living were accepted. Of the ASD group, 78.3% were residing in university dormitories, 16.3% at home, and about 5.4% in rented apartments near the university. For the non-ASD groups, inclusion criteria included a score ≤31 on the AQ. The participant’s score on the Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale [LSAS; von Glischinski et al., 2018] was used as an inclusion criterion for the SP group. Non-ASD participants whose LSAS score was ≥35 [considered an optimal cutoff point for social phobia, von Glischinski et al., 2018] were included in the social phobia (SP) group; those scoring <35 were included in the control group. The initial study sample had 119 participants. Eight 1378 participants were excluded because of AQ scores not meeting inclusion criteria (two participants with ASD scored ≤31 and six participants without ASD scored ≥32). Comparing ages across the three groups revealed a significant group effect F(2, 110) = 3.51, P = 0.033. Post-hoc testing indicated that participants in the ASD group were significantly younger than the SP group. Male dominance was observed in the entire sample. However, no significant difference in sex between the ASD and non-ASD groups (SP, control) was found [χ 2(2, 111) = 2.07, P = 0.354]. As depression was reported as a major comorbidity among those with ASD [Anderson et al., 2018], we examined depression levels using the Beck Depression Inventory-Second Edition [BDI-II; Beck, Steer, & Brown, 1996]. The average depression scores in all three groups indicated nondepression levels [BDI score <13; Beck et al., 1996; Whisman & Richardson, 2015]. Participants were designated to one of three academic affiliation types based on their major: (a) social sciences; (b) exact sciences (including natural sciences, architecture, and engineering); or (c) health sciences. Table 1 presents the academic affiliation distribution. No significant difference in academic affiliation type between the groups was observed (χ 2(4, 105) = 6.77, P = 0.148). Measures LSAS. The LSAS assesses social interaction and social performance anxiety and includes 24 Likert scale items (0–3) that tap into fear and avoidance in social situations [Liebowitz, 1987]. High reliability (r = 0.82, test–retest reliability) and validity (r = 0.83 with other social anxiety inventories) were reported for the self-report version used in this study [Baker, Heinrichs, Kim, & Hofmann, 2002]. State Trait Anxiety Inventory. The State Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) is self-report questionnaire that measures anxiety symptoms, and includes state (STAI-S) and trait (STAI-T) anxiety subscales [Spielberger, Gorsuch, Lushene, Vagg, & Jacobs et al., 1983]. High internal consistency coefficients [0.86–0.95; Spielberger et al., 1983] as well as moderate validity, expressed by a high correlation with other measures of anxiety (0.73–0.85), were reported [Taylor, 1953]. AQ. A self-report questionnaire that quantifies autistic traits among adults with normal IQs [Baron-Cohen et al., Table 1. Academic Affiliation Type Distribution Among Study Groups Social sciences Exact sciences Health sciences ASD Social phobia Control 13 35 1 7 24 0 7 15 3 Abbreviation: ASD, autism spectrum disorder. Zukerman et al./Academics and social anxiety in ASD INSAR 2001]. Scores range from 0 to 50; the higher the score, the more autistic traits a person possesses. Participants rate to what extent they agree with statements on a 4-point Likert scale. Relatively good internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha ranging from 0.63 to 0.77) was reported [Baron-Cohen et al., 2001]. In this study, as in previous studies [Golan, Baron-Cohen, Hill, & Golan, 2006], criteria for the ASD group included AQ scores ≥32 (associated with clinically significant levels of autistic traits [Baron-Cohen et al., 2001] along with a formal ASD diagnosis. Criteria for the non-ASD groups included AQ scores below the cutoff (≤31) and absence of an ASD diagnosis. GPA. GPAs from the first semester of the current academic year and academic affiliation were obtained from an official university-generated report. GPAs were represented by numeric values ranging from 0 to 100. All but six participants gave permission to access grades, and an additional six participants did not take any tests during their first semester, so a GPA was unavailable; therefore, GPA scores were available for 99 participants. Procedure University Ethics Committee approval was obtained prior to study onset. Participants gave signed informed consent to participate (including permission to access grades). Students with ASD were already required to complete questionnaires as part of participation in the university’s integration program; their involvement in the study was limited to consent for use of their data. Participants without ASD were recruited through advertisements in various campus locations and were financially compensated. The questionnaires utilized (LSAS, STAI, AQ) were self-report (a research assistant was available, as necessary). Data from the LSAS and the AQ questionnaires was available for 111 participants. State and trait anxiety subscales of the STAI data were available from 101 and 102 participants, respectively. Data Analysis Analyses were conducted using SPSS version 25 (Arkmonk, NY; IBM Corp., 2013). Completion of missing data was performed only for the STAI-T. Imputation was conducted only when ≤20% of the data was missing and used the “expectation maximization” technique only when data was missing completely at random, using Little’s “missing completely at random” test. Chi-square analysis was used to compare the distribution of academic affiliation and sex ratios among the three groups. Data were examined for normal distribution using SPSS Kolmogrov–Smirnov Test [Lilliefors corrections; Elliott & Woodward, 2007]. Normality was tested individually for the ASD and non-ASD (SP, control) groups as well as INSAR for each group separately (ASD, SP, control). Additionally, a nonparametric (Kruskal–Wallis) analysis was conducted to test group differences in study variables. A one-way analysis of variance was conducted to evaluate group differences for age. As the groups differed in age, age served as a covariate in all analyses. Separate univariate analyses of covariance (ANCOVA), with age as a covariate, were conducted to examine group differences in grades as well as severity of autistic symptoms (AQ scores). A multivariate analysis of covariance (MANCOVA), with age as a covariate, was conducted to examine group differences in social anxiety (LSAS) and trait anxiety (STAI-T). Separate ANCOVAs for each variable followed the MANCOVA with Bonferroni post hoc testing. Pearson correlation analysis was conducted for academic achievement (GPA) and anxiety measures (social anxiety, trait/state anxiety). A high correlation between trait and state anxiety on the STAI was observed (r = 0.833). Thus, we reported on data related only to trait anxiety. In addition, a four-step hierarchical linear regression analysis was performed in order to identify the relative contribution of the study’s variables to the variance in academic achievements (GPA). The analysis was conducted according to Dawson [2014]. The GPA served as an outcome variable, while, in the first step, independent variables included sex and age as demographic control variables. Group affiliation (ASD, non-ASD) was entered in the second step, social anxiety (LSAS score) in the third, and the interaction of group and LSAS score in the fourth. Results Normal distribution was evident for all variables (GPA, LSAS, STAI-T) for ASD and non-ASD (SP, control) groups. Testing three groups separately (ASD, SP, control) indicated a normal distribution for GPA (grades) and STAI-T (trait anxiety) for all groups. LSAS scores (social anxiety) were normality distributed for the ASD and control groups but not for the SP group. Table 2 presents the group differences in GPA, LSAS, STAI, and AQ indices. The average GPA across the entire sample was 77.15 (SD = 13.80). Univariate analysis of mean differences in GPAs between the three groups yielded a significant group effect (Table 2). The control group had the highest GPA, followed by the SP group, then the ASD group. However, the only significant difference in GPA was between the ASD and control groups (Bonferroni post-hoc tests). Psychiatric symptom severity between the three groups was also compared (Table 2). Multivariate analysis for the anxiety measures (LSAS, STAI-T) indicated a significant group effect [F(4, 192) = 14.31, P < 0.001, η2p = 0.23]. Univariate analyses for each anxiety measure indicated significant group effects for social anxiety severity (LSAS) as well as for trait anxiety (STAI-T). By definition, the SP group’s average total LSAS score was >35, a threshold suggestive of social Zukerman et al./Academics and social anxiety in ASD 1379 phobia [von Glischinski et al., 2018]. Among the ASD group, the average LSAS score was also >35. Bonferroni post-hoc testing indicated that both ASD and SP groups had significantly higher levels of social anxiety compared to controls, whose level of social anxiety was in the nonclinical range. Additionally, the mean STAI trait anxiety score for the ASD and SP groups was significantly higher than for controls. As expected, the level of autistic traits (AQ) differed between the groups (Table 2); the ASD group had a higher mean AQ score than the non-ASD groups, above the cutoff score (32) for identifying clinically significant autistic traits [Baron-Cohen et al., 2001], whereas both groups without ASD (SP, control) had levels of autistic traits slightly below mean levels of the general population [Ruzich et al., 2015]. A nonparametric analysis (Kruskal– Wallis) also indicated a significant effect of group affiliation on all study measures. No significant effects of age as a covariate were observed for any of the analyses. Correlations between first semester academic achievement (GPA), anxiety measures (social anxiety: LSAS; trait anxiety: STAI-T), and autistic traits (AQ) were examined. Across all participants, a significant negative correlation between first semester GPA and autistic traits (AQ) was observed r(99) = −0.20, P = 0.024. No significant correlations between GPA and the other study variables were found. When examining correlations between GPA and other study variables for each group separately (ASD, SP, control), a significant positive correlation emerged between GPA and LSAS scores for the ASD group: higher grades were associated with higher social anxiety (Table 3). In contrast, for participants without ASD (SP, control), GPA was significantly negatively correlated with LSAS scores, indicating higher grades were associated with lower social anxiety. Fisher’s Z transformation indicated a significant difference in correlations between ASD and non-ASD groups (Z = −2.76, P = 0.003). No significant correlations between GPA and trait anxiety scores were observed in any group. Finally, we performed a four-step hierarchical regression with academic achievement (GPA) as the dependent variable. Independent variables included sex, a demographic variable strongly associated with ASD [Hartley & Sikora, 2009], and age, in the first step. Group (ASD, non-ASD [SP, control]) Table 3. Academic Achievement and Anxiety Measures (LSAS, STAI-trait) Among Study Groups GPA r n r n r n r n ASD SP Control Non-ASD (SP, control) LSAS-total STAI-trait 0.30* 47 −0.16 27 −0.12 25 −0.29* 52 0.02 41 0.15 27 0.00 25 0.00 52 *P ≤ 0.05; **P ≤ 0.01. Abbreviations: ASD, Autism Spectrum Disorder; GPA, grade point average; LSAS, Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale; STAI, State Trait Anxiety Inventory; SP, social phobia. was entered into the analysis in the second step. Social anxiety (LSAS score), which was significantly correlated with GPA, was entered in the third step. The interaction of group × LSAS score was entered in the fourth step, in a stepwise fashion. Group (ASD, non-ASD), LSAS score (social anxiety), and the interaction of group × LSAS score significantly predicted GPA (academic achievement). Table 4 exhibits the results of the regression analysis. A medium effect size for the regression (Cohens f2) was found [Cohen, 1988; Selya, Rose, Dierker, Hedeker, & Mermelstein, 2012]. Discussion Despite growing interest in the transition of young adults with ASD to university study [White et al., 2016], our findings are novel in a field that includes little empirical research. Our first hypothesis, that both ASD and the SP groups would exhibit lower academic achievement compared to controls, was partially validated. The academic performance (GPA) of university students with ASD was significantly lower than their peers without ASD and low symptoms of social anxiety (controls). Although the SP group had lower academic performance than the controls as well, the difference was not significant. Thus, we suggest that ASD may be associated with lower academic achievement, even among high-functioning cognitively able students. Table 2. Study Measure Means and Standard Deviations Among Study Groups With and Without ASD ASD GPA LSAS STAI-trait AQ SP Control n M SD n M SD n M SD F P Partial eta squared 47 55 46 55 74.03 44.17 44.49 44.18 14.74 23.22 8.98 3.81 27 31 31 31 77.02 49.59 40.80 14.65 14.01 14.06 10.33 3.22 25 25 25 25 83.17 17.54 33.68 15.28 9.79 9.65 9.98 2.76 4.01 23.56 9. 93 940 <0.05 <0.01 <0.01 <0.01 0.08 0.31 0.17 0.95 Abbreviations: AQ, Autism Spectrum Quotient; ASD, autism spectrum disorder; GPA, grade point average; LSAS, Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale; SP, social phobia; STAI, State Trait Anxiety Inventory. 1380 Zukerman et al./Academics and social anxiety in ASD INSAR Table 4. Hierarchical Regression Model for Academic Achievement (Grades) Step Variable B SE β P R2 P R 2Δ 1 Age Sex Age Sex Group Age Sex Group LSAS Age Sex Group LSAS Group × LSAS 0.15 2.01 −1.40 0.03 −6.91 −1.49 −0.00 −7.38 0.73 −1.76 1.56 −7.03 −3.97 8.24 1.47 4.24 1.56 4.21 3.00 1.58 4.22 3.14 1.37 1.51 4.08 3.01 2.02 2.68 −0.00 0.04 0.09 0.00 −0.25 −0.10 0.00 −0.26 0.05 −0.12 0.03 −0.25 −0.30 0.46 0.992 0.633 0.371 0.994 0.024 0.348 0.999 0.021 0.594 0.247 0.702 0.022 0.052 0.003 0.00 0.888 0.00 0.05 0.145 0.05 0.024 0.05 0.226 0.00 0.594 0.15 0.011 0.087 0.003 2 3 4 P Abbreviation: LSAS, Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale. Our second hypothesis, that the autistic trait severity would be negatively correlated with GPA, was confirmed for the entire sample. The ASD group’s average GPA of 74.03 (on a scale of 0–100) was nine points less than the average control GPA (see Table 2), suggesting that an ASD diagnosis and a high level of autistic traits are related to reduced academic performance. Although this finding is in contrast to a previous study of university students with ASD that reported a relatively high GPA (3.27 on a 4.0 scale, corresponding to 82–90 on a 0–100 scale), that study was based on self-reported GPA, a smaller study sample, and no control comparison groups [Gelbar et al., 2015]. Our third hypothesis, that anxiety levels (social anxiety, in particular) would be negatively correlated with academic performance (GPA) among the non-ASD groups, was confirmed, as has been previously reported [Brook & Willoughby, 2015; Russell & Topham, 2012]. This relationship can be understood in the context of social capital [Crosnoe et al., 2003]— the benefit derived from membership in social groups or networks [Jensen & Jetten, 2015]. In academic settings, one’s social network provides access to psychological gains such as emotional support and the sharing of positive values and attitudes regarding academic success that help facilitate it [Astin, 1999; Tinto, 2006]. An academic social network also increases opportunities for instrumental gains such as academic knowledge and skills gathered through collaborative learning [e.g., quality small group interactions in and out of classroom settings; Hommes et al., 2012; VisschersPleijers, Dolmans, de Leng, Wolfhagen, & van der Vleuten, 2006]. Anticipation of social rejection and lack of peer intimacy, conversely, is said to disrupt the attainment of peer support useful in achieving academic goals [Goguen et al., 2010; Schlenker & Leary, 1982]. Moreover, the lack of significant correlations found between trait anxiety and academic achievement across all study groups supports the specificity of the relationship between social anxiety (i.e., distress related to social situations) and academic achievement. In INSAR sum, wider social networks seem to enhance academic achievement among university students, whereas reduced social interactions impair it. Thus, social anxiety, leading to avoidance of social interaction, may reduce academic achievement. However, it is important to note that the effect of social anxiety on the individual’s daily functioning may include not only avoidance of social interaction with peers, but also elevated anxiety when being observed or reduced function when performing in front of others. Our most interesting and novel finding was the diametrically opposed association between social anxiety and academic achievement among students with ASD, resulting from our correlation and regression analyses. For students without ASD, higher social anxiety was negatively associated with GPA, as expected. However, for students with ASD, a positive association was found: higher social anxiety was associated with a higher GPA. Various explanations can be proposed for this fascinating finding. Reduced social motivation [interest in engaging in social interpersonal behavior, Constantino & Gruber, 2012] may affect the relationship between social anxiety and academic achievement in several ways. Reduced social motivation is considered a primary characteristic of ASD [Bachevalier & Loveland, 2006; Chevallier et al., 2014; Riby & Hancock, 2008]. This is evidenced by diminished interest in collaborative activities, social anhedonia, and more restricted social networks among individuals with ASD [Chevallier et al., 2012; van Asselt-Goverts et al., 2015]. Thus, it is possible that social avoidance (as a result of social anxiety) less negatively affects this population because of their already restricted use of social interaction as a resource for beneficial academic outcomes. In addition, collaborative learning, which often involves being observed and “performing” in front of others by demonstrating knowledge, may elicit elevated anxiety among socially anxious individuals (DSM-5, APA). Thus, it is possible that, among those with ASD, social anxiety and avoidance lead to reduced exposure to fearful Zukerman et al./Academics and social anxiety in ASD 1381 situations and facilitate more effective independent learning whereas their academic performance is “protected” from a negative impact that less social capital may have on a student without ASD. Another possible explanation is that, among those with ASD, higher social motivation is associated with less ability to regulate emotion and, consequently, more avoidance of social interactions [White et al., 2014]. Previous research has found positive associations between social motivation and social cognition [Chevallier et al., 2014]; positive correlations between social cognition and executive functioning [Fisher & Happé, 2005; Hughes & Graham, 2002] and IQ [Ahmed & Miller, 2011] have been reported too. Although ASD is typically associated with reduced social motivation [Chevallier et al., 2014], it is possible that within our sample, students with ASD who were characterized by high levels of social anxiety (and presumed social avoidance), also possessed higher levels of social cognition and executive functioning that may have led to a higher GPA. This should be evaluated in future research. The current study has several strengths: the relatively large sample size, especially in the ASD group; two non-ASD comparison groups (high/low social anxiety); use of the standardized AQ for screening participants for ASD, in addition to a formal diagnosis; use of standardized questionnaires to evaluate levels of anxiety and autism trait severity; and officially obtained (not self-report) GPAs as a measure of academic achievement. The current study enables, for the first time, examination of the relationship between anxiety (social anxiety, in particular) and academic performance among a cohort of young adults with ASD in postsecondary education. Significant differences between the ASD and control groups were observed in all study measures; effect size values (η2p, f2) suggest a large (0.17–0.30) group effect on the psychiatric measures. A medium effect size was observed in academic achievement [Cohen, 1988] possibly because it is affected by many factors (e.g., I.Q., motivation, self-perception) not tested in this study. It is also important to note that a self-reported social anxiety symptoms (LSAS) questionnaire was used as an inclusion criterion for the SP group rather than formal diagnosis of social anxiety [DSM-5, APA, 2013]; future research may wish to include a formal diagnostic evaluation. The study has several limitations. Receipt of treatment (e.g., psychiatric medication, psychotherapy) was not factored into analyses (although such treatment would likely reduce the negative impact of social anxiety on GPA rather than enlarge it). Also noteworthy is the academic affiliation of 71% of the ASD group with Exact Sciences departments; this academic preference is in accord with previous research suggesting an association between autistic traits and scientific skills [Baron-Cohen et al., 2001]. Because our comparison groups resembled this academic affiliation type distribution, the findings may not fully represent the general student population. Additionally, possible sex differences in outcome variables were not examined in this study although 1382 sex differences in core ASD symptoms [van Backer, Koot, Scheeren, & Begeer, 2017; Kreiser & White, 2014], as well as executive function and play behavior, have been reported [for review, see Hull, Mandy, & Petrids, 2017]. Previous research has also reported greater social anxiety [Asher, Asnaani, & Aderka, 2017] and higher GPAs [Conger & Long, 2010] among women. ASD is more commonly diagnosed in males [Hull et al., 2017] and the study sample was characterized by male predominance, possibly making the findings more applicable to men with ASD. However, because no significant group differences in male to female ratio were observed, we believe that the study findings cannot be explained by sex effects. Nevertheless, future studies might use a more balanced male/female ratio to examine whether the association between social anxiety and academic achievement among those with ASD is affected by sex. Lastly, it is also important to note that all students with ASD participated in an integration program. Future research would benefit from exploring the potential influence of each of these factors. The current study findings may have important implications for both future research and practical interventions for high-functioning adult students with ASD. Social anxiety is characterized by fear of potential negative evaluation and scrutiny by others [DSM-5, APA, 2013] and those with high levels of social anxiety may hold negative cognitions in relation to social situations such as assuming that others are particularly critical [Leary et al., 1988; Niles et al., 2014]. Accordingly, cognitive behavioral therapies utilized for social anxiety/phobia usually advocate for altering appraisals and diminishing negative cognitions related to social interaction by using cognitive restructuring techniques such as focusing on negative outcome probability or questioning the social cost of negative evaluations [Niles et al., 2014; Rapee & Heimberg, 1997]. CBTs were previously reported as useful in improving functioning among typically developing students with social phobia [Tillfors et al., 2008]. Recent randomized controlled studies have also indicated positive effects of CBT on reduction of anxiety symptoms among individuals with ASD [Murphy et al., 2017; Reaven et al., 2018; Wise et al., 2019]. However, most of these studies were conducted with children or adolescents, focused on reduction of different kinds of anxiety, and used generic CBT protocols not specifically designed for treatment of social anxiety. This study’s findings suggest that, differently than the general population, reduced social motivation, which may be characteristic of those with ASD, might play an important role in affecting the association between their social anxiety and academic achievement. Thus, standard cognitive behavioral therapies mainly focused on reducing fear of negative social evaluation might not meet the needs of the ASD population. A more suitable intervention program, adapted for the special needs of this population, may be warranted. Zukerman et al./Academics and social anxiety in ASD INSAR References Ahmed, F. S., & Miller, L. S. (2011). Executive function mechanisms of theory of mind. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41(5), 667–678. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-010-1087-7 American Psychiatric Association (APA). (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596 Anderson, D. K., Liang, J. W., & Lord, C. (2014). Predicting young adult outcome among more and less cognitively able individuals with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 55(5), 485–494. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12178 Anderson, H., Carter, M., & Stephenson, J. (2018). Perspectives of university students with autism spectrum disorder in Australia. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 48(3), 651–665. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-017-3257-3 Asher, M., Asnaani, A., & Aderka, I. M. (2017). Gender differences in social anxiety disorder: A review. Clinical Psychology Review, 56, 1–12. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2017.05.004 Astin, A. W. (1999). Student involvement: A developmental theory for higher education. Journal of College Student Development, 40(5), 518–529. Bachevalier, J., & Loveland, K. A. (2006). The orbitofrontalamygdala circuit and self-regulation of social-emotional behavior in autism. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30(1), 97–117. Baker, S. L., Heinrichs, N., Kim, H. J., & Hofmann, S. G. (2002). The Liebowitz social anxiety scale as a self-report instrument: A preliminary psychometric analysis. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40(6), 701–715. Baminger, N., & Kasari, C. (2000). Loneliness and friendship in high-functioning children with autism. Child Development, 71, 447–456. Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J., & Clubley, E. (2001). The Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ): Evidence from Asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(1), 5–17. Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Manual for the Beck depression inventory-II. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. Blansky, D., Kavanaugh, C., Boothroyd, C., Benson, B., Gallagher, J., Endress, J., & Sayama, H. (2013). Spread of academic success in a high school social network. PLoS One, 8(2), e55944. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0055944 Brook, C. A., & Willoughby, T. (2015). The social ties that bind: Social anxiety and academic achievement across the university years. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 44(5), 1139–1152. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-015-0262-8 Cassady, J., & Johnson, R. (2002). Cognitive test anxiety and academic performance. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 27, 270–295. https://doi.org/10.1006/ceps.2001.1094 Chamberlain, B., Kasari, C., & Rotheram-Fuller, E. (2007). Involvement or isolation? The social networks of children with autism in regular classrooms. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 230–242. Chamorro-Premuzic, T., & Furnham, A. (2003). Personality traits and academic examination performance. European Journal of Personality, 17, 237–250. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.473 INSAR Chevallier, C., Grèzes, J., Molesworth, C., Berthoz, S., & Happé, F. (2012). Brief report: Selective social anhedonia in high functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(7), 1504–1509. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-011-1364-0 Chevallier, C., Molesworth, C., & Happé, F. (2014). Diminished social motivation negatively impacts reputation management: Autism Spectrum Disorders as a case in point. PLoS One, 7, e31107. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0031107 Christensen, D. L., Bilder, D. A., Zahorodny, W., Pettygrove, S., Durkin, M. S., Fitzgerald, R. T., … Yeargin-Allsopp, M. (2016). Prevalence and characteristics of autism spectrum disorder among 4-year-old children in the autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 37(1), 1–8. https://doi.org/10.1097/DBP. 0000000000000235 Clark, D. M., & Wells, A. (1995). A cognitive model of social phobia. In R. G. Heimberg, M. R. Liebowitz, D. A. Hope, & F. R. Schneier (Eds.), Social phobia: Diagnosis, assessment, and treatment (pp. 69–93). New York: The Guilford Press. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Conger, D., & Long, M. (2010). Why are men falling behind? Gender gaps in college performance and persistence. The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 627, 184–214. https://doi.org/10.1177/0002716209348751 Constantino, J. N., & Gruber, C. P. (2012). Social Responsiveness Scale—Second edition (SRS-2). Torrance, CA: Western Psychological Services. Crosnoe, R., Cavanagh, S., & Elder, G. (2003). Adolescent friendships as academic resources: The intersection of friendship, race, and school disadvantage. Sociological Perspectives, 46(3), 331–352. https://doi.org/10.1525/sop.2003.46.3.331 Dawson, J. F. (2014). Moderation in management research: What, why, when, and how. Journal of Business and Psychology, 29(1), 1–19. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10869-013-9308-7 de Lijster, J. M., Dieleman, G. C., Utens, E. M. W. J., Dierckx, B., Wierenga, M., Verhulst, F. C., & Legerstee, J. S. (2018). Social and academic functioning in adolescents with anxiety disorders: A systematic review. Journal of Affective Disorders, 230, 108–117. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2018.01.008 Elliott, A. C., & Woodward, W. A. (2007). Statistical analysis quick reference guidebook: With SPSS examples. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. Endler, N. S., & Kocovski, N. L. (2001). State and trait anxiety revisited. Anxiety Disorders, 15, 231–245. https://doi.org/10. 1016/S0887-6185(01)00060-3 Fisher, N., & Happé, F. (2005). A training study of theory of mind and executive function in children with autistic spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35(6), 757–771. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-005-0022-9 Gelbar, N. W., Shefyck, A., & Reichow, B. (2015). A comprehensive survey of current and former college students with autism spectrum disorders. The Yale Journal of Biology and Medicine, 88(1), 45–68. Gelbar, N. W., Smith, I., & Reichow, B. (2014). Systematic review of articles describing experience and supports of individuals with autism enrolled in college and university programs. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44(10), 2593–2601. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-014-2135-5 Zukerman et al./Academics and social anxiety in ASD 1383 Gillberg, I. C., Helles, A., Billstedt, E., & Gillberg, C. (2016). Boys with Asperger syndrome grow up: Psychiatric and neurodevelopmental disorders 20 years after initial diagnosis. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(1), 74–82. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-015-2544-0 Goguen, L. M. S., Hiester, M. A., & Nordstrom, A. H. (2010). Associations among peer relationships, academic achievement, and persistence in college. Journal of College Student Retention: Research, Theory & Practice, 12(3), 319–337. Golan, O., Baron-Cohen, S., Hill, J. J., & Golan, Y. (2006). The "reading the mind in films" task: Complex emotion recognition in adults with and without autism spectrum conditions. Social Neuroscience, 1(2), 111–123. https://doi.org/10.1080/ 17470910600980986 Gotham, K., Marvin, A. R., Taylor, J. L., Warren, Z., Anderson, C. M., Law, P. A., … Lipkin, P. H. (2015). Characterizing the daily life, needs, and priorities of adults with autism spectrum disorder from interactive autism network data. Autism, 19(7), 794–804. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361315583818 Hartley, S. L., & Sikora, D. M. (2009). Sex differences in autism spectrum disorder: An examination of developmental functioning, autistic symptoms, and coexisting behavior problems in toddlers. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(12), 1715–1722. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-009-0810-8 Helles, A., Wallinius, M., Gillberg, I. C., Gillberg, C., & Billstedt, E. (2016). Asperger syndrome in childhood—Personality dimensions in adult life: Temperament, character and outcome trajectories. British Journal of Psychiatry Open, 2(3), 210–216. Hommes, J., Rienties, B., de Grave, W., Bos, G., Schuwirth, L., & Scherpbier, A. (2012). Visualising the invisible: A network approach to reveal the informal social side of student learning. Advances in Health Sciences Education: Theory and Practice, 17(5), 743–757. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10459-012-9349-0 Howlin, P., & Magiati, I. (2017). Autism spectrum disorder: Outcomes in adulthood. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 30(2), 69–76. https://doi.org/10.1097/YCO.0000000000000308 Hughes, C., & Graham, A. (2002). Measuring executive functions in childhood: Problems and solutions? Child and Adolescent Mental Health, 7(3), 131–142. Hull, L., Mandy, L. H., & Petrids, K. V. (2017). Behavioral and cognitive sex/gender differences in autism spectrum condition and typically developing males and females. Autism, 26(6), 706–727. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361316669087 Jackson, S. L. J., Hart, L., Brown, J. T., & Volkmar, F. R. (2018). Brief report: Self-reported academic, social, and mental health experiences of post-secondary students with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 48(3), 643–650. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-017-3315-x Jensen, D. H., & Jetten, J. (2015). Bridging and bonding interactions in higher education: Social capital and students’ academic and professional identity formation. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 126. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00126 Kraper, C. K., Kenworthy, L., Popal, H., Martin, A., & Wallance, G. L. (2017). The gap between adaptive behavior and intelligence in autism persists into young adulthood and is linked to psychiatrics co-morbidities. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 47(10), 3007–3017. https://doi.org/10.1007/ s10803-017-3213-2 1384 Kreiser, N. L., & White, S. W. (2014). ASD in females: Are we overstating the core differences in diagnosis? Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 17(1), 67–84. https://doi.org/ 10.1007/s10567-013-0148-9 Leary, M. R., Kowalski, R. M., & Campbell, C. D. (1988). Selfpresentational concerns and social anxiety: The role of generalized impression expectancies. Journal of Research in Personality, 22(3), 308–321. https://doi.org/10.1016/0092-6566(88)90032-3 Lever, A. G., & Geurts, H. M. (2016). Psychiatric co-occurring symptoms and disorders in young, middle-aged, and older adults with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(6), 1916–1930. https://doi.org/ 10.1007/s10803-016-2722-8 Liebowitz, M. R. (1987). Social phobia. Modern Problems of Pharmacopsychiatry, 22, 141–173. https://doi.org/10.1159/ 000414022 Macher, D., Paechter, M., Papousek, I., Ruggeri, K., Harald Fredenthaler, H., & Arendasy, M. (2012). Statistics anxiety, state anxiety during an examination, and academic achievements. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 83, 535–549. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8279.2012.02081.x Maddox, B. B., & White, S. W. (2015). Comorbid social anxiety disorder in adults with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45(12), 3949–3960. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-015-2531-5 Mathews, G., Davies, D. R., Westerman, S. J., & Stammers, R. B. (2000). Human performance. Cognition, stress, and individual differences. Hove, UK: Psychology Press. Moss, P., Howlin, P., Savage, S., Bolton, P., & Rutter, M. (2015). Self and informant reports of mental health difficulties among adults with autism findings from a long-term follow-up study. Autism, 19(7), 832–841. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361315585916 Murphy, S. M., Chowdhury, U., White, S. W., Reynolds, L., Donald, L., Iqbal, Z., … Press, D. A. (2017). Cognitive behavior therapy versus a counseling intervention in the treatment of anxiety among late adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 47, 3446–3457. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-017-3252-8 Newman, L., Wagner, M., Knokey, A. M., Marder, C., Nagle, K., Shaver, D., … Schwarting, M. (2011). The post-high school outcomes of young adults with disabilities up to 8 years after high school. A report from the National Longitudinal Transition Study-2 (NLTS2) (NCSER 2011-3005). Menlo Park, CA: SRI International. Retrieved from. www.nlts2.org/reports/ Niles, A. N., Burklund, L. J., Arch, J. J., Lieberman, M. D., Saxbe, D., & Craske, M. G. (2014). Cognitive mediators of treatment for social anxiety disorder: Comparing acceptance and commitment therapy and cognitive-behavioral therapy. Behavior Therapy, 45(5), 664–677. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2014.04.006 Pintrich, P., & DeGroot, E. (1990). Motivational and self-regulated learning components of classroom academic performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82(1), 33–40. https://doi.org/10. 1037/0022-0663.82.1.33 Rapee, R. M., & Heimberg, R. G. (1997). A cognitive-behavioral model of anxiety in social phobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35(8), 741–756. Reaven, J., Moody, E. J., Grofer-Klinger, L., Keefer, A., Duncan, A., O’Kelley, S., … Blakeley-Smith, A. (2018). Training clinicians to deliver group CBT to manage anxiety in youth with ASD: Zukerman et al./Academics and social anxiety in ASD INSAR Results of a multisite trial. Journal of Counseling and Clinical Psychology, 86, 205–217. https://doi.org/10.1037/ccp0000285 Riby, D. M., & Hancock, P. J. (2008). Viewing it differently: Social scene perception in Williams syndrome and autism. Neuropsychologia, 46(11), 2855–2860. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. neuropsychologia.2008.05.003 Roy, M., Prox-Vagedes, V., Ohlmeier, M. D., & Dillo, W. (2015). Beyond childhood: Psychiatric comorbidities and social background of adults with Asperger syndrome. Psychiatria Danubina, 27(1), 50–59. Russell, G., & Topham, P. (2012). The impact of social anxiety on student learning and well-being in higher education. Journal of Mental Health, 21(4), 375–385. https://doi.org/10. 3109/09638237.2012.694505 Ruzich, E., Allison, C., Smith, P., Watson, P., Auyeung, B., Ring, H., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2015). Measuring autistic traits in the general population: A systematic review of the AutismSpectrum Quotient (AQ) in a nonclinical population sample of 6,900 typical adult males and females. Molecular Autism, 6(2), 1–12. https://doi.org/10.1186/2040-2392-6-2 Schlenker, B. R., & Leary, M. R. (1982). Social anxiety and selfpresentation: A conceptualization and model. Psychological Bulletin, 92(3), 641–669. Selya, A. S., Rose, J. S., Dierker, L. C., Hedeker, D., & Mermelstein, R. J. (2012). A practical guide to calculating Cohen’s f(2), a measure of local effect size, from PROC MIXED. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 111. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00111 Spain, D., Sin, J., Linder, K. B., McMahon, J., & Happé, F. (2018). Social anxiety in autism spectrum disorder: A systematic review. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorder, 52, 51–68. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2018.04.007 Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., Lushene, R., Vagg, P. R., & Jacobs, G. A. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. Steinhausen, H. C., Mohr Jensen, C., & Lauritsen, M. B. (2016). A systematic review and meta-analysis of the long-term overall outcome of autism spectrum disorders in adolescence and adulthood. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 133(6), 445–452. https://doi.org/10.1111/acps.12559 Taylor, J. A. (1953). A personality scale of manifest anxiety. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 48(2), 285–290. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0056264 Tillfors, M., Carlberg, P., Furmark, T., Lewchuapt, S., Spak, M., Eriksson, A., … Andersson, G. (2008). Treating university students with social phobia and public speaking fears: Internet delivered self-help with or without group exposure sessions. Depression & Anxiety, 27, 708–717. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.20416 Tinto, V. (2006). Research and practice of student retention: What next? Journal of College Student Retention: Research, Theory & Practice, 8(1), 1–19. https://doi.org/10.2190/4YNU4TMB-22DJ-AN4W van Asselt-Goverts, A. E., Embregts, P. J., Hendriks, A. H., Wegman, K. M., & Teunisse, J. P. (2015). Do social networks differ? Comparison of the social networks of people with INSAR intellectual disabilities, people with autism spectrum disorders and other people living in the community. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45(5), 1191–1203. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-014-2279-3 van Backer, O.,. T., Koot, H. M., Scheeren, A. M., & Begeer, S. (2017). Sex differences in the reciprocal behavior of children with autism. Autism, 21(6), 795–805. https://doi.org/10. 1177/1362361316669622 Visschers-Pleijers, A. J., Dolmans, D. H., de Leng, B. A., Wolfhagen, I. H., & van der Vleuten, C. P. (2006). Analysis of verbal interactions in tutorial groups: A process study. Medical Education, 40(2), 129–137. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2929.2005.02368 Volkmar, F. R., Jackson, S. L. J., & Hart, L. (2017). Transition issues and challenges for youth with autism spectrum disorders. Pediatric Annals, 46(6), e219–e223. https://doi.org/10. 3928/19382359-20170519-03 von Glischinski, M., Willutzki, U., Stangier, U., Hiller, W., Hoyer, J., Leibing, E., … Hirschfeld, G. (2018). Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale (LSAS): Optimal cut points for remission and response in a German sample. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 25(3), 465–473. https://doi.org/10.1002/cpp.2179 Werling, D. M., & Geschwind, D. H. (2013a). Sex differences in autism spectrum disorders. Current Opinion in Neurology, 26(2), 146–153. https://doi.org/10.1097/WCO.0b013e32835ee548 Werling, D. M., & Geschwind, D. H. (2013b). Understanding sex bias in autism spectrum disorder. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 110(13), 4868–4869. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1301602110 Whisman, M. A., & Richardson, E. D. (2015). Normative data on the Beck Depression Inventory—Second edition (BDI-II) in college students. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 71(9), 898–907. https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.22188 White, S. W., Elias, R., Salinas, C. E., Capriola, N., Conner, C. M., Asselin, S. B., … Getzel, E. E. (2016). Students with autism spectrum disorder in college: Results from a preliminary mixed methods needs analysis. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 56, 29–40. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2016.05.010 White, S. W., Mazefsky, C. A., Dichter, G. S., Chiu, P. H., Richey, J. A., & Ollendick, T. H. (2014). Social-cognitive, physiological, and neural mechanisms underlying emotion regulation impairments: Understanding anxiety in autism spectrum disorder. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 39, 22–36. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijdevneu. 2014.05.012 White, S. W., Ollendick, T. H., & Bray, B. C. (2011). College students on the autism spectrum: Prevalence and associated problems. Autism, 15(6), 683–701. https://doi.org/10.1177/ 1362361310393363 Wise, J. M., Cepeda, S. L., Ordaz, D. L., McBride, N. M., Cavitt, M. A., Howie, F. R., … Storch, E. A. (2019). Open trial of modular cognitive-behavioral therapy in the treatment of anxiety among late adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 50, 27–34. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10578-018-0817-5 Zukerman et al./Academics and social anxiety in ASD 1385 Copyright of Autism Research: Official Journal of the International Society for Autism Research is the property of Wiley-Blackwell and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.