Strategy Training and Reading Comprehension Anne Brailsford, MEd, Fern Snart, PhD, and J. P. Das, PhD A remedial strategy training program, conceptualized within the theoretical framework of the simultaneous-successive model of information processing, was investigated with the intent of improving performance on tests of cognitive synthesis and tasks of reading comprehension. A group of 24 learning-disabled children was divided into an experimental and a matched control group. Both groups received fifteen hours of remediation, the experimental group in cognitive strategy training and the control group in remedial reading, in addition to regular classroom reading instruction. Results indicated significantly greater experimental than control group improvement (pre/post) on four tests of cognitive synthesis and on reading comprehension levels. M ore than seventy years ago Huey (1908) recognized the inseparable ink between the child as a reader and the child as a processor of information. In the same era Thorndike (1917) wrote an article, "Reading as Reasoning," identifying reading as an active thinking process and the reader as a problem solver. Though terminology may have changed over the years, Thorndike's concept of the reader as an active thinker and strategist is still timely. Within the past two decades, human information processing theory has encapsulated the metaphor of the reader as an information processor involved in processing print, actively selecting, coding, storing and retrieving information from the reading material and his own cognitive structure (Pearson & Johnson, 1978). The child experiencing reading comprehension difficulties may have problems with processing print, namely in the effective selection and utilization of appropriate coding strategies for the task. Das, Kirby, and Jarman's (1979) information-integration model suggests a theoretical framework for conceptualizing human information processing, and as such provides a dynamic structure for investigating the reader as a processor of printed information. Much of the impetus for this model was derived from the early work of the Soviet psychologist Luria. Luria (1966) described the integrative nature of the human brain and observed that both simultaneous and successive coding are jointly implicated in every processing activity. Research has isolated simultaneous and successive processes as "psychological realities" (Das et al., 1979; Leong, 1974) and it is suggested that these coding processes are available, to varying extents, to the reader involved in tasks of reading comprehension. The applicability of the model for intervention has previously been investigated, with remedial procedures focusing primarily on successive synthesis. Krywaniuk's (1974) intervention program, with native children, improved performance in word recognition (Schonell Graded Word Reading Test) and Kaufman's (1978), with fourth-grade children, improved performance both in word recognition (Schonell) and mathematical skills (Metropolitan Achievement Test). The efficacy of structuring a remediation program, embedded with the objective of improving performance in reading comprehension by training task-appropriate utilization of information processing strategies, was investigated in the present study. METHOD Selection of Subjects A group of 24, 9 - to 12-year-old learning-disabled children, enrolled in reading resource room programs at two schools in small residential towns in the vicinity of Edmonton, Alberta, was selected. The children scored above IQ 85 on the nonverbal section of the Canadian Cognitive Abilities Test (Thorndike, Hagen & Wright, 1974) and below the 35th percentile on the comprehension subtest of the Gates-MacGinitie Reading Test, Level D (MacGinite, Kamons, Kowalski, MacGinite & MacKay, 1980). The students selected had no diagnosed sensory impairments and spoke English as a native language. They were assigned to either experimental (strategy training) or control (reading resource) groups, so that each group was composed of 12 children, six from each of the participating schools. Both groups contained eight boys and four girls. No significant preintervention differences existed between the groups in IQ or equivalent scores on the Gates-MacGinitie comprehension subtest. Pre-Intervention and Testing Procedures Simultaneous and successive processing were tested using a battery of tests consistently used in factor analytic studies of information-integration theory (Das et al., 1979). Memory for Designs (Das et al., 1979) and Figure Copying (Das et al., 1979) have both loaded reliably on a "simultaneous" factor, and Serial Recall (Ashman & Das, 1980) Free Recall (Das et al., 1979) and Digit Span-Forward (Das et al., 1979) on a "successive" factor. No significant pretest difference between the means of each group on the five simultaneous-successive tests was obtained. To test reading comprehension levels, the grade equivalent scores from the group administered Gates-MacGinitie comprehension subtest were utilized. In addition, an individual assessment of each child's reading cmprehension instructional level was obtained from the administration of The Standard Reading Inventory (McCracken, 1966). The latter test asks the child to read graded passages, retell them in his own words, and respond to probe comprehension questions which serve to elicit information not produced in the initial recall. No Downloaded from ldx.sagepub.com at Karolinska Institutets Universitetsbibliotek on May 24, 2015 significant pretest differences on reading instructional levels were obtained between the groups (experimental mean = grade 2.83, SD = .89; control mean = grade 3.25, SD = .83). The Intervention Phase Both the experimental and control groups continued receiving regular reading instruction in the classroom but each, in addition, had 15 hours of remedial assistance. Control group children received 30 minutes of small group reading instruction daily, from a resource room teacher. Remedial program content emphasized comprehension and interrelated word analysis activities. Experimental group children were seen in groups of two, for 30 minutes daily. Remediation focused on strategic behaviors, specifically those of simultaneous and successive synthesis, and verbalization of the child during task performance. Both of these aspects are seen as important for the appropriate coding and organization of information in problem solving. No task duplicated any materials used in either the cognitive test battery or the reading comprehension tests. Examples of a simultaneous and a successive training task respectively, are as follows: 1. Magic Window (Adapted from the Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children, Kaufman & Kaufman, 1980). With an emphasis on simultaneous coding, the child's task was to identify a picture of a common object, when the latter was gradually revealed, section by section, through a narrow paper "window." Fortyeight pictures, with eight presented on each of six paper discs, were revealed to the children, such that they attempted to predict what an object was after half of it had been shown, and again after the total object had been viewed. This task required the child to synthesize parts into meaningful wholes and effect closure by conceptualizing spatial relationships among the parts. The children's on-task verbalizations were transcribed, and the recalls confirmed that they were indeed utilizing task-appropriate strategies, synthesizing parts into wholes and checking previous predictions through the integration of spatial wholes. 2. Matrix Numbers (Adapted from Matrix Numbers, Kaufman, 1978). With a focus on successive processing strategies, this task required the child to memorize a sequence of randomly chosen numbers within the five cells of a cross-shaped matrix (see Figure 1). Each number appeared only once per matrix. Children were initially shown the entire matrix, and then were shown five matrices in a predetermined sequence, such that only one of the cells contained a number on each presentation. Children attempted to recall the series by writing on an erasable surface and, as with all of the tasks, they were encouraged to verbalize throughout. The process was repeated until successful recall was achieved, and six matrices in all were used. Based on recorded verbal comments, the majority of children revealed the utilization of successive strategies, such as verbal repetition or the tactile use of five fingers to aid in sequential recall. to imagine he was meeting a friend by a particular tree. He was asked to survey each tracking card and the village map and locate the relevant locations as rapidly as possible. The child was encouraged to verbalize his "journeys" and to review his routes orally, e.g., "I knew the road had to go straight up—then I went right at the tree, then at this (the second) intersection. 1 got to the house—I mailed it at house 32." Successful completion of each journey involved perceiving the spatial relationships amongst roads, streets, houses and trees, and comparing the spatial organization with the individual tracking map. Simultaneous processing was implicated for surveying the tracking card and village map, and successive processing for describing the sequential and narrative chain of events from start to completion. The children's recorded verbalizations indicated the utilization of task-appropriate strategies at the conclusion of three timed trials and two verbal review periods. There were 18 different tasks designed for the remedial program, and the emphasis on strategy training was divided relatively equally between simultaneous and successive coding. The complete set of remedial tasks, including several which combine aspects of simultaneous and successive coding, is presently being prepared for publication. Postintervention Testing 3. Tracking (Adapted from a Soviet preschool diagnostic test, Venger & Kholmovska, 1978). This task, focusing on both simultaneous and successive synthesis, required the child to locate specific landmarks by matching journeys on individual tracking cards with a largescale map (see Figure 2). Half the tracking cards illustrated a line drawing from a starting point to a numbered house, and half showed a journey from the same starting position to a lettered fir tree. The village map outlined roads, streets, intersections, houses and trees. The child's tasks was to pretend that he was either a mailman delivering a letter to a house, or At the conclusion of the intervention phase a posttest battery, basically replicating the composition of the pretest battery, was administered to each child. Alternate forms of the Gates-MacGinitie comprehension subtest (Level D, Form 2) and The Standard Reading Inventory (Form B) were utilized. RESULTS Analyses of variance were performed using a two-factor design, with repeated measures on the last factor. The factors were Groups (experimental/control) and Test Scores over Time (pre/post). Significant main effects on the pre/post factor were obtained on all the tests, indicating improved test scores over time for both groups. A significant interaction was obtained on Memory for Designs (^(1.22) Downloaded from ldx.sagepub.com at Karolinska Institutets Universitetsbibliotek on May 24, 2015 dard Reading Inventory are characterized in more detail in Table 2. As one method of more objectively examining improvement scores, we obtained the mean and standard deviation of instructional reading levels over all 24 students at the time of pretest, and examined the number of students in the experimental and control groups respectively who had improved at least one standard deviation by the time of posttest (pretest mean = 3.04, SD = .87). It may be noted utilizing Table 2, that eight of the 12 experimental subjects improved by at least one standard deviation, whereas only two students of 12 in the control group improved by one or more SD. TRACKING WHICH HOUSE? WHICH TREE ? DISCUSSION Figure 2 = 7.75, p < . 0 1 ) , Serial Recall (F(1.22) = 13.51, /?<.001), Free Recall (F(l.22) = 19.72, p < . 0 0 1 ) , and Digit SpanForward (F(1.22) = 6 . 8 1 , p < . 0 5 ) , with the experimental group producing significantly greater improvement than the control group over time. No significant interaction was obtained on the GatesMacGinitie comprehension subtest. However, there was a significant interaction on The Standard Reading Inventory (/^l.22) = 9.29, p<.0\) with the experimental group achieving greater improvement on instructional reading levels than the control group. Pre- and posttest means and standard deviations for the simultaneous-successive battery and the Standard Reading Inventory are presented in Table 1. Since improvement in reading ability is of primary interest within the present study, the group differences on the Stan- The fact that both groups demonstrated significant improvement, over time, on the simultaneous-successive battery and the reading comprehension tests may be attributed to maturation, practice effects, incidental exposure to tasks focusing on the appropriate utilization of simultaneous-successive coding, and also to the regular classroom reading programs. However, the experimental group scores indicated significantly greater improvement than the control group scores on four of the cognitive tests and on the instructional reading levels on The Standard Reading Inventory. These results suggest that a major portion of the improvement may be attributed to the remediation program. Significantly higher scores for the experimental than control group (pre/post) were expected on the tests of cognitive synthesis, on the basis of Krywaniuk (1974) and Kaufman's (1978) previous results and due to the present remediation program's intensive training in the task-appropriate utilization of simultaneous and successive programs. The experimental group was thus able to transfer successful coding strategies from the remedial tasks to the tasks presented on the cognitive synthesis tests. If we assume that simultaneous-successive syntheses are implicated in every act of information processing (Luria, 1966), then we make the assumption that they are employed by the reader in the process of reconstructing meaning from print. A rigorous test of the effectiveness of remediation, in terms of transfer of strategies, should be evident in the examination of the reading comprehension scores to observe if coding strategies have been transferred to tasks of reading comprehension. No significant interaction was obtained on the Gates-MacGinitie comprehension subtest grade equivalent scores. Though reasons must necessarily remain speculative it may be suggested that no significantly higher experimental than control results may be due to the multiple choice nature of the test's format. The latter allows constant access to the text and supplies an in-built organization, in that the questions and a range of possible responses are always available to the reader. It does not place reliance on the active organizational strategies emphasized in the remedial tasks. The Standard Reading Inventory, an individually administered test, demands the active participation of the child, asking him to read a story, verbally reconstruct it, and respond to questions. The experimental group achieved significantly higher instructional reading levels than the control group on this test. This was confirmed by ANOVA as well as by the rigorous criterion of improvement in units of standard deviation. It is suggested that Table 1. P r e - and Posttest Means and Standard Deviations for the Simultaneous-Successive Battery and for the Instructional Reading Levels (Standard Reading Inventory) for the Experimental and Control Groups Instructional Reading Levels (Standard Rdg. Inventory) Mean SD Memory for Designs Mean SD Digit Span Mean SD Serial Recall Mean SD Free Recall Mean SD Experimental Group Pretest Posttest 2.83 4.08 .89 .97 43.58 53.00 9.50 11.39 4.75 6.00 1.13 1.04 32.08 45.17 12.98 10.10 56.41 70.08 9.04 5.63 Control Group Pretest Posttest 3.25 3.60 .83 .74 47.50 48.58 9.92 9.21 4.83 4.83 1.03 .39 31.42 32.58 11.48 9.21 57.33 59.42 7.30 5.58 Downloaded from ldx.sagepub.com at Karolinska Institutets Universitetsbibliotek on May 24, 2015 Table 2. Standard Reading Inventory Instructional Reading Levels 1 4.0 5.0 3.5 3.5 3.5 3.5 6.0 5.0 5.0 3.5 2.5 4.0 4 5 6 7 CO 2.0 3.5 2.0 1.7 3.5 3.5 4.0 2.5 3.5 3.0 1.3 3.5 CM 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 Control Students Grade Levels Pre Post CO Experimental Students 9 10 11 12 the remediation program taught the child to use active strategies for the organization, coding, memorization and retrieval of information, and that these cognitive strategies are necessary in the reconstruction of meaning from print. Hence, a cognitive strategy training program may be a viable addition to a reading resource room program, with the objective of teaching strategies that may be transferred to tasks of reading comprehension. REFERENCES Ashman, A.F., & Das, J.P. Relation between plan- Grade Levels Post Pre 3.5 4.0 3.5 3.5 3.5 3.5 1.5 3.5 3.5 4.0 3.5 1.5 3.5 4.0 4.0 3.5 3.5 4.0 3.5 3.5 3.5 5.0 3.5 1.7 ning and simultaneous-successive processing. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1980, 51. 371-382. Das, J.P., Kirby, J.R., & Jarman, R.F. Simultaneous and successive cognitive processes. New York: Academic Press, 1979. Huey, E.B. The psychology and pedagogy of reading. Cambridge, Mass.: The M.l.T. Press, 1968. (Originally published, 1908). Kaufman, A.S., & Kaufman, N.L. Kaufman assessment battery for children. Circle Pines, Minnesota: American Guidance Service, Inc., 1980. Kaufman, D. The relationship of academic performance to strategy training and remedial techniques: An information processing approach. Doctoral dissertation, University of Alberta, 1978. Krywaniuk, L.W. Patterns of cognitive abilities of high and low achieving school children. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Alberta, 1974. Leong. C.K. An investigation of spatial-temporal information-processing in children with specific PROFESSOR STORIES (From page 281) himself was never so far away from Henderson Chapel as he was then. And as he spoke about the contemplative life, as did Aristotle—the highest expression of human nature—he thought about Aristotle's even more insistent attention to the idea of friendship. In his mind's vision, Meyer kept going back to the unforgettable fact that the scientists of the Third Reich also had a conscience of calling, even as they betrayed everything relating to human decency. He remembered that the scientists of the Third Reich devoted their lives to science and murder, to contemplation and betrayal. And while he spoke about the "natural arrogance of the young that education must dampen and ennoble into maturity," he also thought about the arrogance of tradition, the arrogance of teachers that threatens merely to dampen and extinguish the spirit of the young. And while he extolled his friends to once again take up their conscience, he thought about people he had known during his past who had too much conscience, and who should have better dusted off their common sense before they paraded their conscience. And while Meyer Wolf wove bril- reading disabilities. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Alberta, 1974. Luria, A.R. Human brain and psychological processes. New York: Harper and Row, 1966. MacGinitie, W.H., Kamons, J., Kowalski, R.L. MacGinitie, R.K. & MacKay, T. Gates-MacGinitie Reading Tests, Canadian Edition Teacher's Manual. Ontario, Canada: Thomas Nelson & Sons, 1980. McCracken, R.A. The Standard Reading Inventory Manual. Klamath Falls, Oregon: Klamath Printing Company, 1966. Pearson, D.P., & Johnson, D.D. Teaching reading comprehension. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1978. Thorndike, E.L. Reading as reasoning: A study of mistakes in paragraph reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 1917, 8, 323-332. Thorndike. R.L.. Hagen, E. & Wright, E.N. Canadian Cognitive Abilities Test Manual (Levels A-F). Ontario, Canada: Thomas Nelson & Sons, 1974. Venger, L.A., & Kholmouska, V.V. The diagnostics of intellectual development in preschool children. Moscow: Pedagogika, 1978. (In Russian). ABOUT THE AUTHORS Anne Brailsford is a reading specialist from Edmonton public schools. She received her MEd in educational psychology from the University of Alberta. Fern Snart is an assistant professor of special education at the University of Alberta. She received her PhD in educational psychology from the University of Alberta. J. P. Das is a professor of educational psychology and director of the Centre for the Study of Mental Retardation at the University of Alberta. He received his PhD in psychology from the University of London in Canada. liant metaphors concerning science, he was thinking about human experiments which extended the metaphors of science to the limits of their applicability, past the limits of human comprehensibility. And so while his friends and all the others who would have loved to have been his friend thanked God for Meyer Wolf, thanked God that here was a person who understood the university and its dedication to "preserving the universals of human experience and thought," and while the mob cheered and cheered in appreciation and praise for one of their own, all Meyer Wolf would let himself think about was the hope that the university is different from the Third Reich, that while the professor might volunteer to die for the truth, he should not kill for it. After 40 years in America, Meyer Wolf still could not get over the idea that he was an immigrant, an alien in a strange land. Possibly, for that reason there was still the hope, more than the expectation that, in the end, American science will not disillusion him, that the American university will not betray humanity in pursuit of truth, that America will remain a friendly haven for strangers. Downloaded from ldx.sagepub.com at Karolinska Institutets Universitetsbibliotek on May 24, 2015