Chapter 12 - Learning and Memory 2 TYPES OF LEARNING Associative learning, occurs when an organism forms a connection between two features of its environment. Classical conditioning, which allows organisms to learn about signals that predict important events, falls into this category. Non-associative learning, including the processes of habituation and sensitization, involves changes in the magnitude of responses to stimuli rather than the formation of connections between specific elements or events. Habituation, occurs when an organism reduces its response to unchanging, harmless stimuli, such as the sound of an air conditioner or furnace that becomes background noise. Sensitization involves, an increased response to other environmental stimuli due to repeated exposure to a strong stimulus, seen in exaggerated responses to movement, light, and noise following major disasters like earthquakes. In Classical conditioning, organisms learn that stimuli act as signals predicting the occurrence of important events, with conditioned stimuli (CS) gaining significance through learning, and conditioned responses (CR) being behaviors learned in response to stimuli that reliably predict future events. USING INVERTEBRATES TO STUDY LEARNING Invertebrates, with their large-celled and simple nervous systems, serve as ideal subjects for learning research. “Research on invertebrate learning includes bizarre demonstrations like G. A. Horridge's (1962) experiment showing that headless cockroaches can learn classically conditioned responses." "The sea slug Aplysia californica is commonly used in invertebrate learning research due to its easily observable nervous system." "The gill-withdrawal reflex in Aplysia habituates over time when the siphon is repeatedly touched, resulting in a gradual diminishment of the reflex." "Eric Kandel and colleagues traced the neural pathways responsible for habituation in Aplysia, revealing the role of synapses between sensory neurons and motor neurons." "Kandel demonstrated that repeated touching of the siphon in Aplysia led to reduced neurotransmitter release, depleting available neurotransmitter and producing short-term habituation." CLASSICAL CONDITIONING OF FEAR Many emotional responses, such as exam jitters or a child's fear of dogs, are learned through classical conditioning, with the amygdala playing a crucial role in Chapter 12 - Learning and Memory 1 emotional response conditioning (Wilensky et al., 2006). In classical fear conditioning studies with rats, a stimulus like a tone (CS) paired with an electrical shock (UCS) results in a conditioned fear response (CR) leading to a reduction in behaviors incompatible with fear, such as feeding. Information about the CS and UCS converges in the amygdala, where a high influx of calcium triggers events, similar to Aplysia, resulting in protein synthesis and increased sensitivity to subsequent CS input, making the newly conditioned circuits respond more strongly to the CS alone (Wilensky et al., 2006) CLASSICAL CONDITIONING OF THE EYEBLINK Investigations into conditioned eyeblinks in rabbits by Richard Thompson and colleagues involve pairing a tone (CS) with a puff of air directed at the rabbit's eye (UCS), resulting in the movement of the nictitating membrane, an inner eyelid found in some animals (Christian & Thompson, 2003; Lee & Thompson, 2006; Thompson, Thompson, Kim, Krupa, & Shinkman, 1998). The cerebellum, particularly the interpositus nucleus, plays a crucial role in classical conditioning of eyeblinks, with recordings showing an increase in the interpositus nucleus's response as learning proceeds (Woodruff-Pak & Disterhoft, 2008; Thompson, 1986). Reversible lesion experiments, particularly inactivating the interpositus nucleus by cooling, effectively prevent classical conditioning, indicating the primary responsibility of the interpositus nucleus in forming the classically conditioned response of the nictitating membrane in rabbits (Robleto & Thompson, 2008; Krupa, Thompson, & Thompson, 1993). CEREBELLAR CIRCUITS AND CLASSICAL CONDITIONING The cerebellum, with its unique anatomy, is well-suited for classical conditioning, particularly in its cortex where Purkinje cells receive climbing fibers from the inferior olive and parallel fibers from granule cells, forming inhibitory synapses on the cerebellum's output cells in the deep nuclei. According to James Albus (1971), learning occurs when climbing-fiber and parallelfiber synapses onto a Purkinje cell are activated simultaneously, a proposition supported by Masao Ito's (1984) recordings showing a reduction in Purkinje cell EPSPs lasting up to one hour through long-term depression (LTD). LTD involves a decrease in the number of available glutamate receptors in the Purkinje cell membrane, essential for learning the eyeblink conditioned response (CR) in the inferior olive-climbing fiber system. TRACE CONDITIONING AND EXTINCTION Classical conditioning can be categorized into delay conditioning, where the CS and UCS overlap without a stimulus-free interval, and trace conditioning, which involves a stimulus-free interval between the CS and UCS, requiring the participation of forebrain areas. Chapter 12 - Learning and Memory 2 Research with mutant mice and human patients with cerebellar lesions revealed that abnormal cerebellums interfered with delay conditioning but not trace conditioning, indicating different learning processes for these two types. Trace conditioning, involving forebrain structures like the sensory cortex, hippocampus, and prefrontal cortex, requires conscious, declarative processes, making it a valuable model for studying declarative memories across species. MEMORY Types of memory In the Atkinson-Shiffrin model, information initially enters sensory memory, which can hold a large amount of data briefly. Selected data moves to short-term memory, with a limited capacity of about five to nine items, often leading to loss of previous information. Short-term memory is temporary and involves temporary storage areas managed by a "central executive" process. The final destination is long-term memory, which has few limitations on capacity or duration. Long-term memories include semantic, episodic, and procedural memories, with semantic and episodic memories grouped as declarative memories, recalled consciously, while procedural memories are recalled unconsciously. The model distinguishes explicit and implicit memories, illustrated in cases of anterograde amnesia where patients can form implicit memories despite a lack of explicit recall. The search for the engram, the physical representation of memory in the brain, remains a focus in psychology. Chapter 12 - Learning and Memory 3 BRAIN MECHANISMS IN MEMORY The search for the engram, the physical representation of memory in the brain, involved early efforts by psychologists like Karl Lashley and neurosurgeon Wilder Penfield. Lashley conducted experiments on rats, concluding that all parts of the cortex contribute equally to learning and memory, a concept known as equipotentiality. However, this idea is now considered a mistake, as more recent data suggest uneven participation of different cortex areas in memory. Wilder Penfield, through cortical mapping of patients undergoing seizure disorder surgery, found experiential responses linked to temporal lobe stimulation, focusing interest on the temporal lobe's role in long-term memory. Other researchers using single-cell recordings provided evidence for specific, localized memory storage, with neurons showing relationships to particular types of information, challenging Lashley's equipotentiality concept. THE TEMPORAL LOBE AND MEMORY The temporal lobe's role in memory was explored through studies on patients with anterograde amnesia, such as the well-known case of patient H. M. H. M., who underwent extensive surgery, including removal of parts of the temporal lobe, demonstrated a profound inability to transfer new information from short-term longterm memory while retaining procedural memory. This case supports the distinction between explicit and implicit memories, as well as the Atkinson-Shiffrin model's stage approach to memory. While H. M.'s damage impacted explicit memories, it did not affect implicit memories or stored long-term memories, suggesting that the hippocampus may not be the storage location for the engram. Further studies on monkeys with medial temporal lobe lesions, including the amygdala, hippocampus, and surrounding areas, showed impaired performance on a delayed nonmatching to sample task, emphasizing the role of these structures in the formation of long-term memories. LONG-TERM POTENTIATION (LTP) Chapter 12 - Learning and Memory 4 Long-Term Potentiation (LTP), explored since the 1970s, involves neural mechanisms in the hippocampus crucial for learning and memory. The hippocampus, consisting of Ammon's horn and the dentate gyrus, plays a central role. Input from the cortex enters through the parahippocampal and rhinal cortices, reaching the hippocampus. LTP, demonstrated through rapid electrical shocks, enhances synapses’ efficiency, lasting a long time, and requiring only seconds of input. LTP aligns with Donald Hebb's cellular learning model, involving associativity and cooperativity. NMDA glutamate receptors facilitate these processes. While initially observed in the hippocampus, LTP has been identified throughout the central nervous system, emphasizing its significance in understanding memory formation. LTP AND SPATIAL MEMORY Studies on spatial memory, examining an organism's ability to map locations, provide additional evidence linking Long-Term Potentiation (LTP) to memory. Researchers, like O'Keefe and Dostrovsky, demonstrated that mice use distinct hippocampal activation patterns to represent spatial locations, forming stable maps within minutes of entering a new environment. This rapid formation and stability align with features of both long-term memories and LTP. Investigations using genetic mutations disrupting LTP-related pathways, such as NMDA receptor components, show a negative impact on spatial memory. While animals with impaired LTP can still form spatial maps, these maps lack stability. The impairment leads to the formation of new maps instead of reactivating previous ones, resembling the behavior of patients with memory deficits. Applying NMDA receptor antagonists also impairs spatial learning in rodents and prevents the development of LTP in the hippocampus. THE DIENCEPHALON AND MEMORY The hippocampus and the temporal lobe are closely linked to the thalamus, and disruptions to these structures or their connections can lead to amnesia. Case studies, such as that of patient N. A., who suffered a thalamic lesion from a fencing foil accident, show similarities to the memory loss experienced by H. M. N. A. exhibited anterograde amnesia, preserving intelligence and short-term memory but struggling with forming new explicit memories. Chronic alcoholics with Korsakoff's syndrome, often resulting from thiamine deficiency, display similar anterograde amnesia symptoms. Thiamine deficiency damages the dorsomedial thalamus and mammillary bodies in the diencephalon. Patients with Korsakoff's syndrome also suffer severe retrograde amnesia, possibly due to lesions in various brain areas, including the cerebellum, brainstem, cortex, and diencephalon. Animal studies support these findings, showing that monkeys with thalamic and mammillary body lesions struggle with memory tasks. SEMANTIC MEMORY AND THE CEREBRAL CORTEX Semantic knowledge, encompassing basic facts and language, is widely distributed in the cortex. Different areas of the association cortex activate during semantic Chapter 12 - Learning and Memory 5 memory tasks based on specific concept characteristics. PET scans by Alex Martin and colleagues revealed that when naming animals, participants activated the left medial occipital lobes associated with visual processing, while naming tools activated the left premotor area and the left middle temporal gyrus associated with tool-use concepts. Case studies of patients with association cortex damage, like the one by McCarthy and Warrington, further support the distributed nature of semantic knowledge. Retrieving these memories involves coordinated efforts, and Antonio Damasio suggests the existence of a "convergence zone" responsible for assembling various aspects of a memory into a cohesive whole. Likely candidates for this convergence zone in semantic memories include areas along the left lateral inferior frontal gyrus. These areas become more active when language rules or world knowledge are violated compared to correct language usage or factual information. EPISODIC MEMORY AND THE CEREBRAL CORTEX Episodic memory, involving personal experiences, is distinct from semantic memory. Patients with cortical damage, particularly in the prefrontal areas, may experience source amnesia, retaining semantic knowledge but forgetting how and when they learned specific information. For instance, Patient K. C. retained general semantic knowledge but couldn't remember details about learning to play chess after sustaining damage to specific cortical areas. Episodic memories contribute to our sense of self, and brain imaging studies suggest the involvement of the anterior prefrontal cortex and posterior cingulate cortex in retrieving personal episodic memories. The distinction between fantasy and reality relies on episodic memory, as evidenced by brain activity patterns when participants were asked about the possibility of conversing with real or fantasy figures. Brain areas associated with episodic memory were active when considering reality, while areas linked to semantic processing were more active when contemplating fantasy. Disturbances in this distinction may underlie delusions in some psychological disorders. SHORT-TERM MEMORY AND THE BRAIN The Atkinson-Shiffrin model suggests that short-term memory (working memory) involves the central executive, the phonological loop, the visuo-spatial sketchpad, and an episodic buffer. In terms of brain localization, the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) and the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) are believed to underlie the central executive. These areas contribute to attentional aspects of short-term memory. Patients with prefrontal cortex lesions struggle with tasks like the Wisconsin card-sorting test, indicating difficulties in shifting attention and adjusting to new rules. Research on memory development, such as the object permanence test, suggests that a mature prefrontal cortex is necessary for short-term memory. Chapter 12 - Learning and Memory 6 The ACC also plays an executive role in short-term memory, as evidenced by studies comparing people with large or small short-term memory capacities. People with larger capacities show more ACC activation and use different memory strategies, supporting the ACC's involvement in processing verbal information in short-term memory. THE STRIATUM AND PROCEDURAL MEMORY The striatum, encompassing the basal ganglia and nucleus accumbens, plays a crucial role in procedural memory formation. Involved in motor system functions, these structures are particularly associated with learning and remembering motor patterns. The nucleus accumbens contributes emotional and reward-related aspects to procedural memory. During trial-and-error learning, the striatum encourages exploration and participates in the evaluation of changes leading to greater accuracy and reward. Studies with rats in a maze task revealed that the striatum's neuronal activity is involved in both exploration and exploitation phases of learning. In a specific study with rats learning maze tasks, it was observed that the striatum’s role is more pronounced in procedural than declarative memories. Rats with lesions in structures associated with the hippocampus struggled with declarative memory tasks, where explicit episodic memories are crucial, but performed normally in procedural memory tasks. On the other hand, rats with lesions in the basal ganglia had difficulty with procedural tasks but showed little impairment in declarative tasks. This provides evidence for the distinct involvement of the striatum in procedural memory formation. SHORT-TERM SENSITIZATION PROCESS Single session produces behavior changes lasting minutes. [repetition] Serotonin activates adenylyl cyclase, converting ATP to cAMP. cAMP activates protein kinase A (PKA). PKA effects include prolonged action potential, increased vesicle movement, and opening of more Ca2+ channels, enhancing glutamate release. [retrieval] LONG-TERM SENSITIZATION PROCESS Repeated and spaced-out sensitization trials result in behavioral changes lasting weeks. cAMP-PKA-MAPK-CREB pathway activated. PKA, recurrently activated, triggers MAP kinase. PKA and MAP kinase transported to neural cell body, activating CREB-1 and inhibiting CREB-2. CREB-1 and MAP kinase block inhibitory actions of CREB-2, allowing gene transcription. Two genes expressed: one for ubiquitin carboxyterminal hydrolase (maintaining PKA activity) and another for C/EBP (stimulating new synaptic terminal growth). Biochemical and structural changes account for long-term presynaptic changes. THE EFFECTS OF STRESS ON MEMORY Chapter 12 - Learning and Memory 7 Yerkes-Dodson Law and Stress Effects on Memory Stress effects on memory depend on task complexity. Simple tasks show linear improvement with increasing stress. Complex tasks show improvement up to a point, then decline with stronger stress. Timing and Stress-Related Memory Formation Stress initially enhances memory formation. This enhancement is followed by a refractory period impairing memory formation. This refractory period may protect important memories from interference. Hippocampus and Amygdala Interaction Stress impacts hippocampus and amygdala independently. Case study: Emotional component processed by the amygdala, but hippocampus unable to encode declarative details. Prefrontal Cortex and Stress Stress affects prefrontal cortex functions (coping, decision making, planning, multitasking). Inverted U-shaped relationship with dopamine and norepinephrine release. Implications and Potential Interventions Understanding these processes may help prevent and treat traumatic and false memories. Glucocorticoids released during stress (e.g., cortisol) correlated with more false memories. Propranolol, blocking glucocorticoid effects, might prevent traumatic memories. Manipulation of enzymes in animals shows progress in "erasing" long-term memories, offering potential PTSD treatment approaches. AGING AND MEMORY Age-Related Changes in Learning and Memory Eyeblink conditioning is more difficult and takes longer in older participants. Most cognitive abilities in healthy older adults remain stable. Brain modifications may compensate for age-related declines in function. Brain Activity and Aging Decreased blood flow observed in brain regions essential for memory and cognition. Areas of increased activation suggest brain reorganization to maintain stable cognitive performance despite age-related deficits. Comparisons in Memory Encoding Chapter 12 - Learning and Memory 8 Young and older adults show no differences in hippocampal activation during successful encoding of face-name pairs. Young participants exhibit reduced activation in parietal lobe and posterior cingulate cortex during successful encoding. High-performing older participants show similar areas of deactivation, while lower-performing ones may be in early dementia stages. Alzheimer's Disease and Memory Encoding Participants at risk for Alzheimer's or with mild cognitive impairment demonstrate increased hippocampal activity during encoding. Increased activity may represent the brain's compensatory effort. Eventually, compensation fails as dementia progresses, leading to reduced hippocampal activity below healthy older controls. Chapter 12 - Learning and Memory 9