Attention and Memory Brynn E. Sherman1 (brynn.sherman@yale.edu) Nicholas B. Turk-Browne1, 2 (nicholas.turk-browne@yale.edu) 1 Department of Psychology; 2Wu Tsai Institute, Yale University To appear in: Oxford Handbook of Human Memory. M.J. Kahana & A.D. Wagner (Eds). Oxford University Press. Abstract Attention plays an important and pervasive role in human perception, filtering complex sensory input based on what is most salient or relevant, and gating the entry of this information into conscious awareness. Beyond modulating processing of the current environment, the control of attention over perception has downstream consequences for processing of the past and future, determining what information is available for memory encoding and retrieval. This interaction between attention and memory is bidirectional, as goals, episodes, and knowledge held in memory in turn inform attentional selection and vigilance. These bidirectional interactions can be found across different types of memory and their underlying brain systems. In this chapter, we catalog the ways in which attention and memory interact, focusing on working memory, episodic memory, and learning. By reviewing the neural and behavioral mechanisms underlying these interactions, we reconceptualize attention as serving a broader role in the organization of the mind, and memory as being fundamentally selective and interwoven with perception. Keywords: cuing, selection, modulation, encoding, retrieval, hippocampus Introduction Various factors influence memory encoding and retrieval, including context, semantics, emotion, and motivation — all of which and more are covered in the chapters of this book. However, one of the strongest predictors of memory, and the focus of this chapter, is attention. The term attention has many different meanings, but here we refer to how salience or goals prioritize a subset of external inputs or internal states (Chun et al., 2011). This attentional selection in turn leads to modulation — enhancing or facilitating processing of the targets of attention and suppressing or inhibiting processing of distracting information. By this definition, attention interacts with memory in several ways. Attention has a powerful influence over what we perceive and thus necessarily influences what can be encoded into memory (Uncapher et al., 2011). When oriented internally, attention helps select among representations to be retrieved from memory (Chun & Johnson, 2011). Attentional priorities themselves are often derived from memory, informed by when and where relevant information was found in recent or long-past experiences (Hutchinson & Turk-Browne, 2012). The interactions between attention and memory are thus bidirectional and recurrent, with attention influencing which memories are formed, these memories guiding subsequent attention, and this in turn impacting new encoding and retrieval. Here we review evidence for interactions between attention and memory in behavior and the brain, with an emphasis on how attentional selection operates during encoding and retrieval, and how retrieval influences attentional selection. We highlight effects across a range of brain systems for attention and memory (Figure 1), from shorter-term working memory, to longer-term episodic memory, to more gradual forms of statistical learning. We consider both domaingeneral and specialized mechanisms, for example: evaluating whether attention and working memory reflect a single flexible cognitive process versus two distinct processes, and dissociating influences of attention on long-term memory that are a byproduct of modulation of sensory systems versus a result of direct modulation of memory systems. Exploring this broad literature leads to a comprehensive understanding of how attention and memory coordinate to support cognition. Figure 1. Summary of brain systems involved in attention and memory that are discussed in this chapter. WM: working memory; LTM: long-term memory. The color shading corresponds to the location of specific brain systems (purple: sensory visual cortices; blue: frontoparietal networks; green: hippocampus; pink: neuromodulation stemming from midbrain structures). Dashed lines indicate subcortical structures. Attention and working memory Attention is often thought of in terms of the external world — how we attend to the road while driving or the professor giving a lecture. Yet we can also attend to our internal states. For example, mind-wandering during a lecture involves orienting attention to unrelated thoughts in mind rather than to the content of the lecture. A key insight in understanding the role of attention in memory is that attention can be allocated to the contents of our memories, even when they are not directly supported by stimuli in the current environment (Chun et al., 2011). This process of internal attention dovetails with a conventional definition of working memory: the process by which absent stimuli are actively held or maintained in mind (see Chapters 2.9 and 2.10). Indeed, capacity limits in working memory are thought to emerge from the limitations of attentional selection (Cowan, 1998; Cowan, 1999; Garavan, 1998; Oberauer, 2002). To what extent, though, is internally oriented attention responsible for maintaining information in working memory? Attention can be allocated to the contents of working memory Traditional views of working memory suggest that maintenance is supported by two modality-specific subsystems: a visuospatial sketchpad for holding and manipulating images in mind, and a phonological loop for rehearsing auditory information (Baddeley, 1992). Although these systems accord with intuitions about what it feels like to hold something in working memory, subsequent research suggested that maintenance — particularly in the visuospatial domain — can be understood as an allocation of attention. In a seminal behavioral study, participants responded faster to an external stimulus presented at the same location as an item held in working memory, and working memory accuracy was lower when external attention was drawn away from a to-be-remembered location (Awh et al., 1998). These findings suggest that working memory invokes attention, resulting in enhanced processing for locations currently held in mind. Attention can also be used to select among the contents of working memory. This has been studied using the retro-cuing paradigm, in which participants are cued during a delay period to one or more of several stimuli that are no longer physically present but are being held in memory. Retro-cues provide similar performance benefits as pre-cues, resulting in enhanced memory for retro-cued objects (Griffin & Nobre, 2003; Makovski & Jiang, 2007) and features (Niklaus et al., 2017). This selection during working memory maintenance may boost memory by protecting target items from delay-related degradation, rather than prioritizing the item for retrieval. This interpretation is supported by the fact that switching the retro-cue (i.e., asking participants to shift attention to previously unattended memory contents) does not provide any benefit to memory, presumably because those contents have degraded since the original cue (Matsukura et al., 2007). Indeed, information held outside the focus of attention in working memory is remembered with less precision (LaRocque et al., 2015). Nevertheless, attention can be allocated dynamically and flexibly over time during maintenance, including by relying on temporal statistics to prioritize information likely to be probed at different timepoints (van Ede et al., 2017). Taken together, these data suggest that attention plays an important role in keeping representations active. Neuroimaging studies provide further evidence for this role of attention in working memory. Maintaining a spatial location in memory evokes similar activity patterns in visual cortex as when attending to that location in external space (Awh & Jonides, 2001). This holds for features such as orientation as well (Harrison & Tong, 2009; Serences et al., 2009). The fidelity with which remembered information can be decoded in these cortical regions predicts the precision of behavioral reports of these memories (Emrich et al., 2013). Furthermore, this mirroring of attention and memory persists along the visual hierarchy: orienting attention to maintained faces or scenes evokes patterns of activity in visual cortex analogous to external visual attention to those categories (Lepsien & Nobre, 2006; Lepsien et al., 2011). Beyond modulating sensory cortex, attention to perceived stimuli and attention to remembered stimuli recruit a similar broad network of brain regions (e.g., Nobre et al., 2004; Lepsien et al., 2005; Lepsien et al., 2011; see Corbetta & Shulman, 2002; Xu, 2017, 2018). These data suggest that attention may act on working memory representations similarly to how it acts on perceptual representations. Causal interventions using transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) further support this view. TMS of both sensory cortex (Zokaei et al., 2014) and higher-order attentional regions (Rose et al., 2016) can increase memory for an unattended stimulus. Stimulation in these cases is thought to mimic attention, thus enhancing perceptual processing of the unattended stimulus and rescuing memory degradation. Common neural correlates between attention and working memory have been used to argue that these processes are intricately related, if not two sides of the same coin. Yet, such findings leave open important questions about the nature of the representations being measured: To what extent does neural activity during a working memory delay reflect the contents of attention, as opposed to the contents of memory (over which attention can operate)? In other words, is it possible to actively maintain information (the hallmark of working memory) without internally attending to it? Converging studies utilizing fMRI (Lewis-Peacock et al., 2012) and EEG (LaRocque et al., 2013) attempted to de-confound attention and memory, and found that delay period activity reflects the contents of attention rather than memory more generally. When presented with distractors during a delay period, patterns of brain activity contained information about the category of the distractor image (which presumably had captured attention), rather than the category of the to-be-remembered target image (Lewis-Peacock et al., 2012). Critically, behavioral performance indicated that the target was still held in working memory, despite not being decodable during the delay period. Relatedly, adding a distractor task or intervening items during the delay period disrupts maintenance signals in visual cortex without impairing behavior (Miller et al., 1993; Miller & Desimone, 1994; Miller et al., 1996; Bettencourt & Xu, 2015). These findings reveal that neural representations thought to be related to working memory per se may in fact reflect the allocation of attention, either internally (to memories) or externally (to distractor stimuli). Of course, working memory can survive distraction, suggesting that while visual areas may reflect the contents of attention rather than memory, other brain regions that exhibit persistent delay period activity, such as prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices (see Xu, 2017; Myers et al., 2017), may represent the contents of memory more faithfully. These regions may better track working memory performance in the face of distraction and may coordinate memory-related activity in other cortical regions, including sensory systems, through top-down control. Working memory influences attentional selection The above section discussed how attention can be allocated in working memory, but the reverse may also be true. Indeed, the contents of working memory can drive the allocation of attention. Theories of attention posit that allocating attention voluntarily requires holding a target in mind (see Desimone & Duncan, 1995), a process that fundamentally relies on working memory. This endogenous or goal-directed attention differs from exogenous or stimulus-driven control, in which attention is captured in an obligatory way by salient, bottom-up information, such as abrupt onsets or stimulus changes (Chun et al., 2011). The link between endogenous attention and working memory has been demonstrated by studies of task interference. When working memory is placed under load, performance on a concurrent attention task suffers from increased distractor interference (de Fockert et al., 2001). This is thought to reflect a diminished ability to hold the attentional target in mind when working memory is otherwise consumed with task-irrelevant information. Working memory determines not only whether attention can be allocated, but also how it is allocated. That is, even in cases when there is no direct competition between working memory and attention, the contents of working memory can bias attention. This biasing can occur in space: responses to an attention probe are faster when that probe is presented on the same side of the screen as an object being held in working memory (Downing, 2000). The biasing can also occur over features: visual search is facilitated when the search target matches the feature(s) of an item being held in working memory (Soto et al., 2005). Subsequent work examined whether this biasing of attention by memory is a consequence of the task design and explicit strategy, or whether memory signals can automatically guide attention. Consistent with an automatic effect, items held in memory facilitate search for a matching target even when such matches are statistically improbable (Carlisle & Woodman, 2011), and even when other salient cues that capture exogenous attention are present (Soto et al., 2006). Interactions between working memory and attention can also be revealed by examining how working memory influences physiological responses related to perception and attention. For example, pupil size can be modulated by working memory demands (Unsworth & Robison, 2016b; Zokaei et al., 2019). Capitalizing on the fact that the pupil dilates to low light levels and constricts to higher light levels, pupil response can be used as an index of what information is being attended to in working memory (Figure 2A-B). Specifically, while maintaining an orientation in working memory, the pupil is modulated by the brightness of the oriented grating at encoding, despite the fact that the brightness is irrelevant to the memory task and will not be probed at test. Further, pupil size provides a dynamic, online signature, as it changes when attention is switched between items in working memory (Zokaei et al., 2019). Working memory can also influence attentional guidance via direct influences on the oculomotor system (Figure 2C-D). For example, when asked to report the color of a stimulus from working memory, gaze is directed toward the spatial location in which the stimulus had appeared (van Ede et al., 2019). These cases provide compelling evidence that working memory can powerfully influence attentional selection. However, whether and how these pupillary and oculomotor responses affect subsequent attentional, perceptual, and mnemonic processing remains unstudied. A B ** 4 + Memory array Mean pupil size 3.5 + Delay + Auditory Cue Probe 3 2.5 2 1.5 1 Adjustment Probe 0.5 Da rk Bri ght C D + Memory array + Delay + Probe + Adjustment Probe Gaze position onxaxis (%, dva) Cued it em 4 0.23 L item R item –4 –0.23 0 500 1,000 Time after probe (ms) Figure 2. Working memory modulates physiological responses implicated in visual attention. (A) Participants were shown two Gabor patches of different illumination and different orientations. During the delay period, they were auditorily cued that one of the patches would be probed for orientation report. (B) Pupil size during the delay period was modulated by the task-irrelevant luminance of the to-be-reported item. For example, when cued that the darker patch’s orientation would be probed, the size of the pupil was larger. Adapted with permission from Zokaei et al. (2019). (C) Participants were shown two oriented lines of different colors. At test, they were cued with the color of one of the lines, indicating which orientation to report. (D) Gaze was biased towards the location of the tobe-reported item. Adapted with permission from van Ede et al. (2019). Working memory as internal attention In addition to bidirectional behavioral influences and shared neural substrates, working memory and attention have also been tightly linked at the trait level (see Engle, 2002) and may share cognitive resources (see Chun, 2011). Indeed, individuals with greater working memory capacity perform better on attentional tasks (Kane et al., 2001), and attentional ability accounts for a significant proportion of the variance in working memory capacity (Unsworth & Spillers, 2010; Unsworth et al., 2014). Low working memory capacity individuals demonstrate both more interference from irrelevant distraction (Fukuda & Vogel, 2009; Fukuda & Vogel, 2011) and more attentional lapses (Unsworth & Robison, 2016a). Attention and working memory also covary within an individual over time. Fluctuations in attentional control (Adam et al., 2015) and attention lapses (Unsworth & Robison, 2016a) predict working memory performance on a trialby-trial basis. Although these data implicate attentional mechanisms in working memory success, a recent study with real-time manipulation and analysis of attention provided more direct evidence (deBettencourt et al., 2019). Participants completed a sustained attention task in which colored shapes appeared on the screen and participants reported the shape. The distribution of the shapes over time was skewed such that certain shapes had a much higher probability than others, and thus attentional vigilance was required to not miss rare shapes. Some trials were followed by a working memory probe, in which participants reported the color at each location in an array. This dual-task design allowed for trial-specific measures of attention (response time and accuracy to classify the shape) and working memory (proportion correct on the probe). After confirming a trial-by-trial correlation between these measures, the researchers sought a more causal link. Specifically, they assessed attentional fluctuations in real-time by comparing the response time (RT) from the most recent three sustained attention trials to the distribution of RTs throughout the task. Memory probes were presented when the trailing window RT was either faster than a threshold, related to attentional lapses (i.e., reduced vigilance/habitual responding), or slower, as a control for better attention. Consistent with lapses of attention causing working memory errors, performance was lower for memory probes triggered after a fast vs. slow trailing RT, with fewer items stored and thus lower capacity. Further evidence of shared resources comes from studies demonstrating competition between attention and working memory. For example, when spatial information is held in working memory, attention-demanding visual search is slowed (Woodman & Luck, 2004). Similarly, increasing working memory load leads to more interference from distractors in an attention task (de Fockert et al., 2001; Lavie et al., 2004). Together, these findings suggest that increasing demand on working memory leaves fewer cognitive resources for effortful aspects of attention, such as filtering out distraction or staying on task. Interestingly, such competition may be modality-specific: loading up working memory with non-overlapping spatial information does not impair visual search (Woodman et al., 2001), and interference is reduced when distractors are more similar to the items in working memory (Park et al., 2007). The specificity of these competitive effects suggests that the resources being shared are not generic (e.g., motivation, control), but rather tied more closely to the sensory systems and constraints at play. Notably, such results support the concept that there may be discrete working memory subsystems even within the visual domain (Smith et al., 1995; Hakim et al., 2019), contrasting with prior work that conceptualized two discrete, sensory-specific subsystems: one responsible for both visual and spatial information (visuospatial sketchpad) and one for verbal information (phonological loop; see Baddeley, 1992). Although questions remain about the format of working memory, the body of work demonstrating interactions between visual attention and visual memory has led to the suggestion that working memory may in fact be a form of internal attention rather than a separate memory system (e.g., Awh & Jonides, 2001; Postle, 2006; Chun et al., 2011). The depth of this analogy is still debated (see Xu, 2017; Myers et al., 2017), but it may be most applicable to forms of working memory without significant distraction (e.g., visual short-term memory and retro-cuing tasks), whereas more complex and continuous tasks (e.g., n-back and AX-CPT) may recruit additional processes and brain regions to prevent distraction. Moreover, internal attention itself may not be a singular concept, and may instead depend on multiple attentional components, such as for prioritizing locations and maintaining object information (Hakim et al., 2019). This latter multi-component view of internal attention highlights an interesting fact about the working memory literature: most of the studies described throughout this section focused on the maintenance of either spatial locations or objects/features, but generally not both (c.f., Rao et al., 1997; Postle & D’Esposito, 1999). Natural settings often demand that we integrate these sources of information, in order to keep track of objects in particular locations. Given that such integration has long been fundamental to theories of attention, as in the role of spatial maps in feature integration theory (e.g., Treisman & Gelade, 1980), a fruitful avenue for further exploring links between attention and working memory may be to address more configural, multidimensional forms of working memory. Attention and long-term memory Attention allows us to pursue goals and adapt behavior in the moment, and thus naturally interacts with working memory as a short-term store of information that may be immediately relevant. However, attention can also guide, and be guided by, long-term memory (Hutchinson & Turk-Browne, 2012). To this end, attention helps determine not only which aspects of an experience are encoded into episodic memory to influence future processing (including future deployment of attention), but also which retrieval cues are selected, in turn impacting which episodic memories are retrieved. Here, we describe different conceptions of attention and the relationship of each to episodic memory, we distinguish effects related to encoding vs. retrieval, and we consider how these effects manifest from the known neural mechanisms of attention. Attention during episodic encoding It may seem intuitive that if you pay attention to something, you are more likely to remember it later. However, attention is not a singular construct: attention can operate in different modalities, over objects, features, spatial locations, and points in time (Chun et al., 2011). Further, as noted earlier, attentional selection is not only endogenous or goal-directed, but can also be captured by salient environmental cues in an exogenous or stimulus-driven manner. Thus, understanding how attention interacts with long-term memory requires considering the possibility that different kinds of attention exhibit different interactions. Throughout this section, we focus on various ways in which researchers have manipulated the allocation of attention to memoranda in order to shed light on these complex interactions. One task manipulation intended to broadly disrupt attention is to require processing of two or more streams of information simultaneously. If attention is required for memory encoding, then encoding should be worse under such conditions of divided attention (analogous to the interference between working memory and attention discussed above). Indeed, across a range of tasks, dividing attention during encoding impairs later memory performance (Craik et al., 1996; Fernandes & Moscovitch, 2000; see Craik, 2001). Interestingly, divided attention only modestly influences judgments of familiarity, whereas it more strongly disrupts recollection (Gardiner & Parkin, 1990; see Chapter 5.6). Recollection is thought to reveal richer, more contextual episodic memories that rely upon the hippocampus (see Yonelinas, 2002), suggesting that attention may play a role in relational binding, rather than merely in memory for constituent items. On the other hand, effects of divided attention on associative memory have been linked to disrupted encoding of individual items (Naveh-Benjamin et al., 2003). Converging evidence that attention influences episodic encoding comes from studies of selective attention, a different kind of attention manipulation in which participants prioritize only one of two or more streams of information while ignoring the other(s). For example, in a dichotic listening paradigm with different auditory inputs in each ear, one of which is attended, only the contents of the attended ear are later remembered (Moray, 1959). Likewise, when attending to one category of composite face-scene images, only the image from the attended category is remembered (Yi & Chun, 2005). Selective attention can also be manipulated on a trial-by-trial basis with cuing tasks. For example, spatial cues enhance subsequent memory encoding for items that occur at the cued locations (Uncapher et al., 2011; Turk-Browne et al., 2013). In all of these cases, the impact of attention on memory may be a side effect of attentional modulation of perception (Rees et al., 1999; Rees & Lavie, 2001). During a dichotic listening paradigm, for example, if participants do not perceptually register the contents of the unattended ear, this information may not persist far enough along the sensory processing hierarchy to be encoded into memory systems. Relatedly, selective attention is not all or none, but rather graded based on how deeply stimuli must be processed to perform a task (Craik & Lockhart, 1972). These different levels of processing have distinct mnemonic consequences: when oriented to the phonetic features of a word (e.g., whether it rhymes with another word) vs. to its semantic features, participants perform worse on a standard recognition memory test but better on a test where the words rhymed with the encoded words (Morris et al., 1977). Beyond conceiving of attention as a limited resource that must be divided or a selective process that enhances relevant information and suppresses irrelevant information, sustaining a consistent attentional state over long periods of time is itself effortful and tiring. Such vigilance is also related to episodic memory. For example, lapses in sustained attention — reflected in faster, habitual responses to stimuli over a period of time — are associated with worse subsequent memory for those stimuli (deBettencourt et al., 2018). Our ability to allocate attention is also influenced by the temporal structure of our experience. Shifts in context, or event boundaries, can redirect attention (Zacks et al., 2007), perhaps due to changes in low-level visual information (Huff et al., 2012) and autonomic arousal (Clewett et al., 2020). Such event-based modulation of attention can result in a bias away from associative processing and toward local details, resulting in enhancement of memory for items occurring at the boundaries (Boltz, 1992; Gold et al., 2017; Heusser et al., 2018) and decreased integration across events (Heusser et al., 2018; see Clewett et al., 2019). On the other hand, promoting associative processing within an event (e.g., by encouraging participants to form links between successive items) benefits temporal order memory, and shifting attention away from associative processing (by inserting a distractor task between stimuli) reduces temporal memory (DuBrow & Davachi, 2013). Nevertheless, even with an explicit associative encoding strategy, event boundaries disrupt temporal order memory, suggesting that event boundaries may have shifted attention away from or disrupted associative processing. The studies above involve attention to neutral, innocuous stimuli, but psychological factors such as stress and emotion also control attention (Anderson & Phelps, 2001; Öhman et al., 2001; see Chapters 3.7 and 9.9). Exogenous attentional allocation to emotional stimuli can enhance memory for the arousing stimulus, often at the cost of memory for surrounding (even if relevant) details (see Mather & Sutherland, 2011). For instance, attentional capture by a weapon can enhance memory for the weapon at the cost of memory for the perpetrator (Loftus et al., 1987). Moreover, while the details of arousing stimuli are better remembered, the association between arousing and neutral stimuli does not similarly benefit (Mather et al., 2009). Relatedly, reward can shift attention (see Maunsell, 2004; Awh et al., 2012), perhaps in part explaining the role of reward in episodic memory (e.g., Adcock et al., 2006; Wolosin et al., 2013; Clewett & Mather, 2014; Mason et al., 2017). The behavioral findings discussed above shed light on how differential allocation of attention can influence memory, but are agnostic as to whether these effects result from feedforward modulation of sensory processing or direct modulation of episodic memory processes. Understanding the neural mechanisms of these interactions between attention and memory sheds light on this question. The biased competition theory of attention is a prominent model of how attention operates in sensory cortex (Desimone & Duncan, 1995). Visual environments contain multiple features and objects at different locations, all vying for representation in stimulus-selective visual areas. Attention to one of these objects modulates the amplitude of the neural response it evokes, strengthening its representation relative to unattended objects. As these neural signals propagate forward in the sensory hierarchy, competitive dynamics in each layer amplify this difference between attended and unattended stimuli, resulting in an increasingly exclusive representation of attended stimuli (see Kastner & Ungerleider, 2000). This attentional modulation is observed across levels of the visual hierarchy, including in the cortical areas that directly feed into the hippocampus (O’Craven et al., 1999; Uncapher & Rugg, 2009; Dudukovic et al., 2011; TurkBrowne et al., 2013). Such modulation has also been observed in the hippocampus proper (Uncapher & Rugg, 2009; Carr et al., 2013), though not consistently (Yamaguchi et al., 2004), and may be dependent upon whether attention is drawn to items or relations (Dudukovic et al., 2011; Córdova et al., 2019). Regardless, hippocampal modulation may reflect a form of biased competition, whereby attentionally modulated signals that emanate at earlier stages, perhaps in MTL cortex, propagate downstream. This spreading is consistent with the fact that subsequent memory can be predicted from attentional modulation in both MTL cortex and the hippocampus (Uncapher & Rugg, 2009; Carr et al., 2013; Turk-Browne et al., 2013). Biased competition has also been invoked to explain findings of how emotion influences memory, based on evidence that arousing stimuli are prioritized in sensory processing, resulting in downstream memory enhancements (Mather & Sutherland, 2011). This account also relates to the role of repetition suppression in attention and memory. Repeated stimuli evoke weaker responses in stimulus-selective sensory cortex relative to novel stimuli. This disparity can tilt competition toward novel stimuli, enhancing their encoding into episodic memory (Hutchinson et al., 2016). This would have the advantage of prioritizing the encoding of novel information in memory. Indeed, repetition suppression for a stimulus has been linked to both implicit (Maccotta & Buckner, 2004; Wig et al., 2005; Ward et al., 2013) and explicit (Turk-Browne et al., 2006) memory (see Chapters 2.5 and 5.8). The fact that repetition suppression only occurs for attended stimuli (Eger et al., 2004; Yi & Chun, 2005), combined with the fact that attention enhances memory encoding, further suggests that repetition suppression may be a neural marker of memory formation. Together, these findings are consistent with an account of how attention and memory interact that is centered on biased competition during sensory processing. Beyond the visual system, several regions of frontoparietal cortex are thought to exert attentional control over the way stimuli are represented and routed in the brain (see Noudoost et al., 2010). It follows that these regions may also influence the way in which memories are encoded. In particular, these regions are connected into two networks — the dorsal attention network involved in top-down, goal-directed control and the ventral attention network involved in stimulus-driven reorienting — and these networks can have opposite effects on memory. During encoding, greater activity in dorsal attention regions such as intraparietal sulcus (IPS) is associated with better memory (Uncapher et al., 2011), whereas greater activity in ventral attention regions such as temporoparietal junction (TPJ) is associated with worse memory (Uncapher et al., 2011; Turk-Browne et al., 2013). These networks may impact memory by modulating stimulus representations and biasing competition in visual cortex: functional connectivity between visual regions and IPS prior to encoding predicts better memory, whereas functional connectivity between visual regions and TPJ during encoding predicts worse memory (Uncapher et al., 2011). Thus, the dorsal attention network may facilitate visual processing to enhance the information available for memory encoding, whereas the ventral attention network may redirect processing away from relevant stimuli, reducing the information available. In addition to shifting functional connectivity between visual and frontoparietal regions, attention can also switch connectivity within the visual system. Specifically, attention may alter the flow of information along the sensory hierarchy based on task goals (Al-Aidroos et al., 2012; Bosman et al., 2012). For example, low-level visual regions show increased functional connectivity with the fusiform face area (FFA) when attending to faces and with the parahippocampal place area (PPA) when attending to scenes (Al-Aidroos et al., 2012). Similar modulation has been observed in the MTL, with ventral visual cortex switching functional connectivity between parahippocampal cortex and perirhinal cortex during scene vs. face attention, respectively (Córdova et al., 2016). It remains an open question how these connectivity patterns influence hippocampal representation and episodic memory encoding. Indeed, the effects of attention on the hippocampus may be qualitatively different than effects of attention on cortex. Studies of both animal (Kentros et al., 2004; Muzzio et al., 2009; Fenton et al., 2010) and human (Aly & Turk-Browne, 2016a; Aly & Turk-Browne, 2016b; Córdova et al., 2019) hippocampus suggest that, rather than increasing the amplitude of neural activity, attention increases the stability of neural activity patterns. Specifically, the hippocampus shows sensitivity to attentional state such that, for example, attending to spatial information evokes similar patterns of activity in the hippocampus across trials, whereas attending to item information evokes a different, yet reliable pattern of activity in the hippocampus (Muzzio et al., 2009; Aly & Turk-Browne, 2016a). Furthermore, the stabilization of hippocampal representations by attention is related to episodic memory encoding (Figure 3). Greater pattern similarity to an attentional state template is associated with better subsequent memory for the corresponding task-relevant features of a stimulus (Aly & Turk-Browne, 2016b). Where does the attentional modulation of hippocampus originate? In other words, is representational stability in the hippocampus merely a different manifestation of the feedforward influence of attention on the sensory hierarchy, or does it reflect a more direct routing of attentional signals to the hippocampus through neuromodulation? For example, reward and arousal — which both bias attention — may act on the hippocampal system directly via dopaminergic (Shohamy & Adcock, 2010) and noradrenergic (Mather et al., 2016) neurotransmitter systems, respectively. The acetylcholine system has also been implicated in both attention and memory (Newman et al., 2012). Future studies examining the timing of attentional signals in sensory cortex, attentional control networks, and the hippocampus, as well as signals relating to neuromodulation, will shed light on the nature of attentional modulation of the hippocampus. B same - different template pattern similarity r() A Figure 3. Hippocampal representations are modulated by attention in a way that supports episodic memory encoding (Aly & Turk-Browne, 2016b). (A) Analysis approach and task design. In phase 1, participants were presented with rendered images of rooms in an art museum and instructed to attend either to the layout of the room or the style of the art. Voxel patterns of fMRI activity in the hippocampus from this phase were used as attentional templates for room and art attentional states, respectively. In phase 2, participants incidentally encoded similar but trial-unique images while performing one of two related attention tasks. In art blocks, participants detected one-back repetitions of artwork in the same style/by the same artist (despite being different pieces). In room blocks, participants detected one-back repetitions of the layouts (despite surface differences). The pattern of hippocampal activity for each encoding trial was correlated with the attentional templates for the same vs. different attentional states. In phase 3, participants completed a surprise memory test for the images from phase 2. (B) Subsequently remembered items exhibited greater pattern similarity to the template that matched during their attentional state incidental encoding than to the other template. In other words, memory was enhanced when the hippocampus was in the correct attentional state during encoding. Adapted with permission from Aly & Turk-Browne (2016b). Attention during episodic retrieval The influence of attention on episodic memory arises not only at the time of encoding, determining which information gets stored, but also at the time of retrieval, determining which information gets accessed and reported (see Tarder-Stoll et al., 2020). Indeed, attention can be oriented internally to long-term episodic memories, just as it can be allocated to the contents of short-term, working memories (Chun & Johnson, 2011). This analogy is further supported by findings that episodic memory retrieval can be impaired by the same kinds of distraction as working memory (Fernandes & Moscovitch, 2000; Wais & Gazzaley, 2011), presumably because the distractors compete with the contents of memory for attentional resources (Wais et al., 2010), and by findings that eye movements are spontaneously biased toward the encoded locations of retrieved information (Johansson et al., 2012; Johansson & Johansson, 2014). Dividing attention at retrieval also reduces the likelihood that an item will be retrieved again in a later test (Dudukovic et al., 2009), suggesting that attention also plays a role in reinforcing and/or re-encoding memories at retrieval. These combined effects on both encoding and retrieval position attention as important for disambiguating novel from familiar inputs. This role is echoed by studies of attentional modulation during hippocampal retrieval. Computational models (e.g., Lisman & Grace, 2005) and fMRI evidence (Kumaran & Maguire, 2006; Kumaran & Maguire, 2007; Chen et al., 2011; Duncan et al., 2012) implicate the hippocampus in mismatch detection, when a retrieved memory (e.g., an expectation of familiarity) conflicts with current sensory experience (i.e., unexpected novelty). Mismatch signals are sensitive to attentional demands. For example, MTL activity is enhanced when judging the novelty but not the recency of an item (Dudukovic & Wagner, 2007). Novelty signals in the hippocampus are also sensitive to attentional state, as demonstrated by dissociations across recognition memory tasks involving old, similar, and new items (Hashimoto et al., 2012). When attending to perceptual attributes, the hippocampus responds equivalently to similar and new items (reflecting the need to classify similar items as “new”), whereas when attending to semantic attributes, the hippocampus responds equivalently to similar and old items (reflecting the need to classify similar items as “old”). This is consistent with findings from intracranial EEG, in which hippocampal activity distinguishes similar vs. old items only when the task required such a discrimination (Lohnas et al., 2018). In addition to the hippocampus, the parietal cortex has been consistently implicated in the retrieval of memories (Wagner et al., 2005; Kuhl & Chun, 2014). Given that the parietal cortex is strongly implicated in attention (Corbetta & Shulman, 2002; Xu, 2018), the role of the parietal regions in episodic retrieval was thought to be reflective of its role in attentional selection (Cabeza et al., 2008; Ciaramelli et al., 2008). Notably, however, discrete anatomical subregions of parietal cortex seem to support attention vs. memory retrieval (Hutchinson et al., 2009). Indeed, separate functional subregions of the parietal cortex track successful memory recollection vs. attentional benefits, with some subregions only tracking memory performance, and others tracking attentional components of memory retrieval (Hutchinson et al., 2014). Despite debate about the precise anatomical substrates of attention vs. memory functions in parietal cortex, there is evidence of a unique role for parietal cortex in attention-like memory selection (Figure 4). Specifically, attentional effects on retrieval in frontal and parietal regions can be dissociated from effects in the MTL (Kuhl et al., 2013). In a study designed to tease apart the contributions of these regions to memory retrieval, participants first learned associations between a word and either a face or a scene that appeared on either the left or right of the screen. During a subsequent test phase, participants were cued with a word and asked to report either the category or the location of the associated image. Multi-voxel pattern analysis (MVPA) was applied during the test trials to decode evidence for face vs. scene categories. Such category information could be decoded reliably from frontoparietal regions, but not medial temporal lobe regions, on trials that required category retrieval. However, on location retrieval trials, in which the category information was retrieved incidentally (as assessed in a later post-test), medial temporal lobe regions but not frontoparietal regions exhibited reliable category evidence. A B Figure 4. Frontoparietal regions support goal-directed long-term memory retrieval. (A) During study, participants were presented with word-face or word-scene pairings. After each round of study, they were shown a word and asked to recall one of two source features, either whether the word was presented with a face or a scene, or whether the word’s associated face/scene was presented on the left of the right of the screen. Subsequently, in a surprise post-test they were presented again with words and were asked to recall both the associated category and location, regardless of which was tested initially. (B) Category evidence from the initial test, as a function of post-test performance. On trials in which the category association was tested initially, greater category evidence in prefrontal cortex (PFC) and lateral parietal cortex (LPC) predicted better subsequent memory. In the MTL, category evidence on trials in which the location association was tested initially predicted subsequent memory. Adapted with permission from Kuhl et al. (2013). These findings suggest that parietal cortex may be particularly important for implementing task goals and selecting relevant content from memory. In particular, dorsal and lateral parietal regions — implicated in top-down attentional control (see Corbetta & Shulman, 2002) — are sensitive to retrieval goals, such that the contents of memory representations reflect which feature was cued (Favila et al., 2018). As further support for the role of parietal cortex in memory retrieval, visual information is represented more strongly in lateral parietal regions during memory retrieval than during perception, whereas the reverse is true in occipitotemporal cortex (Xiao et al., 2017; Favila et al., 2018). Although interpreting the role of the parietal cortex in memory retrieval as an attentional selection mechanism is appealing, it is important to note that the evidence above demonstrates that parietal representations can be modulated by attentional state or goal-relevance. Whether the parietal cortex plays a critical role in the selection of memories at retrieval remains an open question. Specifically, patients with parietal lesions do not exhibit profound deficits in memory retrieval (see Cabeza et al., 2008); rather, when reported, the deficits are subtle, such as decreased vividness of memory (Berryhill et al., 2007) and a reduced sense of recollection (as opposed to general familiarity; Davidson et al., 2008). Nonetheless, regardless of the precise neural substrates, considering memory retrieval as a form of attentional selection remains a useful framework (e.g., Anderson & Spellman, 1995; Levy & Anderson, 2002), not only for understanding the effects of attention on memory retrieval, but also for understanding the general properties of memory retrieval across memory systems such as working memory and episodic memory. Guidance of attention by episodic memory The previous section reviewed how attention can guide memory retrieval, but retrieved memories in turn play an important role in guiding attention (see Desimone, 1996; Hutchinson & Turk-Browne, 2012). An early demonstration of such memory-guided attention compared how attention is cued by past experience vs. current sensory information (Summerfield et al., 2006). In this study, participants were pre-exposed to visual scenes and tasked with finding a hidden key target. One day later, participants returned to perform a covert (without eye movements) visual search task in which they again had to find keys. Some of the scenes were familiar from the prior day (memory cue) and other scenes were novel but presented with a brief additional stimulus that oriented participants to the location of the target (visual cue). Each condition was contrasted against a baseline of scenes with equal viewing time but no cue (i.e., familiar scenes that did not have a learned target and novel scenes with a neutral cue, respectively). As expected, visual search was facilitated for the visual-cue condition relative to baseline. However, visual search was also facilitated in the memory-cue condition, even though the target location was only indicated by episodic memory (with no explicit visual cues). Consistent with the role of longterm memory in guiding attention, activity in the hippocampus selectively predicted the performance benefit on the memory-cue trials. Subsequent work using a similar paradigm established that episodic memory guides attention by modulating perceptual processing (Stokes et al., 2012). Both EEG and fMRI correlates of neural responses in visual cortex were modulated by the presence of a memory cue. This can be interpreted as a memory-driven preparatory response in visual cortex that facilitated processing, analogous to the biasing signals observed in standard goal-directed attention (see Noudoost et al., 2010). One key difference from goal-directed attention, however, is that memory-guided attention may reflect hippocampal control over visual cortex, rather than frontoparietal control. Indeed, memory-guided attention can affect processing without an explicit instruction or orientation towards the target (i.e., in the absence of a visual search task), and even after just a single encoding opportunity (Becker & Rasmussen, 2008; Ciaramelli et al., 2009). The proposed role of episodic memory in guiding attention is further supported by evidence that memory — and the hippocampus — are important for determining where we move our eyes (see Meister & Buffalo, 2016). This observation has been exploited to study memory in infants (see Chapter 8.2). For example, in the visual paired comparison task, infants tend to look more to novel than familiar stimuli all else being equal (see Rose et al., 2004). Adults and nonhuman primates also exhibit preferences for novel stimuli (e.g., Manns et al., 2000; Zola et al., 2000). Preferential looking towards novel stimuli is adaptive, as it allows an observer to prioritize extracting new information from the world rather than perseverating on information that is already known. Relatedly, when a familiar stimulus has changed in some way, it would be adaptive to update this representation. Indeed, participants also preferentially look to aspects of remembered stimuli that have changed. This was demonstrated by an early study linking the hippocampus to eye movements (Ryan et al., 2000). In this study, participants first encoded a set of scenes and then were presented with the scenes again after they had been altered by changing the presence or relations of objects in each scene. Healthy individuals tended to look more at the manipulated region of the image, whereas patients with hippocampal damage and associated amnesia did not. In other words, overt attention was guided by discrepancies between perception and memory, a process which depended on the hippocampus. These results dovetail with the previously discussed findings that the hippocampus is sensitive to mismatches between perceived and remembered information (Lisman & Grace, 2005) and critically highlight how such mismatch representations may be consequential for guiding attention. Similar guidance by relational memory has been observed in other paradigms. For example, when faces are superimposed on scenes during encoding, healthy individuals look longer during test at a face that matches the current scene than at other, equally familiar faces that had been paired with other scenes. This behavior is correlated with activation of the hippocampus in fMRI (Hannula & Ranganath, 2009) and is eliminated in patients with hippocampal damage (Hannula et al., 2007). These effects occur rapidly upon encountering familiar stimuli, suggesting that memory interacts with attentional processes triggered by the sensory stimulus itself (Ryan et al., 2007). Interestingly, hippocampal guidance of overt attention can be detected even in the absence of explicit memory for the familiar stimulus (Hannula & Ranganath, 2009; Ryals et al., 2015). Notably, many of the studies above are interpreted as an eye-movement measure of memory, rather than an effect of memory on attention. However, in the real-world, we are rarely confronted with the task of merely retrieving memories. Rather, we use memory to guide future behavior, and these memory-related eye movements may play an important role in guiding our attention. This form of attention is overt, as compared to the guidance of covert attention that occurs while, for example, fixating during visual search. Nevertheless, such overt attention can have dramatic effects on how we select and represent the sensory environment, including because foveal (vs. peripheral) vision is much higher fidelity. Given this attentional guidance by memory, it seems that memory actually guides memory — we use memory to orient our attention in the world, which in turn influences what we subsequently encode into memory. Attention and learning The sections above considered interactions between attention and memory formed after one or a small number of exposures, as is typical for working memory and episodic memory. However, we regularly encounter the same items over time, in similar or different viewpoints, contexts, and tasks. This repeated and varied exposure can enrich our memories for objects, places, or people, resulting in abstracted representations stripped of idiosyncratic features, which generalize better to future experiences and are more useful for prediction and facilitating processing (see Chapters 2.2, 2.8, 6.6, and 7.1). Such memories must be held separate from representations of individual experiences in episodic memory, which depend critically on idiosyncratic features to enable the retrieval of specific moments in time and space. Generalized memory representations must be learned over multiple exposures in which individual (episodic) experiences are integrated rapidly or gradually over time (McClelland et al., 1995; Schapiro et al., 2017). Here, we focus on two such forms of memory integration — statistical learning and category learning — though other forms of learning including reinforcement learning (Mackintosh, 1975; Niv et al., 2015; see Chapter 4.4) and motor skill learning (Nissen & Bullemer, 1987; Wulf et al., 1998; see Chapters 2.4 and 11.4) also have deep interactions with attention. Attention and statistical learning To study how people learn regularities across repeated experiences, a process known as statistical learning, participants are repeatedly presented with configurations (e.g., Fiser & Aslin, 2001) or sequences (e.g., Turk-Browne et al., 2005) of stimuli with hidden structure. For example, shapes might appear sequentially over time in repeated groupings of three, where the first shape (A) is always followed by the second (B) and then the third (C). What appears before A and after C is random and there are no other indications of where the groupings begin and end, necessitating the extraction of transition probabilities to find the boundaries. The emerging picture about the role of attention in statistical learning is complex. On one hand, it is impossible to know in advance, or even after a few experiences, which items will be part of regularities, and thus it is important to track multiple sets of statistics in the environment. On the other hand, truly tracking all possible regularities would subject the mind to combinatorial explosion. The balance of evidence suggests that attention does constrain statistical learning, determining the stimuli over which regularities can be extracted (Jiménez & Méndez, 1999; Jiang & Chun, 2001; Baker et al., 2004; Toro et al., 2005; Turk-Browne et al., 2005; Emberson et al., 2011; Campbell et al., 2012; cf. Saffran et al., 1997; Musz et al., 2015). Whether attention is strictly required for any learning to take place or serves a more modulatory role, whether the influence of attention on learning reflects limited constraints on processing resources (Desimone & Duncan, 1995) or whether attention is selectively allocated in order to uncover relevant structure (Dayan et al., 2000), and how effects differ across tasks (Vickery et al., 2018) and modalities (Frost et al., 2015) remain important questions. Similar to the bidirectional interactions between episodic memory and attention, not only does attention influence statistical learning, but regularities also capture and guide attention. For example, in a visual search task, if the configuration of distractors is repeated and fixed with respect to the target location, participants get faster at responding to the target, an effect known as contextual cuing (Chun & Jiang, 1998). Visual search is also facilitated when target location or identity is paired to distractors that have a consistent identity, even if their locations are variable across trials (Chun & Jiang, 1999; Endo & Takeda, 2004). This and other work suggest that multiple aspects of visual context can guide attention. Such effects can be viewed as resulting from spatial or object-based statistical learning (Orbán et al., 2008; Vickery & Jiang, 2009) and/or from gradual encoding of relational memory (Ryan et al., 2000; Summerfield et al., 2006). Moreover, attention is not only guided towards regions of space that contain targets, but also away from regions containing distracting information (Wang & Theeuwes, 2018). Furthermore, attention can be biased by regularities even in cases where the regularities do not provide predictive information useful for a task. This has been demonstrated by presenting multiple task-irrelevant streams of shapes at the same time and interrupting the streams occasionally with a visual search trial (Zhao et al., 2013). When one of the streams contains regularities, visual search is facilitated at that location, suggesting that the stream had drawn attention. Critically, the location of regularities was randomized with respect to the search targets, so this attentional bias occurred automatically. This was replicated in additional experiments where multiple shape streams differing in color or orientation were interleaved at a single, central location. Regularities in one of those streams induced attentional capture on the interspersed visual search trials for the corresponding dimension. For example, if the sequence of green shapes contained embedded structure, then visual search was faster when the target was green and slower when a distractor was green, compared to a different color like red associated with a random stream. Thus, despite competition amongst multiple streams of information, attention can be spontaneously biased towards the information that contains reliable structure. This has also been demonstrated in infants, who preferentially attend to sources of information with moderately complex regularities that need to be learned (Kidd et al., 2012; Kidd et al., 2014), perhaps a mechanism that allows infants to efficiently acquire the structure of their environment (Saffran et al., 1996; Kirkham et al, 2002; Aslin, 2017; Saffran & Kirkham, 2018). The guidance of attention by regularities can flexibly influence not only the locus but also the spatial scale of attention. Regularities at a global scale drive more global allocation of attention, whereas regularities at a local scale drive attention to local details (Zhao & Luo, 2017). In addition, statistical learning induces object-based attention effects, such that presenting targets at either side of two paired objects produced faster detection than presenting targets at either side of two shapes which were not paired together in learning (Zhao et al., 2014). Thus, regularities can also guide attention within objects learned from repeated conjunctions of features and parts. Why is attention automatically drawn towards regularities? One reason is that structure often conveys goal-relevant information, such as in contextual cuing where the learned structure reliably predicts the target location. Attentional biases to structure may also serve an adaptive purpose in facilitating additional or new learning (Kosie & Baldwin, 2019). There is a chickenand-egg problem, however, with attention needed for learning but learning helping to guide attention. A potential solution could be that simple regularities (e.g., stimulus repetition) might be detected without attention, but once learned, draw attention. This may in turn permit more sophisticated learning, a greater attentional bias, etc., until sufficient learning has occurred and attention can be redirected elsewhere. An alternative solution could be that attention is deployed in an exploratory manner in novel environments, and when it happens to fall on a location or feature with structure, rapid learning captures attention and reduces further exploration. Consistent with this, attention remains biased towards locations that contained structure even after the regularities are replaced by randomness or another unknown set of regularities (Yu & Zhao, 2015). Another interpretation of this emerging literature is that attention is repelled away from randomness rather than towards structure. Indeed, initially encountering random information in a configuration or sequence prior to regularities prevents statistical learning of structure that emerges later (Jungé et al., 2007; Gebhart et al., 2009). Future work is needed to further clarify the nature and purpose of interactions between attention and statistical learning, perhaps by distinguishing between different conceptions of attention and by comparing different measures of statistical learning. Attention and category learning The term statistical learning is often applied to the learning of regularities in how multiple objects are organized over space and time. However, there are also regularities across experiences with individual objects, resulting in abstract categories that structure our world into types and concepts. As with statistical learning, attention plays an important role in category learning. Attention can alter how stimuli are represented in both the mind and brain, leading to different patterns of category classification in different contexts (e.g., Nosofsky, 1986; Johansen & Palmeri, 2002; Mack et al., 2016). Task context can in turn affect attention and influence categorization. For example, interleaved study of two categories biases attention to features that differentiate between the two categories, whereas blocked study of a single category results in attention to features shared within the category (Carvalho & Goldstone, 2017). Furthermore, researchers have distinguished between perceptual attention, which is driven by the physical features of a stimulus such as salience and is correlated with the speed of processing, and decisional attention, which is driven by the relation of features to a category boundary and classification. Perceptual attention may influence category decisions based on a single feature, whereas decisional attention dominates when categorization requires the integration of features across diagnostic dimensions (e.g., Maddox, 2002). Consistent with these findings, models of category learning — in both behavior and neural implementation — consider attention as an important intermediary for learning the mapping between objects and categories (e.g., Kruschke, 1992; Minda & Smith, 2002; Love et al., 2004). Accordingly, attention can be used to arbitrate between models of category learning, for example, by measuring eye movements during a categorization task (Rehder & Hoffman, 2005). In this study, fixations shifted towards highly diagnostic features and away from the least diagnostic features, reflecting learning of the relevant category features. However, in the last block of learning, participants still reliably fixated the least diagnostic dimension, suggesting a sub-optimal allocation of attention. This pattern of attention is consistent with exemplar rather than prototype models of categorization: prototype models suggest that we represent a template containing the most common, or ideal, features of a category, whereas exemplar models posit that we hold each example of a category in memory, and thus would store even idiosyncratic or uninformative features. Indeed, although allocating attention to non-diagnostic features may be sub-optimal from a category learning perspective (Minda & Smith, 2002), it may prove adaptive for memory more generally. That is, learning about the world is not only about learning categories, but also about forming memories of the particular experiences we have, idiosyncratic features and all. Conclusions and Open Questions Throughout this chapter, we have highlighted interactions between multiple forms of attention and memory. Across working memory, episodic memory, and statistical learning, the same pattern of results emerges: attention is important for determining both whether a memory is formed in the first place and, if so, what content it contains; and those memory representations are in turn important for guiding attention. In real life, attention and memory do not operate in isolation. Rather, our mind seamlessly switches attention between information from the current sensory environment, the environment from moments ago, and environments in the more distant past. As we move our eyes across a scene, we perceptually register the part being sampled, we hold in working memory the parts that are out of view, and these representations in turn trigger the retrieval of episodic memories, regularities, and categories. The amalgam of this new experience — mixing present with past — itself gets encoded into working memory and eventually long-term memory, and gets abstracted over during statistical learning. This intricate and recursive web of perceptual and mnemonic experiences highlights the complexity of interactions between cognitive systems: where attention is allocated will influence what kind of memory is formed, and what kind of memory is retrieved will influence the features to which attention is allocated. And because attention has limited capacity over features, locations, and time (e.g., Shapiro et al., 1997; Lavie et al., 2004; Franconeri et al., 2007), these interactions will not always be beneficial. For example, attention to regularities may compete with attention to episodic details (Sherman & Turk-Browne, 2020). What, then, determines which memories guide attention? Not only might working memory and long-term memory compete, but so too might different forms of long-term memory. For example, attention can be guided not only by memory for contextual information stored in the hippocampus, but also by memory for the mapping between the stimulus location and the required response stored in the striatum (Goldfarb et al., 2016). That said, many of the documented cases of attentional guidance from long-term memory are related to the hippocampus. Not only is episodic memory-guided attention dependent upon the hippocampus (e.g., Ryan et al., 2000), but so are contextual cuing (e.g., Chun & Phelps, 1999), statistical learning (e.g., Schapiro et al., 2014), and category learning (e.g., Foerde et al., 2013). Thus, competition within the hippocampus may serve to arbitrate between different kinds of memory and ultimately play a critical role in determining the allocation of attention. Further work on how competition between memory systems influences attention is needed. The shared reliance of working memory and long-term memory on attention highlights the importance of their relationship. What role does attention play in transforming information held in working memory into long-term memory representations? How does information retrieved from long-term memory get transferred into working memory for online use? Can effects of long-term memory on attention be understood as merely effects of working memory on attention? In this vein, it is also important to emphasize the ubiquity of attention in a variety of cognitive processes. Considering how these other modulated factors, such as context, value, and emotion, relate to attentional processing may help to clarify observed bidirectional interactions between attention and memory. References Adam, K. C., Mance, I., Fukuda, K., & Vogel, E. K. (2015). The contribution of attentional lapses to individual differences in visual working memory capacity. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 27(8), 1601-1616. Adcock, R. A., Thangavel, A., Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., Knutson, B., & Gabrieli, J. D. (2006). Reward-motivated learning: Mesolimbic activation precedes memory formation. Neuron, 50(3), 507-517. Aly, M., & Turk-Browne, N. B. (2016a). Attention stabilizes representations in the human hippocampus. Cerebral Cortex, 26(2), 783-796. Aly, M., & Turk-Browne, N. B. (2016b). Attention promotes episodic encoding by stabilizing hippocampal representations. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113(4), E420E429. Al-Aidroos, N., Said, C. P., & Turk-Browne, N. B. (2012). Top-down attention switches coupling between low-level and high-level areas of human visual cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(36), 14675-14680. Anderson, A. K., & Phelps, E. A. (2001). Lesions of the human amygdala impair enhanced perception of emotionally salient events. Nature, 411(6835), 305-311. Anderson, M. C., & Spellman, B. A. (1995). On the status of inhibitory mechanisms in cognition: Memory retrieval as a model case. Psychological Review, 102(1), 68-100. Aslin, R. N. (2017). Statistical learning: A powerful mechanism that operates by mere exposure. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 8(1-2), e1373. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1373 Awh, E., Belopolsky, A. V., & Theeuwes, J. (2012). Top-down versus bottom-up attentional control: A failed theoretical dichotomy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(8), 437-443. Awh, E., & Jonides, J. (2001). Overlapping mechanisms of attention and spatial working memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5(3), 119-126. Awh, E., Jonides, J., & Reuter-Lorenz, P. A. (1998). Rehearsal in spatial working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24(3), 780-790. Baddeley, A. (1992). Working memory. Science, 255(5044), 556-559. Baker, C. I., Olson, C. R., & Behrmann, M. (2004). Role of attention and perceptual grouping in visual statistical learning. Psychological Science, 15(7), 460-466. Becker, M. W., & Rasmussen, I. P. (2008). Guidance of attention to objects and locations by long-term memory of natural scenes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34(6), 1325-1338. Berryhill, M. E., Phuong, L., Picasso, L., Cabeza, R., & Olson, I. R. (2007). Parietal lobe and episodic memory: Bilateral damage causes impaired free recall of autobiographical memory. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(52), 14415-14423. Bettencourt, K. C., & Xu, Y. (2016). Decoding the content of visual short-term memory under distraction in occipital and parietal areas. Nature Neuroscience, 19(1), 150-157. Boltz, M. (1992). Temporal accent structure and the remembering of filmed narratives. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18(1), 90-105. Bosman, C. A., Schoffelen, J. M., Brunet, N., Oostenveld, R., Bastos, A. M., Womelsdorf, T., ... & Fries, P. (2012). Attentional stimulus selection through selective synchronization between monkey visual areas. Neuron, 75(5), 875-888. Cabeza, R., Ciaramelli, E., Olson, I. R., & Moscovitch, M. (2008). The parietal cortex and episodic memory: An attentional account. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(8), 613-625. Campbell, K. L., Zimerman, S., Healey, M. K., Lee, M., & Hasher, L. (2012). Age differences in visual statistical learning. Psychology and Aging, 27(3), 650-656. Carr, V. A., Engel, S. A., & Knowlton, B. J. (2013). Top-down modulation of hippocampal encoding activity as measured by high-resolution functional MRI. Neuropsychologia, 51(10), 1829-1837. Carlisle, N. B., & Woodman, G. F. (2011). Automatic and strategic effects in the guidance of attention by working memory representations. Acta Psychologica, 137(2), 217-225. Carvalho, P. F., & Goldstone, R. L. (2017). The sequence of study changes what information is attended to, encoded, and remembered during category learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 43(11), 1699-1719. Chen, J., Olsen, R. K., Preston, A. R., Glover, G. H., & Wagner, A. D. (2011). Associative retrieval processes in the human medial temporal lobe: Hippocampal retrieval success and CA1 mismatch detection. Learning & Memory, 18(8), 523-528. Chun, M. M. (2011). Visual working memory as visual attention sustained internally over time. Neuropsychologia, 49(6), 1407-1409. Chun, M. M., Golomb, J. D., & Turk-Browne, N. B. (2011). A taxonomy of external and internal attention. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 73-101. Chun, M. M., & Jiang, Y. (1998). Contextual cueing: Implicit learning and memory of visual context guides spatial attention. Cognitive Psychology, 36(1), 28-71. Chun, M. M., & Jiang, Y. (1999). Top-down attentional guidance based on implicit learning of visual covariation. Psychological Science, 10(4), 360-365. Chun, M. M., & Johnson, M. K. (2011). Memory: Enduring traces of perceptual and reflective attention. Neuron, 72(4), 520-535. Chun, M. M., & Phelps, E. A. (1999). Memory deficits for implicit contextual information in amnesic subjects with hippocampal damage. Nature Neuroscience, 2(9), 844-847. Ciaramelli, E., Grady, C. L., & Moscovitch, M. (2008). Top-down and bottom-up attention to memory: A hypothesis (AtoM) on the role of the posterior parietal cortex in memory retrieval. Neuropsychologia, 46(7), 1828-1851. Ciaramelli, E., Lin, O., & Moscovitch, M. (2009). Episodic memory for spatial context biases spatial attention. Experimental Brain Research, 192(3), 511-520. Clewett, D., DuBrow, S., & Davachi, L. (2019). Transcending time in the brain: How event memories are constructed from experience. Hippocampus, 29(3), 162-183. Clewett, D., Gasser, C., & Davachi, L. (2020). Pupil-linked arousal signals track the temporal organization of events in memory. Nature Communications, 11(1), 1-14. Clewett, D. V., & Mather, M. (2014). Not all that glittered is gold: Neural mechanisms that determine when reward will enhance or impair memory. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 8, 1-3. Corbetta, M., & Shulman, G. L. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(3), 201-215. Córdova, N. I., Tompary, A., & Turk-Browne, N. B. (2016). Attentional modulation of background connectivity between ventral visual cortex and the medial temporal lobe. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 134, 115-122. Córdova, N. I., Turk‐Browne, N. B., & Aly, M. (2019). Focusing on what matters: Modulation of the human hippocampus by relational attention. Hippocampus, 29(11), 1025-1037. Cowan, N. (1998). Attention and memory: An integrated framework (Vol. 26). Oxford University Press. Cowan, N. (1999). An embedded-processes model of working memory. In A. Miyake & P. Shah (Eds.), Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control (p. 62-101). Cambridge University Press. Craik, F. I. (2001). Effects of dividing attention on encoding and retrieval processes. In H.L. Roediger III, J.S. Nairne, I. Neath, & A.M. Surprenant (Eds.), Science conference series. The nature of remembering: Essays in honor of Robert G. Crowder (p. 55-68). American Psychological Association. Craik, F. I., Govoni, R., Naveh-Benjamin, M., & Anderson, N. D. (1996). The effects of divided attention on encoding and retrieval processes in human memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 125(2), 159-180. Craik, F. I., & Lockhart, R. S. (1972). Levels of processing: A framework for memory research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11(6), 671-684. Davidson, P. S., Anaki, D., Ciaramelli, E., Cohn, M., Kim, A. S., Murphy, K. J., ... & Levine, B. (2008). Does lateral parietal cortex support episodic memory?: Evidence from focal lesion patients. Neuropsychologia, 46(7), 1743-1755. Dayan, P., Kakade, S., & Montague, P. R. (2000). Learning and selective attention. Nature Neuroscience, 3(11), 1218-1223. deBettencourt, M. T., Keene, P. A., Awh, E., & Vogel, E. K. (2019). Real-time triggering reveals concurrent lapses of attention and working memory. Nature Human Behaviour, 3(8), 808-816. deBettencourt, M. T., Norman, K. A., & Turk-Browne, N. B. (2018). Forgetting from lapses of sustained attention. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(2), 605-611. de Fockert, J. W., Rees, G., Frith, C. D., & Lavie, N. (2001). The role of working memory in visual selective attention. Science, 291(5509), 1803-1806. Desimone, R. (1996). Neural mechanisms for visual memory and their role in attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 93(24), 13494-13499. Desimone, R., & Duncan, J. (1995). Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 18(1), 193-222. Downing, P. E. (2000). Interactions between visual working memory and selective attention. Psychological Science, 11(6), 467-473. DuBrow, S., & Davachi, L. (2013). The influence of context boundaries on memory for the sequential order of events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142(4), 1277-1286. Dudukovic, N. M., DuBrow, S., & Wagner, A. D. (2009). Attention during memory retrieval enhances future remembering. Memory & Cognition, 37(7), 953-961. Dudukovic, N. M., Preston, A. R., Archie, J. J., Glover, G. H., & Wagner, A. D. (2011). Highresolution fMRI reveals match enhancement and attentional modulation in the human medial temporal lobe. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(3), 670-682. Dudukovic, N. M., & Wagner, A. D. (2007). Goal-dependent modulation of declarative memory: Neural correlates of temporal recency decisions and novelty detection. Neuropsychologia, 45(11), 2608-2620. Duncan, K., Ketz, N., Inati, S. J., & Davachi, L. (2012). Evidence for area CA1 as a match/mismatch detector: A high‐resolution fMRI study of the human hippocampus. Hippocampus, 22(3), 389-398. Eger, E., Henson, R. N. A., Driver, J., & Dolan, R. J. (2004). BOLD repetition decreases in object-responsive ventral visual areas depend on spatial attention. Journal of Neurophysiology, 92(2), 1241-1247. Emberson, L. L., Conway, C. M., & Christiansen, M. H. (2011). Timing is everything: Changes in presentation rate have opposite effects on auditory and visual implicit statistical learning. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64(5), 1021-1040. Emrich, S. M., Riggall, A. C., LaRocque, J. J., & Postle, B. R. (2013). Distributed patterns of activity in sensory cortex reflect the precision of multiple items maintained in visual short-term memory. Journal of Neuroscience, 33(15), 6516-6523. Endo, N., & Takeda, Y. (2004). Selective learning of spatial configuration and object identity in visual search. Perception & Psychophysics, 66(2), 293-302. Engle, R. W. (2002). Working memory capacity as executive attention. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11(1), 19-23. Favila, S. E., Samide, R., Sweigart, S. C., & Kuhl, B. A. (2018). Parietal representations of stimulus features are amplified during memory retrieval and flexibly aligned with top-down goals. Journal of Neuroscience, 38(36), 7809-7821. Fenton, A. A., Lytton, W. W., Barry, J. M., Lenck-Santini, P. P., Zinyuk, L. E., Kubík, Š., ... & Olypher, A. V. (2010). Attention-like modulation of hippocampus place cell discharge. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(13), 4613-4625. Fernandes, M. A., & Moscovitch, M. (2000). Divided attention and memory: Evidence of substantial interference effects at retrieval and encoding. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129(2), 155-176. Fiser, J., & Aslin, R. N. (2001). Unsupervised statistical learning of higher-order spatial structures from visual scenes. Psychological Science, 12(6), 499-504. Foerde, K., Race, E., Verfaellie, M., & Shohamy, D. (2013). A role for the medial temporal lobe in feedback-driven learning: Evidence from amnesia. Journal of Neuroscience, 33(13), 56985704. Franconeri, S. L., Alvarez, G. A., & Enns, J. T. (2007). How many locations can be selected at once? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33(5), 10031012. Frost, R., Armstrong, B. C., Siegelman, N., & Christiansen, M. H. (2015). Domain generality versus modality specificity: The paradox of statistical learning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(3), 117-125. Fukuda, K., & Vogel, E. K. (2009). Human variation in overriding attentional capture. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(27), 8726-8733. Fukuda, K., & Vogel, E. K. (2011). Individual differences in recovery time from attentional capture. Psychological Science, 22(3), 361-368. Garavan, H. (1998). Serial attention within working memory. Memory & Cognition, 26(2), 263276. Gardiner, J. M., & Parkin, A. J. (1990). Attention and recollective experience in recognition memory. Memory & Cognition, 18(6), 579-583. Gebhart, A. L., Aslin, R. N., & Newport, E. L. (2009). Changing structures in midstream: Learning along the statistical garden path. Cognitive Science, 33(6), 1087-1116. Gold, D. A., Zacks, J. M., & Flores, S. (2017). Effects of cues to event segmentation on subsequent memory. Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications, 2(1), 1-15. Goldfarb, E. V., Chun, M. M., & Phelps, E. A. (2016). Memory-guided attention: Independent contributions of the hippocampus and striatum. Neuron, 89(2), 317-324. Griffin, I. C., & Nobre, A. C. (2003). Orienting attention to locations in internal representations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15(8), 1176-1194. Hakim, N., Adam, K. C., Gunseli, E., Awh, E., & Vogel, E. K. (2019). Dissecting the neural focus of attention reveals distinct processes for spatial attention and object-based storage in visual working memory. Psychological Science, 30(4), 526-540. Hannula, D. E., & Ranganath, C. (2009). The eyes have it: Hippocampal activity predicts expression of memory in eye movements. Neuron, 63(5), 592-599. Hannula, D. E., Ryan, J. D., Tranel, D., & Cohen, N. J. (2007). Rapid onset relational memory effects are evident in eye movement behavior, but not in hippocampal amnesia. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(10), 1690-1705. Harrison, S. A., & Tong, F. (2009). Decoding reveals the contents of visual working memory in early visual areas. Nature, 458(7238), 632-635. Hashimoto, R., Abe, N., Ueno, A., Fujii, T., Takahashi, S., & Mori, E. (2012). Changing the criteria for old/new recognition judgments can modulate activity in the anterior hippocampus. Hippocampus, 22(2), 141-148. Heusser, A. C., Ezzyat, Y., Shiff, I., & Davachi, L. (2018). Perceptual boundaries cause mnemonic trade-offs between local boundary processing and across-trial associative binding. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 44(7), 1075-1090. Huff, M., Papenmeier, F., & Zacks, J. M. (2012). Visual target detection is impaired at event boundaries. Visual Cognition, 20(7), 848-864. Hutchinson, J. B., Pak, S. S., & Turk-Browne, N. B. (2016). Biased competition during longterm memory formation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 28(1), 187-197. Hutchinson, J. B., & Turk-Browne, N. B. (2012). Memory-guided attention: Control from multiple memory systems. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(12), 576-579. Hutchinson, J. B., Uncapher, M. R., & Wagner, A. D. (2009). Posterior parietal cortex and episodic retrieval: Convergent and divergent effects of attention and memory. Learning & Memory, 16(6), 343-356. Hutchinson, J. B., Uncapher, M. R., Weiner, K. S., Bressler, D. W., Silver, M. A., Preston, A. R., & Wagner, A. D. (2014). Functional heterogeneity in posterior parietal cortex across attention and episodic memory retrieval. Cerebral Cortex, 24(1), 49-66. Jiang, Y., & Chun, M. M. (2001). Selective attention modulates implicit learning. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Section A, 54(4), 1105-1124. Jiménez, L., & Méndez, C. (1999). Which attention is needed for implicit sequence learning? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25(1), 236-259. Johansen, M. K., & Palmeri, T. J. (2002). Are there representational shifts during category learning? Cognitive Psychology, 45(4), 482-553. Johansson, R., Holsanova, J., Dewhurst, R., & Holmqvist, K. (2012). Eye movements during scene recollection have a functional role, but they are not reinstatements of those produced during encoding. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38(5), 1289-1314. Johansson, R., & Johansson, M. (2014). Look here, eye movements play a functional role in memory retrieval. Psychological Science, 25(1), 236-242. Jungé, J. A., Scholl, B. J., & Chun, M. M. (2007). How is spatial context learning integrated over signal versus noise? A primacy effect in contextual cueing. Visual Cognition, 15(1), 1-11. Kane, M. J., Bleckley, M. K., Conway, A. R., & Engle, R. W. (2001). A controlled-attention view of working-memory capacity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130(2), 169183. Kastner, S., & Ungerleider, L. G. (2000). Mechanisms of visual attention in the human cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 23(1), 315-341. Kentros, C. G., Agnihotri, N. T., Streater, S., Hawkins, R. D., & Kandel, E. R. (2004). Increased attention to spatial context increases both place field stability and spatial memory. Neuron, 42(2), 283-295. Kidd, C., Piantadosi, S. T., & Aslin, R. N. (2012). The Goldilocks effect: Human infants allocate attention to visual sequences that are neither too simple nor too complex. PloS One, 7(5), e36399. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0036399 Kidd, C., Piantadosi, S. T., & Aslin, R. N. (2014). The Goldilocks effect in infant auditory attention. Child Development, 85(5), 1795-1804. Kirkham, N. Z., Slemmer, J. A., & Johnson, S. P. (2002). Visual statistical learning in infancy: Evidence for a domain general learning mechanism. Cognition, 83(2), B35-B42. Kosie, J. E., & Baldwin, D. (2019). Attention rapidly reorganizes to naturally occurring structure in a novel activity sequence. Cognition, 182, 31-44. Kruschke, J. K. (1992). ALCOVE: An exemplar-based connectionist model of category learning. Psychological Review, 99(1), 22-44. Kuhl, B. A., & Chun, M. M. (2014). Successful remembering elicits event-specific activity patterns in lateral parietal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(23), 8051-8060. Kuhl, B. A., Johnson, M. K., & Chun, M. M. (2013). Dissociable neural mechanisms for goaldirected versus incidental memory reactivation. Journal of Neuroscience, 33(41), 16099-16109. Kumaran, D., & Maguire, E. A. (2006). An unexpected sequence of events: Mismatch detection in the human hippocampus. PLoS Biology, 4(12), e424. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3000442 Kumaran, D., & Maguire, E. A. (2007). Match–mismatch processes underlie human hippocampal responses to associative novelty. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(32), 8517-8524. LaRocque, J. J., Eichenbaum, A. S., Starrett, M. J., Rose, N. S., Emrich, S. M., & Postle, B. R. (2015). The short-and long-term fates of memory items retained outside the focus of attention. Memory & Cognition, 43(3), 453-468. LaRocque, J. J., Lewis-Peacock, J. A., Drysdale, A. T., Oberauer, K., & Postle, B. R. (2013). Decoding attended information in short-term memory: An EEG study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25(1), 127-142. Lavie, N., Hirst, A., De Fockert, J. W., & Viding, E. (2004). Load theory of selective attention and cognitive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133(3), 339-354. Lepsien, J., Griffin, I. C., Devlin, J. T., & Nobre, A. C. (2005). Directing spatial attention in mental representations: Interactions between attentional orienting and working-memory load. NeuroImage, 26(3), 733-743. Lepsien, J., & Nobre, A. C. (2006). Attentional modulation of object representations in working memory. Cerebral Cortex, 17(9), 2072-2083. Lepsien, J., Thornton, I., & Nobre, A. C. (2011). Modulation of working-memory maintenance by directed attention. Neuropsychologia, 49(6), 1569-1577. Levy, B. J., & Anderson, M. C. (2002). Inhibitory processes and the control of memory retrieval. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(7), 299-305. Lewis-Peacock, J. A., Drysdale, A. T., Oberauer, K., & Postle, B. R. (2012). Neural evidence for a distinction between short-term memory and the focus of attention. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24(1), 61-79. Lisman, J. E., & Grace, A. A. (2005). The hippocampal-VTA loop: Controlling the entry of information into long-term memory. Neuron, 46(5), 703-713. Loftus, E. F., Loftus, G. R., & Messo, J. (1987). Some facts about “weapon focus”. Law and Human Behavior, 11(1), 55-62. Lohnas, L. J., Duncan, K., Doyle, W. K., Thesen, T., Devinsky, O., & Davachi, L. (2018). Timeresolved neural reinstatement and pattern separation during memory decisions in human hippocampus. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(31), E7418-E7427. Love, B. C., Medin, D. L., & Gureckis, T. M. (2004). SUSTAIN: A network model of category learning. Psychological Review, 111(2), 309-332. Maccotta, L., & Buckner, R. L. (2004). Evidence for neural effects of repetition that directly correlate with behavioral priming. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(9), 1625-1632. Mack, M. L., Love, B. C., & Preston, A. R. (2016). Dynamic updating of hippocampal object representations reflects new conceptual knowledge. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113(46), 13203-13208. Mackintosh, N. J. (1975). A theory of attention: Variations in the associability of stimuli with reinforcement. Psychological Review, 82(4), 276-298. Maddox, W. T. (2002). Learning and attention in multidimensional identification and categorization: Separating low-level perceptual processes and high-level decisional processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28(1), 99-115. Makovski, T., & Jiang, Y. V. (2007). Distributing versus focusing attention in visual short-term memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14(6), 1072-1078. Manns, J. R., Stark, C. E., & Squire, L. R. (2000). The visual paired-comparison task as a measure of declarative memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 97(22), 12375-12379. Mason, A., Farrell, S., Howard-Jones, P., & Ludwig, C. J. (2017). The role of reward and reward uncertainty in episodic memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 96, 62-77. Mather, M., Clewett, D., Sakaki, M., & Harley, C. W. (2016). Norepinephrine ignites local hotspots of neuronal excitation: How arousal amplifies selectivity in perception and memory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39, E200. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X15000667 Mather, M., Gorlick, M., & Nesmith, K. (2009). The limits of arousal's memory impairing effects on nearby information. The American Journal of Psychology, 122(3), 349-369. Mather, M., & Sutherland, M. R. (2011). Arousal-biased competition in perception and memory. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(2), 114-133. Matsukura, M., Luck, S. J., & Vecera, S. P. (2007). Attention effects during visual short-term memory maintenance: Protection or prioritization? Perception & Psychophysics, 69(8), 14221434. Maunsell, J. H. (2004). Neuronal representations of cognitive state: Reward or attention? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(6), 261-265. McClelland, J. L., McNaughton, B. L., & O'Reilly, R. C. (1995). Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: Insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory. Psychological Review, 102(3), 419-457. Meister, M. L., & Buffalo, E. A. (2016). Getting directions from the hippocampus: The neural connection between looking and memory. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 134, 135-144. Miller, E. K., & Desimone, R. (1994). Parallel neuronal mechanisms for short-term memory. Science, 263(5146), 520-522. Miller, E. K., Erickson, C. A., & Desimone, R. (1996). Neural mechanisms of visual working memory in prefrontal cortex of the macaque. Journal of Neuroscience, 16(16), 5154-5167. Miller, E. K., Li, L., & Desimone, R. (1993). Activity of neurons in anterior inferior temporal cortex during a short-term memory task. Journal of Neuroscience, 13(4), 1460-1478. Minda, J. P., & Smith, J. D. (2002). Comparing prototype-based and exemplar-based accounts of category learning and attentional allocation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28(2), 275-292. Moray, N. (1959). Attention in dichotic listening: Affective cues and the influence of instructions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 11(1), 56-60. Morris, C. D., Bransford, J. D., & Franks, J. J. (1977). Levels of processing versus transfer appropriate processing. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 16(5), 519-533. Musz, E., Weber, M. J., & Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2015). Visual statistical learning is not reliably modulated by selective attention to isolated events. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 77(1), 78-96. Muzzio, I. A., Kentros, C., & Kandel, E. (2009). What is remembered? Role of attention on the encoding and retrieval of hippocampal representations. The Journal of Physiology, 587(12), 2837-2854. Myers, N. E., Stokes, M. G., & Nobre, A. C. (2017). Prioritizing information during working memory: Beyond sustained internal attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21(6), 449-461. Naveh-Benjamin, M., Guez, J., & Marom, M. (2003). The effects of divided attention at encoding on item and associative memory. Memory & Cognition, 31(7), 1021-1035. Newman, E. L., Gupta, K., Climer, J. R., Monaghan, C. K., & Hasselmo, M. E. (2012). Cholinergic modulation of cognitive processing: insights drawn from computational models. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 6(24), 1-19. Niklaus, M., Nobre, A. C., & van Ede, F. (2017). Feature-based attentional weighting and spreading in visual working memory. Scientific Reports, 7, 42384. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep42384 Nissen, M. J., & Bullemer, P. (1987). Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures. Cognitive Psychology, 19(1), 1-32. Niv, Y., Daniel, R., Geana, A., Gershman, S. J., Leong, Y. C., Radulescu, A., & Wilson, R. C. (2015). Reinforcement learning in multidimensional environments relies on attention mechanisms. Journal of Neuroscience, 35(21), 8145-8157. Nobre, A. C., Coull, J. T., Maquet, P., Frith, C. D., Vandenberghe, R., & Mesulam, M. M. (2004). Orienting attention to locations in perceptual versus mental representations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(3), 363-373. Nosofsky, R. M. (1986). Attention, similarity, and the identification–categorization relationship. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 115(1), 39-57. Noudoost, B., Chang, M. H., Steinmetz, N. A., & Moore, T. (2010). Top-down control of visual attention. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 20(2), 183-190. O'Craven, K. M., Downing, P. E., & Kanwisher, N. (1999). fMRI evidence for objects as the units of attentional selection. Nature, 401(6753), 584-587. Oberauer, K. (2002). Access to information in working memory: exploring the focus of attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28(3), 411-421. Öhman, A., Flykt, A., & Esteves, F. (2001). Emotion drives attention: Detecting the snake in the grass. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130(3), 466-478. Orbán, G., Fiser, J., Aslin, R. N., & Lengyel, M. (2008). Bayesian learning of visual chunks by human observers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(7), 2745-2750. Park, S., Kim, M. S., & Chun, M. M. (2007). Concurrent working memory load can facilitate selective attention: Evidence for specialized load. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33(5), 1062-1075. Postle, B. R. (2006). Working memory as an emergent property of the mind and brain. Neuroscience, 139(1), 23-38. Postle, B. R., & D’Esposito, M. (1999). “What”—then—“where” in visual working memory: An event-related fMRI study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11(6), 585-597. Rao, S. C., Rainer, G., & Miller, E. K. (1997). Integration of what and where in the primate prefrontal cortex. Science, 276(5313), 821-824. Rees, G., & Lavie, N. (2001). What can functional imaging reveal about the role of attention in visual awareness?. Neuropsychologia, 39(12), 1343-1353. Rees, G., Russell, C., Frith, C. D., & Driver, J. (1999). Inattentional blindness versus inattentional amnesia for fixated but ignored words. Science, 286(5449), 2504-2507. Rehder, B., & Hoffman, A. B. (2005). Thirty-something categorization results explained: Selective attention, eyetracking, and models of category learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31(5), 811-829. Rose, N. S., LaRocque, J. J., Riggall, A. C., Gosseries, O., Starrett, M. J., Meyering, E. E., & Postle, B. R. (2016). Reactivation of latent working memories with transcranial magnetic stimulation. Science, 354(6316), 1136-1139. Rose, S. A., Feldman, J. F., & Jankowski, J. J. (2004). Infant visual recognition memory. Developmental Review, 24(1), 74-100. Ryals, A. J., Wang, J. X., Polnaszek, K. L., & Voss, J. L. (2015). Hippocampal contribution to implicit configuration memory expressed via eye movements during scene exploration. Hippocampus, 25(9), 1028-1041. Ryan, J. D., Althoff, R. R., Whitlow, S., & Cohen, N. J. (2000). Amnesia is a deficit in relational memory. Psychological Science, 11(6), 454-461. Ryan, J. D., Hannula, D. E., & Cohen, N. J. (2007). The obligatory effects of memory on eye movements. Memory, 15(5), 508-525. Saffran, J. R., Aslin, R. N., & Newport, E. L. (1996). Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science, 274(5294), 1926-1928. Saffran, J. R., & Kirkham, N. Z. (2018). Infant statistical learning. Annual Review of Psychology, 69, 181-203. Saffran, J. R., Newport, E. L., Aslin, R. N., Tunick, R. A., & Barrueco, S. (1997). Incidental language learning: Listening (and learning) out of the corner of your ear. Psychological Science, 8(2), 101-105. Schapiro, A. C., Gregory, E., Landau, B., McCloskey, M., & Turk-Browne, N. B. (2014). The necessity of the medial temporal lobe for statistical learning. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 26(8), 1736-1747. Schapiro, A. C., Turk-Browne, N. B., Botvinick, M. M., & Norman, K. A. (2017). Complementary learning systems within the hippocampus: A neural network modelling approach to reconciling episodic memory with statistical learning. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 372(1711), 20160049. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2016.0049 Serences, J. T., Ester, E. F., Vogel, E. K., & Awh, E. (2009). Stimulus-specific delay activity in human primary visual cortex. Psychological Science, 20(2), 207-214. Shapiro, K. L., Raymond, J. E., & Arnell, K. M. (1997). The attentional blink. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 1(8), 291-296. Sherman, B. E., & Turk-Browne, N. B. (2020). Statistical prediction of the future impairs episodic encoding of the present. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 117(37), 22760-22770. Shohamy, D., & Adcock, R. A. (2010). Dopamine and adaptive memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(10), 464-472. Smith, E. E., Jonides, J., Koeppe, R. A., Awh, E., Schumacher, E. H., & Minoshima, S. (1995). Spatial versus object working memory: PET investigations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7(3), 337-356. Stokes, M. G., Atherton, K., Patai, E. Z., & Nobre, A. C. (2012). Long-term memory prepares neural activity for perception. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(6), E360E367. Soto, D., Heinke, D., Humphreys, G. W., & Blanco, M. J. (2005). Early, involuntary top-down guidance of attention from working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31(2), 248-261. Soto, D., Humphreys, G. W., & Heinke, D. (2006). Working memory can guide pop-out search. Vision Research, 46(6-7), 1010-1018. Summerfield, J. J., Lepsien, J., Gitelman, D. R., Mesulam, M. M., & Nobre, A. C. (2006). Orienting attention based on long-term memory experience. Neuron, 49(6), 905-916. Tarder-Stoll, H., Jayakumar, M., Dimsdale-Zucker, H. R., Günseli, E., & Aly, M. (2020). Dynamic internal states shape memory retrieval. Neuropsychologia, 138, 107328. Toro, J. M., Sinnett, S., & Soto-Faraco, S. (2005). Speech segmentation by statistical learning depends on attention. Cognition, 97(2), B25-B34. Treisman, A. M., & Gelade, G. (1980). A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12(1), 97-136. Turk-Browne, N. B., Golomb, J. D., & Chun, M. M. (2013). Complementary attentional components of successful memory encoding. NeuroImage, 66, 553-562. Turk-Browne, N. B., Jungé, J. A., & Scholl, B. J. (2005). The automaticity of visual statistical learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134(4), 552-564. Turk-Browne, N. B., Yi, D. J., & Chun, M. M. (2006). Linking implicit and explicit memory: Common encoding factors and shared representations. Neuron, 49(6), 917-927. Uncapher, M. R., Hutchinson, J. B., & Wagner, A. D. (2011). Dissociable effects of top-down and bottom-up attention during episodic encoding. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(35), 1261312628. Uncapher, M. R., & Rugg, M. D. (2009). Selecting for memory? The influence of selective attention on the mnemonic binding of contextual information. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(25), 8270-8279. Unsworth, N., Fukuda, K., Awh, E., & Vogel, E. K. (2014). Working memory and fluid intelligence: Capacity, attention control, and secondary memory retrieval. Cognitive Psychology, 71, 1-26. Unsworth, N., & Robison, M. K. (2016a). The influence of lapses of attention on working memory capacity. Memory & Cognition, 44(2), 188-196. Unsworth, N., & Robison, M. K. (2016b). Pupillary correlates of lapses of sustained attention. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 16(4), 601-615. Unsworth, N., & Spillers, G. J. (2010). Working memory capacity: Attention control, secondary memory, or both? A direct test of the dual-component model. Journal of Memory and Language, 62(4), 392-406. van Ede, F., Chekroud, S. R., & Nobre, A. C. (2019). Human gaze tracks attentional focusing in memorized visual space. Nature Human Behaviour, 3(5), 462-470. van Ede, F., Niklaus, M., & Nobre, A. C. (2017). Temporal expectations guide dynamic prioritization in visual working memory through attenuated α oscillations. Journal of Neuroscience, 37(2), 437-445. Vickery, T. J., & Jiang, Y. V. (2009). Associative grouping: Perceptual grouping of shapes by association. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71(4), 896-909. Vickery, T. J., Park, S. H., Gupta, J., & Berryhill, M. E. (2018). Tasks determine what is learned in visual statistical learning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(5), 1847-1854. Wais, P. E., & Gazzaley, A. (2011). The impact of auditory distraction on retrieval of visual memories. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18(6), 1090-1097. Wais, P. E., Rubens, M. T., Boccanfuso, J., & Gazzaley, A. (2010). Neural mechanisms underlying the impact of visual distraction on retrieval of long-term memory. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(25), 8541-8550. Wagner, A. D., Shannon, B. J., Kahn, I., & Buckner, R. L. (2005). Parietal lobe contributions to episodic memory retrieval. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(9), 445-453. Wang, B., & Theeuwes, J. (2018). Statistical regularities modulate attentional capture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 44(1), 13-17. Ward, E. J., Chun, M. M., & Kuhl, B. A. (2013). Repetition suppression and multi-voxel pattern similarity differentially track implicit and explicit visual memory. Journal of Neuroscience, 33(37), 14749-14757. Wig, G. S., Grafton, S. T., Demos, K. E., & Kelley, W. M. (2005). Reductions in neural activity underlie behavioral components of repetition priming. Nature Neuroscience, 8(9), 1228-1233. Wolosin, S. M., Zeithamova, D., & Preston, A. R. (2013). Distributed hippocampal patterns that discriminate reward context are associated with enhanced associative binding. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142(4), 1264-1276. Woodman, G. F., Vogel, E. K., & Luck, S. J. (2001). Visual search remains efficient when visual working memory is full. Psychological Science, 12(3), 219-224. Woodman, G. F., & Luck, S. J. (2004). Visual search is slowed when visuospatial working memory is occupied. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11(2), 269-274. Wulf, G., Höß, M., & Prinz, W. (1998). Instructions for motor learning: Differential effects of internal versus external focus of attention. Journal of Motor Behavior, 30(2), 169-179. Xiao, X., Dong, Q., Gao, J., Men, W., Poldrack, R. A., & Xue, G. (2017). Transformed neural pattern reinstatement during episodic memory retrieval. Journal of Neuroscience, 37(11), 29862998. Xu, Y. (2017). Reevaluating the sensory account of visual working memory storage. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21(10), 794-815. Xu, Y. (2018). The posterior parietal cortex in adaptive visual processing. Trends in Neurosciences, 41(11), 806-822. Yamaguchi, S., Hale, L. A., D'Esposito, M., & Knight, R. T. (2004). Rapid prefrontalhippocampal habituation to novel events. Journal of Neuroscience, 24(23), 5356-5363. Yonelinas, A. P. (2002). The nature of recollection and familiarity: A review of 30 years of research. Journal of Memory and Language, 46(3), 441-517. Yi, D. J., & Chun, M. M. (2005). Attentional modulation of learning-related repetition attenuation effects in human parahippocampal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 25(14), 35933600. Yu, R. Q., & Zhao, J. (2015). The persistence of the attentional bias to regularities in a changing environment. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 77(7), 2217-2228. Zacks, J. M., Speer, N. K., Swallow, K. M., Braver, T. S., & Reynolds, J. R. (2007). Event perception: A mind-brain perspective. Psychological Bulletin, 133(2), 273-293. Zhao, J., Al-Aidroos, N., & Turk-Browne, N. B. (2013). Attention is spontaneously biased toward regularities. Psychological Science, 24(5), 667-677. Zhao, J., & Luo, Y. (2017). Statistical regularities guide the spatial scale of attention. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 79(1), 24-30. Zhao, L., Cosman, J. D., Vatterott, D. B., Gupta, P., & Vecera, S. P. (2014). Visual statistical learning can drive object-based attentional selection. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 76(8), 2240-2248. Zokaei, N., Board, A. G., Manohar, S. G., & Nobre, A. C. (2019). Modulation of the pupillary response by the content of visual working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116(45), 228082-22810. Zokaei, N., Manohar, S., Husain, M., & Feredoes, E. (2014). Causal evidence for a privileged working memory state in early visual cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(1), 158-162. Zola, S. M., Squire, L. R., Teng, E., Stefanacci, L., Buffalo, E. A., & Clark, R. E. (2000). Impaired recognition memory in monkeys after damage limited to the hippocampal region. Journal of Neuroscience, 20(1), 451-463.