See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/230878361 Construct Validity of Three Depersonalization Measures in Trauma-Exposed College Students Article in Journal of Trauma & Dissociation · October 2012 DOI: 10.1080/15299732.2012.678470 · Source: PubMed CITATIONS READS 17 843 3 authors, including: Christy A Blevins Frank Weathers Overton Brooks VA Medical Center Auburn University 9 PUBLICATIONS 3,081 CITATIONS 113 PUBLICATIONS 28,870 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE All content following this page was uploaded by Frank Weathers on 23 May 2016. The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file. SEE PROFILE This article was downloaded by: [Auburn University] On: 25 October 2012, At: 10:21 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK Journal of Trauma & Dissociation Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/wjtd20 Construct Validity of Three Depersonalization Measures in TraumaExposed College Students a a Christy A. Blevins MS , Frank W. Weathers PhD & Elizabeth A. Mason PhD a a Department of Psychology, Auburn University, Auburn, Alabama, USA Accepted author version posted online: 05 Apr 2012.Version of record first published: 18 Sep 2012. To cite this article: Christy A. Blevins MS, Frank W. Weathers PhD & Elizabeth A. Mason PhD (2012): Construct Validity of Three Depersonalization Measures in Trauma-Exposed College Students, Journal of Trauma & Dissociation, 13:5, 539-553 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15299732.2012.678470 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representation that the contents will be complete or accurate or up to date. The accuracy of any instructions, formulae, and drug doses should be independently verified with primary sources. The publisher shall not be liable for any loss, actions, claims, proceedings, demand, or costs or damages whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with or arising out of the use of this material. Journal of Trauma & Dissociation, 13:539–553, 2012 Copyright © Taylor & Francis Group, LLC ISSN: 1529-9732 print/1529-9740 online DOI: 10.1080/15299732.2012.678470 Construct Validity of Three Depersonalization Measures in Trauma-Exposed College Students CHRISTY A. BLEVINS, MS, FRANK W. WEATHERS, PhD, and ELIZABETH A. MASON, PhD Downloaded by [Auburn University] at 10:21 25 October 2012 Department of Psychology, Auburn University, Auburn, Alabama, USA Depersonalization is a type of dissociation characterized by feelings of unreality and detachment from one’s sense of self. Despite a history rich in clinical description, the construct of depersonalization has proven difficult to define and measure. Available measures vary substantially in content, and all have relatively limited psychometric support. In this study the content validity, internal consistency, and convergent and discriminant validity of 3 depersonalization measures were compared in a sample of 209 trauma-exposed college students. Measures were the Dissociative Experiences Scale (E. M. Bernstein & F. W. Putnam, 1986), Cambridge Depersonalization Scale (CDS; M. Sierra & G. E. Berrios, 2000), and Multiscale Dissociation Inventory (MDI; J. Briere, 2002). All 3 measures exhibited adequate to high internal consistency for the depersonalization–derealization items. Based on D. Westen and R. Rosenthal’s (2003) procedure for quantifying construct validity, the CDS and MDI demonstrated the best fit with the predicted pattern of correlations with measures of other constructs. The CDS and MDI also demonstrated the strongest evidence of content validity. Overall, the results most strongly support the use of the CDS and MDI for assessing depersonalization in this population. KEYWORDS depersonalization, dissociation, construct validity, self-report Received 20 October 2011; accepted 1 March 2012. Address correspondence to Christy A. Blevins, MS, Auburn University, 226 Thach Hall, Auburn, AL 36849. E-mail: cat0011@tigermail.auburn.edu 539 Downloaded by [Auburn University] at 10:21 25 October 2012 540 C. A. Blevins et al. Depersonalization is a type of dissociation characterized by feelings of unreality and detachment from one’s sense of self that often leads to significant distress and impairment (Hunter, Phillips, Chalder, Sierra, & David, 2003). Associated symptoms include emotional numbing, altered perceptions of the physical self, loss of sense of agency, altered perceptions of autobiographical memories, heightened self-awareness, and altered visual perceptions (Sierra & Berrios, 2001). It is important to note that individuals experiencing depersonalization are not considered delusional or psychotic (Hunter et al., 2003). Although once considered rare, depersonalization is in fact commonly observed in a variety of settings. The lifetime prevalence of transient depersonalization symptoms ranges from 26% to 74% in student and nonclinical populations, and clinically significant symptoms are reported by 1%–2% of community samples and 42%–91% of psychiatric inpatients (Hunter, Sierra, & David, 2004). When symptoms of depersonalization become persistent or recurrent and are associated with significant distress or impairment, a diagnosis of depersonalization disorder may be warranted (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). Although the prevalence of depersonalization disorder has been estimated as relatively low, depersonalization as a dimensional construct is frequently described in the general population (e.g., Hunter et al., 2004). Since its introduction in the literature more than 100 years ago, investigators have extensively described the clinical features of depersonalization and proposed a number of conceptual models (see Sierra & Berrios, 1997, for a full discussion). Currently, the most comprehensive conceptualization of depersonalization is what Sierra and Berrios (1998) characterized as a neurobiological model. This model, which has been supported in a growing number of studies (e.g., Phillips et al., 2001; Sierra et al., 2002), postulates that depersonalization is a hardwired biological response involving simultaneous emotion inhibition and heightened vigilance. Depersonalization evolved to help individuals cope with life-threatening situations in which they have no control over their surroundings and the source of danger is unknown. In such situations, in which the typically adaptive fight-or-flight response may not be feasible, the “freezing” response of depersonalization may increase one’s chances of survival by inhibiting potentially disorganizing emotional arousal and increasing vigilant alertness, thereby enabling the individual to scan the environment for important information. When this response becomes generalized to situations not involving a threat to life, the result is a disturbing experience that combines an enhanced sensory clarity with a sudden lack of emotional feeling. Despite increasingly well-articulated theoretical models and a growing empirical literature on depersonalization (Sierra & Berrios, 2001), the transition from clinical description to operational definition and scientific investigation has proven difficult. First, depersonalization is a subjective Downloaded by [Auburn University] at 10:21 25 October 2012 Journal of Trauma & Dissociation, 13:539–553, 2012 541 experience with no characteristic behavioral manifestations (Radovic & Radovic, 2002) and thus is assessed primarily through self-report. Second, individuals use ambiguous language to describe their depersonalization experiences, such as “unreal” and “as if,” which poses a number of semantic difficulties (Radovic & Radovic, 2002). Third, depersonalization overlaps with derealization. Although the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition, Text Revision (DSM–IV–TR) distinguishes between derealization, defined as “an alteration in the perception or experience of the external world so that it seems strange or unreal” and depersonalization, defined as “alteration in the perception or experience of the self so that one feels detached from, and as if one is an outside observer of, one’s mental processes or body” (American Psychiatric Association, 2000, p. 822), there is no conclusive evidence that the two phenomena are distinct (Sierra & Berrios, 2001). Indeed, Dugas, who coined the term depersonalization in the late 1800s, recognized the essential equivalence of these two terms: “[In depersonalization] the individual feels a stranger amongst things, or if one prefers, things appear strange to him” (as translated by Sierra & Berrios, 1996, p. 456). Evidence from factor analytic studies has empirically supported this lack of distinction (Briere, Weathers, & Runtz, 2005; Stockdale, Gridley, Balogh, & Holtgraves, 2002). For example, Briere et al. (2005) conducted a factor analysis of the Multiscale Dissociation Inventory (MDI) and found that depersonalization and derealization scale items loaded onto one factor, suggesting that depersonalization and derealization represent a single underlying dimension. Given these challenges, it is not surprising that a consensus definition of depersonalization has not emerged, which thus hinders the development of psychometrically sound measures. Several measures have been developed, but they vary substantially in content and therefore assess either somewhat different constructs or different aspects of the same construct. Furthermore, none has been extensively validated. To address these concerns, the present study examined the content validity, internal consistency, and convergent and discriminant validity of three self-report measures of depersonalization: the Dissociative Experiences Scale (DES; Bernstein & Putnam, 1986), Cambridge Depersonalization Scale (CDS; Sierra & Berrios, 2000), and MDI (Briere, 2002). Predicted patterns of correlations were based on a literature review that indicated significant associations between depersonalization and anxiety (e.g., Cox & Swinson, 2002), obsessive-compulsive disorder (e.g., Simeon, Stein, & Hollander, 1995; but see Sierra & Berrios, 2000), posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD; e.g., Bremner, Steinberg, Southwick, Johnson, & Charney, 1993), depression (e.g., Mula, Pini, & Cassano, 2007), borderline personality disorder (e.g., Zanarini, Ruser, Frankenburg, & Hennen, 2000), somatization and conversion (e.g., Spitzer, Spelsberg, Grabe, Mundt, & Freyberger, 1999), and schizotypy (e.g., Watson, 2001; but see Simeon, Guralnik, Knutelska, & 542 C. A. Blevins et al. Downloaded by [Auburn University] at 10:21 25 October 2012 Nelson, 2004). In the context of current theory and empirical findings, the following hypotheses were posited: Hypothesis 1: Internal consistency. Scores on the three depersonalization measures will demonstrate high internal consistency (α ≥ .80). Hypothesis 2: Convergent validity. Scores on the three depersonalization measures will demonstrate strong convergent validity, correlating highly (r ≥ .80) with one another. Hypothesis 3: Discriminant validity. Following convergent correlations, scores on the depersonalization measures will correlate (a) most strongly (r = .50–.60) with other types of dissociation and closely related constructs such as depression, borderline personality disorder, and facets of the schizophrenia spectrum; (b) moderately (r = .40) with related constructs such as anxiety and traumatic stress; (c) weakly (r = .20) with obsessive-compulsive and paranoia symptoms, constructs less related to depersonalization but potentially associated through their correlation with anxiety; and (d) near zero (r = .10) with mania and antisocial personality features, constructs least theoretically related to depersonalization. METHOD Participants Participants were 209 undergraduates (81 male, 126 female, 2 unreported) enrolled in psychology courses at a large southeastern university who completed the study for extra credit. Participants were recruited using flyers that described the study as involving the assessment of stressful life events. The sample was predominantly Caucasian (n = 165; 79%) or African American (n = 28; 13%). Mean age was 19.9 years (SD = 2.0). To be included in the final analyses, participants were required to endorse at least one event that met Criterion A1 for PTSD in the DSM–IV–TR (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). Trauma-exposed participants were selected based on the relationship between trauma and dissociation and to reduce restriction of range resulting from the floor effect typically found on measures of psychopathology in nonclinical samples. The present study focused on the assessment of depersonalization as a dimensional construct and not as a taxon. Measures Measures were administered as part of a larger battery that included a demographics form, three measures of health beliefs and behaviors not relevant to the present study, the three depersonalization measures (DES, CDS, Downloaded by [Auburn University] at 10:21 25 October 2012 Journal of Trauma & Dissociation, 13:539–553, 2012 543 and MDI), the Life Events Checklist (LEC), and the Personality Assessment Inventory (PAI). The DES is a 28-item measure of disturbances in identity, memory, and awareness; depersonalization; and absorption. Respondents rate symptom frequency from 0% to 100% in increments of 10%. The DES does not define a time period for symptom assessment. Although the DES was created as a general dissociation measure, several factor analytic studies have identified three subscales: amnesia, absorption, and depersonalization (for a review, see Stockdale et al., 2002). In the present study, the 6-item depersonalization subscale (Items 7, 11, 12, 13, 27, and 28) identified in Carlson et al.’s (1991) original factor analysis of the DES and used in most subsequent studies was analyzed. The DES has been found to have strong psychometric properties (Bernstein & Putnam, 1986; van IJzendoorn & Schuengel, 1996). Although most of these studies apply to the DES as a whole and are not specific to the depersonalization subscale, available psychometric evidence for the DES depersonalization subscale supports its internal consistency (.84–.87) and temporal stability (.84–.92; Dubester & Braun, 1995) as well as its criterionrelated validity, in that DES depersonalization scores were able to predict depersonalization disorder diagnosis (Simeon, Smith, Knutelska, & Smith, 2008). There are several limitations to the DES as a measure of depersonalization, all having to do with content validity (see Haynes, Richard, & Kubany, 1995). First, the content validation process for the DES was not explicitly described, so there is insufficient information as to how depersonalization was defined and how items were matched to key aspects of the construct. Second, the depersonalization subscale used in most DES studies includes an item assessing auditory hallucinations, which is inconsistent with current conceptualizations of depersonalization that require symptoms to be nonpsychotic (Hunter et al., 2003). Third, DES depersonalization items represent more extreme forms of depersonalization, such as out-of-body experiences, and underrepresent less extreme phenomena associated with depersonalization. The CDS is a 29-item measure of depersonalization. Respondents rate items on separate scales for frequency (0 = never to 4 = all the time) and duration (1 = few seconds to 6 = more than a week) and report symptoms that have occurred in the past 6 months. The CDS was developed partly in response to the reliance of existing measures on overly narrow definitions of depersonalization. To address this issue, the authors defined depersonalization as a syndrome including not only feelings of unreality and detachment but also emotional numbing, heightened self-observation, and perceptual distortions. The initial item pool for the CDS was based on a comprehensive literature review and Sierra and Berrios’s (1998) neurobiological model. An effort was made to represent both extreme (e.g., “I have the feeling of being outside my body”) and more subtle (e.g., “I feel detached from Downloaded by [Auburn University] at 10:21 25 October 2012 544 C. A. Blevins et al. memories of things that have happened to me—as if I had not been involved in them”) phenomena. Initial reliability and validity evidence for the CDS supports its psychometric merit. The CDS has been found to demonstrate excellent internal consistency (.89) and split-half reliability (.92) as well as good criterion-related validity in that CDS scores were able to differentiate individuals with depersonalization disorder from individuals with anxiety and temporal lobe epilepsy (Sierra & Berrios, 2000). The utility of the CDS in nonclinical populations is limited to a few studies using an abridged version of the CDS in a German general population (e.g., Michal et al., 2009) and several small healthy control groups (e.g., Sierra et al., 2002). The MDI is a 30-item measure that assesses six types of dissociation: disengagement, depersonalization, derealization, emotional constriction, memory disturbance, and identity dissociation. Respondents rate symptom frequency on a 5-point scale (1 = never to 5 = very often) and report symptoms that have occurred in the past month. Raw scale scores are calculated by summing the five item scores for each scale, which may then be converted to standardized T scores. The MDI was standardized using university, clinical, and community samples. Briere (2002) found evidence of good reliability and validity for the MDI. Internal consistency coefficients for the MDI depersonalization and derealization scales ranged from .77 to .93 across the three samples. In addition, the MDI depersonalization and derealization scales demonstrated good criterion-related validity in that MDI scores predicted PTSD diagnosis. The major limitation of the MDI is the lack of additional psychometric studies, with only one such study currently in the literature (Briere et al., 2005). Another limitation concerns the splitting of depersonalization items (e.g., “Feeling like you didn’t belong in your body”) and derealization items (e.g., “Suddenly things around you not feeling real or familiar”) into separate scales. Given the lack of support for distinguishing depersonalization and derealization (as previously discussed; see Briere et al., 2005), and given that the DES depersonalization subscale and the CDS include both types of items, the MDI depersonalization and derealization items were combined into a single scale for all analyses in the present study. A final limitation of the MDI is that the content validation process for the scales was not fully described, and thus it is not clear how the initial item pool was created or how items were mapped onto scales. The LEC, the self-report trauma assessment of the Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale (Blake et al., 1995), is a list of 17 categories of potentially traumatic events. Respondents indicate whether they have experienced, witnessed, or learned about or were never exposed to each type of event. If participants endorsed at least one LEC category, they identified the worst event and completed several items assessing whether that event met DSM–IV–TR Criterion A1 for PTSD. The LEC demonstrated adequate Journal of Trauma & Dissociation, 13:539–553, 2012 545 Downloaded by [Auburn University] at 10:21 25 October 2012 psychometric properties in an undergraduate sample (Gray, Litz, Hsu, & Lombardo, 2004). The PAI (Morey, 2007) is a 344-item multiscale inventory that assesses a broad range of psychopathology and personality traits. The PAI scales have been found to have consistently high internal consistency and test– retest reliability. In addition, support for the validity of the clinical scales has been demonstrated in numerous correlational and criterion-group studies (Morey, 2007). The following PAI scales and subscales were analyzed in the current study: Inconsistency, Infrequency, Negative Impression Management, Conversion, Anxiety, Obsessive-Compulsive, Traumatic Stress, Depression, Mania, Paranoia, Schizophrenia, Identity Problems, and Antisocial Features. Procedure Measures were administered online in a university computer lab. After providing informed consent, participants completed a demographics form, followed by the LEC. Depersonalization measures were administered in random order within one block, and the health measures were administered in random order within a second block. The presentation of the two blocks was also randomized. The PAI was always administered last. A total of 277 participants completed the questionnaire session. Of those, 68 were excluded based on (a) an index event that did not meet Criterion A1 (n = 28), (b) an invalid PAI profile as indicated by an Inconsistency score ≥73 T or an Infrequency score ≥75 T (n = 38; Morey, 2007), or (c) having completed only the demographics questionnaire and no other measure (n = 2). Therefore, the final sample consisted of 209 participants. Data Analyses Missing data were analyzed using PASW 18 software. Missing data were considered missing completely at random and were addressed by multiple imputation, following guidelines in Enders (2010). Twenty complete data sets were generated and pooled. The results discussed here were based on the pooled data set. Descriptive statistics and internal consistency coefficients for all measures were examined. Next, zero-order correlations were calculated to evaluate convergent and discriminant relationships among the DES, CDS, MDI, and PAI. Finally, statistics developed by Westen and Rosenthal (2003) were used to investigate construct validity by examining the extent to which the observed pattern of correlations matched the pattern of correlations predicted by theory. Westen and Rosenthal’s (2003) procedure is based on contrast analysis and permits the evaluation of specific hypotheses regarding patterns 546 C. A. Blevins et al. of correlations. The first statistic, r alerting-CV , reflects the degree to which the ordering of predicted versus obtained correlations is consistent. The second statistic, r contrast-CV , is a more stringent test of the fit between the predicted and obtained correlations that takes into account sample size, median intercorrelations among criterion, and the magnitudes of correlations between target and criterion measures. In calculating r contrast-CV , we used Poythress et al.’s (2010) procedure to convert Z scores into t values. RESULTS Downloaded by [Auburn University] at 10:21 25 October 2012 Descriptive Statistics and Internal Consistency Descriptive statistics are presented in Table 1. Mean depersonalization and dissociation scores were somewhat higher than scores found in previous studies using student and other nonclinical samples (e.g., Briere, 2002; van IJzendoorn & Schuengel, 1996). Consistent with the first hypothesis, the CDS and the MDI depersonalization scale exhibited high internal consistency (αs = .93 and .90, respectively), and the DES depersonalization scale exhibited adequate internal consistency (α = .75). Inter-item correlations were analyzed as an additional index of internal consistency. In general these fell in the recommended range of .15 to .50 (Clark & Watson, 1995) for all three measures, with a range of .12 to .61 (M = .33) for the DES, .09 to .67 (M = .34) for the CDS, and .21 to .69 (M = .49) for the MDI. Correlation Analyses Convergent and discriminant correlations are shown in the left side of Table 2. Consistent with the second hypothesis, the highest correlations were found for the convergent correlations among the depersonalization measures, which ranged from r = .63 (p < .01) between the DES and CDS to r = .82 (p < .01) between the CDS and MDI. Consistent with the third hypothesis, somewhat weaker but still strong correlations were found between the three depersonalization measures and other dissociation measures, with rs ranging from .42 to .52 for the DES, .53 to .68 for the CDS, and .51 to .73 for the MDI. Furthermore, moderate correlations were found between the depersonalization measures and Identity Problems, Anxiety, Conversion, and Traumatic Stress, with rs ranging from .31 to .37 for the DES, .40 to .46 for the CDS, and .42 to .50 for the MDI. Finally, the weakest correlations were found between the depersonalization measures and Obsessive-Compulsive, Mania, and Antisocial Features, with rs ranging from .16 to .25 for the DES, .22 to .28 for the CDS, and .22 to .31 for the MDI. A few discriminant correlations were higher than expected, specifically the correlations between each of the depersonalization measures and Journal of Trauma & Dissociation, 13:539–553, 2012 547 TABLE 1 Descriptive Statistics for the DES, CDS, MDI, and PAI Downloaded by [Auburn University] at 10:21 25 October 2012 Measure DES Depersonalization/ Derealization Absorption Amnesia CDS MDI Depersonalization/ Derealization Disengagement Emotional Constriction Memory Disturbance Identity Dissociation PAI validity scale NIM PAI clinical scales SOM-C ANX ARD-T ARD-O DEP MAN PAR SCZ BOR-I ANT No. of items Possible range Observed range M SD α 28 6 0–100 0–100 9–52 5–70 20.8 13.7 9.3 7.6 .90 .75 16 6 29 0–100 0–100 0–290 7–67 6–57 0–226 26.2 13.6 25.9 12.4 8.1 28.4 .87 .66 .93 10 10–50 10–42 14.1 5.4 .90 5 5 44–125 46–131 44–125 46–131 73.7 60.0 18.2 18.1 .86 .91 5 5 45–172 47–283 45–159 47–224 66.8 54.8 22.1 21.2 .80 .78 9 44–144 44–92 50.8 9.8 .68 8 24 8 8 24 24 24 24 6 24 43–114 34–103 41–99 25–89 35–111 25–103 29–112 32–124 36–89 36–115 43–105 34–96 41–99 30–86 35–101 29–86 36–86 33–92 36–86 38–90 48.8 55.6 55.5 50.6 54.0 54.0 54.9 49.4 57.8 57.0 9.0 11.7 13.3 11.7 12.3 11.4 11.0 11.1 11.3 11.2 .79 .91 .89 .66 .91 .84 .85 .85 .65 .83 Notes: N = 209. T scores derived from a trauma-exposed adult community standardization sample are reported for MDI Disengagement, Emotional Constriction, Memory Disturbance, and Identity Dissociation scores. T scores derived from a census-matched standardization sample are reported for the PAI scales. Raw scores are reported for the DES scales, the CDS, and the MDI Depersonalization/Derealization scale. DES = Dissociative Experiences Scale; CDS = Cambridge Depersonalization Scale; MDI = Multiscale Dissociation Inventory; MDI Depersonalization/Derealization = MDI Depersonalization and Derealization Scales combined; PAI = Personality Assessment Inventory; NIM = Negative Impression Management; SOM-C = Conversion; ANX = Anxiety; ARD-T = Traumatic Stress; ARD-O = ObsessiveCompulsive; DEP = Depression; MAN = Mania; PAR = Paranoia; SCZ = Schizophrenia; BOR-I = Identity Problems; ANT = Antisocial Features. Schizophrenia (rs = .51–.61) and Paranoia (rs = .34–.41). To examine the influence of negative response bias, we computed partial correlations, controlling for Negative Impression Management, and report them in parentheses in Table 2. After we controlled for Negative Impression Management, the associations between the depersonalization measures and PAI scales that remained significant included DES with Schizophrenia (r = .24); CDS with Depression, Identity Problems, and Schizophrenia (rs = .24–.33); and MDI with Depression, Identity Problems, Schizophrenia, Anxiety, Traumatic Stress, and Mania (rs = .18–.34). 548 C. A. Blevins et al. TABLE 2 Predicted and Observed Correlations Between Depersonalization Measures and Criterion Measures, Raw λs, and Integer Values of Raw λs Predicted correlations and λs Observed correlations Downloaded by [Auburn University] at 10:21 25 October 2012 Criterion variable DES-DP/DR CDS MDI-DP/DR MDI-ECON MDI-DENG DES-ABS MDI-MEMD MDI-IDDIS DES-AMN PAI-DEP PAI-BOR-I PAI-SCZ PAI-ANX PAI-SOM-C PAI-ARD-T PAI-ARD-O PAI-PAR PAI-MAN PAI-ANT DES-DP/DR r(pr) .63∗ .73∗ .52∗ .42∗ .51∗ .52∗ .51∗ .47∗ .43∗ .32∗ .51∗ .35∗ .31∗ .37∗ .16∗ .34∗ .25∗ .25∗ (.48∗ ) (.62∗ ) (.42∗ ) (.26∗ ) (.37∗ ) (.36∗ ) (.33∗ ) (.35∗ ) (.13) (.10) (.24∗ ) (.10) (–.02) (.08) (.00) (.03) (.11) (.12) CDS r(pr) .82∗ .66∗ .58∗ .60∗ .68∗ .53∗ .55∗ .56∗ .46∗ .61∗ .40∗ .42∗ .44∗ .22∗ .40∗ .28∗ .22∗ (.72∗ ) (.59∗ ) (.43∗ ) (.47∗ ) (.56∗ ) (.31∗ ) (.43∗ ) (.27∗ ) (.24∗ ) (.33∗ ) (.11) (.06) (.10) (.05) (.05) (.13) (.06) MDI-DP/DR r(pr) .71∗ .64∗ .62∗ .73∗ .61∗ .51∗ .56∗ .45∗ .60∗ .45∗ .42∗ .50∗ .22∗ .41∗ .31∗ .23∗ (.66∗ ) (.52∗ ) (.51∗ ) (.63∗ ) (.44∗ ) (.37∗ ) (.28∗ ) (.25∗ ) (.34∗ ) (.20∗ ) (.08) (.21∗ ) (.05) (.09) (.18∗ ) (.08) Predicted r Raw λs Raw λs as integers .80 .80 .80 .60 .60 .60 .50 .50 .50 .50 .50 .50 .40 .40 .40 .20 .20 .10 .10 .34 .34 .34 .14 .14 .14 .04 .04 .04 .04 .04 .04 −.06 −.06 −.06 −.26 −.26 −.36 −.36 3 3 3 1 1 1 0 0 0 0 0 0 −1 −1 −1 −3 −3 −4 −4 Notes: N = 209. Partial correlations, controlling for PAI Negative Impression Management, are reported in parentheses. DES-DP/DR = Dissociative Experiences Scale, Depersonalization/Derealization Scale; CDS = Cambridge Depersonalization Scale; MDI-DP/DR = Multiscale Dissociation Inventory, Depersonalization and Derealization Scales; MDI-ECON = MDI Emotional Constriction Scale; MDIDENG = MDI Disengagement Scale; DES-ABS = DES Absorption Scale; MDI-MEMD = MDI Memory Disturbance Scale; MDI-IDDIS = MDI Identity Dissociation Scale; DES-AMN = DES Amnesia Scale; PAI = Personality Assessment Inventory; DEP = Depression; BOR-I = Identity Problems; SCZ = Schizophrenia; ANX = Anxiety; SOM-C = Conversion; ARD-T = Traumatic Stress; ARD-O = Obsessive-Compulsive; PAR = Paranoia; MAN = Mania; ANT = Antisocial Features. ∗ p < .01. Effect Size Indices r alerting-CV and r contrast-CV Predicted correlations and λ values used to compute r alerting-CV and r contrast-CV are shown on the right side of Table 2. The results of these analyses are shown in Table 3. Large values for r alerting-CV and r contrast-CV were found for the DES depersonalization scale, the CDS, and the MDI depersonalization scale (r alerting-CV = .88, .88, and .90, respectively; r contrast-CV = .70, .78, and .83, respectively). This suggests substantial correspondence between theoretical predictions and observed correlations. The 95% confidence interval around the DES r contrast-CV effect size did not overlap the MDI confidence interval. This suggests a weaker match between predicted and observed correlations for the DES than for the MDI. Neither of the other two comparisons of r contrast-CV effect size confidence intervals were non-overlapping. Journal of Trauma & Dissociation, 13:539–553, 2012 549 Downloaded by [Auburn University] at 10:21 25 October 2012 TABLE 3 Effect Size Statistics r alerting-CV and r contrast-CV for Depersonalization Measures Quantity DES-DP/DR CDS MDI-DP/DR r alerting-CV r contrast-CV 95% CI From To Z contrast t contrast pcontrast .88 .70 .88 .78 .90 .83 .62 .76 11.68 13.92 <.001 .72 .83 13.98 18.07 <.001 .78 .87 15.43 21.19 <.001 Notes: DES-DP/DR = Dissociative Experiences Scale, Depersonalization/Derealization Scale; CDS = Cambridge Depersonalization Scale; MDI-DP/DR = Multiscale Dissociation Inventory, Depersonalization and Derealization Scales; CI = confidence interval. DISCUSSION In this study, the psychometric properties of three depersonalization measures were examined in trauma-exposed college students. First, scores on the three measures exhibited adequate (DES) to strong (CDS and MDI) internal consistency as indicated by coefficient alpha and satisfactory internal consistency as indicated by inter-item correlations. Second, all three measures demonstrated substantial convergent validity. However, the strongest convergent correlation was between the CDS and MDI, which was stronger than the correlation between the DES and either the CDS or MDI. Third, the results generally support the discriminant validity of the depersonalization measures. The DES, CDS, and MDI correlated most strongly with other measures of dissociation; less strongly with measures of moderately related constructs, such as anxiety and PTSD; and least strongly with measures of relatively unrelated constructs, such as mania and antisocial features. Finally, the high degree of congruence between the predicted and observed patterns of correlations was reflected in large effect size indices r alerting-CV and r contrast-CV . The MDI depersonalization scale demonstrated the best match between predicted and observed correlations, followed by the CDS, then the DES depersonalization scale. Content validity was also examined. Because the CDS is based on a comprehensive literature review and clear theoretical model, its items appear to provide the best coverage of the depersonalization construct, including core and associated symptoms. Items on the MDI depersonalization and derealization scales reflect a narrower definition of depersonalization, and coverage is limited primarily to core symptoms of feelings of unreality and detachment. Items on the DES depersonalization scale appear to underrepresent the full construct, focusing primarily on more extreme forms of detachment. In sum, Downloaded by [Auburn University] at 10:21 25 October 2012 550 C. A. Blevins et al. the MDI and CDS demonstrate better content validity, internal consistency, convergent validity, and construct validity as measured by rcontrast-CV . Thus, overall the findings most strongly support the use of the MDI and CDS as measures of depersonalization in this population. This study has several limitations. First, data are based on self-report and thus are subject to response biases. The possible effects of inconsistent, careless, and random responding were addressed by excluding participants who exceeded the recommended cutoffs on PAI scales measuring these response styles. In addition, we examined the effects of negative impression management by calculating partial correlations between the depersonalization measures and all other measures, controlling for the PAI Negative Impression Management scale. Second, Westen and Rosenthal’s (2003) procedure requires a priori selection of external correlates. Although a variety of relevant variables were used in this study, the findings might have differed with the selection of alternative correlates. Third, participants were a nonclinical sample. Although an effort was made to increase variability on measures of psychopathology by including participants with self-identified trauma exposure, participants were likely relatively well adjusted overall. Consistent with this concern, the distributions of scores does not extend to the full possible range. Nonetheless, there does not appear to be a significant restriction of range, and the obtained correlations closely fit the predicted size and pattern. Furthermore, one aim of this study was to contribute to the available evidence of the validity of the depersonalization measures in a nonclinical population. Given the reported prevalence of depersonalization in normal individuals (Hunter et al., 2004), establishing evidence for the appropriate use of self-report depersonalization measures outside of a clinical setting is an important step in student and community research. Given the limited number of available psychometric studies of depersonalization measures, this study contributes to the literature by examining the convergent and discriminant validity of three depersonalization measures and by generalizing findings to a nonclinical sample. The pattern of convergent and discriminant validity coefficients supports the conceptualization of depersonalization as a unique type of dissociation. In light of this finding, measures assessing specific types of dissociation (e.g., the CDS) or multiple types of dissociation (e.g., the MDI) may be considered more useful than global measures of general dissociation. As noted by Briere et al. (2005), the assessment of specific dissociative symptoms allows for more accurate information and specific clinical intervention. Detailed data from well-validated measures of depersonalization provide clinicians with a useful tool in creating treatment goals and tracking treatment progress. Depersonalization is a distressing condition that may lead to fears of loss of control, functional impairment due to disturbances in early attentional and perceptual processing (Guralnik, Schmeidler, & Simeon, 2000), and interpersonal stress due to a profound sense of emotional disconnection. Journal of Trauma & Dissociation, 13:539–553, 2012 551 Downloaded by [Auburn University] at 10:21 25 October 2012 When comorbid with other disorders, depersonalization is associated with greater clinical severity and poorer response to treatment (Mula et al., 2007). Simeon (2004) suggested that depersonalization may be underdiagnosed for several reasons, including clinicians’ unfamiliarity with the construct and the reluctance of individuals experiencing depersonalization to discuss their symptoms because of their fear of being perceived as crazy by others and difficulty describing the symptoms. Completing a measure that describes such depersonalization experiences may alone be therapeutic to individuals struggling to articulate their experience. For these reasons, the continued process of construct validation of depersonalization measures is warranted to enhance scientific understanding of depersonalization, provide clinicians with a validated instrument to screen for depersonalization, and facilitate the development of empirically supported interventions. REFERENCES American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text rev.). Washington, DC: Author. Bernstein, E. M., & Putnam, F. W. (1986). Development, reliability, and validity of a dissociation scale. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 174, 727–735. Blake, D. D., Weathers, F. W., Nagy, L. M., Kaloupek, D. G., Gusman, F. D., Charney, D. S., & Keane, T. M. (1995). The development of a clinician-administered PTSD scale. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 8, 75–90. Bremner, J. D., Steinberg, M., Southwick, S. M., Johnson, D. R., & Charney, D. S. (1993). Use of the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV dissociative disorders for systematic assessment of dissociative symptoms in posttraumatic stress disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 1011–1014. Briere, J. (2002). Multiscale Dissociation Inventory professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Briere, J., Weathers, F. W., & Runtz, M. (2005). Is dissociation a multidimensional construct? Data from the Multiscale Dissociation Inventory. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 18, 221–231. Carlson, E. B., Putnam, F. W., Ross, C. A., Anderson, G., Clark, P., Torem, M., . . . Braun, B. G. (1991). Factor analysis of the Dissociative Experiences Scale: A multicenter study. In B. G. Braun & E. B. Carlson (Eds.), Proceedings of the Eighth International Conference on Multiple Personality and Dissociative States (p. 16). Chicago, IL: Rush. Clark, L. A., & Watson, D. (1995). Constructing validity: Basic issues in objective scale development. Psychological Assessment, 7, 309–319. Cox, B. J., & Swinson, R. P. (2002). Instruments to assess depersonalizationderealization in panic disorder. Depression and Anxiety, 15, 172–175. Dubester, K. A., & Braun, B. G. (1995). Psychometric properties of the Dissociative Experiences Scale. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 183, 231–235. Enders, C. K. (2010). Applied missing data analysis. New York, NY: Guilford Press. Downloaded by [Auburn University] at 10:21 25 October 2012 552 C. A. Blevins et al. Gray, M. J., Litz, B. T., Hsu, J., & Lombardo, T. W. (2004). Psychometric properties of the Life Events Checklist. Assessment, 11, 330–341. Guralnik, O., Schmeidler, J., & Simeon, D. (2000). Feeling unreal: Cognitive processes in depersonalization. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 103–109. Haynes, S. N., Richard, D. C. S., & Kubany, E. S. (1995). Content validity in psychological assessment: A functional approach to concepts and methods. Psychological Assessment, 7, 238–247. Hunter, E. C. M., Phillips, M. L., Chalder, T., Sierra, M., & David, A. S. (2003). Depersonalization disorder: A cognitive-behavioural conceptualisation. Behavior Research and Therapy, 41, 1451–1467. Hunter, E. C. M., Sierra, M., & David, A. S. (2004). The epidemiology of depersonalization and derealisation—A systematic review. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 39, 9–18. Michal, M., Wiltink, J., Subic-Wrana, C., Zwerenz, R., Tuin, I., Lichy, M., . . . Beutel, M. E. (2009). Prevalence, correlates, and predictors of depersonalization experiences in the German general population. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 197, 499–506. Morey, L. C. (2007). Personality Assessment Inventory professional manual (2nd ed.). Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Mula, M., Pini, S., & Cassano, G. B. (2007). The neurobiology and clinical significance of depersonalization in mood and anxiety disorders: A critical reappraisal. Journal of Affective Disorders, 99, 91–99. Phillips, M. L., Medford, N., Senior, C., Bullmore, E. T., Suckling, J., Brammer, M. J., . . . David, A. S. (2001). Depersonalization disorder: Thinking without feeling. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 108, 145–160. Poythress, N. G., Lilienfeld, S. O., Skeem, J. L., Douglas, K. S., Edens, J. F., Epstein, M., & Patrick, C. J. (2010). Using the PCL-R to help estimate the validity of two self-report measures of psychopathy with offenders. Assessment, 17, 206–219. Radovic, F., & Radovic, S. (2002). Feelings of unreality: A conceptual and phenomenological analysis of the language of depersonalization. Philosophy, Psychiatry, and Psychology, 9, 271–279. Sierra, M., & Berrios, G. E. (1996). A case of depersonalization. History of Psychiatry, 7, 451–461. Sierra, M., & Berrios, G. E. (1997). Depersonalization: A conceptual history. History of Psychiatry, 8, 213–229. Sierra, M., & Berrios, G. E. (1998). Depersonalization: Neurobiological perspectives. Biological Psychiatry, 44, 898–908. Sierra, M., & Berrios, G. E. (2000). The Cambridge Depersonalisation Scale: A new instrument for the measurement of depersonalisation. Psychiatry Research, 93, 153–164. Sierra, M., & Berrios, G. E. (2001). The phenomenological stability of depersonalization: Comparing the old with the new. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 189, 629–636. Sierra, M., Senior, C., Dalton, J., McDonough, M., Bond, A., Phillips, M. L., . . . David, A. S. (2002). Autonomic response in depersonalization disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59, 833–838. Simeon, D. (2004). Depersonalization disorder: A contemporary overview. CNS Drugs, 18, 343–354. Downloaded by [Auburn University] at 10:21 25 October 2012 Journal of Trauma & Dissociation, 13:539–553, 2012 553 Simeon, D., Guralnik, O., Knutelska, M., & Nelson, D. (2004). Dissection of schizotypy and dissociation in depersonalization disorder. Journal of Trauma & Dissociation, 5(4), 111–119. Simeon, D., Smith, R. J., Knutelska, M., & Smith, L. M. (2008). Somatoform dissociation in depersonalization disorder. Journal of Trauma & Dissociation, 9, 335–348. Simeon, D., Stein, D. J., & Hollander, E. (1995). Depersonalization disorder and self-injurious behavior. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 56, 36–39. Spitzer, C., Spelsberg, B., Grabe, H., Mundt, B., & Freyberger, H. J. (1999). Dissociative experiences and psychopathology in conversion disorders. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 46, 291–294. Stockdale, G. D., Gridley, B. E., Balogh, D. W., & Holtgraves, T. (2002). Confirmatory factor analysis of single- and multiple-factor competing models of the Dissociative Experiences Scale in a nonclinical sample. Assessment, 9, 94–106. van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Schuengel, C. (1996). The measurement of dissociation in normal and clinical populations: Meta-analytic validation of the Dissociative Experiences Scale (DES). Clinical Psychology Review, 16, 365–382. Watson, D. (2001). Dissociations of the night: Individual differences in sleeprelated experiences and their relation to dissociation and schizotypy. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110, 526–535. Westen, D., & Rosenthal, R. (2003). Quantifying construct validity: Two simple measures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 608–618. Zanarini, M. C., Ruser, T., Frankenburg, F. R., & Hennen, J. (2000). The dissociative experiences of borderline patients. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 41, 223–227. View publication stats