01 Andresen 058577 (bc-t) 2/12/05 4:41 pm Page 387 Article Abstract The paper relies on Vygotsky’s thesis that preschool children in role play are acting in the zone of proximal development (ZPD). One aim is to specify this thesis with respect to language development. The empirical investigations show that language is the central means of creating pretence. By explicit metacommunication, children collaboratively negotiate the plot, transform meanings and distinguish fiction from reality. Thus, metacommunication functions as a verbal frame, determining the meanings within play. Thereby children overcome sympraxic language use which is characteristic of toddlers. Another result is that role play changes during the preschool years. The paper argues that these changes can be subsumed under a general developmental phenomenon, namely the transition of interpsychic into intrapsychic processes. A point of special interest is why preschoolers, through role play, can act in the ZPD, although their ability to cooperate with other children is only at a nascent stage. To explain this, the paper discusses several aspects of psychosocial development. Key Words child–child-interaction, decontextualization of language, language development, metacommunication, role play, zone of proximal development Helga Andresen University of Flensburg, Germany Role Play and Language Development in the Preschool Years The fourth year of life is a year of change (Nelson, 1996; Vygotsky, 1987). According to Vygotsky, toddlers turn into preschool children at about 3 years of age. He pointed out that role play arises as a new phenomenon during this time, and postulated that preschool children, while negotiating shared pretence, act in the zone of proximal development (ZPD). This applies especially to language use, above all to altering relations between language and its situational context. Vygotksy argues that until the age of about 3 linguistic meaning is dominated by the activities the children actually are involved in and by the concrete objects they are dealing with. The general reason for this lies in the dominating role that perception plays in toddlers’ minds. Culture & Psychology Copyright © 2005 SAGE Publications (London, Thousand Oaks, CA and New Delhi) www.sagepublications.com Vol. 11(4): 387–414 [DOI: 10.1177/1354067X05058577] 01 Andresen 058577 (bc-t) 2/12/05 4:41 pm Page 388 Culture & Psychology 11(4) Vygotsky’s assumption fits well with Bühler’s supposition that the (phylogenetic and) ontogenetic origins of language are empraxic or sympraxic in kind, emerging out of the Me, Here and Now of the actual speech situation (Bühler, 1934/1990). This means that early language use is bound to presently ongoing face-to-face communication and that linguistic utterances function as part of acting practices, indivisibly intertwined with their non-linguistic context, as Bruner (1983) has shown with his analysis of the ‘formats’ which are typical of early child–adult interaction. Luria (1967) related Bühler’s supposition to Vygotsky’s developmental theory, characterizing language development as a process of increasing ability to make use of language in a decontextualized way, starting with the maximally contextualized sympraxic mode in the early years and ending with the maximally decontextualized mode which is required to master written language in all its functions. This notion of language development has been criticized by various authors and from various perspectives: for example, as over-emphasizing the intellectual function of language or as neglecting the cultural conditions and varieties of reading and writing (for a deep discussion of the latter point, see Olson, 1994, ch. 2). Since my concern here is to investigate changes in language use and abilities at the beginning of and during the preschool years, I shall neglect questions tied to reading and writing.1 Vygotsky’s remarks on the importance of role play for language development in the preschool years can be understood as assuming that in role play children go beyond sympraxic language use for the first time. Several aspects of role play are of special interest with regard to the ZPD as much as the decontextualization thesis. First, role play involves interaction between children and not between child and adult, as is the case with teaching, which, since Vygotsky’s days, has been the field mostly connected with the ZPD. Secondly, despite its cognitive dimensions, role play undoubtedly is emotionally based. And, finally, it is an oral and not a literate practice. The general question to ask is: which characteristics of role play might be responsible for its postulated decisive function for language development? The general answer to this question is: above all it is the collaborative negotiation of pretence that makes role play so important. As will be shown later in detail, language serves as the central means to create fictitious meanings and plots and thus to create the whole play. For example, by saying ‘Let’s pretend this [pointing to an iron in her hand] is our phone’, 4-year-old Ingrid transforms the real meaning of the iron into the play meaning and then uses it as if it were a telephone.2 Transformations like this one are an instance of metacommunication 388 01 Andresen 058577 (bc-t) 2/12/05 4:41 pm Page 389 Andresen Role Play and Language Development because children thereby communicate about the play meanings (Schwartzman, 1978). Since the verbal devices to transform ‘real’ meanings into play meanings are central to changes of language use induced by role play, the empirical investigations of this paper will devote special attention to metacommunication. The aims of this paper are: • to empirically analyze children’s language in role play, especially relations between utterances made within the frame of play, on the one hand, and metacommunicative utterances about the play, on the other hand, to answer the question how, especially by which linguistic means, do children generate pretence; • to investigate changes of role play and language use related to it during the preschool years; to this end, the play of beginning preschoolers will be compared to a type of play which developmentally functions as a precursor for role play (Oerter, 1993), and to the role play of children at the end of the preschool phase; • to examine Vygotsky’s thesis that preschool children in role play act in the ZPD and to specify it with respect to language use; and • to propose explanations of why in the preschool years it is just through role play that children can act in the ZPD. In order to set the results of the analyses made within a wider developmental perspective, the structure of interactions between preschool children in the play context will be compared with the structure of early child–adult interaction. As metacommunication is crucial to pretending, functions of metacommunication for play in general and for role play in particular will be outlined first. Metacommunication and Play As Bateson (1955) pointed out, play necessarily is accompanied by metacommunication because players continuously have to signal to each other that their behavior is intended as play in order to avoid misunderstandings, that is, to avoid being taken seriously. So, for example, while playfully biting a playmate one has to send the message: this bite is not (intended as) a bite. Otherwise, if the bite were taken as a real bite, play would turn into serious struggle. According to Bateson, metacommunication constructs a frame which characterizes play as play and marks the boundary between behavior within play and behavior out of play. The metacommunicative message is paradoxical because, on the one hand, it states that the behavior in 389 01 Andresen 058577 (bc-t) 2/12/05 4:41 pm Page 390 Culture & Psychology 11(4) question should not be interpreted as serious but, on the other hand, within the play-frame one has to act as if the playful behavior really were that behavior which it represents. Cooperation between children playing, for example, as mother and child, requires both corresponding to each other’s actions as if they really were mother and child and simultaneously being aware of the fact that they are indeed acting in roles. So, the play-mother is in the position to allow or forbid the play-child to do something, which is not the case between two children. Thus, while within play the ‘child’ has to obey the ‘mother’, at the level of metacommunicative discussions about the play’s progress both children have equal rights. Metacommunication marks the context of behavior or actions; their meanings must be interpreted relative to the context they are situated in, namely play. According to Bateson, it is just this feature of play and metacommunication that makes it so important for development and learning: while playing and producing metacommunication, children learn that all behavior is contextual and that meaning generally must be determined in relation to its context. Children come to recognize this when they actively construct and mark the play-frame, thus consciously differentiating between playful and non-playful behavior.3 The empirical analysis below will show that children use several techniques to construct and mark the play-frame and that the clear distinction between play and non-play is crucially important for collaboratively negotiating the performance. Metacommunication in connection with role play serves several functions. It lays out the boundaries between play and reality, for example by such formulas as pretend, for fun (German: wohl, aus Spaß, im Spiel) or even by whole statements like it’s only play, it’s not real (German: das ist nur Spiel, das ist nicht in echt). By metacommunication children transform meanings of things, persons, actions and the whole situation: for example, pretend you are the father, now it is morning (German: du bist wohl der Vater, jetzt ist es Morgen). Finally, children plan the next steps of the plot, by leaving the play and then speaking about what to do next: for example, pretend then you break your leg (German: und dann brichst du dir wohl das Bein). According to Bateson, metacommunication may be explicit or implicit. The examples given above are all instances of explicit metacommuncation because the fictitious character of the play, of its transformations and its actions, is either marked by special verbal entities like pretend (wohl, aus Spaß) or the propositional content of the utterance in question refers to the play as play. But children involved in role play also produce implicit metacommunication. 390 01 Andresen 058577 (bc-t) 2/12/05 4:41 pm Page 391 Andresen Role Play and Language Development Griffin (1984) focuses on the metacommunicative processes by which children conjointly transform meanings. She establishes seven categories of metacommunicational processes and positions them on a scale according to the criterion, whether the particular transformation is located more within the play-frame or more out of it. Totally within frame are utterances and actions belonging to the category ‘enactment’ (Griffin, 1984, p. 81); they develop the plot without proposing any explicit tranformations. The meaning of pretend behavior, then, is clear to the children because of previous discussion or by ‘tradition’, if the children usually play together and share a lot of play experience. Totally out of frame are utterances and actions belonging to the category ‘Overt Proposals to Pretend’ (Griffin, 1984, p. 87), by which children explicitly talk about the transformed meanings and the play. The examples cited above all belong to this category. The remaining five categories of the scale are all cases of implicit metacommunication, varying as to the degree to which they are within or outside the playframe. To illustrate implicit metacommunication in role play, one example, taken from the Flensburg data (see below), will be cited, representing Griffin’s category of Ulterior Conversation. In a mother–child play the ‘daughter’ says: ‘Then I’ll telephone with my friend Anna’ (denn telefonier ich mit meiner Freundin Anna) and immediately afterwards starts to dial and to talk with an imaginary person. By this utterance, the friend Anna comes into play; she has not been mentioned before. So, while carrying on the play, the ‘daughter’ gives the information to the ‘mother’, whom she is telephoning. This information is necessary to jointly develop the play further, and consequently the ‘mother’ takes up the pretend meaning and adjusts her own behavior to it. The utterance must be classified as metacommunicative because the ‘daughter’ creates the new person just by this utterance and at the same time informs the ‘mother’ about it. Otherwise it would be difficult for the latter to interpret the telephone conversation; typically, unclear performances lead to questions and discussions between the playmates, then mostly switch to explicit metacommunication (Pellegrini, 1982, 1984, 1985a, 1985b). The utterance in question has to be classified as implicit because the girl does not leave the play-frame. The utterance would be possible in a respective non-play situation when a girl rings up her friend and wants to inform her mother about it, but the exact formulation my friend Anna would be slightly strange in a real-life situation because one might expect that the mother would know that Anna is the daughter’s friend. Both mother and daughter could presuppose this 391 01 Andresen 058577 (bc-t) 2/12/05 4:41 pm Page 392 Culture & Psychology 11(4) knowledge, resulting in just saying the name and dropping the further information. But in the play situation both persons do not share this knowledge, because Anna has not been introduced earlier. It is indeed remarkable that the utterance actually made gives this necessary information; it indicates that the girl really intends to inform her playmate about it. It also indicates that the speaker is able to anticipate the hearer’s knowledge and thus to take her perspective. This is especially remarkable if one realizes the age of the two girls, namely 5;0;16 (‘daughter’) and 4;8;3 (‘mother’); according to Piaget they are both in the period of egocentric thinking and thus unable to deliberately take the perspective of another person. This example obviously shows that implicit metacommunication is more difficult and more demanding than explicit metacommunication. An advantage of explicit metacommunication is that the children are not urged to conjoin and simultanously differentiate between the perspective of their role, the perspective of their real-life identity and the perspectives of the co-players, their preconditions and needs. A disadvantage is that explicit metacommunication interrupts play and its dynamic flow. On the contrary, implicit metacommunication makes it possible to stay within play and to continuously develop it (Griffin, 1984). Explicit Metacommunication and Role Play: Two Examples To analyze the relations between explicit metacommunication about play and communication within play, two pieces excerpted from a mother–child play between Ingrid (3;10;13) and Hilde (4;2;17) shall be looked at as examples. Ingrid pretends to be the mother, Hilde the daughter. The first draft of the transcript has been transferred into a format that displays the classification of the utterances as out of and within the play-frame. The criteria by which to analyze an utterance as explicitly metacommunicative are as follows: • The utterance contains explicit markers of the fictitious character of the play action like pretend (German wohl, aus Spaß), thus indicating that the utterance refers to the play. • The children call each other by their real names, whereas within play they address each other by their role names. • The propositional content of the utterance refers to play, like you are not playing well. 392 01 Andresen 058577 (bc-t) 2/12/05 4:41 pm Page 393 Andresen Role Play and Language Development • The utterance is directly related to a preceding metacommunicative utterance, e.g. an answer to a question. • The child speaks with her normal voice, in contrast to a special voice while acting in the role. • The children address each other with the (German) personal pronoun du (you), whereas within their roles they use the more distant pronoun sie. Speaker Communication within play 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 So what’s wrong child? I: H: I: H: I: H: I: H: I: For fun my hand is broken, ok? How’s that? ( ) you’ll have to go to hospital so let’s pretend you’re crying ’cos ’cos ’cos ’cos you broken something No but I’m just crying a bit hmn What’s the matter? Broke my hand Then you’ve got to go to hospital quick ( ) But I have ( ) Yes H: I: H: I: H: I: H: I: H: Metacommunication about play but then ( ) you have to lie down quick and cook with one hand hold on tight . . . let’s pretend you’re falling over. gong gong when you stand up let’s pretend you’ve broken your leg Hmn When you walk let’s pretend you walk ok, right? ( ) What’ll I do now with my leg? Yes First I’ve broken my leg ( ) Now let’s pretend you’re gonna fall down ( ) Ouch Now you’ve fallen down Yes In this piece, evidently, the children act more out of play than within it. The explicit metacommunicative utterances serve the functions of planning what to do next and of interpreting the performed play actions. The children explicitly plan and comment on each single step to happen in play. The meanings of the play acts—breaking an arm and a leg—should be fairly clear to the girls because they have nominated them immediately before. Nevertheless, after Hilde has performed the acts, Ingrid states what has just happened in order to make totally clear the meaning of Ingrid’s motions. The metacommunicative utterances determine and clarify the meanings of the girls’ actions within play and thereby render their 393 01 Andresen 058577 (bc-t) 2/12/05 4:41 pm Page 394 Culture & Psychology 11(4) cooperation. Clearly, the explicit metacommunicative utterances make up the context of the utterances and actions within the play-frame. It is tempting to compare the structure of the girls’ interaction with the structure of early mother–child interactions analyzed by Bruner (1983). In those formats, the meaning of each utterance is determined as much by its place in the course of interaction as by its relation to the non-verbal action performed simultaneously. Mother and child start to establish the formats at about the middle of the first year, when the child neither understands nor produces speech; so, the formats help the child to recognize that the mother’s verbal utterances systematically refer to the non-verbal context, that is, to the ongoing actions and to the objects present to mother and child in the actual situation. Bateson (1955) postulates the existence of metalinguistic rules which determine how linguistic signs are related to non-linguistic entities like objects, persons, actions and places. These rules themselves, of course, cannot be linguistic in nature. Bruner analyzed the development of mother–child interaction within the formats over many months and showed that the child constructs knowledge and anticipation of the interaction sequences and thus successively internalizes the structure of the format. This happens during the second half of the first year. First, the child adjusts his own vocalizations, miming and gestures to this structure until, more and more, he takes over the active part of the ongoing communication. Ritualization and repetition of the interaction make it possible for the child to recognize its structure. Above all, it is the close and fixed relations between verbal utterances and the non-verbal context that give children the chance to realize that the vocal activity of the mother refers to something beyond it and to realize the meanings of the utterances. Therefore the formats may be taken as an instantiation of those metalinguistic, non-verbal rules postulated by Bateson. These considerations explain why early language use must be sympraxic; otherwise, children would have no chance to grasp the symbolic function of language.4 In the formats each utterance corresponds to a non-verbal act, thus making evident the meaning of the utterance. In the role play above, the meanings of the behavior and the utterances within play are determined by other verbal utterances, namely by metacommunication about the fictitious meanings. Of course, the non-verbal context is also important for role play, because the children are handling objects. But between this context and the transformed meanings within play has been inserted a frame of explicit metacommunication consisting of verbal signs. Therefore it is impossible to use language sympraxically 394 01 Andresen 058577 (bc-t) 2/12/05 4:41 pm Page 395 Andresen Role Play and Language Development in role play; transforming the meaning of persons, objects, places and the whole setting necessarily disconnects language and its real, situational context, to define the context anew and to settle the transformed meanings. This process, I hope to have made clear up to now, is a verbal one where language is no longer used indivisibly intertwined with the given context but serves as an instrument with which to create a new context. In order to avoid misunderstandings, it must be stated that the pattern of the first piece of role play, containing very short steps of play and correlated explicit metacommunication, is neither the only nor the typical pattern to be found. In order to illustrate this, another piece of the same girls’ play will be given and briefly commented on. Speaker 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 Communication within play H: Metacommunication about play Let’s pretend for fun Daddy hmn is dead and Da hmn Granny and Grandad are away Here Mummy I: Granny and Grandad went away last night ’cos they’re fed up they had to go shopping and go far () ( )They’re sick today they’re sick today they can’t come today but then your auntie will come for sure then your auntie will come soon Yes H: I: H: I: H: I: Your auntie is here auntie is here. Maria Maria oh (. . .) () Auntie is here your auntie H: I: Maria is here Maria is here she’s here already will you open up? Deng Maria? Nobody’s there I’ll ring up Maria () For fun let’s pretend this is our phone Hallo yes well yes Maria are you coming today? You’re coming now good good bye. ( ) H: I: H: I: ( ) You were speaking with the iron What? You were just speaking with the iron Yes ( ) I can just do magic with an iron Here, the distribution of communication within play and metacommunication about play shows a different pattern than in the first piece. At the beginning, in a metacommunicative discourse the girls conjointly make up and think out a new situation: father is dead and 395 01 Andresen 058577 (bc-t) 2/12/05 4:41 pm Page 396 Culture & Psychology 11(4) grandmother and grandfather have gone out shopping. So, mother and daughter stay alone at home, but then Aunt Maria is expected to come. After having worked out this setting, they get into play simulating Maria’s arrival. Hilde then creates a complication by stating that Maria is actually not there in front of the door. Ingrid conforms to this imagined situation and carries on the plot by declaring she will ring up Maria. But now a new complication arises because in the play room there is no telephone. Therefore Ingrid decides to transform a toy iron into a telephone. Typically she then shifts onto the metacommunicative level, by stating Let’s pretend this is our phone (German Aus Spaß ist das wohl unser Telefon). Having said this, she immediately goes back into the play, pretending to have a telephone conversation with Maria. But again an unexpected complication occurs because Hilde has not realized the transformation and in an indignant tone states that her friend has spoken with an iron. So both girls again shift to explicit metacommunication and Ingrid tries to explain her behavior. Very appropriately and imaginatively, she calls the transformation ‘doing magic’. In this piece it is not the case that each utterance and performed act within play is planned and commented on by corresponding metacommuncative utterances. Rather, after having achieved the setting the girls perform their play. The meaning of the utterances and of their non-verbal behavior during pretend play can be interpreted because of the foregone negotiations. But when Ingrid wants to use the iron as a telephone she considers it to be necessary to explicitly perform the transformation. Hilde’s reaction shows that Ingrid was right in doing so. Apparently for Hilde it is not self-evident to use an iron as if it were a telephone. Her reaction is an impressive proof of the supposition that language is the decisive instrument of transforming meanings in pretend play. To my mind, both Ingrid and Hilde need the explicit performance in order to imagine the transformed meaning so that they can continue their play and coordinate their behavior.5 Finally, I will mention some further points of importance for pretend play. Hilde’s odd formulation Let’s pretend for fun Daddy is dead (German Aus Spaß ist der Vater wohl gestorben) very clearly shows that she does not have in mind the lexical meaning of fun but rather uses the two words aus Spaß as a formula indicating pretence. In line 9 Ingrid, playing the mother, says your auntie (German deine Tante) and not our or the. Her use of this personal pronoun very clearly shows that she differentiates between the perspective of the mother and the perspective of the daughter, because the aunt of the daughter is not the aunt of the mother as well. This kind of differentiation in the 396 01 Andresen 058577 (bc-t) 2/12/05 4:41 pm Page 397 Andresen Role Play and Language Development field of personal pronouns is found very often in pretend play. I shall come back to this point later on. To summarize: up to now I have argued that metacommunication is essential for play and that in role play language is of special importance to create pretence. The transformations are accompanied by the generating of a verbal frame, inserted between the real situational context and the meanings of actions and utterances within play. Thus, while performing role play, children relate verbal signs to other verbal signs and not just to their non-verbal context, as is the case with sympraxic language use.6 Pretend play does not have a canonical, fixed form. With increasing age, children change their play, even during the preschool years. Therefore, the next section deals with such changes. I shall concentrate on role taking, on the importance of concrete objects for transformations and play performance, and on explicit and implicit metacommunication. But before this, some information about the data should be given. The Data The empirical basis of the analysis consists of thirty-six video tapes of two children each. Altogether, forty-eight children between 3 and 6 years of age took part in the research project. Of each age group nine pairs have been taped, combining two girls, two boys and one boy with one girl, respectively. The children were recruited from three kindergardens in Flensburg, a town situated in the most northern part of Germany. The videos were taken in play rooms of the kindergardens, that is, in an environment familiar to the children. The children were given no special instructions; they had just been told that they were allowed to play there. We simply hoped that many children would start pretend play—and so it happened. The children knew that they were being filmed, but almost no one showed signs of shyness or silliness. They were quite used to being filmed because the colleague who took the tapes had been present in the kindergardens some days before, filming the children during their usual activities. Out of all videos those sequences where the children played pretend were chosen, transcribed and analyzed under various aspects. Pretend Play and Role Play We differentiated between pretend play and role play. Pretend play is defined as any play including fictitious meanings: for example, taking a box to be a cooking-pot and building-blocks to be food, thus 397 01 Andresen 058577 (bc-t) 2/12/05 4:41 pm Page 398 Culture & Psychology 11(4) simulating the preparation of a meal by putting the blocks into the pot and stirring them. Role play is defined as pretend play with role taking. As criteria for identifying role play there served explicit role taking, for example Pretend I am the father, or addressing each other with the role name, such as Here, mother—What’s the matter, child? A comparison of the amounts in which the children of our study realized pretend or role play led to the following results. No child of the youngest group (3 to 4) performed any role play but they did perform pretend without role play. Both middle age groups (4 to 5 and 5 to 6) performed role play tentatively twice as long as pretend play without role taking, whereas the oldest children (6 to 7) performed pretend play twice as long as role play. Pretend play of the youngest and of the oldest group differed in many respects. The plays of the older ones were much more complex, the players interacted very intensively with each other and their actions were more independent of handling objects. To illustrate pretend play of the youngest children an example of two boys—Jonathan (3;4;29) and Marius (3;11;23)—will be described and compared to the role play of Hilde and Ingrid, in order to negotiate characteristic changes of play, interaction and language use at the beginning of the preschool period. Parallel Play: An Example The boys have a telephone each; they are sitting side by side on chairs, watching and imitating each other. Jonathan takes the receiver and dials; Marius does so as well. They both repeat their actions until Jonathan simulates speaking with a (really existing) girl friend on the phone. Marius immediately after also begins to speak with the girl, repeating the utterances of Jonathan. So, both boys simulate phoning with someone, but instead of acting complementarily, by simulating phoning with each other, they perform exactly the same actions, verbally and non-verbally. During the course of the boys’ interactions their actions become faster and faster. There is no simulation of talking any more; taking up and putting down the receiver and producing short verbal formulas (hello, bye) make up the whole fun. Overall, broken up by a short episode with handling a hairdryer, the whole phone play lasts for 5 minutes and 41 seconds.7 The interaction shows all characteristics of ritualized play (Garvey, 1977) and of parallel play. Oerter (1993) understands parallel play as a precursor of role play. Therefore, parallel play is essentially important at the developmental transition from toddler to preschool age. It is typical for parallel play that the children coordinate their 398 01 Andresen 058577 (bc-t) 2/12/05 4:41 pm Page 399 Andresen Role Play and Language Development behavior—watching and imitating each other—but that they do not cooperate. They do not act complementarily, either on the level of handling concrete objects or on the more abstract level of verbal and social behavior.8 This is exactly the case with the two boys. A few minutes later, the boys shift into a play corner with a cooking stove for children, toy dishes, and so on. They are not engaged in parallel play any longer, but they do not cooperate either. The general pattern of their play is that one boy first does something—for example, simulates cooking a meal—watched by his peer, and just afterwards, when he is going to do something else, the other one repeats the same actions. So again, they do not act complementarily. Their kind of play is typical for the youngest children of our sample. By contrast, the two girls, Hilde and Ingrid, clearly act within roles, as mother and daughter. They act complementarily: for example, one girl preparing a meal, then passing it to the other one, who has laid the table and who serves it. And very clearly, they deal with the same topic; they collaboratively develop the plot as much at the metacommunicative level outside the play-frame as at the play level. The two boys, as stated, do not perform role play. But nevertheless, they pretend to talk with an absent person or to cook a non-existent meal. For them pretending seems to be bound to handling objects in their real functions (the telephones are real telephones) or to handling objects which are very similar to those adults deal with, like toy dishes. So, in the next section I shall look at the functions of concrete objects for pretend and role play and ask whether changes occur during the preschool years. Handling Objects Concerning actions with concrete objects, all empirical investigations show similar results (Cole & La Voier, 1985; Elkonin, 1980; Field, de Stefano, & Koweler, 1982; Garvey, 1977; Vygotsky, 1981). At the beginning of the pre-school years object-related play predominates and the objects are handled according to their real functions. There follows a phase of object-related play where the objects are used with transformed meanings (like the iron used as a telephone). At the end of the preschool years children perform fantasy play which is much more person-related, much more independent of objects, and makes use of objects further removed from their real functions. Most investigations identify the middle phase, characterized by transformations, with role play and refer to Vygotsky, who stressed the importance of objects for taking and enacting roles. 399 01 Andresen 058577 (bc-t) 2/12/05 4:41 pm Page 400 Culture & Psychology 11(4) Our data show the same pattern of development. To illustrate the differences in handling objects at the first and the second stage and to clarify the functions of objects for creating fiction, the play of Jonathan and Marius, on the one hand, and of Hilde and Ingrid, on the other hand, will be examined. The boys do not produce any explicit transformations as is typical for the two girls and for other children at the age of 4 and 5 in our data. But Jonathan, Marius and other children at the age of 3 very often produce onomatopoeia, which Griffin (1984) classifies as ‘underscoring’. She claims that with utterances and actions belonging to this category children non-verbally stay within play (e.g. they simulate pouring a liquid out of a jug into a cup) while verbally they are outside play. Saying, for example, pssst to simulate the pouring of the liquid takes the place of the real event and would be odd if it were uttered in a corresponding real-life situation. According to Griffin, underscoring allows players to stay emotionally within the play world while at the same time clarifying and transforming meanings. The youngest ones in the Flensburg corpus produce underscorings very narrowly attached to objects. In the example cited, Jonathan holds a toy jug and a cup in his hands and simulates pouring an imagined liquid from the jug into the cup. The meaning of his action and even of the underscoring verbal utterance becomes clear through the objects. The verbal utterance is embedded in the non-verbal action, it fills a small gap in this action and its meaning is determined by the objects and by their typical use. Older children such as Hilde and Ingrid use onomatopoeia as well. In the second reported piece of their play Hilde simulates the opening of a door by saying deng accompanied by rapidly moving her hand in front and back again. She does not handle any object at all. The meaning of her underscoring utterance and her gesture is determined by the verbal context, by the foregone and the following utterances. So, this is another instance of relating verbal signs to other verbal signs in role play. By contrast, the meanings of the boys’ underscorings are determined by objects, that is, the non-verbal context. Above, Oerter’s reference to the subjective value that things have for young children has been reported. To my mind, it is clear that Jonathan and Marius are quite fascinated by the objects and that each one wants to handle them on his own. In Hilde and Ingrid’s play there is an episode which shows that, on the one hand, specific objects are very attractive for them and evoke the wish to act with them, but that, on the other hand, the girls are eager (and able) to integrate their wish into the rules of their roles. Ingrid, who plays the mother, has been busy at 400 01 Andresen 058577 (bc-t) 2/12/05 4:41 pm Page 401 Andresen Role Play and Language Development the cooking stove for some time, while Hilde lays the table. After a while, Hilde obviously wants to work at the stove. She goes there, remarking that now the ‘daughter’ will start to cook. Ingrid accepts this with the remark The child is big, she is already able to do so. This reaction shows that Ingrid feels it necessary to reason that it is possible and suitable for the daughter to cook. It is an illustration of the cited observation that during the preschool period role play gets more and more dominated by action plans and role-specific rules in contrast to the object-related play of younger children (Garvey, 1977). Changes in Metacommunication Several studies have obtained the result that explicit metacommunication about play decreases at the end of the preschool years (e.g. Auwärter, 1986; Sachs, Goldmann, & Chaille, 1984). Sachs et al. and Auwärter found that at the beginning of this developmental period the rate of explicit metacommunication is relatively low, and that it then increases until it at last declines again. Our data perfectly correspond to this pattern. The explanation as to why younger children very rarely produce explicit metacommunication is easy to give. The reason is that they do not perform role play at all, so there is no need to communicate about what is going to happen and how actions and utterances should be interpreted. In the Flensburg data explicit metacommunication starts with role play—and then, in the play of the 4-year-olds, it is produced excessively. Garvey (1977) states that role play seems to be overloaded with explicit markers of the fictitious character of the ongoing action. As to our data, this holds for the 4- and 5-years-olds, whereas the 6year-old children produce much less explicit metacommunication. The examples of Hilde and Ingrid clearly show that for them it is obviously necessary to assure themselves and the playmate whether they are acting at the level of fiction or of real life and to speak about the plot and the transformations they perform. The finding that older children produce less explicit metacommunication is of special interest. At first glance it may be surprising because from a scientific view it would be suggestive to propose that explicit metacommunication demands complex cognitive and communicative abilities which can be developed only on the basis of complex communicative skills which are beyond the scope of 4-year-old children. But metacommunication does not vanish out of play when children grow older; on the contrary, the pretend play of older children is much more complex than in the earlier years and contains a lot of 401 01 Andresen 058577 (bc-t) 2/12/05 4:41 pm Page 402 Culture & Psychology 11(4) transformations. Qualitative analyses of the older children’s play in the Flensburg corpus show that they produce more implicit metacommunication than the younger ones. So, during the preschool period metacommunication changes from explicit to implicit performance. As I stated earlier, implicit metacommunication is much more demanding than explicit metacommunication because the child must also have in mind planning the plot, transforming, interpreting and communicating the fictitious meanings, while at the same time enacting the play. Since transformations of persons, things and situations are central to role play, the children cannot presuppose the meanings of the actions and utterances, and so these meanings must be communicated. It is obvious that children, when they developmentally start with role play, need to speak overtly about transformations and plots as they are not yet able to perform these implicitly. If one looks at metacommunication from this perspective, the change from explicit to implicit metacommunication during the preschool years can be ascribed to a general process of ontogenetic development which has been described by Vygotsky (1986, p. 228). According to him, internal mental processes arise out of external, interactive and communicative processes in earlier stages. He formulated this phenomenon as the transition from interpsychic to intrapsychic processes and functions during development. Thus, I propose, all the changes of play reported here can be subsumed under this general line of development: children between 4 and 6 gain more independence from concrete objects; the older ones can act in fiction without imagining specific roles with typical action patterns as support and they become able to produce and understand more implicit metacommunication. Their play thus becomes much more determined by inner plans and processes of interpretation instead of handling concrete objects and communicating explicitly about roles, actions and transformations. But when difficulties arise, the possibility of leaving the play-frame and shifting onto the level of explicit metacommunication is still at hand (Andresen, 2002; Griffin, 1984; Pellegrini, 1982, 1984, 1985a, 1985b). Egocentric Speech and Metacommunication About Play It is remarkable that another mental function, thoroughly investigated by Vygotsky, originates at the beginning of the preschool age and at its end has undergone a change from external to internal performance: egocentric speech, which at about 6 years has altered to inner speech (Vygotsky, 1986; Wertsch, 1985). According to Vygotsky, the roots of 402 01 Andresen 058577 (bc-t) 2/12/05 4:41 pm Page 403 Andresen Role Play and Language Development language and speech are social, communicative in nature. At about 3 years egocentric speech springs out of interactive speech, thus starting a process of differentiation between the social and intellectual functions of speech. Egocentric speech serves to regulate behavior: by speaking to herself the child directs her own actions. Metacommunication connected with play also fulfills regulatory functions. While by egocentric speech children direct their own behavior, by metacommunication about play they direct their own actional and interpretive processes as well as those of their partners. In respect to changes of sympraxic speech during the preschool years, both egocentric speech and explicit metacommunication, are of interest—but in a different, complementary way. With egocentric speech the child uses language independently from the dynamic ongoing interaction with another person. So, this side, the personal one, of the situational context is absent and the child has to undertake the structuring and regulating of his actions by himself—whereas in early development this is done by adults. Young children, using egocentric speech, typically handle objects—these objects being present to the child in the particular situation. As the child knows the aims and the objects of his intended actions and as he is acting on his own, there is no need to name them explicitly. Egocentric speech is thus shortened and syntactically incomplete because the speaker can presuppose a lot of knowledge to be relevant for the ongoing action. Therefore egocentric speech is still narrowly tied to the non-verbal context at hand, and word–object relations predominate (Wertsch, 1985). In role play, actors cannot take the non-verbal context as given because the central motive for playing is to transform meanings. Therefore the children must establish the play meanings, and—as the data show—the younger preschoolers especially have to do so by explicit verbal communication. So, for role play, while language is still tied to interaction with other persons, the meanings of the situational context are at the participants’ disposal, and so the inextricable interrelation between speech and the non-verbal context of sympraxic language use becomes loosened. To summarize: during the preschool age both language-mediated processes, metacommunication connected with role or pretend play and egocentric speech, undergo a change from outer, explicit, loud performance to inner, implicit, silent realization. Egocentric speech constitutes the beginning of a development in which social, interactive language (interpsychic) alters into intrapsychic processed language. With this, the meanings of the situational context can be presupposed and do not need to be commented on. On the contrary, with role play, 403 01 Andresen 058577 (bc-t) 2/12/05 4:41 pm Page 404 Culture & Psychology 11(4) interactive, social language use is preserved, whereas the meanings of the situational context shift and must be thematized. Metacommunication, necessary to transform and communicate meanings, changes from explicit to implicit performance. Wertsch (1985) stresses that with egocentric speech word–object relations are still prevalent in children’s speech because they presuppose the context given and because speech then refers and points to it directly. With respect to the results of Hickmann (1980a, 1980b, 1982, 1985) and Karmiloff-Smith (1979), who analyze children’s language use while telling stories, he argues that children do not relate signs to other signs until the age of approximately 8. Only then are children able to interrelate them: for example, to establish indexical relations within a text correctly. This—according to Wertsch—is a precondition for the ability to operate with a new category of objects, namely objects that are created by language. But an analysis of children’s role plays clearly shows that already 4year-olds are indeed able to create objects and meanings by linguistic means: for example, Aunt Maria, in the play of Hilde and Ingrid, who comes into existence through Hilde’s utterance on the metacommunicative level and whose existence afterwards can be presupposed within the play. If the children could not create new meanings and communicate them to each other, role play could not take place at all. Pellegrini (1982, 1985a, 1985b) investigated the use of endophoric and exophoric linguistic expressions in two different play contexts, namely role and construction play. Both types of linguistic entities are deictic: endophora point to other linguistic entities, whereas exophora point to the non-verbal context.9 Pellegrini found that with role play children significantly use more endophoric expressions than with construction play. Language use related to the latter is similar to egocentric speech because the deictic words refer to objects and actions that are present before the children’s eyes. Thanks to the essential function of transformations, this does not hold for role play. Elsewhere (Andresen, 2002) I have analyzed in detail how children in role play negotiate personal reference. In accordance with Pellegrini, one result is that persons (roles taken by the players or imaginative persons) first get introduced into play by lexemes designating them— e.g. Aunt Maria, my friend Anna. Later on, during the course of the play, the children point to those lexemes by endophorically used personal pronouns. Even 4-year-olds manage to use personal pronouns systematically and correctly, thus differentiating among the persons spoken about, between their own perspectives as speaker or hearer, and between their role and their real-life identity. 404 01 Andresen 058577 (bc-t) 2/12/05 4:41 pm Page 405 Andresen Role Play and Language Development These results clearly show that preschool children indeed are able to create sign–sign relations and—as I hope to have made plausible by now—must be able to do so if they perform role play. So, in this respect—the ability to relate verbal signs to other verbal signs—our investigations may be taken as a specification of Vygotsky’s thesis that preschool children during role play act in the ZPD. Questions Investigations of children’s interactional development show that not only role play but interaction between children of the same age as well is a new phenomenon in the preschool years. In our data cooperative and complementary interaction starts with role play, and role play starts with explicit metacommunication. The question thus arises, why is it possible for children in role play to act in the ZPD although their ability to perform child–child interaction is only at a nascent stage? Would it not be more expected and reasonable if children were able to act together in the ZPD only after several years of experiences in interaction with other children of the same age and until then they developed new communicative abilities by interactions with adults only? This holds even more as role play is an especially demanding situation where they have to create and communicate fictitious meanings and continually indicate whether they are acting in reality or in fiction. Approaches to Answers The children’s motivation for role play is clearly emotionally based. Vygotsky (1981) locates the roots of these plays in a gap between the child’s desires and reality: the child strongly wishes to act like an adult, but at the same time he clearly knows that he really is neither able nor allowed to do so. This leads to an inner tension that is solved by producing fantasy and fiction. Vygotsky claims that toddlers’ wishes are bound to the actual situation and are directed to single acts. If the child does not get them fulfilled at once he usually becomes angry and often starts to cry. In contrast to this, preschool children are not as much dependent on the actual situation as the younger ones; their wishes are of a more general kind. So they are impressed not only by single persons; rather, power and ability of adults in general attract them. Assignments of persons in role play very clearly show this: dogs are often called doggy, children child, wives wife, and so on. All these expressions are used like proper names, which is very unusual in real 405 01 Andresen 058577 (bc-t) 2/12/05 4:41 pm Page 406 Culture & Psychology 11(4) life. This shows that, although role plays indeed are inspired by experiences with certain persons, children do not wish to act like a particular person in a particular situation, but rather like a man, woman or a doctor in general. Vygotsky (1987) characterizes preschooler’s thinking as perceptual and yet increasingly generalizing. Both characteristics are perfectly realized in role play, as will be sketched out now in respect to psychic and social development. Role Play and Psycho-social Development Role play is common play, interactive and social in nature.10 The examples of Hilde and Ingrid clearly show that the girls conjointly construct the play; together they negotiate the plot and the transformations, and this takes place explicitly, by verbal communication. Handling objects and transforming their meanings verbally makes it easier to pretend, and so do roles. Younger children, especially, prefer not only familiar, but also complementary roles, like mother–child or doctor–patient. Their action patterns are related to and clearly differentiated from each other, which makes it easier for the children to act appropriately. Vygotsky in regard to children’s play states: no roles without rules; no rules without roles. Preschool children need the perceptual role concept to follow the action rules. Later, in the school years, they are able to follow rules as such, but very often rule plays— e.g. running away and chasing—are accompanied by adding perceptual roles to them—e.g. robber and policeman. The role concept perfectly corresponds to the concrete thinking of preschool children. It entails generalizations because it is not tied to the here, now and me (Bühler) like toddler’s sympraxic language use and thinking. Rather, in role play preschool children go beyond the given context of the actual situation and orientate their behavior towards typical, socially determined action patterns. Compared to school children, on the other hand, preschoolers’ play is more concrete, tied to the perceptual role concept. Mead (1934) understands role play as the first stage of identity development. That is why children, in taking roles and enacting them, for the first time come to terms with adults, that is, with those persons who control and take care of them. In role play children generate a first distance to themselves because by imagining, for example, being a mother or a doctor and acting like them, they take the perspectives of adults on actions that they are used to performing from their own perspectives as young children. This distance holds even if a child 406 01 Andresen 058577 (bc-t) 2/12/05 4:41 pm Page 407 Andresen Role Play and Language Development plays a child, since then in his fantasy he is not just himself but rules out action patterns that he supposes to be typical for children. Berger and Luckmann (1966) in addition to Mead’s Generalized Other introduced the Significant Other, designating the child’s caretakers, those persons she is attached to and familiar with from birth on. Note that the first role plays that children come to perform are family plays (Andresen, 2002; Garvey, 1977). The onset of this play is thus related to the Significant Other, but as the play’s aim and content are directed to roles and not to the imitation of single persons, role play brings about a first orientation towards the Generalized Other: society comes into sight. Therefore role play can be understood as a bridge: from interaction as a real child with particular, real persons in intimate situations to interaction along the lines of socially determined action patterns, imagined in a pretend world, but nevertheless negotiated with peers in actual interactions. At this point it is interesting to look again at metacommuncation, comparing metacommunication related to early adult–child interaction to metacommunication connected with role play. Concerning the formats between mother and child, Bruner (1983) has shown that when the child knows the structure of the format and becomes able to take over the active part, the mother starts to explicitly thematize who of them both shall be the actor. So the child, already at the beginning of language development, learns that action roles are not fixed but may shift and can be negotiated by explicit metacommunication.11 Those roles, like actor and observer, speaker and hearer, are tied to the ongoing action in the actual speech situation (Bühler, 1934/1990). But when preschool children by explicit metacommunication raise questions of role taking and role performance, the roles at their disposal are no longer tied to specific, single, ongoing actions; rather, they are bundles of socially determined action rules. This difference between early adult-child interaction and peer interaction connected with role play corresponds to another difference. By raising the question of whether the child wants to take the active part, the mother pretends that both partners, she and the child, are equally competent. But actually it is still the adult who structures the interaction, scaffolding a frame within which the child fits his actions. Even later on, when the mother extends the format and increases its complexity, she still builds up the context of their common actions, which can then be presupposed. Contrary to this, in the role play context children really negotiate the play and its context conjointly. Up to now, the question of why preschoolers can act in the ZPD through role play has partly been answered. The role concept perfectly 407 01 Andresen 058577 (bc-t) 2/12/05 4:41 pm Page 408 Culture & Psychology 11(4) corresponds to their increasing ability to generalize and at the same time makes it possible to imagine and perform role-specific and typical actions. They take their nearby environment with familiar persons as a starting point, whereas the play itself is orientated towards the future, towards future possibilities beyond the narrow scope of a preschooler’s life. Concrete objects and roles help the children to pretend and thereby go beyond the real situation. Thus, role play stimulates preschoolers’ development, fostering that growing ability to perceptually generalize which is typical of preschool age. But still another question is left open, namely why is it possible that children in just such a demanding situation as role play start to interact with other children of the same age? That is, why is it in just this situation that they become able to structure and perform interaction without the help of older and more competent persons? Origins of Role Play Crucial to role play is its emotional origin. As described earlier, preschool children feel the strong need to act like adults; and it is this desire that children have in common. If one observes children playing role play, it is striking to notice how eager they are to maintain the play—even if they must do or sustain things they otherwise would not accept. Since they want to act in a pretend world, they are urged to reach an agreement. The common wish to pretend, to go beyond the real situation, leads to the deliberate creation of a new type of context—namely fiction. To reach their goal, the children cannot presuppose the meanings of objects and actions any longer, but must negotiate transformed meanings. By planning the play and negotiating the transformations, they happen to structure their interaction by themselves. So, role play with other children, forced by strong desires, seems to be especially appropriate to learning how to responsibly structure interaction and thus to emancipate oneself from the sustaining help of adults.12 Role Play, Language and Context Finally, some aspects concerning altering relations between language and its contexts during the first years of life will be pointed out, summarizing the considerations up to now. As Bruner (1983) has analyzed in an examplary fashion, adults involve children in communication some time before children are able 408 01 Andresen 058577 (bc-t) 2/12/05 4:41 pm Page 409 Andresen Role Play and Language Development to understand or produce language. In early child–adult interaction, verbal utterances are narrowly attached to objects and non-verbal actions. This tight connection between utterances and their non-verbal context is essential for the child’s first steps into language and characteristic of sympraxic language use during the earliest years. With role play, children no longer presuppose the context given, but rather transform the meanings of persons, objects and actions, thereby using language as the central instrument to generate the altered meanings. In doing so, they differentiate between word and object, between language and its context. role play 对语⾔的帮助 As stated earlier, while interacting together in role play, children go beyond sympraxic language use. In using language as an instrument to generate fictitious meanings they decontextualize language. Decontextualization in this sense does not mean to free language and behavior from any context, but to deliberately put them into a new context, i.e. play. The children stipulate the new, playful meanings overtly. While doing so, they realize that linguistic signs are arbitrary and conventional—a precondition for being able to use language flexibly without being bound to specific contexts (Brockmeier, 1997). Paradoxically, the described process of decontextualization takes place in a very special, singular situation: the new meanings matter only during this particular play, between these particular persons. They are bound to the specific, ongoing interaction of the very playmates, often shifting several times within the same play. Being attached to concrete, dialogically structured interaction serves as a precondition for preschool children to go beyond sympraxic language use in the described way. It is precisely the possibility of enacting fictitious actions immediately, sustained by the imaginative role concept and transformations of objects, which enables young children to develop pretend plots at all. The gap between the linguistic abilities of preschoolers during play and while producing monologic narratives (Hickmann, 2003) shows the importance of the interactive, dialogic and dynamic character of role play. Above, changes of pretend play during the preschool years have been understood as an instance of Vygotsky’s rule that intrapsychic processes originate from interpsychic ones. During the same age, egocentric speech changes into inner speech, thus undergoing a similar process of interiorization. Vygotsky regarded the emergence of the intellectual function of language with egocentric speech as an important developmental step. The analysis of role play shows that at the same time social, interactive language also changes: children become able to structure interaction responsibly, without the support of adults, 409 01 Andresen 058577 (bc-t) 2/12/05 4:41 pm Page 410 Culture & Psychology 11(4) and by the same process they decontextualize language, an important step towards the ability to deliberately have language at their disposal. Despite all differences, what egocentric speech and explicit metacommunication about role play have in common is to direct behavior and inner processes. Since Vygotsky’s days, egocentric (and inner) speech, the ability to self-regulate and the ability to establish relations between linguistic signs, have been brought together and regarded as important for the development of metacognition (Wertsch, 1985). Around the world children five to seven years of age are seen as entering a new phase of development. Virtually all societies in which there is formal schooling begin at this age, and quite often children are given new responsibilities . . . . At least part of the reason for adult’s newfound confidence is children’s growing ability to internalize various kinds of rules that adults give them and to follow them even in the absence of the rule-making adult, that is, their growing ability to self-regulate. Another reason is that children of this age are able to talk about their own reasoning and problem-solving activities in a way that makes them much more easily educable in many problem-solving activities: that is to say they are capable of certain kinds of especially useful metacognition. (Tomasello, 1999, p. 191) The investigations of role play put forward here draw attention to interaction between preschool children and argue that it is not only interaction with adults that contributes to the growing ability to selfregulate, to explore and to follow rules. In addition, in the preschool years role play between children is accompanied by important changes in interaction and language use, with children thus promoting each other. 促进⼉童之间彼此协调 Notes 1. In Germany, teaching of written language starts at school, not at kindergarden or preschool, normally at the age of 6. 2. Note that the given example originally is produced in German. It is not useful to translate it literally because each language has its own markers to indicate pretence. In Germany (especially northern Germany, where my tapes were made) children mark pretence by the words wohl or aus Spaß, used as a kind of formula to indicate that now they are speaking about fictitious meanings. The original utterance of Ingrid was: ‘Aus Spaß ist das wohl unser Telefon’ (Let’s pretend this is our phone). 3. Watching role play, it is clear that children are conscious about whether they are acting in a pretend or in a real world. Otherwise, they would get confused and could no longer cooperate. 4. According to Tomasello, a few months after the beginning of the formats—namely at about 9 months of age—infants gain the ability to direct their attention simultanously to an object and to a communication partner. At the same time they start to recognize the other as an intentional 410 01 Andresen 058577 (bc-t) 2/12/05 4:41 pm Page 411 Andresen Role Play and Language Development 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 12. agent like themselves (Tomasello, 1999, p. 68). This process makes up a precondition of the use of conventional signs because their meanings are established by common practices between persons and are related to non-linguistic entities. The triangulation (i.e. the relation between child—adult—object) not accidentally recalls the organon model of Bühler (1934/1990), which states that three relations of a verbal sign are essential: its relation to speaker, to hearer and to the objects and facts of the world. Bruner’s (1983) analysis of the formats clearly shows that common object-related communicational practices between child and adult start before the 9-month revolution (Tomasello, 1999), thus providing a basis of the triangulation process. According to Vygotsky (1981), toddlers take words for attributes of things and thus are unable to detatch words from the objects they refer to. Paradoxically, this thinking helps the preschool child to transform meanings by using language. Calling an iron a telephone creates the image of the fictitious object. So, toddlers’ ways of looking at things and words are both made use of and superseded by role play. Concerning the developmental importance of increasing abilities to generate sign–sign relations and not just sign–object relations, see Wertsch (1985). Garvey (1977), who analyzed ritualized play, refers to a ritual with a length of 2 minutes as relatively long. Thus the example of the two boys is remarkably long. The reason for this—according to Oerter (1993)—lies in the subjective value of things for toddlers: toddlers are eager to get and try things just for themselves. This fits well with Vygotsky’s (1987) claim that in this developmental period perception is the dominant mental function. In Bühler’s (1934/1990) terminology: endophora function within the kontextliches Zeigfeld that is made up by the verbal context; exophora function within the dingliches Zeigfeld that is made up by the non-verbal context. Older children are able to play role plays on their own, enacting different roles and simulating dialogues monologically. This ability presupposes an even greater ability to take and conjoin different perspectives of different persons at the same time. Preschoolers are still at the beginning of their development to consciously change perspectives and have to rely on dialogic and interactive negotiation of role play. This is an impressive instance of Bateson’s (1955) claim that in everyday communication the levels of communication and metacommuncation are actually interwoven. In a more formal sense one can express this by saying that metacommunication and communication are interdependent due to a process of mutual feedback (cf. Klüver, 2002). I think it to be necessary to take this developmental step with peers because if one wants to leave the given context and to intentionally determine the context of one’s actions, one must not rely on other persons doing the job by proxy. Budwig, Strage and Bamberg (1986), analyzing young children’s growing ability to understand that other children take another perspective than they themselves, reach the same conclusion: 411 01 Andresen 058577 (bc-t) 2/12/05 4:41 pm Page 412 Culture & Psychology 11(4) ‘Learning to communicate across differences in perspectives does not develop out of interactions with more experienced collaborators alone’ (p. 102). References Andresen, H. (1985). Schriftspracherwerb und die Entstehung von Sprachbewusstheit. Opladen: Westdeutscher Verlag. Andresen, H. (2002). Interaktion, Sprache und Spiel: Zur Funktion des Rollenspiels für die Sprachentwicklung im Vorschulalter. Tübingen: Narr. Auwärter, M. (1986), Development of communicative skills: The construction of fictional reality in children’s play. In J. Cook-Gumperz, W.A. Corsaro, & J. Streeck (Eds.), Children’s worlds and children’s language (pp. 205–230). Berlin: de Gruyter. Bateson, G. (1955). A theory of play and fantasy: A report on theoretical aspects of the project for study of the role of paradoxes of abstraction in communication. Approaches to the study of human personality: American Psychiatric Association psychiatric research reports, 2, 39–51. (Paper delivered to a Symposium of the American Psychiatric Association on Cultural, Anthropological, and Communications Approaches, 11 March 1954, at Mexico City.) Berger, P.L., & Luckmann, T. (1966). The social construction of reality. Garden City, NY: Doubleday. Brockmeier, J. (1997). Literales Bewusstsein. Schriftlichkeit und das Verhätnis von Sprache und Kultur. Munich: Fink. Bruner, J.S. (1983). Child’s talk: Learning to use language. New York: Norton. Budwig, N., Strage, A., & Bamberg, M. (1986). The construction of joint activities with an age-mate: The transitions from caregiver-child to peer play. In J. Cook-Gumperz, W.A. Corsaro, & J. Streeck (Eds.), Children’s worlds and children’s language (pp. 83–108). Berlin: de Gruyter. Bühler, K. (1990). Theory of language: The representational function of language. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. (Original work published 1934.) Cole, D., & La Voier, J.C. (1985). Fantasy play and related cognitive development in 2-to-6-year-olds. Development Psychology, 21, 231–240. Elkonin, D.B. (1980). Psychologie des Spiels. Cologne: Pahl Rugenstein. Field, O., de Stefano, L., & Koewler, J.H. (1982). Fantasy play of toddlers and preschoolers. Development Psychology, 18, 503–508. Garvey, C. (1977). Play. London: Fontana. Griffin, H. (1984). The coordination of meaning in the creation of a shared make-believe reality. In I. Bretherton (Ed.), Symbolic play. The development of social understanding (pp. 73–100). London: Academic Press. Hickmann, M. (1980a). Creating reference in discourse: A developmental analysis of linguistic cohesion. In J. Kreiman & A. Ojeda (Eds.), Papers from the Parasession on Pronouns and Anaphora: Sixteenth Regional Meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society (pp. 192–203). Chicago, IL: Chicago Linguistic Society. Hickmann, M. (1980b). The context dependence of linguistic and cognitive processes. In M. Hickmann (Ed.), Proceedings of a working conference on the 412 01 Andresen 058577 (bc-t) 2/12/05 4:41 pm Page 413 Andresen Role Play and Language Development social foundations of language and thought. Chicago, IL: Center for Psychosocial Studies. Hickmann, M. (1982). The development of narrative skills. Pragmatic and metapragmatic aspects of discourse cohesion. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Chicago. Hickmann, M. (1985). The implications of discourse skills in Vygotsky’s theory. In J. Wertsch (Ed.), Culture, communication and cognition: Vygotskian perspectives (pp. 237–257). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Hickmannn, M. (2003). Children’s discourse: Person, space and time across languages. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1979). A functional approach to child language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Klüver, J. (2002). An essay concerning sociocultural evolution: Theoretical principles and mathematical models. Dordrecht: Kluwer. Luria, A.R. (1967). Die Entwicklung der Sprache und die Entstehung psychischer Prozesse. In H. Hiebsch (Ed.), Ergebnisse der sowjetischen Psychologie (pp. 165–316). Berlin: Akademie. Mead, H. (1934). Mind, self and society: From the standpoint of a social behaviorist. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Nelson, K. (1996). Language in cognitive development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Oerter, R. (1993). Psychologie des Spiels: Ein handlungstheoretischer Ansatz. Munich: Quintessenz. Olson, D.R. (1994). The world on paper: The conceptual and cognitive implications of writing and reading. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Pellegrini, A.D. (1982). The construction of cohesive text by preschoolers in two play contexts. Discourse Process, 5, 101–108. Pellegrini, A.D. (1984). The effect of dramatic play on children’s generation of cohesive text. Discourse Process, 7, 57–67. Pellegrini, A.D. (1985a). The narrative organization of children’s fantasy play: The effects of age and play context. Educational Psychology, 5, 17–25. Pellegrini, A.D. (1985b). Relations between preschool children’s symbolic play and literate behavior. In L. Galda & A.D. Pellegrini (Eds.), Play, language and stories: The development of children’s literate behavior (pp. 79–97). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Sachs, J., Goldmann, J., & Chaille, C. (1984). Planning in pretend play: Using language to coordinate narrative development. In A. Pellegrini & T. Yawkey (Eds.), The development of oral and written language in social contexts (pp. 119–127). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Schwartzman, H. (1978). Transformations: The anthropology of children’s play. New York: Plenum. Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Vygotsky, L.S. (1981). Das Spiel und seine Rolle für die psychische Entwicklung des Kindes. In H. Röhrs (Ed.), Das Spiel—ein Urphänomen des Lebens (pp. 129–146). Wiesbaden: Akademische Verlagsgesellschaft. Vygotsky, L.S. (1986). Thought and language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. 413 01 Andresen 058577 (bc-t) 2/12/05 4:41 pm Page 414 Culture & Psychology 11(4) Vygotsky, L.S. (1987). Arbeiten zur psychischen Entwicklung der Persölichkeit (2 vols.). Cologne: Pahl Rugenstein. Wertsch. J.V. (1985). Vygotsky and the social formation of mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Biography HELGA ANDRESEN is a Professor of Linguistics and Mother Tongue Education at Flensburg University, Germany. Her research focuses on language development, written language acquisition and language awareness. She has published several books in German dealing with these topics. ADDRESS: Helga Andresen, Universität Flensburg Auf dem Campus, 1 D-24943 Flensburg, Germany. [email: andresen@uni-flensburg.de] 414