Neuropsychological Rehabilitation An International Journal ISSN: (Print) (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/pnrh20 Spatial exploration strategy training for spatial neglect: A pilot study Joan Toglia & Peii Chen To cite this article: Joan Toglia & Peii Chen (2020): Spatial exploration strategy training for spatial neglect: A pilot study, Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, DOI: 10.1080/09602011.2020.1790394 To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/09602011.2020.1790394 View supplementary material Published online: 20 Jul 2020. Submit your article to this journal View related articles View Crossmark data Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=pnrh20 NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL REHABILITATION https://doi.org/10.1080/09602011.2020.1790394 Spatial exploration strategy training for spatial neglect: A pilot study Joan Toglia a,b and Peii Chen c,d a School of Health and Natural Sciences, Mercy College, Dobbs Ferry, NY, USA; bRehabilitation Medicine Department, New York Presbyterian Hospital, Weill Cornell Medical Center, New York, NY, USA; c Kessler Foundation, West Orange, NJ, USA; dDepartment of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Rutgers University, Newark, NJ, USA ABSTRACT ARTICLE HISTORY Spatial neglect is a syndrome due to impaired neural networks critical for spatial attention and related cognitive and motor functions. Affected individuals also have impaired self-awareness of their own neglect symptoms. The present randomized controlled study was the first proof-of-concept pilot examining the multi-context treatment approach using a protocol of spatial exploration strategy training in one brief session (20–30 minutes). The therapist provided supportive feedback and semi-structured guidance to promote strategy learning and self-discovery of omission errors. 40 patients with left-sided neglect after right brain stroke were included. The results showed that the treatment reduced lateralized bias toward the ipsilesional side of space but did not improve overall detection performance. Impaired general self-awareness of daily-life spatial difficulties was found independent of treatment outcome. This implies that judgment regarding responsiveness to treatment should not be made based on an awareness interview or the severity of neglect symptoms. Lastly, the treatment showed the potential of improving online contextual self-awareness of spatial abilities. A collaborative and interactive approach that focuses on helping the patient self-discover, monitor and self-manage their errors, appears to have a potential for decreasing neglect symptoms. Future studies are required to examine additional aspects of the multi-context treatment approach. Received 28 October 2019 Accepted 3 June 2020 KEYWORDS Stroke rehabilitation; Brain injury; Occupational therapy; Cognitive rehabilitation; Anosognosia Introduction Spatial neglect is a syndrome due to impaired neural networks critical for spatial attention and related cognitive and motor functions. Affected individuals pay little or insufficient attention toward the side of space contralateral to their CONTACT Joan Toglia jtoglia@mercy.edu School of Health and Natural Sciences, Mercy College, 555 Broadway, Dobbs Ferry, NY 10522, USA Supplemental data for this article can be accessed at https://doi.org/10.1080/09602011.2020.1790394 © 2020 Informa UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis Group 2 J. TOGLIA AND P. CHEN injured cerebral hemisphere (Corbetta & Shulman, 2011; Mesulam, 1999), manifested as a failure to report, respond, or initiate movement toward stimuli presented in the contralesional side of space (Heilman et al., 2012). Left-sided neglect after right brain stroke is more common than right-sided neglect after left brain stroke (Chen, Chen, et al., 2015a). Spatial neglect is a major hidden disability post-stroke, slowing functional recovery and impeding rehabilitation outcomes (Chen, Hreha, et al., 2015b; Wee & Hopman, 2008). Treatment for spatial neglect can be categorized into two major approaches: the bottom-up stimulus-driven approach and the top-down strategy-learning approach (Bowen et al., 2013; Marshall, 2009). The bottom-up approach may facilitate neural changes in spatial functions through various sensory stimulation or visuomotor adaptation pathways. Examples of the bottom-up approach to spatial neglect rehabilitation include optokinetic stimulation (Kerkhoff et al., 2006), neck vibration (Schindler et al., 2002) and prism adaptation (Goedert et al., 2020). Unlike the bottom-up approach, the top-down approach is focused on explicit learning of strategies and repetitive practice of strategy application. The most commonly used top-down treatment for spatial neglect is visual scanning (Chen et al., 2018; Weinberg et al., 1977). During treatment, patients with left-sided neglect learn to use a visual search pattern by placing a visually salient stimulus (e.g., bright-coloured stripe) on the left edge of a defined workspace (e.g., a table or a newspaper page). They start searching from the salient stimulus toward the right, non-neglected side of space. This strategy helps patients to read and locate objects. The lighthouse strategy is also commonly taught to patients with spatial neglect (Niemeier, 1998; Niemeier et al., 2001). It is a visual imagery technique wherein patients learn to scan a scene by turning their head from side to side, like a sweeping beam of light from a lighthouse. The literature shows that these top-down trainings improve spatial neglect and functional activities (Bowen et al., 2013; Winstein et al., 2016). However, the top-down treatment approach can be challenging, especially for patients who have impaired self-awareness of their neglect symptoms or the consequences of their spatial deficits, i.e., anosognosia for spatial neglect (Berti et al., 1996; Chen & Toglia, 2019; Jehkonen et al., 2001; Ronchi et al., 2014), and for patients who have difficulty in sustaining their attention on a specific task for a period of time (I. H. Robertson, 1990; I. H. Robertson et al., 1995). Patients with self-awareness deficits may not actively seek treatment or be engaged in treatment activities. Patients with sustained attention deficits may be limited in their learning ability. Beyond the dichotomy of the bottom-up vs. top-down approaches, the multicontext treatment approach, based on a Dynamic Interactional Model of Cognition (Toglia, 2011; Toglia & Cermak, 2009; Toglia et al., 2019), suggests a highly personalized strategy training approach that focuses on helping a patient to self-recognize, monitor and manage cognitive symptoms across variations in tasks. The multi-context treatment approach aims to remediate spatial neglect NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL REHABILITATION 3 through supportive guidance and collaborative interaction between the patient and the therapist to facilitate self-awareness and optimize functional performance. Others have also proposed solutions using verbal or video feedback during a specific visuospatial task (Tham & Tegner, 1997) or every day activities (Soderback et al., 1992; Tham et al., 2001). For example, Soderback and colleagues (1992) video-recorded four patients with left-sided neglect who were asked to find a piece of dough in a refrigerator, cut the dough into sixteen pieces and distribute them evenly on a baking tray. During video review, the patient was guided in identifying neglect symptoms to increase self-awareness. Strategies for improving performance were suggested, including systematic search from left to right, and exploration of the entire workspace by feeling the left-end edge of the baking tray. The same tasks and procedures were repeated four times over 7 days. In addition to increased performance on the baking tasks, performance also improved on the Albert’s Test (Albert, 1973), a target cancellation test (Soderback et al., 1992). Another case series study conducted by Tham and colleagues (2001), asked four patients to perform the same self-selected activity again (e.g., reading newspaper or gardening) immediately after receiving feedback and discussing strategies. After four weeks of intervention (one hour a day, five days a week), spatial neglect improved based on a target cancellation test and the Baking Tray Test (Tham & Tegner, 1996). In addition to spatial neglect, self-awareness of one’s own disability improved as well (Tham et al., 2001). However, the self-awareness deficit addressed in Tham et al. (2001) was not specifically related to spatial neglect, and the measure used did not separate general, offline self-awareness from contextual, online self-awareness (Toglia & Kirk, 2000). The present study was the first proof-of-concept pilot examining the multicontext treatment approach for spatial neglect as well as self-awareness deficits using a randomized controlled study design. Specifically, we created a protocol for spatial exploration strategy training. The therapist guides a patient in specific research-informed strategies based on the patient’s performance of a spatial task at the time. The methods used for guidance adhere to supportive feedback and help the person self-discover omission errors themselves (Schrijnemaekers et al., 2014). The protocol also incorporates common neurorehabilitation principles such as repetition (i.e., multiple practice trials in a given session) and includes varying task features (i.e., changes in size of explored space and in distractor items). To this end, we examined three a priori hypotheses in the present study. Hypothesis 1 was that a single session of spatial exploration strategy training would improve spatial neglect symptoms. By definition, patients who are generally unaware of their own spatial difficulties (offline anosognosia) do not recognize the need for treatment of spatial neglect, and thus may receive little benefit from strategy training. Thus, Hypothesis 2 was that better general, offline self-awareness of spatial neglect symptoms in real-life situations would be associated with better treatment outcomes. Lastly, 4 J. TOGLIA AND P. CHEN the protocol was focused on helping patients with spatial neglect to self-recognize, monitor and manage their own symptoms during strategy training. Thus, Hypothesis 3 was that online self-awareness of spatial difficulties would be better after intervention in the treatment group, relative to the control group. Materials and methods Participants and Procedures. Forty-four individuals were recruited from five inpatient rehabilitation facilities in the metropolitan area of New York City and provided informed consent, from January to December 2003. The study was approved by each facility’s Institutional Research Board. While participating in the study, all the patients were receiving rehabilitation services. Informed consent and authorization to disclose protected health information for research was obtained prior to testing. Inclusion criteria for study participation were (1) right unilateral cerebrovascular accident, diagnosed by neuroradiologic findings (CT or MRI scan), (2) presence of spatial neglect as shown in at least one of the four designated tests (described in the following section), administered by an occupational therapist, (3) ability to follow two-step directions and to provide informed consent, as determined by the primary unit physician, (4) functional use of one hand, (5) visual acuity of at least 20/80 (with corrective lenses if applicable), and (6) no prior history of significant previous psychiatric disorder, neurological disorder, or chronic alcoholism. Participating patients were randomly assigned to one of two intervention groups: the control group or the treatment group. Randomization was based on the last digit of a patient’s medical record number, reported by the patient’s primary therapists to the research therapist. Recruitment terminated once each of the two intervention groups reached 20 participants who completed five activities in the sequence shown in Figure 1. During intervention, the treatment group received a semi-structured, therapist-guided spatial exploration strategy training while completing a 12-page object search task; the control group completed the task without receiving any guidance or feedback from the therapist. Object Search Task. A patient was presented with 12 pages of line drawings of objects, one page at a time. There were four task conditions based on page size and the presence of background non-object distractors. The task condition Figure 1. Sequence of the activities in the present study. NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL REHABILITATION 5 changed every 3 pages (Figure 2), in this order: Small-sized page (8 ½ x11 in.) with no distractor, large-sized page (17 × 22 in.) with no distractor, small-sized page with distractors, and large-sized page with distractors (see examples of stimuli in Figure 3). The small-sized and large-sized page conditions differed in the blank space among stimuli while the stimulus size remained the same across conditions (i.e., stimuli were more scattered on a large than small page). Each page contained 24 common and recognizable objects, such as door, spoon, bell, etc. 12 of the objects scattered on the left side, and the other 12 objects on the right side. Each page was presented in midline of the patient who was instructed to, “tell me all of the objects that you see.” If the patient was unable to name the object, the therapist instructed him/her to describe the object’s function or point to it. Inaccurate naming or inability to recognize objects were documented, but the person was given full credit for ability to locate an object, even when misidentification occurred. Thus, performance on each page was rated based on object detection, ranging from 0 to 24. In addition, we also assessed performance using the laterality index and the location of the first object detected. The laterality index was calculated to determine the degree of asymmetrical performance (i.e., the ratio of the number of items detected on the left side to the total number of items detected). The location of the first object detected was recorded and coded as 1, 2, or 3 to indicate the left, centre, or right region of a given page. Spatial Exploration Strategy Training. The therapist provided the spatial exploration strategy training to patients in the treatment group during the object search task. The objectives were to teach the patient exploration strategies, to help increase self-awareness, and to promote a sense of control over symptoms of spatial neglect (Toglia, 2004; Toglia & Cermak, 2009). The same steps were followed for each of the 12 object search pages (see further information in the Supplemental Material). Step 1: Object Search. The therapist presented the patient with a new page of stimuli and asked them to report all the objects they detected. Performance was recorded and coded as described above. Step 2: Verbal Feedback. If omissions occurred during step 1, the therapist generally informed the patient where objects were missed (left or right side) but did not point out errors or orient the patient to the neglected space. The objective was to encourage the patient to spontaneously orient to the general area where the objects had not been detected. Figure 2. Sequence and conditions of the 12-page object search task. 6 J. TOGLIA AND P. CHEN Figure 3. Examples of stimuli used in the object search task: (a) small page without distractor and (b) small page with distractors. Step 3. Strategies. If there were still missing objects after verbal feedback, the therapist proceeded to strategy exploration training. This included use of a tactile and spatial imagery strategy, a visual anchor strategy, and a stimulus reduction strategy, in this order or until all objects on a page were detected. The tactile and mental imagery strategies were designed to assist the person in creating a representation of space and orienting them to space that had not been explored. Errors were not directly pointed out. Instead, guidance was provided to help the patient attend to neglected space and self-discover omissions themselves. The visual anchor strategy was designed to build upon the tactile and mental imagery strategy by suggesting the utilization of an external stimulus visible to the patient. The stimulus served as the visual anchor as well as visual feedback for the person to know that their exploration reached the left side. Finally, the stimulus reduction strategy encouraged the patient to cover part of the page using a blank piece of paper to reduce visual stimuli so that they could self-recognize missed items. Step 4: Self-reflection and Strategy Reinforcement. Before proceeding to the next page regardless of the performance, the therapist would ask the participant “Why do you think you originally may have missed some of those items?” The person was encouraged to answer using their own words. The therapist would end the conversation with “The methods I showed you will help you check yourself to make sure you have seen everything on both sides of space. Remember to use [the strategies above]. It will help you monitor the tendency to miss things.” The patient was provided positive feedback each time spontaneous initiation of the strategies was observed on subsequent pages. Spatial Neglect Assessment Battery. We considered two outcome measures that determined patients’ performance of a battery of four tests for spatial neglect including line crossing, star cancellation, baking tray task, and picture scanning. Except for the baking tray task (Tham & Tegner, 1996), all the other tests were extracted from the Behavioral Inattention Test (B. Wilson et al., 1987) and were NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL REHABILITATION 7 included in the total detection score (i.e., the total number of items detected), which was one of the outcome measures. The other outcome measure was the overall laterality index of the four tests, which was defined as the total number of items detected on the left side divided by the total number of items detected. A laterality index between 0.48 and 0.52 was considered within the normal “unbiased” range while we determined an index above 0.52 as right-sided neglect and below 0.48 as left-sided neglect (Halligan et al., 1991). It is important to note that a given patient can demonstrate a poor performance below the abnormality cutoff but does not show lateralized neglect. Line Crossing. The test consists of 40 lines, each 2.5 cm in length, in various orientations, printed on a piece of paper (20 × 26 cm). The lines are arranged in seven columns of six lines each, except for the central column of four lines (not included in scoring). The examiner demonstrated by crossing out two of the four centre lines and asked the participant to cross out all the other lines. The maximum score is 36 (18 left and 18 right). Star Cancellation. The test is a visual search task for small stars on a 30 × 21 cm page, where there are distractor items that included 52 large stars, 10 short words, and 13 capital letters. All stimuli are randomly interspersed. Among the 56 small stars (targets), there are two small stars in the centre of the page that the examiner crosses out for demonstration before the patient begins taking the test. The maximum score is 54 (27 left and 27 right). Baking Tray Task. The test presents a 50 × 75 cm hardboard and 16 blocks of wood, and the task is to place the blocks over the hardboard as symmetrically as possible, as though the blocks were buns being placed on a baking tray to be put in the oven. Symmetrical placement required that eight blocks be placed on each half of the tray. The number of blocks placed on each half of the board is counted (maximum score = 16). When a block is placed directly in midline, a score of .5 is counted for each area. Due to the scoring method, this test was only included in the overall laterality index but excluded from the total detection score of the entire battery. Picture Scanning. The test consists of three 11 × 14′ coloured photographs of (1) meal on a plate, (2) bathroom sink with toiletries, and (3) view of a room. Each photograph has various objects arranged about the midline. The instruction is to name and point to all the objects in each picture. There are two alternate versions of the test. The two versions present the same objects in different locations, in each of the photographs. In the present study, pictures were presented in the same order to each patient, with version A used before intervention and version B after intervention. The score is the total number of items detected on the right and left side of each photograph. Centre items are not scored. The maximum score or total number of objects across the three photographs is 24 (12 left and 12 right). Errors in naming or misidentification of objects are recorded but do not lower the score. 8 J. TOGLIA AND P. CHEN General Self-Awareness for Spatial Difficulty. After completing the spatial neglect assessment battery as the baseline measure (Figure 1), we asked the patients to answer 17 questions about their self-perceived spatial difficulties in daily-life situations (Table 1). The answer to each question could be “not at all,” “a little,” “somewhat,” or “a lot,” which were coded from 1 to 4 respectively. Their primary occupational therapists were also asked to answer those questions. These therapists had interacted with the patients a minimum of four days and completed assessment for functional independence with the patients, and thus they had observed their patients in various daily-life situations. If an activity described in a given question was not applicable to the patient or not observed by the therapist, then the question was skipped. The final score of each questionnaire was the average of all the available answers provided. We then generated the offline self-awareness deficit score by subtracting the patient self-rated score from the therapist-rated score, with a greater value indicating that a greater level of general self-awareness deficits.Toglia, 2004). Online Self-Awareness Deficit. Immediately after intervention, the patient was asked to estimate their performance in the 12-page object search task (Figure 1). The self-estimation was compared to the total number of objects detected across the 12 pages (maximum number of objects = 288) and scored dichotomously. Specifically, under the following circumstances, a score of 0 was assigned for no online self-awareness deficit: (1) if they rated performance as “no difficulty” and if they detected at least 91% (≥ 262) of the objects, (2) if they indicated they had a “little difficulty” and if they detected at least 71% (≥ 204) of the objects, (3) If they indicated they had “some difficulty” and if they detected at least 50% (≥ 144) of the objects, and (4) if they indicated they had “lot of difficulty” and if they detected fewer than 50% (≤ 143) of the objects. If Table 1. Questionnaire for self-perceived spatial difficulties in daily-life situations. 1. Since your stroke, do you experience … difficulty locating, finding or seeing everything around you? 2. Since your stroke, do you experience … difficulty understanding what you read? 3. Since your stroke, do you experience … tendency to miss food or items on your plate / food tray? 4. Since your stroke, do you experience … tendency to make errors dialing phone numbers? 5. Since your stroke, do you experience … tendency to misread time on clock or watches? 6. Since your stroke, do you experience … difficulty finding items in the bathroom closet or sink? 7. Since your stroke, do you experience … difficulty finding all the items on tables, shelves, closets, draws or counters? 8. Since your stroke, do you experience … tendency to skip words or sentences when reading? 9. Since your stroke, do you experience … a tendency to miss things on the left side? 10. Since your stroke, do you experience … a tendency to bump into walls, doorways, furniture or people on the left side? 11. Since your stroke, do you experience … tendency to forget about your left arm or foot? 12. Since your stroke, do you experience … a tendency to miss numbers, letters or words on the left side? 13. Since your stroke, do you experience … a tendency to shave only one side of your face or put makeup only on one side? 14. Since your stroke, do you experience … a tendency to comb one side of your hair? 15. Since your stroke, do you experience … a tendency to miss food on the left side? 16. Since your stroke, do you experience … a tendency to crowd writing on the right side of a page? 17. To what extent are any difficulties in finding or seeing things on the left side interfering with your overall ability to function? NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL REHABILITATION 9 a given patient did not meet any of the above circumstances, it was determined that they overestimated their performance and were given a score of 1 for the presence of online self-awareness deficits. Analysis Methods. All the analyses were performed using STATA/SE16.0, and the alpha level was set at .05. We described participant characteristics (nonmodifiable variables) using median and interquartile range (IQR) for continuous variables, and percentages for categorical variables, and performed group comparisons using the U test for continuous variables and chi-squared for categorical variables. If any of the non-modifiable variables showed a potential difference (p < .1) between groups, they would be included in the main analyses that examined a given a priori hypotheses. Results and discussion Participant Characteristics. Three patients (two in the control group and one in the treatment group) did not meet all the inclusion criteria. One patient in the treatment group was unable to complete testing because of medical problems unrelated to the study procedures. A total of 40 patients, 20 in each group, were included in the final analysis. Table 2 presents the characteristics of participating patients within each group. Two variables were potentially different between groups. The treatment group was younger and had more male participants than the control group (both p < .1). These two variables, age and sex, were then included in the following analyses that examined a priori hypotheses. Hypothesis 1: A single session of spatial exploration strategy training would improve spatial neglect symptoms. To test this hypothesis, we conducted a 2 × 2 repeated-measure ANCOVA with the intervention group (control vs. treatment), assessment time (before vs. after intervention), and the interaction term of the two as predictors to examine each of the two outcome measures (the total score and the overall laterality index of the spatial neglect assessment battery), and added age and sex as covariates. First, we examined the total detection score of the spatial neglect assessment battery, and the analysis showed a main effect of group, F(1,36) = 39.77, p < .001, partial η² = .51, but no main effect of assessment time, F(1,36) = .34, p = .561. The group by assessment time interaction was significant, F(1,36) = 6.59, p = .014, partial η² = .15. Post hoc Tukey Honest Significant Difference (HSD) tests revealed that (1) at baseline, the control group had a better score than the treatment group but after the intervention, the two groups did not differ, and (2) the control group’s score declined after intervention by an average of 6.8 points (Table 3) while the interaction did not change the treatment group’s score statistically. Figure 4(a) illustrates this interaction. Next, we examined the overall laterality index of the spatial neglect assessment battery using the same analysis model and procedures. The analysis yielded no main effect of group, F(1,36) = 3.71, p = .062, or assessment time, F 10 J. TOGLIA AND P. CHEN Table 2. Participant characteristics. Variable Control Group (n = 20) Treatment Group (n = 20) Group comparison result: p value Age (in year) 75 (63–78.5) 64.5 (57–74.5) .066 Sex: .058 Male 7 (35%) 13 (65%) Female 13 (65%) 7 (35%) Race/Ethnicity: .387 White 17 (85%) 13 (65%) Black 3 (15%) 5 (25%) Asian 0 1 (5%) Hispanic 0 1 (5%) Education: .394 < High School 2 (10%) 3 (15%) High school 7 (35%) 6 (30%) Associate degree or some college 1 (5%) 5 (25%) College 4 (20%) 3 (15%) Graduate school 6 (30%) 3 (15%) Time post stroke (in day) 26 (16.5–39) 22 (18–41) .828 Stroke type: .429 Ischemic 17 (85%) 15 (75%) Hemorrhage 3 (15%) 5 (25%) Visual field cut: .758 Yes 11 (55%) 9 (45%) No 9 (45%) 9 (45%) Missing information* 0 2 (10%) Infarct location in the right cerebral .116 hemisphere: Cortical with frontal involvement 2 (10%) 5 (25%) Cortical without frontal 6 (30%) 4 (20%) involvement Subcortical 6 (30%) 3 (15%) Mixed 2 (10%) 8 (40%) Missing information* 4 (20%) 0 General (offline) self-awareness 1.35 (1–1.93) 1.38 (1.12–1.85) .764 deficit score Notes: Categorical variables are described in counts (%) and analysed using X 2. Continuous variables are summarized in median (IQR) and compared using the U test. *Missing information was information unavailable from medical charts of a given rehabilitation hospital and was not included in group comparison analysis. (1,36) = .82, p = .370, but the group by assessment time interaction was significant, F(1,36) = 4.76, p = .035, partial η² = .11. Post hoc Tukey HSD tests showed that (1) at baseline, the control group was less lateralized than the treatment group, and the two groups were not different after intervention, and (2) the control group’s laterality index did not change significantly after intervention but the treatment group’s became less lateralized (see Table 3 and Figure 4(b)). In summary, although patients were randomly assigned to a group, the control group had better performance than the treatment group at baseline, shown in both the detection score and the laterality index. The control group’s performance declined after intervention, in terms of the detection score, but not in terms of the laterality index, suggesting that their lateralized spatial bias remained unchanged but their ability to make correct responses in the spatial neglect assessment battery deteriorated. In contrast, the treatment group’s performance improved after the spatial exploration strategy training, Table 3. Scores of the Spatial Neglect Assessment Battery before and after intervention, in means (SD). Line crossing Group Baking tray task Picture scanning Overall After Before After Before After Before After Before After 30.0 (8.6) .38 (.21) 27.3 (9.4) .34 (.22) 34.7 (13.4) .32 (.20) 31.5 (15.4) .30 (.21) NA .29 (.29) NA .35 (.30) 17.0 (4.4) .39 (.17) 16.0 (5.1) .28 (.18) 81.6 (22.9) .34 (.16) 74.8 (27.7) .32 (.19) 26.1 (10.5) .31 (.22) 27.6 (8.8) .35 (.18) 30.6 (15.1) .23 (.20) 31.7 (15.3) .31 (.19) NA .06 (.11) NA .20 (.27) 15.3 (4.9) .34 (.17) 17.0 (5.3) .31 (.19) 71.9 (28.2) .23 (.15) 76.2 (27.3) .29 (.17) Notes: The overall detection score is the sum of three test scores (excluding the Baking Tray Task; see the Methods section for the scoring method of this test), and the overall laterality index is the average of the indices from all four tests. Abbreviation: NA, not applicable. NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL REHABILITATION Control Group (n = 20) Detection Score Laterality Index Treatment Group (n = 20) Detection Score Laterality Index Star cancellation Before 11 12 J. TOGLIA AND P. CHEN Figure 4. Treatment efficacy on spatial neglect severity in terms of (a) detection score and (b) laterality of the spatial neglect assessment battery before and after intervention. The higher the detection score, the better the visuospatial performance. The higher the laterality index, the more symmetrical the performance (less bias toward the ipsilesional side of space). Error bar = SE. in terms of laterality but not of detection ability, suggesting that the treatment was effective in reducing lateralized spatial bias but did not change their ability to make correct responses in visuospatial tasks. Thus, the finding on laterality was consistent with the a prior hypothesis. However, the finding of the detection score was not as expected. Hypothesis 2: General, offline self-awareness of spatial difficulties was associated with better treatment outcome. Based on the U test, the two groups did not differ in self-awareness deficit score (p = .764; see the last row of Table 2), suggesting they had a similar level of general, offline self-awareness deficits. To examine the hypothesis, we performed a multivariate regression analysis on each outcome measure. Intervention group, self-awareness deficit score, and the interaction of the two were independent variables, and the baseline outcome measure, age, and sex were included as covariates. The hypothesis would be supported if the interaction effect was significant. For the total detection score, none of the independent variables showed a significant effect (all p values > .1). For the overall laterality index, none showed a significant effect either (all p values > .3). Thus, we did not find evidence supporting the hypothesis, suggesting that general self-awareness of spatial deficits might be independent of treatment outcome. Hypothesis 3: Online emergent self-awareness of spatial difficulties would be better after intervention in the treatment group, relative to the control group. 15 (75%) of the control group and 8 (40%) of the treatment group showed online self-awareness deficits as defined by overestimation of their own performance relative to their actual performance over the 12 pages of object search. This group difference was found significant, X 2(2) = 5.01, p = .025. Thus, the hypothesis was supported by the result, suggesting that the spatial exploration strategy training improved online self-awareness of spatial difficulties among patients with spatial neglect. Post hoc Hypothesis: Performance continued improving during the spatial exploration strategy training. In order to explore the possible accounts for the NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL REHABILITATION 13 unexpected finding related to Hypothesis 1, we inspected the two groups’ performance during intervention. Due to the small sample size, we were unable to perform statistical analyses to examine the effect of object search condition embedded in the fixed page order (Figure 2). Thus, we described the apparent pattern as shown in Figure 5 and performed between-group comparisons in the differences between the 1st and 12th pages. While the number of objects detected by the control group declined (Figure 5 (a)), the treatment group appeared to improve from the first trial to the last trial within the first condition (small page without distractor) and within the third condition (small page with distractors) although the overall trend was also a decline of the number of objects detected over the 12 pages. The control group declined more than the treatment group (M ± SD = −7.6 ± 1.3 vs. −3.2 ± 1.3; t test: p = .020). The spatial exploration strategy training appeared to reduce lateralized bias (laterality index closer to .5) after the first page, and the laterality changed little from the 2nd to the 6th pages over two task conditions where there were no background distractors (Figure 5(b)). Then the laterality index dropped (more biased toward the right side) at the 7th page when the third condition (small page with distractors) was introduced, but after provided with feedback and strategies, laterality improved again over the next two task conditions (Figure 5(b)). On the other hand, the control group’s laterality index declined most apparently at the 7th page and declined further, probably due to too few of the objects detected in the later conditions and later trials. Overall, the control group became more biased but the treatment group improved (M ± SD = −.17 ± .05 vs. .06 ± .05; t test: p = .002). Lastly, the treatment group changed their initial search location from the right side toward the centre after the first two trials of the first condition and eventually more toward the left side during the last task condition (pages 10–12). The control group kept initiating their search more on the right side over the entire intervention period (Figure 5(c)). In comparison to the control group, the treatment group showed greater difference between the 1st and 12th pages (M ± SD = .05 ± .20 vs. −.7 ± .24; t Figure 5. Performance during intervention in terms of (a) numbers of objects detected, (b) laterality index, and (c) initial search location across the four consecutive task conditions: small page without distractor (S w/o D), large page without distractor (L w/o D), small page with distractors (S w/ D), and large page with distractors (L w/ D) over 12 pages. Each data point is the median value of each group corresponding to a given trial. The white circles and dash lines represent the control group, and the black circles and solid lines represent the treatment group. 14 J. TOGLIA AND P. CHEN test: p = .022). Overall, the spatial exploration strategy training prevented the treatment group’s detection performance from decline, and led the treatment group to initiate their object search from a centre-left location. General discussion We conducted a randomized controlled study that examined the effect of a brief 20-to-30-minute session of spatial exploratory strategy training among individuals with left-sided spatial neglect after right-brain stroke. The design and delivery methods of the intervention were based on principles from the multi-context treatment approach of the Dynamic Interactional Model of Cognition (Toglia, 2011; Toglia et al., 2019; Toglia & Cermak, 2009). The therapist provided supportive feedback and semi-structured guidance to promote strategy learning and self-discovery of omission errors. The spatial exploratory strategy training focused on helping the patient recognize the need to use strategies for deploying attention to the entire workspace and preventing omission errors. In particular, the guidance provided by the therapist involved hands-on and conversational interactions with the patient. This may have enhanced online self-awareness and self-efficacy. Self-efficacy is an individual’s perceived belief in their capabilities to manage performance errors or complete tasks successfully. As the person recognizes that they have missed items on the left side, motivation for change requires the belief that there are methods that one can use to prevent errors or enhance performance. In acquired brain injury, selfefficacy for managing symptoms has been associated with a better treatment engagement and quality of life (Belanger et al., 2019; Brands et al., 2014). It has also been found to be a mediating variable for perceived functional improvements in people with stroke (Nott et al., 2019). Although self-efficacy has not been specifically studied in patients with neglect, evidence from other studies suggests that using methods that provide an opportunity for patients to recognize and manage their own neglect symptoms, may be important in improving self-efficacy and functional outcomes (Brands et al., 2017; Nott et al., 2019). During the intervention, the treatment group showed improvement in laterality, and greater initiation of search on the left side, despite increasing task difficulty. The overall number of items detected on the outcome measure using the 4-test spatial neglect assessment battery, however, did not significantly change after intervention. The control group’s performance showed a decline in the number of items detected and unchanged laterality or search origin. These findings suggest that the spatial exploratory strategy training improves lateralized spatial bias, which was the signature symptom of spatial neglect but may not improve sustained attention that is required to complete visuospatial tasks. Moreover, findings from the control group suggest that repetition of visual search activities in the absence of therapist guidance and feedback does not improve spatial bias and may lead to fatigue or reduced engagement NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL REHABILITATION 15 in visual search tasks. Given the brevity of the spatial exploratory strategy training protocol in the present study, the changes observed in attention to the left side are impressive. The findings suggest, however, that once the lateralized bias is decreased, intervention may need to also address non-lateralized spatial attention deficits. Strategy learning can be challenging, especially for patients who have impaired self-awareness of their neglect symptoms or the consequences of their spatial deficits (Berti et al., 1996; Chen & Toglia, 2019; Jehkonen et al., 2001; Ronchi et al., 2014). In the present protocol, we specifically included supportive feedback, semi-structured guidance and strategy training to help the person self-detect and self-monitor errors in visuospatial tasks. Our second a priori hypothesis was that patients with greater degrees of general, offline self-awareness deficits would have more difficulty learning strategies, indicated by poorer treatment outcomes. The findings, however, did not support the hypothesis. The severity of general self-awareness deficits, outside the context of the task, did not appear to be associated with treatment outcome. However, online, emergent awareness of spatial difficulties improved in the treatment group compared to the control group, thus supporting our third a priori hypothesis. The ability to participate in and thus respond to the spatial exploration strategy training appears to be more dependent on online awareness or that which emerges from direct experience with task performance. The experience of performing repeated visual search tasks with guidance to facilitate self-recognition and attend to omitted parts of space, appears to have facilitated online awareness and attention to the left side in some patients, regardless of initial general, offline awareness. This disassociation between general, offline awareness and online awareness has been observed in patients with spatial neglect (Chen & Toglia, 2019) and different clinical populations (Goverover et al., 2014; O’Keeffe et al., 2007; Robertson & Schmitter-Edgecombe, 2015), supporting the Dynamic Comprehensive Model of Awareness (Toglia & Kirk, 2000). General offline awareness is based on patients’ daily-life experience, which relies on retrospective self-report, knowledge of task attributes (e.g., clock reading, hair brushing, way finding) as well as self-perceptions of abilities and beliefs about certain tasks or in general. Selfawareness of neglect symptoms after performing tasks that help the patient self-recognize task errors (Chen & Toglia, 2019) appears to be important in strategy learning and thus may be associated with treatment outcome. The Dynamic Comprehensive Model of Awareness hypothesizes that offline awareness is slower and more resistant to change, compared to online awareness. With repeated online awareness experiences over time, and across different situations, offline awareness is hypothesized to gradually improve, however this requires future study. The present findings are of great importance in the development of personalized treatment for spatial neglect and related self-awareness deficits. Currently, 16 J. TOGLIA AND P. CHEN no treatment protocol exists to improve self-awareness in patients with spatial neglect following a stroke. The majority of recommended treatment options (Bowen et al., 2013; Hebert et al., 2016; Winstein et al., 2016) rarely take selfawareness deficits into account although anosognosia is a common symptom among patients with spatial neglect (Chen & Toglia, 2019). For patients with spatial neglect, their self-awareness of their own neglect symptoms is impaired, presenting an obstacle to rehabilitation efforts. Thus, the treatment for selfawareness cannot be isolated from the treatment for spatial neglect in this population. Addressing self-awareness when treating patients for spatial neglect can enhance treatment outcome, which is the core idea of the multi-context treatment approach of the Dynamic Interactional Model of Cognition (Toglia, 2011; Toglia et al., 2019; Toglia & Cermak, 2009). As online awareness emerges, the patient is immediately encouraged to use strategies to manage neglect symptoms, thus simultaneously building a sense of mastery and encouraging active participation in treatment. It is important that methods used to enhance online awareness are closely integrated with strategy training and efforts to foster self-efficacy Lower self-efficacy has been associated with anxiety and depression (Brands et al., 2019). If awareness of neglect symptoms emerges without tools for the person to use to manage their neglect symptoms, there may be a risk of negative emotional responses such as anxiety or depression (for related clinical implications, see Chen & Toglia, 2019). One interesting finding from the present study is the effect (or lack of) on the spatial neglect assessment battery, which consisted of four tests including line crossing, star cancellation, baking tray task (not included in the total detection score), and picture scanning. The treatment group showed no change in the total number of items detected after intervention although their lateralized bias was reduced, and the control group showed a lower number of items detected after intervention while their lateralized bias was unchanged. This indicates that patients were able to locate targets on both left and right sides of the space in a more symmetrical manner after intervention but they were unable to locate more targets overall. Thus, the present protocol is in favour of improving spatial neglect, but only partially. Spatial neglect has been evaluated and defined by abnormal lateralized bias toward the ipsilesional side of space. Non-lateralized spatial symptoms exist as part of the spatial neglect syndrome but have received relatively little attention. Halligan and colleagues (1991) classified patients who omitted approximately equal numbers of items on both sides of a paper-andpencil test, i.e., the Behavioral Inattention Test (Wilson et al., 1987), as having non-lateralized inattention. These patients’ test scores may be below the cutoff score for the diagnosis of spatial neglect but their performance does not necessarily manifest lateralized bias. It is possible that patients have limited global attentional capacity and difficulty sustaining attention over the course of a test. The spatial exploration strategy training examined in the NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL REHABILITATION 17 present study was not designed to improve sustained attention (Robertson et al., 1995; Wilson et al., 2000) but may have helped keep sustained attention at the same level pre and post-intervention, preventing the treatment group from performance decline. Alternative to the account of sustained attention, which is focused on the time-related sustainability, abnormality in the size of the attended spatial area at a given moment may be another possibility. We hypothesize that spatial neglect does not only impair the ability to allocate attention to the contralesional side of space but also narrow the attended spatial area. The metaphor of a moving spotlight has been used to theorize and generate discussions about mechanisms of spatial attention (Cave & Bichot, 1999; Eriksen & Murphy, 1987; Hamilton et al., 2010; Posner et al., 1980). The moving feature is emphasized much more than other features, such as the aperture size, of the attention spotlight. This is true in both cognitive psychology (studying spatial attention) and neuropsychology (studying spatial neglect). In the scenario of the present study, patients might be able to attend to the left and right side after intervention (in which they had learned spatial exploration strategies), but the area covered by their attentional searchlight remained abnormally narrowed so that they find a limited number of items even after they shifted attention. This argument is supported by a previous study suggesting abnormally small attention aperture being a feature of spatial neglect. Chen and Goedert (2012) asked stroke survivors with right brain damage and healthy individuals to draw a clock face on a blank piece of paper, which is a task that participants determine the workspace (clock perimeter) before allocating specific information within the workspace (clock numbers and hands). The authors found that patients without spatial neglect produced the clock in a similar size to that produced by healthy controls, but patients with spatial neglect produced smaller clocks. The size of the clock did not correlate with the location of the drawing on the paper (Chen & Goedert, 2012). Either way, the spatial exploration strategy training was not designed to address impairment in sustained attention or attention aperture. Future studies may address these possibilities into strategy training for improving spatial exploration. Study Limitations. This is a pilot study on a novel treatment approach for spatial neglect. Only one brief session was conducted. It is unknown how many sessions are required to observe effects being transferred to real-life situations or whether there is any long-term effect. However, we demonstrated that in the current relatively small sample size, the intervention improved lateralized bias. Future studies are required to examine the efficacy of multiple sessions in larger sample sizes and with a better-balanced study design, which avoids any potential violation of assumptions for utilizing repeated-measures ANCOVA or other parametric analyses. Another limitation is the difference between groups at baseline where the control group had better performance in both detection score and laterality index than the treatment group. This could create some 18 J. TOGLIA AND P. CHEN ambiguity in interpretation. Nonetheless, the analysis results supported the presence of the treatment effect. In addition, the study included patients with a variety of symptom severity. Patients with milder neglect might score at ceiling in any test included in the outcome measure before or after intervention, and their performance changes could not be adequately measured. A larger sample size would allow for analyses of learning patterns within subgroups that differ in level of severity. Conclusion A brief 20–30 minutes spatial exploration strategy training, which integrates supportive feedback and semi-structured guidance to promote strategy learning, reduces lateralized bias toward the ipsilesional side of space among individuals with left-sided spatial neglect after right-brain stroke. Impaired general selfawareness of daily-life spatial difficulties was found to be independent of treatment outcome. This implies that judgment regarding responsiveness to treatment should not be made based on an awareness interview or the severity of neglect symptoms. A collaborative and interactive approach that focuses on helping the patient self-discover, monitor and self-manage their errors, appears to have a potential for decreasing neglect symptoms. The intervention appeared to have improved self-awareness deficits within the task, which in turn, helped patients learn the strategies and attend to the left side of space. Methods that capture patterns of intra-individual change, learning and online awareness along with transfer to other activities need to be further explored. Future students need to examine additional aspects of this approach, particularly transfer and generalization beyond the visual object search task. The current intervention provided the opportunity to observe carryover of strategies from one page to the next across similar search tasks (near transfer). A next step would be to incorporate techniques to help the person apply strategies to other situations. For example, after the visual object search task, the patient could be given the opportunity to apply the same strategies to functional tasks such as searching for specific items in a cabinet or closet, or on a webpage for an online store. Guided questions that ask the patient to identify other situations that the same strategies could apply, could be used across different tasks or situations (Toglia, 2011). Finally, the role of online awareness and self-efficacy in contributing to functional outcome needs to be further explored. Acknowledgements We thank occupational therapists and participating patients in the Rusk Institute of New York University Medical Center, Burke Rehabilitation Hospital, Helen Hayes Hospital, New York Presbyterian Hospital, and Kings Harbor Multicare Center. NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL REHABILITATION 19 Disclosure statement No potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s). Funding This research was partially supported by an Extramural Research Development Award (Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development), National Institutes of Health No. 2G11HD035965, Pilot grant awarded to Joan Toglia, through award received by Mercy College, Dobbs Ferry, NY. ORCID Joan Toglia http://orcid.org/0000-0002-6902-6853 Peii Chen http://orcid.org/0000-0002-6724-7046 References Albert, M. L. (1973). A simple test of visual neglect. Neurology, 23(6), 658–658. https://doi.org/ 10.1212/wnl.23.6.658 Belanger, H. G., Vanderploeg, R. D., Curtiss, G., Armistead-Jehle, P., Kennedy, J. E., Tate, D. F., Eapen B. C., Bowles A. O. & Cooper, D. B. (2019). Self-efficacy predicts response to cognitive rehabilitation in military service members with post-concussive symptoms. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 1190–1203. https://doi.org/10.1080/09602011.2019. 1575245 Berti, A., Ladavas, E., & Della Corte, M. (1996). Anosognosia for hemiplegia, neglect dyslexia, and drawing neglect: Clinical findings and theoretical considerations. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 2(5), 426–440. https://doi.org/10.1017/ S135561770000151X Bowen, A., Hazelton, C., Pollock, A., & Lincoln, N. B. (2013). Cognitive rehabilitation for spatial neglect following stroke. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, 7, CD003586. https://doi. org/10.1002/14651858.CD003586.pub3 Brands, I., Custers, M., & van Heugten, C. (2017). Self-efficacy and quality of life after low-intensity neuropsychological rehabilitation: A pre-post intervention study. Neurorehabilitation, 40 (4), 587–594. https://doi.org/10.3233/nre-171446 Brands, I., Kohler, S., Stapert, S., Wade, D., & van Heugten, C. (2014). Influence of self-efficacy and coping on quality of life and social participation after acquired brain injury: A 1-year follow-up study. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 95(12), 2327–2334. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apmr.2014.06.006 Brands, I. M. H., Verlinden, I., & Ribbers, G. M. (2019). A study of the influence of cognitive complaints, cognitive performance and symptoms of anxiety and depression on self-efficacy in patients with acquired brain injury. Clinical Rehabilitation, 33(2), 327–334. https://doi.org/10. 1177/0269215518795249 Cave, K. R., & Bichot, N. P. (1999). Visuospatial attention: Beyond a spotlight model. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 6(2), 204–223. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03212327 Chen, P., Chen, C. C., Hreha, K., Goedert, K. M., & Barrett, A. M. (2015a). Kessler Foundation neglect assessment Process uniquely measures spatial neglect during activities of daily living. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 96(5), 869–876. https://doi.org/10. 1016/j.apmr.2014.10.023 20 J. TOGLIA AND P. CHEN Chen, P., & Goedert, K. M. (2012). Clock drawing in spatial neglect: A comprehensive analysis of clock perimeter, placement, and accuracy. Journal of Neuropsychology, 6(2), 270–289. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1748-6653.2012.02028.x Chen, P., Hreha, K., Kong, Y., & Barrett, A. M. (2015b). Impact of spatial neglect in stroke rehabilitation: Evidence from the setting of an inpatient rehabilitation facility. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 96(8), 1458–1466. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apmr.2015.03.019 Chen, P., Pitteri, M., Gillen, G., & Ayyala, H. (2018). Ask the experts how to treat individuals with spatial neglect: A survey study. Disability and Rehabilitation, 40(22), 2677–2691. https://doi. org/10.1080/09638288.2017.1347720 Chen, P., & Toglia, J. (2019). Online and offline awareness deficits: Anosognosia for spatial neglect. Rehabilitation Psychology, 64(1), 50–64. https://doi.org/10.1037/rep0000207 Corbetta, M., & Shulman, G. L. (2011). Spatial neglect and attention networks. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 34(1), 569–599. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-061010-113731 Eriksen, C. W., & Murphy, T. D. (1987). Movement of attentional focus across the visual field: A critical look at the evidence. Perception and Psychophysics, 42(3), 299–305. https://doi.org/ 10.3758/bf03203082 Goedert, K. M., Chen, P., Foundas, A. L., & Barrett, A. M. (2020). Frontal lesions predict response to prism adaptation treatment in spatial neglect: A randomised controlled study. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 30(1), 32–53. https://doi.org/10.1080/09602011.2018. 1448287 Goverover, Y., Genova, H., Griswold, H., Chiaravalloti, N., & DeLuca, J. (2014). Metacognitive knowledge and online awareness in persons with multiple sclerosis. Neurorehabilitation, 35(2), 315–323. https://doi.org/10.3233/nre-141113 Halligan, P. W., Robertson, I., Pizzamiglio, L., Homberg, V., Weber, E., & Bergego, C. (1991). The laterality of visual neglect after right hemisphere damage. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 1(4), 281–301. https://doi.org/10.1080/09602019108402259 Hamilton, R. H., Stark, M., & Coslett, H. B. (2010). Increased effect of target eccentricity on covert shifts of visual attention in patients with neglect. Cortex, 46(1), 68–76. https://doi.org/10. 1016/j.cortex.2009.02.005 Hebert, D., Lindsay, M. P., McIntyre, A., Kirton, A., Rumney, P. G., Bagg, S., Bayley M., Dowlatshahi D., Dukelow S., Garnhum M., Glasser E., Halabi M.-L., Kang E., MacKay-Lyons M., Martino R., Rochette A., Rowe S., Salbach N., Semenko B., … Teasell, R. (2016). Canadian stroke best practice recommendations: Stroke rehabilitation practice guidelines, update 2015. International Journal of Stroke, 11(4), 459–484. https://doi.org/10.1177/1747493016643553 Heilman, K. M., Watson, R. T., & Valenstein, E. (2012). Neglect and related disorders. In K. M. Heilman & E. Valenstein (Eds.), Clinical neuropsychology (5th ed, pp. 296–348). Oxford University. Jehkonen, M., Ahonen, J. P., Dastidar, P., Koivisto, A. M., Laippala, P., Vilkki, J., & Molnar, G. (2001). Predictors of discharge to home during the first year after right hemisphere stroke. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica, 104(3), 136–141. https://doi.org/10.1034/j.16000404.2001.00025.x Kerkhoff, G., Keller, I., Ritter, V., & Marquardt, C. (2006). Repetitive optokinetic stimulation induces lasting recovery from visual neglect. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience, 24 (4-6), 357–369. http://content.iospress.com/articles/restorative-neurology-andneuroscience/rnn00356 Marshall, R. S. (2009). Rehabilitation approaches to hemineglect. The Neurologist, 15(4), 185– 192. https://doi.org/10.1097/NRL.0b013e3181942894 Mesulam, M. M. (1999). Spatial attention and neglect: Parietal, frontal and cingulate contributions to the mental representation and attentional targeting of salient extrapersonal NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL REHABILITATION 21 events. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 354(1387), 1325–1346. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.1999.0482 Niemeier, J. P. (1998). The lighthouse strategy: Use of a visual imagery technique to treat visual inattention in stroke patients. Brain Injury, 12(5), 399–406. https://doi.org/10.1080/ 026990598122511 Niemeier, J. P., Cifu, D. X., & Kishore, R. (2001). The lighthouse strategy: Improving the functional status of patients with unilateral neglect after stroke and brain injury using a visual imagery intervention. Topics in Stroke Rehabilitation, 8(2), 10–18. https://doi.org/10.1310/ 7UKK-HJ0F-GDWF-HHM8 Nott, M., Wiseman, L., Seymour, T., Pike, S., Cuming, T., & Wall, G. (2019). Stroke self-management and the role of self-efficacy. Disability and Rehabilitation, 1–10. https://doi.org/10. 1080/09638288.2019.1666431 O’Keeffe, F., Dockree, P., Moloney, P., Carton, S., & Robertson, I. H. (2007). Awareness of deficits in traumatic brain injury: A multidimensional approach to assessing metacognitive knowledge and online-awareness. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 13(1), 38–49. https://doi.org/10.1017/s1355617707070075 Posner, M. I., Snyder, C. R. R., & Davidson, B. J. (1980). Attention and the detection of signals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 109(2), 160–174. https://doi.org/10.1037/00963445.109.2.160 Robertson, I. H. (1990). Digit span and visual neglect: A puzzling relationship. Neuropsychologia, 28(2), 217–222. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(90)90103-u Robertson, K., & Schmitter-Edgecombe, M. (2015). Self-awareness and traumatic brain injury outcome. Brain Injury, 29(7-8), 848–858. https://doi.org/10.3109/02699052.2015.1005135 Robertson, I. H., Tegner, R., Tham, K., Lo, A., & Nimmo-Smith, I. (1995). Sustained attention training for unilateral neglect: Theoretical and rehabilitation implications. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 17(3), 416–430. https://doi.org/10.1080/ 01688639508405133 Ronchi, R., Bolognini, N., Gallucci, M., Chiapella, L., Algeri, L., Spada, M. S., & Vallar, G. (2014). (Un)awareness of unilateral spatial neglect: A quantitative evaluation of performance in visuo-spatial tasks. Cortex, 61, 167–182. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2014.10.004 Schindler, I., Kerkhoff, G., Karnath, H. O., Keller, I., & Goldenberg, G. (2002). Neck muscle vibration induces lasting recovery in spatial neglect. Journal of Neurology Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 73(4), 412–419. https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.73.4.412 Schrijnemaekers, A. C., Smeets, S. M., Ponds, R. W., van Heugten, C. M., & Rasquin, S. (2014). Treatment of unawareness of deficits in patients with acquired brain injury: A systematic review. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 29(5), E9–E30. https://doi.org/10.1097/01. HTR.0000438117.63852.b4 Soderback, I., Bengtsson, I., Ginsburg, E., & Ekholm, J. (1992). Video feedback in occupational therapy: Its effects in patients with neglect syndrome. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 73(12), 1140–1146. https://doi.org/10.5555/uri:pii:0003999392901119 Tham, K., Ginsburg, E., Fisher, A. G., & Tegner, R. (2001). Training to improve awareness of disabilities in clients with unilateral neglect. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 55(1), 46–54. https://doi.org/10.5014/ajot.55.1.46 Tham, K., & Tegner, R. (1996). The baking tray task: A test of spatial neglect. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 6(1), 19–26. https://doi.org/10.1080/713755496 Tham, K., & Tegner, R. (1997). Video feedback in the rehabilitation of patients with unilateral neglect. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 78(4), 410–413. https://doi.org/ 10.1016/s0003-9993(97)90234-3 Toglia, J. (2004). Dynamic assessment and unilateral neglect [PhD]. Columbia University, New York, NY. Dissertation Abstracts International (UMI No. 3129036). 22 J. TOGLIA AND P. CHEN Toglia, J. (2011). A dynamic interactional model of cognition in cognitive rehabilitation. In N. Katz (Ed.), Cognition, occupation, and participation across the life span: Neuroscience, neurorehabilitation, and models of intervention in occupational therapy (3rd ed, pp. 161–202). AOTA. Toglia, J., & Cermak, S. A. (2009). Dynamic assessment and prediction of learning potential in clients with unilateral neglect. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 63(5), 569–579. https://doi.org/10.5014/ajot.63.5.569 Toglia, J., Golisz, K. M., & Goverover, Y. (2019). Cognition, perception, and occupational performance. In B. A. B. Schell & G. Gillen (Eds.), Willard & Spackman’s occupational therapy (13th ed, pp. 901–941). Wolters Kluwer. Toglia, J., & Kirk, U. (2000). Understanding awareness deficits following brain injury. Neurorehabilitation, 15(1), 57–70. https://doi.org/10.3233/NRE-2000-15104 Wee, J. Y., & Hopman, W. M. (2008). Comparing consequences of right and left unilateral neglect in a stroke rehabilitation population. American Journal of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 87(11), 910–920. https://doi.org/10.1097/PHM.0b013e31818a58bd Weinberg, J., Diller, L., Gordon, W. A., Gerstman, L. J., Lieberman, A., Lakin, P., … Ezrachi, O. (1977). Visual scanning training effect on reading-related tasks in acquired right brain damage. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 58(11), 479–486. https://www. ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/931586 Wilson, B., Cockburn, J., & Halligan, P. W. (1987). Behavioural Inattention test. Thames Valley Test Company. Wilson, F. C., Manly, T., Coyle, D., & Robertson, I. H. (2000). The effect of contralesional limb activation training and sustained attention training for self-care programmes in unilateral spatial neglect. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience, 16(1), 1–4. https://content. iospress.com/articles/restorative-neurology-and-neuroscience/rnn00128 Winstein, C. J., Stein, J., Arena, R., Bates, B., Cherney, L. R., Cramer, S. C., Deruyter F., Eng J. J., Fisher B., Harvey R. L., Lang C. E., MacKay-Lyons M., Ottenbacher K. J., Pugh S., Reeves M. J., Richards L. G., Stiers W., Zorowitz R. D., & Outcomes, R. (2016). Guidelines for adult stroke rehabilitation and recovery: A guideline for healthcare professionals from the American Heart Association/American stroke Association. Stroke, 47(6), e98–e169. https:// doi.org/10.1161/STR.0000000000000098