The Relationship Between Emotional Intelligence and Psychological Well-being among Employees in the Caregiving Profession HONOURS RESEARCH PROPOSAL Faculty of Economic and Management Sciences (Department of Industrial Psychology) at the University of the Free State Supervisor: Dr. Harunavamwe April 2017 NO PART OF THIS DOCUMENT MAY BE REPLICATED WITHOUT PRIOR PERMISSION FROM UFS DEPARTMENT OF INDUSTRAIL PSYCHOLOGY. TABLE OF CONTENTS CHAPTER 1: GENERAL INTRODUCTION 1.1 Introduction and Problem Statement 1 1.2 Research Questions 3 1.3 Research Objectives 3 1.4 Research Hypotheses 3 1.5 Outline of the Study 4 1.6 Summary 5 CHAPTER 2: PSYCHOLOGICAL WELL-BEING 6 2.1 Introduction 6 2.2 Nature and Definition of Psychological Well-being 6 2.3 Approaches to Psychological Well-being 7 2.3.1 The Hedonic Approach 7 2.3.2 The Eudemonic Approach 7 2.4 Components of Psychological Well-being 8 2.4.1 Cognitive Components 8 2.4.1.1 Life Satisfaction 8 2.4.1.2 Domain Satisfaction 8 2.4.2 Affective Components 9 2.4.2.1 Positive Affect 9 2.4.2.2 Negative Affect 9 2.5 The Dynamic Equilibrium Theory 9 2.6 Ryff’s Multidimensional Model of Psychological Well-being 11 2.6.1 Self-Acceptance 12 2.6.2 Positive Relations with Others 2.6.3 Autonomy 12 12 2.6.4 Environmental Mastery 12 2.6.5 Purpose of Life 13 2.6.6 Personal Growth 13 2.7 Summary 13 CHAPTER 3: EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE 14 3.1 Introduction 14 3.2 Nature and Definition of Emotional Intelligence 14 3.3 The Components of Emotional Intelligence 15 3.4 Models of Emotional Intelligence 16 3.4.1 Bar-On’s Model of Emotional Intelligence 16 3.4.2 The Ability Model of Emotional Intelligence 17 3.5 Summary 18 CHAPTER 4: RESEARCH METHODOLOGY 19 4.1 Introduction 19 4.2 Selection of test persons 19 4.3 Data Gathering 19 4.3.1 Ryff’s Psychological Well-being Scale 19 4.3.1.1 Nature and Composition 20 4.3.1.2 Reliability 20 4.3.1.3 Validity 20 4.3.1.4 Rationale for inclusion 21 4.3.2 Mayer, Salovey and Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test 21 4.3.2.1 Nature and Composition 21 4.3.1.2 Reliability 21 4.3.1.3 Validity 21 4.3.1.4 Rationale for inclusion 22 4.4 Statistical Methods 22 4.4.1 Descriptive statistics 22 4.4.2 Inferential statistics 23 4.4.2.1 Pearson product-moment correlation 23 4.4.2.1.1 Reasons for inclusion... 23 4.4.2.1.2 Discussion of the Pearson product-moment correlation 23 4.4.2.2 T-test for independent groups / One-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) 24 4.4.2.2.1 Reasons for inclusion of the T-test for independent groups 25 4.4.2.2.2 Discussion of the T-test for independent groups 25 4.5 Summary 26 Bibliography 27 LIST OF FIGURES Figure 2.1: Heady and Wearing’s (1991) stocks and flows framework 10 Figure 2.2: Ryff’s Model of Psychological Well-being 11 Figure 4.1: Pearson’s Product Moment Correlation (Definitional Formula) 24 Figure 4.2: Pearson’s Product Moment Correlation (Calculation Formula) 24 CHAPTER 1 GENERAL INTRODUCTION 1.1 Introduction and Problem Statement Caregiving is a stressful and emotionally draining profession and puts individuals employed in such lines of work under considerable amounts of stress. Although stress may help an individual become more focused, chronic and excessive stress has detrimental effects, such as feeling pressured and being overwhelmed (Moss, Good, Gozal, Kleinpell & Sessler, 2016). Psychological stress develops when individual’s external demands exceed their adaptive abilities (Moss et al., 2016). Caregiving, especially nursing, is considered to be a stressful profession because it requires coping with high emotional, cognitive and physical demands (Donoso, Demerouti, Hernández, Moreno-Jiménez & Cobo, 2015). This continued exposure to these demands can be associated with physical and psychological problems (Donoso et al., 2015), which negatively impact a person’s psychological and overall well-being. Internationally, the burden of caregiving is felt heaviest by individuals in the nursing profession. Por, Barriball, Fitzpatrick and Roberts (2011) hold that in the UK, an estimate of up to 25% of nursing students drop out of their preparation programme because of the high demands of the occupation. Maslach and Leiter (2014), provided supporting research showcasing that work-stress may predict psychological disability in initially healthy employees. Additionally, Carod-Artal and Vázquez-Cabrera (2013) indicated that the demands of caregiving have been associated with decreases in well-being, quality of care among nursing staff and high absenteeism. The effects of globalisation on developing countries worsen the situation, as internationally recognised quality of care becomes the bar of measurement. This puts considerable amounts of stress on caregivers as they attempt to meet these requirements with the little they have (CarodArtal & Vázquez-Cabrera, 2013). Research conducted in Nigeria on physiotherapists, Ibikinle, Umeadi and Akosle (as cited in Okwaraji & Aguwa, 2014), reported that 66.2% of subjects exhibited emotional exhaustion, 65.2% showed high levels of depersonalisation and 75.6% displayed high levels of reduced personal accomplishment. In Malawi, maternal staff reported high rates of burnout with close to three quarters of subjects (72%) reporting emotional exhaustion, 43% reporting depersonalisation and 74% experienced reduced personal accomplishment (Carod-Artal & Vázquez-Cabrera, 2013). Research seems to find a prevalence of negative attitudes and low psychological and physical well-being among caregivers with 1 many expressing strong feelings to leave the caregiving profession (nursing) (Okwaraji & Aguwa, 2014). Within the South African context, a cross-sectional study conducted in a public hospital reported that physicians had higher levels of occupational stress compared to the average working population (Carod-Artal & Vázquez-Cabrera, 2013). Further reports indicate that only 25% of registered nurses working in critical care units are qualified as critical care nurses (Nagel, Towell, Nel and Foxall, 2016). This staff shortage leads to the employment of newly graduated and inexperienced registered nurses who have not yet developed appropriate knowledge and competence, or the emotional preparation needed for such a highly charged area of practice. This lack of emotional preparedness can lead to stress, depression and regression in psychological well-being. Nagel et al., (2016) revealed that when faced with demands that threaten their well-being, caregivers in the health services leave their working environments, or the profession altogether. The solution to this problem is twofold. Firstly, it is essential that nurses continue to be recruited to critical care units, but it is also imperative that experienced critical care nurses be retained, as their experience is invaluable in such challenging environments (Nagel et al., 2016). Secondly, Nagel et al. (2016) draw from De Beer, Brysiewizz and Bhengu’s (2011) research and posit that in order to manage the demands of such taxing environments, caregivers need a broad but specialist knowledge base and sound decision-making skills to function appropriately. These skills, which can be summed up as emotional intelligence, can reduce the effects of stress, burnout and psychological distress, improving feelings of ownership towards work. Growing interest and research into how people can manage the complex demands of their work place has led to the identification and management of emotions. Today, researchers such as Por et al. (2011) believe that the ability to process emotionally relevant information can have an effect on an individual’s life outcomes such as achieving success at work and their general well-being. This ability, termed emotional intelligence (EI), has been found to foster adaptive methods of coping with social challenges, social stress and interpersonal conflicts; promoting the development of supportive social networks; decreasing negative and increasing positive emotions; and enhancing emotional regulation (Di Fabio & Kenny, 2016). EI involves a set of emotional abilities to effectively use information from emotions, allowing people to have adaptive coping with stressful life events (Sánchez-Álvarez, Extremera & Fernández-Berrocal, 2 2016), therefore, EI is conceived of as an indicator of psychological adjustment and a key precursor to feelings associated with well-being. 1.2 Research Questions From the preceding problem statement, the following research questions can be identified: • Primary Question: Does a relationship exist between emotional intelligence and well-being among employees in the caregiving profession? • Secondary Question: Do differences exist in well-being among employees in the caregiving professions with regards to gender? 1.3 Research Objectives The following research objectives can be identified from the aforementioned research questions: • Primary Objective To determine by means of a non-experimental research design whether a relationship exists between emotional intelligence and well-being among employees working in the caregiving profession. • Secondary Objective To determine by means of a non-experimental research design whether differences exist between males and females working in the caregiving profession. 1.4 Research Hypothesis Resulting from the previous research objectives, the following hypotheses will be investigated in this study: • Hypothesis 1 Null Hypothesis (Ho) 3 There is no statistically significant relationship between scores achieved on emotional intelligence and well-being among employees working in the caregiving profession. Alternative Hypothesis (H1) There is no statistically significant relationship between scores achieved on emotional intelligence and well-being among employees working in the caregiving profession. • Hypothesis 2 Null Hypothesis (Ho) There is no statistically significant differences with regards to gender between scores achieved on well-being among employees working in the caregiving profession. Alternative Hypothesis (H1) There is a statistically significant difference with regards to gender between the scores achieved on well-being among employees working in the caregiving profession. 1.5 Outline of the Study Chapter 1 This chapter encapsulates the general introduction as well as the problem statement that will be the focus of the study. The research questions, -objectives and hypotheses are outlined as well. Chapter 2 The first variable, emotional intelligence, is focused on, with the provision of several definitions as well as the conceptualisation of emotional intelligence for the purposes of this study. Emotional intelligence is then linked to the caregiving profession, and further discusses. 4 Chapter 3 The second variable, well-being, is discussed and conceptually defined. Following, is how well-being is linked to the caregiving profession as well as the relationship between emotional intelligence and well-being. Chapter 4 Chapter 4 concerns itself with the research methodology. The methods that were used to conduct the research such as the selection of research participants, data gathering and the statistical methods employed are discussed in detail. 1.6 Summary A general introduction along with an outline of the problem statement was provided. Explicit focus was placed on the health of individuals employed in the caregiving profession, and how their work exposes them to high physical and mental demands. Furthermore, focus was also placed on the difficulty of finding a balance and maintaining that balance of well-being due to the taxing profession of caregiving. The effects of emotional intelligence were briefly outlined as a means of regulating the stresses of caregiving and achieving a greater sense of well-being and health. The specific research questions, -objectives, and –hypotheses were specified. In conclusion, the basic outline the study would follow was also included. Following in sequence will be a focus on relevant literature, which will provide an overview of the variables: emotional intelligence and well-being. 5 CHAPTER 2 PSYCHOLOGICAL WELL-BEING 2.1 Introduction Seifert (2015) holds that well-being is a dynamic concept that includes subjective, social and psychological dimensions, as well as health related behaviours. However, the question of how well-being should be defined, let alone spelt (wellbeing or well-being) still remains largely unresolved (Dodge, Daly, Huyton & Sanders, 2012), and has given rise to blurred and overly broad definitions of well-being. One thing is sure though, amidst all the debates and attempts to define the concept, there exists general consensus that it involves a state of being comfortable, healthy or happy as well as the partial absence of illness (Rathnakana, 2014). The elusiveness of defining well-being comes about because of the complex and multi-faceted nature of the construct (Pollard & Lee, 2003), and as Thomas (2009) argued, the intangibility faced when attempting to measure it (as cited in Dodge et al., 2012). Bradburn (1969) made one of the earliest attempts in defining well-being by shifting his focus towards the psychological reactions of ordinary people in their daily lives (Dodge et al., 2012). Bradburn’s interest was on how individuals coped with their daily difficulties, and highlighted how psychological well-being, which he referred to as happiness, was the variable that stands out as being of primary importance (Dodge et al., 2012). From this backdrop, today, psychological well-being is a tradition centred on personal growth, on the way to manage life challenges, and on the effort to achieve goals (Montes-Berges & Augusto-Landa, 2014). 2.2 Nature and Definition of Psychological Well-being According to Dolan, Layard and Metcalfe (as cited in Dodge et al., 2012), the nature of psychological well-being is multi-faceted, however, it is generally agreed that three aspects can be distinguished; • Evaluative well-being, involving global assessments of how people evaluate their lives, or their satisfaction with life. • Affective or hedonic well-being, involving measures of feelings such as happiness, sadness and enjoyment. • Eudemonic well-being, which focuses on judgements about the meaning or purpose of one’s life and appraisals of constructs such as fulfilment, autonomy and control. 6 From the aforementioned aspects, it becomes clear that the hedonic and eudemonic well-being constructs seep into each other, and that complete separation of the two is an impossibility. Due to this, defining psychological well-being, becomes ever more difficult, as researcher’s attempts end up focused more on the dimensions or descriptions of psychological well-being rather than on the definition (Dodge et al., 2012). Nonetheless, this has not deterred researchers from trying with Brown (1992) defining psychological well-being as “well-being that could be related to the quality of life, however defined, or to life satisfaction, however defined” (as cited in Fatemi & Asghari, 2016: 189). Brown’s (1992) definition is too broad, and provides no basis of conceptualisation and measurement. Therefore, Chen, Jing, Hayes and Lee’s (2012) definition seems more practical for the purposes of this study. Chen et al., (2012) hold that psychological well-being involves perceived thriving in life in the face of existing challenges of life, such as pursuing meaningful goals, growing and developing as a person, and establishing quality ties with others. This definition works well as it can be aligned with Ryff’s (1989) dimensions and model of psychological well-being which will be covered later in the study. 2.3 Approaches to Well-being Two broad traditions have historically been employed to explore well-being, the hedonic and eudemonic traditions. 2.3.1 The Hedonic Tradition The hedonic tradition can be traced back to Aristippus, a Greek philosopher who believed that the goal of life is to experience maximum pleasure (Boniwell, 2016). This view equates wellbeing with happiness and is often operationalised as the balance between positive and negative affect (Irshad, 2015). People virtually always experience moods and emotions, which have a hedonic component that is pleasant, signalling a positive reaction, and if unpleasant, signalling a negative reaction (Dodge et al., 2012). Kehneman (2006) expanded on this by mentioning that positive affects are not simply the opposite of negative affects, but rather, both should be seen as carrying valuable information. The umbrella term used to denote hedonic measures is subjective well-being (Samman, 2007). 2.3.2 The Eudemonic Tradition The eudemonic perspective to a good life originated from Aristotle, the originator of eudemonia (from daimon – meaning true nature) (Boniwell, 2016). Aristotle deemed happiness 7 to be a vulgar idea, stressing that not all desires are worth pursuing as, even though some of them may yield pleasure, they would not necessarily produce happiness (Boniwell, 2016). Aristotle, as Boniwell (2016) holds, believed true happiness is found by leading a virtuous life and doing what is worth doing, and that realising human potential is the ultimate human goal. This view was further developed throughout the centuries, with Maslow and Rogers being the first 20th century eudaemonists. This perspective then, links itself well with humanistic psychology and highlights positive psychological functioning and human development (Dodge et al., 2012). The eudemonic tradition assesses how well people are living with their true selves, emphasizing personal growth, mastery, life purpose and meaning (Di Fabio & Kenny, 2016). Di Fabio and Kenny (2016) add that the eudemonic viewpoint holds that psychological wellbeing is derived from a sense of fulfilment, meaning and self-realisation. 2.4 Components of Psychological Well-being Psychological well-being as posited by Diener (as cited in Irshad, 2005) has two distinct components; cognitive and affective components. However, simplified emotional reactions such as anger, pride or joy (affective components) usually also involve cognitive appraisals and interpretations, thus Hansen’s (2012) assertion of unclear distinctions holds true. 2.4.1 Cognitive Components Cognitive components have their basis on evaluative beliefs or attitudes about one’s life (Irshad, 2015). Three aspects of cognitive well-being are reported being life satisfaction, partnership satisfaction as well as domain satisfaction (Hansen, 2012). However, this study will consider partnership satisfaction as part of the life satisfaction aspect. 2.4.1.1 Life Satisfaction Life satisfaction, as Hansen (2012) explains, refers respectively to the overall assessment of one’s quality of life and relationship(s). Better understood, Rojas (2014) explains that a person’s evaluation of their own life involves a judgemental process. Hence life satisfaction is an evaluation of a happy life, based on the individual’s judgement of their own life. 2.4.1.2 Domain Satisfaction This component states that life can be approached as a general construct of many specific domains (Rojas, 2006), and that satisfaction in the domains of life ultimately determine life satisfaction. Consequently, a relationship between life satisfaction and domain satisfaction can 8 be assumed. In his earlier work, Rojas (2004) expanded that the domain satisfaction approach attempts to understand a general appraisal of life as a whole, on the basis of a multidimensional component of specific appraisals going from a small number to an almost infinite recount of all imaginable human activities and spheres of being. 2.4.2 Affective Components Irshad (2015) maintains that affective components reflect the amount of peasant and unpleasant feelings that people experience in their lives. It is the frequency and intensity of positive and negative emotions and mood (Luhmann, Hawkley, Eid & Cacoppo, 2013). 2.4.2.1 Positive Affect Positive affect, as Tran (2015) explains, refers to a trait that describes individual differences in positive emotional experience. These emotions or feelings reflect a level of pleasurable engagement with the environment, and can be brief, longer lasting or more stable trait-like feelings (Cohen & Pressman, 2006). It is easy to confuse positive affect with positive emotions, thus Miller (2011) makes the distinction that although positive affect overlaps significantly with positive emotions, they are not identical. Positive affect is more closely related to mood states, whereas positive emotions involve positive feelings as well as characteristic patterns of psychological arousal, thoughts and behaviours (Miller, 2011). 2.4.2.2 Negative Affect Since positive and negative ae opposites on a continuum, one would be justified in thinking that the lack of positive engagement leads to negative affect, however, this is not necessarily true (Cohen & Pressman, 2006). Negative affect as defined by Stringer (2013) is feelings of “emotional distress” more specifically, the common variance between anxiety, sadness, fear, anger, guilt and shame, irritability and other unpleasant emotions. Carey (1988) explains elegantly that negative affect is a broad and pervasive predisposition to experience negative emotions that has further influences on cognition, self-concept and worldview. 2.5 The Dynamic Equilibrium Theory Originally proposed by Headey and Wearing in 1989, the dynamic equilibrium theory, also known as the set-point theory, suggests links between personality, life events, well-being and ill-being (Dodge et al., 2012). The preposition made is that individuals have unique baseline levels of well-being that are determined by their personality (Irshad, 2015). Furthermore, 9 Headey and Wearing (1992) as cited in Irshad (2015), argued that individuals with certain personalities are likely to experience certain types of events, and these events influence ones’ average level of well-being. Unusual events, as Irshad (2015) explains, can move a person above or below this baseline level, however, the individual will eventually return to the baseline, resulting in the normalisation of life events. This assertion mirrors the work of Brickman and Campbell (1971) who built the body of theory from which the equilibrium theory draws heavily upon. Headey and Wearing proposed a model for equilibrium theory which is aimed at understanding how people cope with change, and how their levels of well-being are affected as a result (Dodge et al., 2012). The assumption then, is that change in well-being occurs only when a person deviates from their equilibrium pattern of events, due to external forces (Irshad, 2015). Consequently, Headey and Wearing propose a definition of well-being in which well-being is shown “as depending on prior equilibrium levels of well-being and of life events, and also on recent events” (Dodge et al., 2011: 227). This reflects their framework for analysing subjective well-being which considers the relationship between stocks and flows: Figure 2.1: Heady and Wearing’s (1991) stocks and flows framework Using the model, Headey and Wearing proposed that differences between individuals in terms of subjective well-being are due to “stable stocks” (stable personal characteristics), and because of these stable stocks, each person has a level of subjective well-being which represents their own “normal” equilibrium level (Dodge et al., 2012). Stocks, as Dodge et al., (2012) explain, are used to deal with specific life experiences (flows) so that satisfaction is enhanced and 10 distress diminished. Consequently, the model becomes more appropriate in regarding subjective well-being as a fluctuating state rather than a stable trait. Though detailed and easily comprehensible, the theory and model fall short in that they treat subjective and psychological well-being as identical and interchangeable constructs. 2.6 Ryff’s Multidimensional Model of Psychological Well-being Ryff (1989) analysed various approaches to happiness in different subfields of psychology (Boniwell, 2016) and came to the conclusion that research on well-being up to that time, largely translated only into happiness (Henriques, 2014). For Ryff, well-being was more lined with optimal psychological functionality rather than happiness, and this lead to her development of a multidimensional model of well-being. Her model of psychological well-being includes six related yet distinct components, and rests on the assumption that individuals strive to function fully and realise their unique talents (Chen et al., 2012). Personal Growth Purpose in Life Positive Relationships Self-Acceptance Psychological Well-Being Autonomy Environmental Mastery Figure 2.2: Ryff’s Model of Psychological Well-being 11 2.6.1 Self – Acceptance Self-acceptance refers to the degree to which one has a positive attitude towards oneself and one’s past life, past behaviours and choices (Irshad, 2015). Self-acceptance is the most recurrent criterion of well-being, and previous perspectives emphasise it as well. It is a prerequisite for mental health, as well as characteristics of self-actualisation, optimal functioning and maturity (Henriques, 2014). Seifert (2005) maintains that high scorers possess a positive attitude toward the self; acknowledge and accept multiple aspects of self, including good and bad qualities and feel positive about past life. While low scorers feel dissatisfied with their self; are disappointed with what has occurred with past life and wish to be different than what who they are. 2.6.2 Positive Relations with Others Many theories stress the importance of warm, trusting interpersonal relations. The ability to love is viewed as a central component of psychological well-being (Henriques, 2014). Irshad (2015) holds that people with high quality, satisfying relationships with others are concerned about the welfare of others, have a capacity for strong empathy and intimacy. On the other hand, people scoring low on this dimension have few close, trusting relationships with others, are isolated and frustrated in interpersonal relationships and unwilling to make compromises to sustain important ties with others. 2.6.3 Autonomy There is considerable emphasis on such qualities such as self-determination, independence and regulation of behaviour from within (Seifert, 2005). Self-actualisers, according to Ryff (as cited in Irshad, 2015) show resistance to enculturation, have an internal locus of control and evaluate the self with personal standards, as opposed to the individual who relies on others judgements to make decisions, conforming to social pressures and is concerned with others expectations and evaluations. 2.6.4 Environmental Mastery This is the degree to which one feels competent to meet the demands of their situation (Henriques, 2014), and their ability to manage life, choosing environments suitable to their psychic conditions (Ishrad, 2005). Individuals scoring high exhibit a control over a complex array of external activities, and are able to choose or create contexts suitable to personal needs 12 and values. However, individuals who struggle to master their environments have difficulty managing everyday affairs and remains unaware of surrounding opportunities. 2.6.5 Purpose of Life This domain deals with having life gaols and a belief that one’s life is meaningful. Furthermore, Henriques (2014) adds that a clear comprehension of life’s purpose, a sense of directedness and intentionality form part of the criteria of maturity. Thus one who functions positively has goals, intentions, and a sense of direction, unlike a low scorer who experiences a lack of sense of direction, sees no purpose to their past, and holds no sense of purpose in life (Ryff, as cited in Seifert, 2005). 2.6.6 Personal Growth The final facet of Ryff’s model is the continuation of developing one’s potential to grow as a person and being open to new experiences (Irshad, 2005). While low scorers experience a sense of personal stagnation, boredom with life and feelings of inability to develop new attitudes and behaviours, the self-actualiser sees the self as growing and expanding, is open to new experiences and changes in ways which reflect more self-knowledge and effectiveness (Henriques, 2014). 2.7 Summary This section focused on providing a comprehensive definition of psychological well-being and outlined some issues faced when trying to define psychological well-being. Two traditions of well-being were outlined as well as how these traditions intersect and aren’t truly separate, but rather intertwined with each other. Furthermore, the components of psychological well-being were briefly discussed, along with the dynamic equilibrium theory. Lastly, Ryff’s multidimensional theory and model of psychological well-being, was touched upon. 13 CHAPTER 3 EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE 3.1 Introduction Emotional intelligence is a fairly new concept that came from the acknowledgement that social and emotional competencies are an important part of both performance effectiveness, as well as measures of personal and professional success (Codier & Odell, 2014). The concept of emotional intelligence, dubbed by Salovey and Mayer (1990), was initially described as being a group of interconnected abilities that aid the processing of information regarding emotions by acting as a guide toward cognition and behaviour (Mayer, Salovey & Caruso, 2008 as cited by Beauvais, Stewart & DeNisco, 2014). The concept of emotional intelligence arose from Thorndike in his theory of ‘social intelligence’ in which he defines social intelligence as – the ability to understand and manage men and women, boys and girls – to act wisely in human relations (Thorndike (1920) as cited in Shaheen and Shaheen (2016)). The concept of emotional intelligence was then carried out by Howard Gardner in his theory of Multiple intelligences, wherein he identifies interpersonal intelligence and intrapersonal intelligence as two of the nine primary forms of intelligence (Codlier and Odell, 2014). The research of Drs. Reuven Bar-On, John Mayer, and Peter Salovey provide much of the theoretical foundation for the concept (Codier and Odell, 2014). Emotional intelligence went on to gain the public’s attention and was popularised in the mid – 90’s when Dr. Dan Goleman published his best seller list book ‘’Emotional Intelligence’’, in which he referred to emotional intelligence as a mixed model that includes social behaviours, traits, and competencies (Beauvais, et al, 2014). 3.2 Nature and Definition Emotional intelligence can be seen as an eye-catching term, but it is also controversial and highly debatable. There is no consensus regarding its single definition (Irshad, 2015). Irshad (2015) is of the opinion that emotional intelligence helps individuals in achieving the quality of life that will make them successful and content by means of guiding the actions of individuals. According to Meyer et al. (2008) as cited in Karimi, Leggat, Donohue, Farrell and Couper (2013); Emotional intelligence refers to the ability to identify, assess, manage and control oneself and one’s reactions to the emotions of others. Wolff (2005) as cited in Ackley (2016), states that the Goleman model defines emotional intelligence as the ability to recognise 14 one’s own feelings and the feelings of others, such that emotionally intelligent individuals are able to motivate themselves, and appropriately deal with their own emotions as well as the emotions of others. Salovey and Mayer, (1990) as cited in Irshad (2015), define emotional intelligence as being a form of intelligence that involves the ability to monitor one’s own and others’ feelings and emotions, to distinguish among them and to use this information to guide one’s thinking and actions. Mayer and Salovey (1997) as cited in Shaheen and Shaheen (2016) further refined this definition in terms of four factors which are perceiving, using, understanding and managing emotions. As they define “the ability to perceive accurately, appraise and express emotion, the ability to access and/or generate feelings when they facilitate thought; the ability to understand emotion and emotional knowledge; and the ability to regulate emotions to promote emotional and intellectual growth” (Mayer and Salovey (1997) as cited in Shaheen and Shaheen (2016)). For the purpose of this study mention will be given to the definition by Mayer, Salovey and Caruso (2004) as cited in Sharma, Dhar & Tyagi (2015) which defines emotional intelligence as, ‘’the capacity to reason about emotions, and of emotions to enhance thinking. It includes the abilities to accurately perceive emotions, to access and generate emotions so as to assist thought, to understand emotions and emotional knowledge, and to reflectively regulate emotions so as to promote emotional and intellectual growth’’. The theoretical basis of the current study is emotional intelligence and psychological wellbeing; the aforementioned definition will lend itself to the provision of a better understanding of the inter-relationship shared by the two constructs moderated by emotional intelligence. Thus allowing the study to explore emotional intelligence as a solution to psychological wellbeing. 3.3 The Components of Emotional Intelligence People who are emotionally intelligent perceive themselves to be confident and are better able to understand, control and manage their emotions (Wilson, 2014). As such, Heffernan et al (2010) as cited in Wilson (2014), identifies four components or determinants of emotional intelligence: 1. Wellbeing – the degree to which one accurately perceives emotions, their level of selfesteem being it high or low, and their positive or negative outlook on life. 2. Self-control – one’s ability to access and generate emotions to assist thought in order to regulate and control one’s own emotions and how well one deals with stress. 15 3. Emotionality – possessing emotional knowledge, understanding emotions and being able to show empathy, and communicate one’s feelings. 4. Sociability – referring to one’s ability to reflectively regulate emotions in order to stimulate emotional and intellectual skills and to better social skills and assertiveness in dealing with others. 3.4 Models of Emotional Intelligence Several models of emotional intelligence are currently used widely in organisational, psychological, and educational research; and are all used to conceptualize, define, operationalize and measure emotional intelligence differently (Codier and Odell, 2014). For instance, Mayer et al, (2008) as cited in Codier and Odell (2014) give mention to Drs. Bar-On and, Mayer and Salovey, who had different models of emotional intelligence. 3.4.1. Bar-On’s Model of Emotional Intelligence Dr. Reuven Bar-On based his model on his own rigorous research wherein he views the factors he identified as skills that can be learned and improved (Ackley, 2016). According to Bar-On’s model as defined by Ackley (p274, 2016), emotional intelligence comprises of the follow five components: 1. Self-perception which consist of self-regard, self-actualization, and emotional selfawareness. 2. Self-expression is comprised of emotional expression, assertiveness, and independence. 3. Interpersonal is comprised of interpersonal relationships, empathy, and social responsibility. 4. Decision making is comprised of problem solving, reality testing, and impulse control. 5. Stress management is comprised of flexibility, stress tolerance, and optimism. In the application of this model Dr. Bar-On made use of a self-report personality test by developing the concept of emotional intelligence as a personality construct which he measured using the Emotional Quotient Inventory (EQ-I). The Emotional Quotient Inventory is a selfreport test consisting of 133 questions on a 5-point Likert scale, that evaluates emotional and social functioning. The instrument is used to decipher emotional intelligence, personal development, and emotional and social competencies by specifically measuring intrapersonal, interpersonal, stress management, adaptability, and general mood skills (Statistic Solutions, 2016). 16 3.4.2. The Ability Model of Emotional Intelligence Drs. John Mayer and Peter Salovey on the other hand established a model of emotional intelligence that describes emotional intelligence as a set of abilities that can be measured using a performance instrument (Mayer et al (2008) as cited in Cordier and Odell, 2014). Ackley (2016) explains that Salovey and Mayer see emotional intelligence as being a form of inherent intelligence, a largely inborn set of abilities that impact the ways in which people manage their own emotions and understand and influence emotions in others. Salovey and Mayer identify four branches within their overall concept of emotional intelligence (Mayer, Salovey, & Caruso, 2002 as cited in Ackley, 2016). The following branches describe how people recognize and manage their own emotions and how people may try to influence the emotions of others: 1. The perceiving emotions branch involves the ability to identify emotions within the self, others, and as represented by objects, such as pictures, as well as the ability to express emotions accurately. 2. The emotional facilitation of thought branch involves the use of emotions to prioritize thought and utilizes feelings as aids to judgment. Changes in mood lead to changes in perspective. 3. The understanding and analysing emotions branch involves the accurate labelling of emotions, understanding emotions and relationships, understanding complex feelings, and understanding transitions between emotions. 4. The reflective regulation of emotion branch involves the ability to stay open to feelings, reflectively engaging and detaching from feelings as appropriate, and managing emotions in oneself and attempting to influence them in others. According to Codier and Odell (2014), the above mentioned models differ widely in their definition of the nature of emotional intelligence, the type of measurement they utilize (selfreport vs performance criteria) and the validity and reliability of the instruments used to measure them. For the purpose of this study the ability model of emotional intelligence will be used. This study has distinguished emotional intelligence as consisting of four components; these being well-being (the ability to identify emotions in self and others), self-control (the ability to use emotions to reason), emotionality (the ability to understand emotions) and sociability (the ability to manage emotions in self and in emotional situations). 17 According to the Universal Class (2017), the ability model supports the above definition by recognising that emotional intelligence includes four distinct types of ability: 1. Emotional perception. The ability to identify emotions in self and others Through facial expression, body language, pictures, voices, and so on, a person can identify the emotions of others. 2. Use of emotion. The ability to use emotions to reason. The second activity which proposed by the Ability Model relates to a person's ability to use emotions -whether it is their own emotions or another person's emotions -- in order to achieve a desired outcome or to reason. 3. Understanding emotions. The ability to understand emotions. This ability is built upon an understanding of the complexity of emotions. 4. Managing emotions. The ability to manage emotions in self and in emotional situations. Someone's ability (or lack thereof) to regulate emotions in both themselves and others. The instrument most commonly used to measure ability emotional intelligence according to Codier and Odell (2014), is the Mayer, Salovey and Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test, version 2 (MSCEITv2), an instrument with rigorously established validity and reliability. Based on the belief that emotional intelligence is an inherent intelligence or ability, the MSCEIT attempts to measure one’s capacity for learning emotional intelligence competencies (Ackley, 2016). The MSCEITv2 measures emotional intelligence ability by evaluating the performance of emotional tasks, and has been used widely in the general emotional intelligence research literature as well as in numerous nursing emotional intelligence research studies (Mayer et al, 2008, as cited in Codier and Odell, 2014). 3.5 Summary Provided in this chapter were various definitions of emotional intelligence. The one which will be used for the purpose of this study is the definition by Mayer, Salovey and Caruso (2004). The four components of emotional intelligence which are; well-being, self-control, emotionality and sociability were discussed. Finally, different models of emotional intelligence were provided with specific focus on the ability model of emotional intelligence. 18 Chapter 4 Research Methodology 4.1 Introduction This chapter will outline the research methodology that will be used for the purpose of this study. The framework of this chapter will comprise; the selection of respondents and a discussion of the data gathering methods that will be used. Lastly, a description of the statistical methods which will be used in data analysis for testing the research hypotheses, will be provided. 4.2 Sample Selection The data for this study will be collected from a sample of 81 registered nurses drawn from the population of at least 270 Post-basic nursing students enrolled at the University of the Free States’ School of Nursing (Idalia Loots), in Bloemfontein. This sampling frame is in accordance with Sekaran’s rule of thumb for determining sample size, wherein, he states that the minimum size of the sample should be 30% of the population (Slideshare, 2014). A selfreport questionnaire will be used to capture demographic characteristics (age, gender); and nursing related details (location of work: rural/metropolitan, years of experience in nursing, qualification etc.). This study will make use of probability sampling where all elements in the population have an equal chance of being chosen for the sample. The simple random method of sampling will be used. This entails assigning numbers per element, distributing these numbers on a table and then selecting the numbers at random. 4.3 Data Gathering In order to measure psychological well-being, Ryff’s Psychological Well-being Scale (RPWBS) will be utilised, while the Mayer, Salovey and Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCEITv2) will assist with the assessment of emotional intelligence trait in the subjects. 4.3.1 Ryff’s Psychological Well-being Scale This section focuses on the dimensions of Ryff’s Psychological Well-being Scale (RPWBS), paying particular attention to its nature and composition, reliability, validity and the rationale for inclusion. 19 4.3.1.1 Nature and Composition The RPWBS was developed by Carol Ryff, who believed that pervious perspectives on operationalising well-being were a theoretical (independent of theory) and decentralised (Springer & Hauser, 2006). To address this shortcoming, Ryff (1989) suggested a multidimensional model of psychological well-being, compromised of six psychological dimensions (Lee & Taniguchi, 2014); self-acceptance, positive relations with others, autonomy, environmental mastery, purpose of life and personal growth (Kállay & Rus, 2014), it is these dimensions that the scale measures. 4.3.1.2 Reliability A variety of versions of the RPWBS ranging from short-item scales to long and extensive ones are available (Lee & Taniguchi, 2014). While the scale is highly acclaimed, many a researcher, Kafka and Kozma (2002); Van Dierendonck (2004), have questioned the reliability of the scale (Springer & Hauser, 2006). However, Akin (2008) refers back to Ryff’s own attempts in assessing the reliability of her scale, reporting internal consistency reliability coefficients of: .80 for autonomy, .90 for environmental mastery, .87 for personal growth, .91 for positive relations with others, .90 for purpose in life, and .93 for self-acceptance, through test-retest. Despite this doubt, researchers (Villar, Triadó & Celdrán, 2010; Akin, 2008; and Chen & Chan, 2005) have reported Cronbach alphas along the same ranges with only slight variation, largely attributable to demographic characteristics (Kálly & Rus, 2014). According to Burns and Machin (2009), the use of the larger 84- and 54-item scales is an improvement on previous validation and reliability studies that have used the shorter scales. 4.3.1.3 Validity While some studies (Kitamura et al., 2004; Fernandes, Vasconcelos-Raposo & Teixeira, 2010) have shown an acceptable level of factorial validity, others such as Kim, Kim, Cha and Lim (2007) (as cited in Lee & Tanguchi, 2014) showed lower validity. However, it should be noted that this difference is due to demographical characteristics, and that evidence on factorial validity of the instrument was provided by empirical studies that used exploratory factor analysis (EFA) and confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) (Kállay & Rus, 2014). Overall, the scale is limited and as Springer and Hauser (2006) hold, “it would appear that the structure of PWB is limited to face validity” (p. 9). 20 4.3.1.4 Rationale for inclusion Satisfactory internal consistencies ranging from .86 to .93 have been reported for the RPWBS. This internal consistency, coupled with the lack of scale application in the South African context, are sufficient reason to conduct the study using this particular measuring instrument. 4.3.2 Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCEIT v2.0) This section focuses on the dimensions of Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCEIT v2.0), paying particular attention to its nature and composition, reliability, validity and the rationale for inclusion. 4.3.2.1 Nature and Composition The MSCEIT V2.0 is a newly developed, 141-item scale designed to measure the following four branches (specific skills) of EI: (a) perceiving emotions, (b) using emotions to facilitate thought, (c) understanding emotions, and (d) managing emotions. Each of the four branches is measured with two tasks. Perceiving emotions is measured with the faces and pictures tasks; facilitating thought is measured with the sensations and facilitation tasks; understanding emotions is measured with blends and changes tasks; and managing emotions is measured with emotion management and emotional relationships tasks (Mayer. J, Salovey. P, Caruso. R, and Sitarenios. G, 2003). 4.3.2.2 Reliability Codier. E, and Odell. E (2013) confirm that the MSCEIT v2.0 has undergone rigorous reliability and validity testing and that significant evidence exists that it has a factor structure reflective of the ability emotional intelligence model. The split-half reliability coefficient for the total emotional intelligence score is reported to be 0.94. The split half reliability for the total score is 0.94. Reliability scores for the sub-scores range from 0.65 to 0.78. Test–retest reliability for the MSCEIT is r − .86, p < .001 ( Brackett et al., 2006; Brackett and Mayer, 2003 ; Mayer et al., 2003 as cited in Codier. E & Odell. E, 2013). 4.3.2.3 Validity Validity evidence is presented that pertains to the MSCEIT's test content, its factorial structure, its convergence and divergence with other measures, and its predictions of important life criteria (Mayer. J, Salovey. P, & Caruso. D, 2017). Additionally, Mayer et al (2017) emphasise that Validity data provide empirical justification for MSCEIT’s use in that: 21 - Face validity is readily apparent in the tasks employed by the test - Content validity is also strong, as the scale items provide a good representation of the Four Branch Model - Findings to date point to suitable construct validity and unique predictive validity 4.3.2.4 Rationale for inclusion The validity and reliability of the MSCEIT v2.0 are considered acceptable and its’ face and construct validity do not overlap with personality construct models. Also, the MSCEIT v2.0 is the only test that measures the entire four-branch ability model which will be used for the purpose of this study. For these reasons mentioned above; the study will draw on the MSCEIT v2.0 for measuring emotional intelligence as defined by Mayer, Salovey and Caruso (2004) as cited in Sharma, Dhar & Tyagi (2015) earlier in the proposal. 4.4 Statistical Methods 4.4.1 Descriptive Statistics Bless, Smith and Sithole (2015) provide a definition for descriptive statistics as procedures for summarising information about a set of data or measurements. The aim is to acquire a holistic idea of the data distribution by summarising large data amounts in order to draw useful conclusions. Descriptive statistics employ the use of frequency distributions in order to describe variables. Frequency distribution is a statistical table that indicates how many individuals in a sample fall into each set of categories. They can be displayed visually as a bar or as a graph. Furthermore, descriptive statistics also contain measures of Central Tendency, which is the point in the distribution of a variable on which data are central. Three major types of estimates of central tendency exist; (a) Mean = the average ;(b) the median = the score found at the exact middle of the set of values (distribution); (c) the mode – the most frequently occurring value in the set of scores (Stagnor, 2015). Measures of dispersion are also part of descriptive statistics. Dispersion refers to the extent to which the observations (scores in a sample) are spread out around the measures of central tendency (Stagnor, 2015). The common measures of dispersion are the range and standard deviation. By definition, the range is a measure of dispersion equal to the maximum observed score minus the minimum observed score on a variable (the highest value minus the lowest value). The standard deviation is a measure of dispersion equal to the 22 square root of the variance, it is a more accurate and detailed estimate of dispersion because an outlier can greatly exaggerate the range (Stagnor, 2015). 4.4.2 Inferential Statistics These are mathematical techniques, or statistical tests, which allow the researcher to make statements about populations based on data retrieved from samples (Bless et al, 2015). Stagnor (2015) supports the above stance in his definition of inferential statistics; as numbers that are used to specify the characteristics of a population on the basis of the data in a sample. These numbers are p-value. The techniques to be applied in this study are the Pearson’s product moment correlation and t-test for independent groups. 4.4.2.1 Pearson’s Product Moment Correlation The Pearson’s product-moment correlation is a measure of the linear dependence between two variables, giving a value between +1 and -1 where 1 is total positive linear correlation, 0 is no linear correlation and -1 is total negative linear correlation 4.4.2.1.1 Reasons for use of the Pearson’s Product Moment Correlation Since the studies sample size is n>30(n=81), this correlation coefficient is best suited in providing conclusive results. Sudbury, Steyn and Wessel’s (2008) hold that if n>30, the scores of the variables of the target population tend towards a normal distribution and a parametric statistical method is implied. Moreover, the Pearson’s product-moment correlation provides a single, objective measure of the direction, as wells as the strength of a relationship between two variables, and in this case, burnout and personality characteristics. 4.4.2.1.2 Discussion of the Pearson’s Product Moment Correlation The Pearson’s product-moment correlation coefficient (r) focuses on the relationship between variables and looks to find a relationship between variable X and Y. Since the correlation will be applied on a sample, the study will use the sample correlation coefficient. Sudbury et al (2008) assert that the size of r is indicative of the strength of the relationship, while the positive sign of r indicates a direct correlation while a negative sign is symptomatic of an inverse correlation (Sudbury et al., 2008). To determine the Pearson’s product moment correlation, the following formula can be used: Definitional Formula: 23 Figure 4.1: Pearson’s Product Moment Correlation (Definitional Formula) Where: rxy = the correlation coefficient between variables x and y Zx and Zy = individual’s standard scores on the variables x and y N = Sample size Calculation Formula: Figure 4.2: Pearson’s Product Moment Correlation (Calculation Formula) Where: rxy = the correlation coefficient between variable x and y Σx = the sum of the scores of the variable x Σy = the sum of the scores of variable y Σxy = the sum of the product of the scores for variables N = Sample size 4.4.2.2 T-test for independent groups/One-way analysis of Variance (ANOVA) A T-test is a statistical test used to determine whether the observed means are statistically different (Stagnor, 2015) 24 The One-way Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) is a statistical procedure designed to compare the means of the dependent variable across the conditions of an experimental research design. (Stagnor, 2015) Given these definitions, the use of the t-test for independent groups is best suited because of the independence between the units to be measured, bearing in mind that the objective is determining whether or not differences in well-being exist with regards to gender. 4.4.2.2.1 Reasons for inclusion of the t-test for independent groups The t-test for independent groups compares the means between two unrelated groups on the same continuous, dependent variable (Statistics.laerd.com). The decision to use the t-test for independent groups stems from the fact that the two groups (male and female) to be tested on the same continuous, dependent variable (psychological well-being) are independent (gender). 4.4.2.2.2 Discussion of the t-test for independent groups The t-test for independent groups as mentioned above, determines whether there is a statistically significant difference between the means in two unrelated groups (statistics.laerd.com). Independent groups, are groups in which the participants in each group are different, this means that an individual in one group cannot also be a member of the other group and vice versa. Since the secondary objective of the study focuses on gender, which classifies an individual as either male or female – not both, this test is best suited. A few assumptions are met when deciding to work with the t-test for independent groups. However, it is not uncommon with real-world data that a few of these assumptions are violated (statistics.laerd.com): 1. The independent variable should be measured on a continuous scale (measured at the internal or ratio level) 2. The independent variable consists of two categorical, independent groups 3. There should be independence of observations – there is no relationship between the observations in each group or between the groups themselves 4. There should be no significant outliers – data points that do not follow the usual pattern 25 5. The dependent variable should be approximately normally distributed for each group of the independent variable 6. There needs to be homogeneity of variables When reporting the result of an independent t-test, included must be the t-statistic value, the degrees of freedom (df) and the significance value of the test (p-value). The format of the test result is: t(df) = t-statistic, p = significance value. For the purpose of this study, the statistical computer program (SPSS program) will be used to process the data. 4.5 Summary This chapter paid attention to the proposed research methodology for the study. Focus was on the selection of a sample and how data would be collected from said sample. The RPWBS and MSCEITv2 were identified as the most appropriate methods of gathering data, after which a discussion of statistical techniques to be used in order to test the hypotheses was provided. The Pearson product moment correlation is proposed to address the question pertaining to the relationship between burnout and personality traits, while the t-test for independent groups will address the differences in well-being with regards to gender. 26 Bibliography/Reference List Ackley, D. (2016). Emotional Intelligence: A Practical Review of Models, Measures, And Applications [Electronic Version]. Consulting Psychology Journal: Practice and Research, 68(4), 269-286. DOI: 10.1037/cp. b0000070. Akin, A. (2008). The Scales of Psychological Well-Being: A Study of Validity and Reliability. Faculty of Education, Department of Educational Sciences, Sakarya University, Sakarya, Turkey. Beauvais, A.M., Stewart, J.G. & DeNisco, S.M. (2014). Emotional Intelligence and Spiritual Well-Being [Electronic Version]. Journal of Christian Nursing, 31(3), 166-171. Bless, C., Higson-Smith, C., & Sithole, L. (2013). Fundamentals of Social Research Methods: An African Perspective (5th ed.). Cape Town: Juta. Boniwell, I. (2008). What Is Eudaimonia? The Concept of Eudaimonic Well-Being and Happiness. Retrieved March 17, 2017, from https://www.positivepyschology.org.uk/theconcept-of-eudaimonic-well-being/ Burns, R.A. & Machin, M.A. (2009). Investigating The Structural Validity of Ryff’s Psychological Well-Being Scales Across Two Samples. University of Southern Queensland, Department of psychology: Toowoomba, Queensland. Carod-Artal, F.J. & Vázquesz-Cabrera C. (2013). Work-Related Mental Consequences: Implications of Burnout On Mental Health Status Among Health Care Providers [Electronic Version]. Journal of Academy of Medical Sciences of Bosnia and Herzegovina, 23(1), 22-28. DOI: 10.5455/aim.2015.23.22-28. Chen, F.F., Jing, Y., Hayes, A. & Lee, J.M. (2012). Two Concepts or Two Approaches? A Bifactor Analysis of Psychological and Subjective Well-Being [Electronic Version]. Journal of Happiness Studies. DOI: 10.1007/510909-012-9367-x. Codier, E. & Odell, E. (2013). Measured Emotional Intelligence Ability and Grade Point Average in Nursing Students [Electronic Version]. Nurse Education Today. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.nedt.2013.06.007 Cohen, S. & Pressman, S.D. (2006). Positive Affect and Health [Electronic Version]. Association of Psychology Science, 15(3). 27 DiFabio, A. & Kenny, M. (2016). Promoting Well-Being: The Contribution of Emotional Intelligence [Electronic Version]. Frontiers in Psychology, 7:1182, DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01182. Dodge, R., Daly, A.P., Huyton, J. & Sanders, L.D. (2012). The Challenge of Defining Wellbeing [Electronic Version]. International Journal of Wellbeing, 2(3), 222-235. DOI: 10.5502/ijw1v2i3.4 Donoso, L.M.B., Demerouti, E., Hernández, E.G., Moreno-Jiménez, B. & Cobo, I.C. (2015). Positive Benefits of Caring On Nurses’ Motivation and Well-Being: A Diary Study About the Role of Emotional Regulation Abilities at Work [Electronic Version]. International Journal of Nursing Studies, 52, 804-816. DOI: 10.1016/j.ijnurstu.2015.01.002. Fatemi, A.H. & Asghari, A. (2016). The Role of Psychological Well-Being On University EFL Learners’ Attributional Patterns [Electronic Version]. Journal of Educational and Social Research, 6(1), 189-197. DOI: 10.5901/jesr. 2016.v6n1p189. Hajimia, H. (2014). Research Method – Sampling. Retrieved March 28, 2017, from https://www.slideshare.net/hafizahhajimaia/research-method-sampling Independent T-Test for Two Samples. (n.d.). Retrieved March 28, 2017, from https://statistics.laerd.com/spss-tutorials/independent-t-test-using-spps-statistics.php Irshad, S. (2015). Impact of Emotional Intelligence On Psychological Well-Being Among 1st Year Medical Students of Public Colleges [Electronic Version]. International Journal of Innovation and Scientific Research, 15, 50-63. Karimi, L., Leggat, S.G., Donohue, L., Farrell, G. & Couper, G.E. (2013). Emotional Rescue: The Role of Emotional Intelligence [Electronic Version]. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 70(1), 176-186. DOI: 10.1111/jan.12185. Kállay, E. & Rus, C. (2014). Psychometric Properties of The 44-Item Version of Ryff’s Psychological Well-Being Scale [Electronic Version]. European Journal of Psychological Assessment. DOI: 10.1027/1015-5759/a000163. Lee, H.E. & Taniguchi, E. (2014). A Cross-Cultural Validation of the Six-Factor Model of Psychological Well-Being. 28 Luhmann, M., Hawkley, L.C. Eid, M. & Cacioppo, J.J. (2013). The Frame and The Distinction Between Affective and Cognitive Well-Being [Electronic Version]. Journal of Research in Personality, 46(4), 461-441. DOI: 10.1016/j.jrp.2012.04.004. Mayer, J.D., Salovey, P., Caruso, D.R., & Sitarenios, G. (2003). Measuring Emotional Intelligence with the MSCEIT v2.0 [Electronic Version]. American Psychological Association, Inc. DOI: 10.1037/1528-3542.3.1.97 Mayer, J.D., Salovey, P., & Caruso, D.R. (2017) MSCEIT™ Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test. Retrieved March 27, 2017, from http://www.mhs.com/product.aspx?gr=cli&prod=msceit&id=resources#techInfo Miller, D.N. (2011). Positive Affect. In S. Goldstein & J.A. Naglieri (Eds.), Encyclopaedia of Child Behaviour and Development (pp. 1121-1122). Vol. 1. USA: Springer US. Montes-Berges, B. & Augusto-Landa, J. (2014). Emotional Intelligence and Affective Intensity as Life Satisfaction and Psychological Well-Being Predictors On Nursing Professionals [Electronic Version]. Journal of Professional Nursing, 30(1), 80-88. DOI: 10.1016/j.profnurs.2012.12.012. Moss, M., Good, V.S., Gozal, D., Kleinpell, R. & Sessler, C.N. (2016). An Official Critical Care Societies Collaborative Statement: Burnout Syndrome in Critical Care Health Care Professional: A Call for Action [Electronic Version]. American Journal of Critical Care, 25(4), 368-376. DOI: 10.4037/ajcc2016133 Nagel, Y., Nel, E. & Foxall, F. (2014). The Emotional Intelligence of Registered Nurses Commencing Critical Care Nursing [Electronic Version]. Curationis, 39(1), 91606. DOI: 10.4102/curationis. v39i1.1606. Okwaraji, F.E. & Aguwa, E.N. (2014). Burnout and Psychological Distress Among Nurses in Nigerian Tertiary Health Institution [Electronic Version]. Journal of African Health Sciences, 14(1), 237-245. DOI: 10.4314/ahs. v14i1.37. Por, J. Barriball, L., Fitzpatrick, J. & Roberts, J. (2011). Emotional Intelligence: Its Relationship to Stress, Coping, Well-Being and Professional Performance in Nursing Students [Electronic Version]. Nurse Education Today, 31, 855-860. DOI: 10.1016/j. nedt2010.12.023. 29 Rathnakara, S.K.A.K. (2014). The Impact of Emotional Intelligence On Psychological WellBeing of Public and Private Sector Executives: Perspective of Postgraduate Students. Proceedings of The HR Conference, University of Sri Jayewardenepura, vol. 1, No. 1. Rojas, M. (2006). Life Satisfaction and Satisfaction Domains of Life: Is It a Simple Relationship? [Electronic Version]. Journal of Happiness Studies, 7, 467-497. DOI: 10.1007/510902-006-9009-2. Ryff, C. (1989). Happiness Is Everything, Or Is It? Explorations On the Meaning of Psychological Well-Being [Electronic Version]. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57(6), 1069-1081. Samman, E. (2007). Psychological and Subjective Well-Being: A Proposal for Internationally Comparable Indicators. Oxford Poverty & Human Development Initiative (OPHI), Department of International Development, Queen Elizabeth House, University of Oxford. Seifert, T.A. (2015). The Ryff Scales of Psychological Well-Being. Retrieved March 17, 2017, From Centre Of Inquiry At Wabash College Web Site: https://www.liberalarts.wabash.edu/ryff-scales/ Shaheen, S. & Shaheen, H. (2016). Emotional Intelligence in Relation to Psychological WellBeing Among Students [Electronic Version]. The International Journal of Indian Psychology, 3(4), 63. DOI: 18.01.115/20160304. Sharman, J., Dhar, R.L. & Tyagi, A. (2015). Stress as A Mediator Between Work-Family Conflict and Psychological Health Among the Nursing Staff: Moderating Role of Emotional Intelligence [Electronic Version]. Journal of Applied Nursing Research, 30, 268-275. DOI: 10.1016/j. apnr2015.01.010. Softsolutions (2015). Research Methods for Business by Uma Sekaran 6th Edition. Retrieved March 28, 2017, from https://www.slideshare.net/shahrozerashid007/research-methods-forbusiness-by-uma-sekaran-6th-edition-full-book-pdf Springer, K.N. & Hauser, R.M. (2006). An Assessment of the Construct Validity of Ryff’s Scales of Psychological Well-Being: Method, Mode and Measurement Effects. University of Wisconsin-Madison, Department of Sociology and Centre for Demography of Health and Aging. 30 Stangor, C. (2015). Research Methods for The Behavioural Sciences (5th ed.). Thousand Oaks CA: Cengage. Stringer, D.M. (2013). Negative Affect. In M.D. Gellman, & J.R. Turner (Eds.), Encyclopaedia of Behavioural Medicine (pp. 1303-1304). Vol. 1. USA: Springer New York. Sánchez-Álvarez, N., Extremera, N. & Fernández-Berrocal, P. (2016). The Relation Between Emotional Intelligence and Subjective Well-Being: A Meta-Analytic Investigation [Electronic Version]. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 11(3), 276-285. DOI: 10.1080/17439760.2015.1058968. Tran, N. (2015). Positive Affectivity: Why Some People Are Happier Than Others. Retrieved March 15, 2017, from https://positivepsychologyprogram.com/positive-affectivity/ Wilson, J. (2014). The Knowledge and Use of Emotional Intelligence by Registered Nurses and Healthcare Support Workers in an Acute Hospital Setting. Proceedings of The RCN Research Conference, Sheffield Hallam University. Retrieved from 31