THE EFFECT OF EMPATHY INDUCTION ON AGGRESSIVE BEHAVIOR OF BULLY-VICTIMS A Thesis presented to the Faculty of the College of Arts and Sciences BATANGAS STATE UNIVERSITY LIPA CITY Marawoy, Lipa City In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Bachelor of Science inPsychology IRIS KATHLEEN S. DOMION CHRISTINE M. GLORIENE NIESHA MARIE L. LALIMAN IAN PAOLO H. MANGOBOS JEANNEN L. ONA May, 2020 APPROVAL SHEET This thesis entitled “THE EFFECT OF EMPATHY INDUCTION ON AGGRESSIVE BEHAVIOR OF BULLY-VICTIMS”prepared and submitted by Iris Kathleen S. Domion, Christine M. Gloriene, Niesha Marie L. Laliman, Ian Paolo H. Mangobos, and Jeannen L. Ona in partial fulfillment of the degree of Bachelor of Science in Psychology, has been examined and recommended for acceptance and approval for Oral Examinations. SHELYN S. EXTRA Adviser PANEL OF EXAMINERS Approved by the Committee on Oral Examinations with a grade of ___________. Accepted and approved in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the Degree of Bachelor of Science inPsychology. _______ Date DR. NERRIE E. MALALUAN Dean of Colleges ACKNOWLEDGEMENT The researchers wish to express their deepest gratitude to the people who have extended their assistance for the success of this study. This work would not have been possible to come to the present shape without the supervision and support given by numerous people particularly the following: It is with deep sense of gratitude to acknowledge the efforts and guidance of our supervisor, Ms. Shelyn S. Extra. Her advices, valuable comments, enthusiasm, and provisions greatly helped on the success of this study. Her expertise and knowledge have also helped the researchers on the completion of this work. To all friends, family, relatives and everyone who have shared their untiring and countless support, either emotionally, morally, spiritually, and financially, and for their immeasurable love and motivation. And above all, to Almighty God, for the wisdom and power of mind he bestowed upon us, strength, and good health in order to finish this research. We thank you. I.K.S.D C.M.G N.M.L.L I.P.H.M J.L.O vi DEDICATION This is wholeheartedly dedicated to all the people in our lives who are behind the success of this work and who have extended their help and support to accomplish this study. To our Family, who have constantly provide their moral, emotional, spiritual, and financial support and who have motivated and gave us unconditional love when we thought of giving up, these achievements are all for you; To our Experimental Psychology Professor, Ms. Shelyn S. Extra, who believed in our abilities and who gave her endless support and guidance, whose expertise and knowledge were genuinely shared, this study would not be possible if not for you; And above all, To Great Almighty, For His guidance, for giving us strength, power of mind, protection, and skills. Iris Christine Niesha Ian Jeannen vi ABSTRACT Title: THE EFFECTS OF EMPATHY INDUCTION ON AGGRESSIVE BEHAVIOR OF BULLY- VICTIMS Author: DOMION, IRIS KATHLEEN GLORIENE, CHRISTINE LALIMAN, NIESHA MARIE MANGOBOS, IAN PAOLO ONA, JEANNEN L. Type of Document: UNDERGRADUATE THESIS No. of Pages: 93 Name and Address of Institution: BATANGAS STATE UNIVERSITY LIPA CITY MARAWOY, LIPA CITY Course: BS PSYCHOLOGY ________________________________________________________________________ Aggressive behavior can cause physical or emotional harm to others ranging from verbal to physical abuse. It is a reactionary and impulsive behavior that often results in breaking household rules or the law. One of the most significant predictors of violence is an vi individual’s ability to empathize with others. Empathy plays a vital role in managing and controlling aggression. This study aims to discover the effects of empathy induction on aggressive behavior of bully-victims. The researchers aim to establish a study that will determine how aggressive behavior can be manifested by bully-victims, ages 15-20 years old. The sample consists of twelve (12) bully-victims or adolescents that are both bullies and victims selected through online survey. Researchers used true experimental design in gathering information and a 20-item Aggression Scale was used as the main data gathering instrument to measure the aggressive behavior of the participants. To assess the effects of the empathy induction on behavior, 2 video clips were utilized to induce empathy; one (1) cognitive empathy inducing clip entitled “Why I Bully You” and one (1) affective empathy inducing film entitled “Ian” for experimental group’s interventions. Results indicate that empathy induction implemented by the researcher has a significant effect on aggressive behavior of participants. viii TABLE OF CONTENTS TITLE PAGE ………………………………..… Page i APPROVAL SHEET …………………………………… ii ACKNOWLEDGMENT …………………………………… iii DEDICATION ……………………………………. iv ABSTRACT ……………………………………. v TABLE OF CONTENTS……………………………………. vii LIST OF TABLES Table 1. (Pre-Test Results of Control and Experimental Group)…………….…………… .46 Table 2. (Post Test Results of Control and Experimental Group)……………….………....48 Table 3. (Summary of Computations on Test of Difference Between the Pre-test Results of the Control and Experimental Group.…….….……51 Table 4. (Summary of Computations on Test of Difference Between the Post Test Results of the Control and Experimental Group……………..52 Table 5. (Summary of Computations on Test of Difference Between Pre-Test and Post Test Results…54 viii Table 6. (Proposed Program and Activities)………………61 LIST OF FIGURES Figure 1. (Conceptual Paradigm)……………………………38 LIST OF APPENDICES………………………………………….73 Appendix A. (Consent Form)…………………………….74 Appendix B. (Instrumentation)…………………………...76 Appendix C. (Statistical Computation)……………….......81 CHAPTER 1 THE PROBLEM Introduction…………………………………………..1 Statement of the Problem………………………….....5 Scope, Limitations, and Delimitations of the Study…5 Significance of the Study…………………………….6 2 REVIEW OF RELATED LITERATURE Conceptual Literature………………………………...9 Research Literature…………………………………..26 Synthesis……………………………………………..32 Conceptual Framework………………………………37 Hypothesis……………………………………………39 Definition of Terms…………………………………..39 3 RESEARCH METHODOLOGY Research Design……………………………………..41 Subjects of the Study………………………………...42 Data Gathering Instrument…………………………..43 Data Gathering Procedure…………………………...43 Analysis of Data……………………………………..45 ix viii 4 PRESENTATION, ANALYSIS, AND INTERPRETATION OF DATA (topical based on specific problems)………………………………46 5 SUMMARY, CONCLUSIONS, AND RECOMMENDATIONS Summary…………………………………………....57 Findings…………………………………..................58 Conclusions…………………………………………59 Recommendations…………………………………..61 APPENDICES CURRICULUM VITAE 1 CHAPTER I THE PROBLEM Introduction The Era of 21st Century is said to be the age of mental, social and personality disintegration that is why aggressive behavior has become a topic of vital importance and a major concern in most societies. People are under the period of anxiety, struggle, frustration, hate, and under the strain of aggression that could soon lead to violent offenses and greater involvement in crime. In most part of the world, violence is disturbingly common and certainly creating chaos that makes the world peace and harmony at sake. A 2002 report from the World Health Organization found that 4,400 people die each year due to act of violence, underscoring the public health relevance of understanding and preventing aggressive behavior. (Krug et al., 2002) Sigmund Freud defined the tendency of aggression as an innate, independent, instinctual disposition in man. The instinct theorist view aggression as a part of the basic nature of humans. Aggressiveness, on the other hand, can also be considered as learnt behavior because there are environmental factors that increase the risk of involvement of aggressive and violent acts. In psychology, the term aggression refers to a range of behaviors that can result in both physical and psychological harm to yourself, others, or objects in the environment. (Cherry, 2020) Mostly, human engage in aggression when they seek to cause harm or pain 2 to another person. In psychological literature, aggression refers to a wide spectrum of behaviors intended to harm another individual who is motivated to avoid being harmed. Commonly, aggression is perceived as purely physical harm such as hitting and pushing, but there is a psychological aggression that can also be damaging. Intimidating or verbally berating another person is an example of verbal, mental and emotional aggression. Aggressive forms of behavior can be characterized by verbal or physical attack. It may be appropriate (self-protective) or, alternatively, it may be destructive to oneself and others. (Ferris & Grisso, 1996). For the past years, bullying has been a big issue surrounding the world. To prevent such an issue, it is necessary to provide information and understand the topic. Bullying represents a form of aggressive behavior. It occurs due to the anticipation of someone’s desirable goal. According to Olweus (2001), bullying is a unique but complex form of interpersonal aggression, which takes many forms, serves different functions, and is manifested in different patterns of relationships. There are numerous definitions of bullying, but they all refer to bullying as repeated intimidation, over time, of a physical, verbal and psychological nature, of a less powerful person by a more powerful person or group of persons. It is repetitive and encompasses an intrinsic power imbalance between the bully and the person being bullied, who generally is incapable of self-defense. The longterm effects of bullying and being aggressive in childhood are reported to be 3 potential risk factors for adolescent depression, suicidal behavior and self-harm. Children exposed to bullying often become violent towards others. One of the most significant predictors of violence is an individual’s ability to empathize with others. Empathy is the ability to recognize, understand, and share the thoughts and feelings of another person, animal, or fictional character. Empathy helps us cooperate with other people, build friendships, make moral decisions, and intervene when we see others being bullied. Empathy, according to Happ & Melzer (2014), refers to the capacity to feel what another is feeling. Examples include the sadness felt when tragedy strikes a friend or the pleasure vicariously experienced in relation to another's joy. It is, then, the sharing of an emotional experience. Humans begin to show signs of empathy in infancy and the trait develops steadily through childhood and adolescence. Still, most people are likely to feel greater empathy for people like themselves and may feel less empathy for those outside their family, community, ethnicity, or race. Developing empathy is crucial for establishing relationships and behaving compassionately. It is an essential part of social and emotional development, affecting an individual’s behavior toward others and the quality of social relationships. It involves experiencing another person’s point of view, rather than just one’s own, and enables prosocial, or helping behaviors that come from within, rather than being forced. Therefore, empathy plays an important role in reducing 4 aggressive and violent behavior like bullying. It just takes serious practice to give empathy the right way. As students of psychology studying human mind and behavior, the research is very relative and essential on the field. The researchers chose to study aggressive behavior because it is a familiar term and a key concept in the study of human behavior. Study aims to investigate the effect of empathy on aggressive behavior of bully-victims. It intends to evaluate if empathy, elicited by empathy inducing films, would influence the participants’ aggressive behavior. Furthermore, the main goal of the research is to create a psychological intervention and activities based on the result of the study. It could be an activity that will boost empathic concern to effectively handle aggressive behavior. 5 Statement of the Problem Generally, this study aims to discover the effects of empathy induction on aggressive behavior of bully-victims. Specifically, this study seeks to answer the following problems: 1. What are the pre-test results of the participants? 2. What are the post test results of the participants? 3. Is there a significant difference between the pre-test results of the participants? 4. Is there a significant difference between the post test results of the participants? 5. Is there a significant difference between the pre-test and post test results of the participants? 6. What are the proposed intervention and activities based on the findings? Scope, Limitation and Delimitations of the Study At this span of life, aggression is also a major characteristic among adolescents. The study is limited to aggressive behavior of bully-victims in relation to empathy induction. The participants of the study were bully-victims selected through online survey. The study was only delimited to twelve (12) participants composed of six 6 (6) men and six (6) women. In this study, both genders were considered and adolescents’ ages 15-20 years old were selected. The study seeks to know if empathy induction has an effect on the aggressive behavior of the participants. The researchers used a Bully-Victim Survey Form to assess bullying and victimization experiences in selection of the respondents. Meanwhile, the researchers used a 20-item Aggression Scale as the main data gathering instrument to measure the aggressive behavior of the participants. The researchers will only use two (2) video clips for empathy induction: one (1) cognitive empathy inducing clip entitled “Why I Bully You” and one (1) affective empathy inducing film entitled “Ian” for experimental group’s interventions. Significance of the Study Aggressive behavior can be learned through various sources like family, school, peers, neighbours, culture, through television or violent video games. Aware of the need to control aggression that resulted on different forms of violence, the researchers would find the results of the study meaningful and substantial: To Students. This study will be helpful to students who have aggressive tendency and behavior due to their condition and experiences. It is essential for them to know how aggression could be regulated and controlled. 7 To Parents. Family plays a significant role in the potential development of early aggressive behavior that’s why this study is important for the parents to understand their children’s emotion and guide them in handling anger, aggression and frustration. To Educational Practitioners. School environment plays a very vital role in the development and prevention of adolescent aggressive behavior. This study will help them to be familiar with the different emotional needs of their students and serves as a guide to create a more comfortable and safe atmospheres for students to learn in. To Psychologists. Fostering empathy may contribute in clinical interventions for prevention and intervention of future aggressive behavior that leads to delinquency and other antisocial behavior if not prevented. This will give them ideas about better design and implementation of treatment programs and therapies. To Media Practitioners. This research will contribute on promoting a healthy media environment as they play a crucial role in developing aggressive and violent behaviors among children. This will help in implementing future programs on media literacy or violence prevention. To Government Officials. Community plays a vital role in the society development of aggression as they are the chief agents of socialization. This study 8 will help in identifying ways and means to reduce or regulate aggression in the community and to promote the use of nonviolent conflict resolution strategies. To Future Researchers. This study willlead them to create different studies in aggression. It will serve as a reference or guide for future researchers who will be doing the same or related kind of research for further expansion of the subject 9 CHAPTER II REVIEW OF RELATED LITERATURE Presented in this chapter are the discussions of studies relative to the present research. This contains concise different literature and studies that will be helpful to the present study. The researchers review important concepts from considerable numbers of previous studies and research work from thesis from both foreign and local resources related to study. Conceptual Literature In order to understand fully, the researchers seek writing from journals and electronic sources. This briefly presents the review of related literature that provided the researcher with the background of determining the problems of bullyvictims. Empathy Ickes (1997) defined empathy as a complex interpersonal phenomenon in which observation, memory, knowledge, and reasoning are united to allow insights into the thoughts and feelings of others. It describes an affective response that involves the perception and the understanding of the emotional state of someone else. Empathic individuals are thought to utilize information about emotional states in others to constrain potentially harmful behaviors. Empathy is 10 associated with helping and comforting another individual, and is assumed to inhibit antisocial and aggressive behavior. According to Goldstein & Michaels (1985), the word empathy can be traced back to the Greek word empatheia, which “implies an active appreciation of another person’s feeling experience”. Meanwhile, Strayer (1987) mentioned another predecessor of the term empathy which is, “einfühlung”, meaning “feeling into”. The term “einfühlung” was introduced into the English language by Edward B. Titchener in the early 1900s. Since then, the definition of empathy has evolved to include two distinct components: affect and cognition. As a broad term, Killen & Smetana (2006) defined empathy as “an affective response that stems from the apprehension or comprehension of another’s emotional state or condition and is similar to what the other person is feeling or would be expected to feel in the given situation.” In short, empathy is the ability to understand and share in another person’s emotions and feelings. Most researchers agree that empathy consists of a cognitive and an affective component. Hoffman (2000) referred the term empathy to two related, yet different human abilities: mental perspective taking (cognitive empathy) and the vicarious sharing of emotion (affective empathy). There are evidences to suggest that cognitive empathy is distinct from affective empathy. Cognitive empathy is the ability of one person to understand another’s emotions, whereas affective empathy 11 is the ability for a person to share the emotions of others. (Crothers, L.M. et.al, 2014). Cognitive Empathy. Cognitive empathy refers to the skills of recognizing others’ emotions and understanding others’ perspectives. Cognitive empathy allows the ability to apprehend, appreciate, and tolerate the viewpoints of others while engaging in non-aggressive behaviors. Cognitive empathy also evolves with age. Children with lower levels of cognitive empathy are less able to interpret the feelings of others. If children have low cognitive empathy, they may create inaccurate attributions about other children’s behaviors and engage in forms of reactive aggression. (Hogan, 1969) According to Beadle, J. & de la Vega, C. (2019), one subdomain of cognitive empathy is perspective taking, which involves mentally putting oneself in another person’s shoes in order to understand their thoughts and feelings. Perspective taking may engage such processes as imagination, autobiographical memory, and future thinking as an individual attempts to determine another person’s thoughts and feelings. There is also evidence that actively attempting to engage in perspective taking can result in an increase in momentary levels of emotional empathy. Moreover, Maibom (2012) defined cognitive empathy as the capacity to represent other people’s mental states (theory of mind). When cognitive empathy is said to be required for affective empathy, it is best interpreted in terms of theory 12 of mind. More often, however, cognitive empathy is thought of in terms of perspective taking. Perspective taking is considered and evaluated mostly as a monolithic ability. The broadest definitions of cognitive empathy are actually redundant with the theory of mind perspective and not specific to emotions, while narrower definitions limit the scope of cognitive empathy to detecting emotions, projecting oneself into another’s situation, or imagining how another person is feeling. In any case, cognitive empathy represents a top-down analysis reliant on higher brain structures. Affective Empathy. Affective empathy is the ability to understand and share the emotions of others. Individuals with affective empathy experience an appropriate emotional response when confronted with the mental state of another person. Those high in affective empathy are able to share the pain of a victim and experience the emotional state and discomfort of the victim. It has been suggested that children high in affective empathy avoid aggression for causing distress in others and thus themselves. Affective empathy reflects the innate, automatic capacity to respond with arousal to the signs of discomfort or affective states of the other. It is initiated through direct contact, which ensures access to species-universal information that activates affective empathy mechanisms. (Preston and De Waal, 2002). 13 Definitions of affective empathy also vary. The conceptualization most similar to simulation theory is that of “emotional contagion”—the exact matching of emotions (Hatfield, Cacioppo, & Rapson, 1994). Most conceptualizations refer to resonant emotional experiences that are congruent in valence rather than identical. Closely related to this conception of affective empathy are sympathy and personal distress, which the psychological literature distinguishes on the basis of their social consequences. Both include a negative emotional reaction caused by witnessing another’s distress; however, sympathy is characterized by an altruistic motivation to reduce that person’s suffering, while personal distress connotes a selfish motivation to reduce one’s own suffering—either by helping the other person or escaping the situation, whichever is less costly. During this evolution, some researchers preferred a definition containing either the affective or cognitive component, but not both, although it is widely recognized today that empathy contains both of these components. For researchers that emphasize the affective component of empathy, the match or sharing between the emotions of the target and of the observer is stressed. Thus, the observer must experience the same emotion the target does. Others have preferred a definition of empathy that centers on cognition. These cognitive definitions often relate empathy to social cognitive and role-taking skills/perspective taking skills (Eagle & Wolitzky, 1997). 14 The distinction between cognitive and affective empathy might be best understood by reference to two competing theoretical views of how humans understand other humans’ mental states more generally. Theory of Mind Perspective. The theory of mind perspective describes an evolved psychological capacity for understanding behavior using something akin to scientific theory (Gopnik & Meltzoff, 1998). Using a fairly cold form of information processing, humans apply a system of rules derived from their own experiences to represent the mental states of others. The accurate representation of others’ thoughts and feelings confers obvious benefits, because it allows us to accurately monitor others’ intentions, predict their behavior, and enjoy the various advantages associated with social living. Simulation Perspective. In contrast to the theory of mind perspective, the simulation perspective suggests that we instinctively mimic others’ mental activity and use our own reactions to understand what they are thinking and feeling (Gallese & Goldman, 1998). The automatic and unconscious activation of neural representations matching the perceived mental state of others has been referred to as the perception-action mechanism (Preston & De Waal, 2002). The PAM provides a bottom-up, phylogenetically ancient mechanism for representing the mental states of others that is far less intellectually demanding and cold than the top-down processes described by the theory of mind account. 15 Empathy Induction Maibom (2012) divulged that empathy is normally induced by seeing, believing, or imagining that the person is experiencing an emotion or is in an emotion-inducing situation. In principle, we can empathize with any emotion someone else experiences, as long as we are capable of experiencing that emotion ourselves. In practice, however, most empathy research concerns empathy with a person in some form of distress. Induction comes in the form of stimulated experimental situations. With this measure, children are shown videos, audiotapes, or realistic enactments that elicit empathic responses. Meta-analyses and reviews on emotion research have shown the use of film clips to be one of the most effective methods of mood induction. Grodal & Kramer (2010) stated that film clips are the most commonly used stimuli in mood induction. Films that provide interaction with the body surface may thus induce similar feelings in the viewer. Film experience consists of a double empathic sharing: the sharing of emotions with characters and the sharing of emotions with other physically present or imagined viewers. Maffei (2019) stated that emotion induction are emotional imagery, presentation of visual stimuli (slides and videos), presentation of sound or music, recollection of autobiographical memories and, more recently, virtual reality. In the theory of mind debate, imagining being in the other person’s situation is thought to play an important role in us representing the mental states of others. 16 Simulation theory is considered a major competitor to the theory, the idea that we use something like a theory to ascribe mental states to people. The simulation theory supposes that when we imagine ourselves in another’s situation, we have thoughts, feelings, etc. very similar to those we would have if we were in that situation ourselves. Empathic induced affects, according to Dodaj et.al., (2012), are combined with the general state of the mental images of others. Empathic distress is the core feeling state of empathy, and this cognitive appraisal determines whether the person will have an emotional arousal. Moreover, Howard (2014) mentioned that the use of emotional film clips provides several advantages compared to other methods of emotion elicitation, including ease of standardization and a high degree of ecological validity. Film clips allow for presentations of more emotional and complex stimuli in relatively short periods of time compared to other methods (e.g., static images).a variety of methods to elicit specific emotions experienced by individuals, including, but not limited to, the use of static images, vignettes, interactions with confederates, and films. In addition, Philippot (2010) proved that longer film clips conveying a larger amount of information for the elicitation of complex emotions (such as empathy) may provide a viable alternative to eliciting empathic emotion. As noted before, film clips have the ability to employ particularly complex stimuli in a short amount of time. On the other hand, Maffei (2019) stated that duration should be 17 long enough to provide the viewer with an understanding of the plot, leading to a coherent and stable change of his/ her affective state. Shorter clips (i.e. one minute or less) may not be long enough to induce a complex emotion, engage the attention of the viewer and making possible the process of “willing suspension of disbelief” which is critical for the ecology of the emotional experience achieved through film watching. Meanwhile, using longer clips (i.e. three minutes or more) may increase the difficulty to disengage from the stimulus, thus prompting carryover effects on the following excerpts within the experimental paradigm. Aggressive Behavior Aggression is usually defined as behavior deliberately aimed to harm individuals and/or objects. Another often cited description of aggression is Parke and Slaby’s (1983), definition as a “behaviour that is aimed at harming or injuring another person or persons”. Intent to harm and the victim feeling hurt are two features common to most definitions of both physical and nonphysical forms of aggression (Galen & Underwood, 1997). However, Hay (2005) suggested intent is a developmental phenomenon, making it difficult to measure. Subsequent investigations of aggressive behaviors resulted in a number of other terms used to describe forms of aggression. Behaviors that are aimed at inflicting relationship or social harm are viewed as covert in nature, and include indirect, covert, relational, and social aggression. Aggression has several elements including physical and social, verbal and nonverbal, and reactive and proactive 18 components, however, it can be summed up in two types – physical and relational aggression (Fraser, et al., 2005). Physical aggression. Physical aggression may be the most widely recognized form of overt or direct aggression and also one of the most serious forms of antisocial behavior. Physical aggression has also been defined by Tremblay & Nagin (2005) as the use of force against another person, which may or may not include the use of objects. In fact, physical aggression is one of the criteria for the diagnosis of conduct disorder (i.e., “often bullies, threatens, or intimidates others,”). It usually takes place in a face-to-face confrontation and includes behaviors that threaten or cause bodily injury, such as making threats of harm, fighting, and violent crimes (Loeber & Hay, 1997). Verbal aggression. Parke & Slaby (1983) characterized verbal aggression by threats of physical aggression and yelling. Kochenderfer-Ladd and Ladd (2001) distinguished overt verbal aggression (e.g., name-calling, insults, threats) and indirect verbal aggression (e.g., talking behind the victim’s back). Indirect aggression. Indirect aggression, according to Lagerspetz et al., (1988), was the first term to emerge to describe the topography of nonphysical behavior. It is described as behaviors that do not confront the victim, including gossiping, rejection, and exclusion. Buss (1961) first used the term indirect aggression to describe aggressive behavior in which the aggressor was not easily observed. Later, Björkqvist et al. (1992) defined indirect aggression as “a type of 19 behavior in which the perpetrator attempts to inflict pain in such a manner that he or she makes it seem as though there has been no intention to hurt at all” Relational aggression. Crick and Grotpeter (1995) defined relational aggression as a form of aggression that targets the closeness of friendships. Relationally aggressive youth are usually found to be more socially savvy, with the ability to manipulate the social hierarchy to their advantage. Relational aggression, sometimes referred to as social aggression, is characterized as actions designed to damage another’s self-esteem, social status or friendship patterns. It consists of both direct (e.g., telling someone they are not your friend anymore unless they do something for you) and indirect behaviors (e.g., spreading rumors). Relational aggression is aimed at inflicting harm by manipulating peer relationships, including behaviors such as gossiping, rumor spreading, social exclusion, and ostracizing. It is further characterized by high levels of intimacy, exclusivity, and jealousy. Social aggression. Social aggression as defined by Galen and Underwood (1997) has been described as behaviors aimed at damaging the victim’s selfesteem or reputation, through rejection, rumors, or social exclusion. Also viewed as in direct behavior, social aggression is viewed as subtle, indirect behavior that is just as hurtful as physical aggression. Social aggression is aimed at damaging another’s self- esteem and social status. They posit that social aggression can be direct (e.g., verbal rejection, negative facial expressions) or indirect (e.g., rumors, 20 social exclusion). Other behaviors manifested through social aggression include hurting another’s social status or friendships through nonverbal and verbal social exclusion, gossip, and friendship manipulation. Moreover, Dodge and Coie (1987) introduced the distinction between reactive and proactive aggression in children. Reactive aggression.Reactive aggression is described as an impulsive response to a perceived threat or provocation, often associated with high emotional arousal, anxiety, and anger. It is a behavior that occurs in response to specific situational conditions, such as threats and provocations, and is accompanied by changes in the physiological and emotional level. Reactive aggression is committed in “anger or frustration or in response to provocation”. It is angry, defensive, and responsive to a real or perceived threat. Dodge and Coie (1987) discovered that reactive aggression is connected with attention difficulties and adjustment concerns with peer relationships. Children use reactive aggression to alleviate their own anger, frustration, or stress in a social situation and typically demonstrate remorse after they have behaved in this manner. Reactively aggressive youth hold more hostile attributions than their nonaggressive peers and their aggression is motivated by both accurate and inaccurate perceptions of a real or perceived threat. Proactive aggression. Proactive aggression, on the other hand, is defined as behavior intended to achieve personal gain through attainment of status or desired 21 objects. It is deliberate behavior focused on reaching an instrumental (not relational) goal. This type of aggression is focused on self-enhancement through material or territorial gain (i.e., external reward). Also, it is described as instrumental, organized, cold-blooded, and motivated by the anticipation of reward. Research has shown that along with individual, family, school and economic factors, early aggressive behavior and poor peer relationships are precursors of fighting, delinquency, and drug involvement. Dodge et al. (1987) theorized that a primary distinction between aggressive and non-aggressive children is the purpose for engaging in aggression. It is “motivated by the desire to reach a specific goal” and is systematically planned and unprovoked. It can be used to gain power, dominate, or intimidate for the aggressor’s personal satisfaction. Children who use proactive aggression are also known as “offensive aggressors” and “coldblooded” aggressor. Proactive aggressors exhibit methodical and intentionally-driven behaviors, which are related to delinquency, criminality, and social withdrawal. However, proactively aggressive children are often viewed as leaders with a sense of humor. The study of aggressive behavior has a long history in the social sciences. From the frustration aggression hypothesis in social learning theory came the assertion that aggression is behavior intended to harm or driven by frustration or passion. 22 Bully-Victim Pepler et al., 2010 mentioned that youth spend much of the day interacting with peers in schools, neighborhoods, communities, and through social media, and bullying behaviors almost always occur within the peer context. Bullying and victimization are more likely in classrooms characterized by peer norms that support bullying and by high peer conflict. Bullying has been most studied in the school context, and the positive or negative climate of the school impacts the frequency of bullying and victimization. As these findings suggest, bullying and victimization do not occur in isolation. Rather, bullying stems from complex interactions between individuals and the contexts in which they function, both proximal (i.e., family, peers, school climate) and distal (i.e., societal, cultural influences). (Swearer & Hymel, 2015) Dodaj et al. (2012) stated that there are four characteristic profiles that can differ in bullying: children who are exposed to bullying or victims, children who bully others or bullies, children who experience bullying but themselves behave violently or bully-victims, as well as the children who are not in any way involved in bullying, or neutral children. Bullies and victims may share risk factors such as low academic achievement, poor social skills, low socioeconomic status, and family discord. Individuals who are victimized at home and bully at school may be part of the bully-victim group as well. Bully-victims are characterized as those that 23 are at risk for a variety of behavioral problems, such as addiction to alcohol, delinquency, etc. Kochel et al. (2015) stated that “bully-victims” are both bullies and the victims of bullying. They suffer from a distinctive set of problems. There is an evidence that victimized bullies engage in acts of aggression or violence. When children experience bullying, they have a tendency to become emotionally withdrawn. It is important to note that anger and rage is one possible emotional response to bullying. In some cases, kids who are bullied are also bullies themselves. They demonstrate many of the same behaviors as do bullies and victims. The interesting, and very sad, part comes later, when they reach adulthood and experience long lasting psychological effects that are more severe than that experienced by either bullies or victims alone. Some kids occupy the middle of the bullying food chain. They get bullied by dominant individuals but they also perpetrate bullying themselves. Rejected, victimized, and aggressive, these “bully-victims” tend to have more psychological problems than either “pure bullies” or “pure victims”. Both type may suffer from psychiatric disorders, and both are at increased risk for committing severe, violent offenses as they get older. (Sourander, 2011) Bully-victims typically tend to have the highest level of adjustment difficulties among all children involved in bullying, showing symptoms of both internalizing and externalizing problems (Nansel et al., 2001). Researchers have 24 long demonstrated that being involved as both a perpetrator and victim seems to compound the impact of bullying, with bully-victims experiencing worse outcomes than either bullies or victims, being at greater risk for anxiety, depression, low self-esteem, self-harm, suicidal ideation and suicidality, physical injury, substance abuse, negative attitudes toward school, absenteeism, poor perceptions of school safety, aggression, and delinquency. (Berkowitz & Benbenishty, 2012) Those who bully and who are bullied (bully-victims) are found consistently to be at greater risk for mental health problems than bullies or victims alone (Benedict et al., 2015). Particularly in regards to internalizing symptoms, bullyvictims report higher levels of mental health challenges, specifically anxiety, depression, and thought problems, than bullies or victims. Additionally, bullyvictims demonstrate the poorest psychosocial functioning of all groups, with increased peer rejection, poorer relationships with classmates, and increased loneliness. Adolescents Aggression is a highly studied area in psychosocial literature, particularly adolescent aggression. Adolescence is a time of rapid change for youth both physically and cognitively. An adolescent passes through great physical, mental, emotional and social stress during this period. During the last decade, the study of adolescent aggressive behavior has increasingly focused upon the fact that 25 aggression is not only physical by its nature, but it can be of various forms. There can be many reasons of aggression among adolescents but the period of adolescence itself is a major cause of aggression. That is why it has been said that adolescence is the period of stress, strain and strife. They become at greater risk for anxiety, depression, and suicidal behavior. During the transition from middle to high school, bullying tends to peak. At this vulnerable stage of a child’s development, social, verbal, and electronic bullying can be more abusive than any kind of physical bullying. (PREVNet, 2019). Aggression and violence during childhood and adolescence have been the focus of much research over the past several decades. These researchers have found that serious forms of aggression remain relatively stable from childhood through adulthood; however, Loeber and Hay (1997) argue that mild forms of aggression may not begin for some children until early or late adolescence. Despite Loeber and Hay's findings, very little research has been conducted on mild forms of aggression, such as bullying, during the middle years. One notable gap in the evolving literature on bullying and victimization during early adolescence is the role that peers play in promoting bullying and victimization by either reinforcing the aggressor, failing to intervene to stop the victimization, or affiliating with students who bully. 26 Research Literature In the process of gathering data, the researchers had referred to some materials which were believed to have to their current endeavours. Reviews of related foreign and local researches, journals and study are presented that serve as a basis of the present study. Research on human aggression has been a flourishing industry in the 20 th century. The study of Scrimgeour (2007) entitled “Empathy and Aggression: A Study of the Interplay Between Empathy and Aggression in Preschoolers” found that high levels of empathy were significantly correlated with low levels of aggression. Conceivably, the development of empathy reduced the presence of aggressive behavior in participants. The less empathic participants were significantly associated with high rates of physical aggression. These findings underline the importance of the development of empathy and prosocial behavior and their ability to inhibit aggressive acts towards others in preschool children. He also concluded that if more children were encouraged to explore their feelings and to understand the feelings of their peers, then perhaps statistics of school violence could be reduced and school shootings could be a phenomenon of the past. Moreover, Bohart & Greenberg (1997) cited in their study, entitled “Empathy reconsidered: New direction in psychotherapy” , that empathy serves as a learned lesson and as an inhibitor of further aggressive acts to a child who vicariously experienced pain through another child’s experience. As children 27 advance cognitively, the more likely they are able to understand and control their emotions as well as take another’s perspective. This cognitive achievement allows for a reduction in potential aggressive conflicts and welcomes the probable occurrence of prosocial behaviors. He supported the notion that children from a young age experience empathy and that their empathic skills develop with age. Furthermore, in the study of Stanger et al. (2015) entitled “Empathy Inhibits Aggression in Competition: The Role of Provocation, Emotion, and Gender”, the researchers indicate that empathy can reduce reactive aggression in team sport athletes in competition. The results showed that empathy reduced males’ reported likelihood to aggress toward an opponent in sport. However, the suppressing effect of empathy on aggression occurs only in women at high provocation. Their findings proved that empathy reduced participants’ aggression. Empathy has the potential to attenuate aggression in sport, a context in which individuals could be aggressive in their efforts to outperform others. Perspective taking may strengthen one’s cognitive ability to counteract the arousal that could lead to aggression, thereby attenuating aggression in athletes during competition. The inhibitory effect of empathy on aggression may be weaker in men compared to women, during competition. In addition, Minde (1992) supported the idea that the development of empathy and prosocial behavior helps decrease levels of overall aggression. He examined the developmental changes of aggression in preschool children. Results 28 showed that the aggressive children displayed a delay in empathic and perspective taking skills. These results support previous research and theories on the notion that empathy is a key factor in the development of prosocial behavior. Chalmers & Townsend (1990) supported the claim that perspective taking has received empirical support with its relationship to empathy and its effects on aggression. Girls who participated in a training program to increase their perspective taking skills also experienced a significant increase in their empathy skills, so perspective taking is likely related to empathy. Research on empathy trainings further supports these findings. After attending intervention programs on empathy-related learning participants behave less aggressively and show fewer social problems. As empathy training promotes the reduction of negative social behaviors to the benefit of prosocial interactions with others, (affective) empathy mediates social behaviors. The study of Sohravardi (2015) also revealed that empathy training caused reduction on aggression. The development of empathy with training programs has positive effects on reduction of aggression and increase compatibility in students at elementary schools. Empathy training will improve interpersonal skills as well as management skills and recognition of emotions. Eventually, the ability to understand and manage emotions will result in interpersonal skill improvement and become less aggressive. 29 From a review of research, Dodaj et al. (2012) summarized that empathy plays an important role in reducing violent behavior. The higher the level of empathy, the lower the tendency towards violent behavior. During testing of the multidimensional construct of empathy, it was found that both the cognitive and affective components of empathy reduce aggressive behavior. However, some studies have shown that only the affective component of empathy plays a significant role in reducing aggressive behavior. The results in children who are bullies show that the affective component of empathy has an important role in reducing empathy in relation to the cognitive component. Jolliffe and Farrington (2004) claimed that cognitive empathy to have a stronger relationship than affective empathy with some types of aggression, such as offending. The researchers emphasized that it is possible that bullies have cognitive empathy, but have reduced affective empathy. Probably the lack of affective empathy separated those who were prone to repeated, constant aggressive behavior. From the above, it can be said that it is necessary to establish a clear distinction between the cognitive and affective components of empathy. They found a significant positive relationship between empathy and antisocial behavior. Also, this correlation proved higher among adolescents and young adults compared to children. Their findings suggested that the association was stronger for cognitive than for affective empathy, weaker for adults than adolescents, and moderated by intelligence and socioeconomic status (SES). Empathic concern and 30 compassion probably encourage an individual to aggressive behavior because these individuals are emotionally able to assess and predict negative consequences that would have negative impact on other people. In agreement with Jolliffe and Farrington (2004), Freeman (1984) concluded that cognitive empathy plays a crucial role in the development of empathy. Results of the studies strongly indicated that “empathy is a multifaceted construct including both cognitive and affective dimensions.” Among preschoolers of both sexes, the cognitive expression of empathy seems to emerge prior to the affective one” Results from the 54 children indicated that cognitive empathy is easier for younger kids to express than affective empathy. To shed light on her findings, it is quite possible that the younger children were able to experience affective empathy, but lacked the verbal skills to express themselves clearly. On the other hand, Lovett and Sheffield (2007) found that affective empathy to have a stronger inverse relationship with aggression and bullying than cognitive empathy. They looked at affective empathy and found a negative relationship between affective empathy and aggression in adolescents, with the relationship being stronger when behavioral measures of empathy were used compared to questionnaire measures. Likewise, Kemp et al. (2007) found systematic negative associations between affective empathy and antisocial behavior, corroborating the results of Lovett and Sheffield (2007). 31 Meanwhile, Feshbach (1983) pointed out that the overall findings suggest that empathy plays a significant role in the control of aggression and argued that empathy hinders both types of aggression. The researchers found that the children who participated in the training program showed an increase in prosocial activity during the course of the training and also showed somewhat of a decrease in their tendency to act aggressively. He explains that the empathetic person is able to understand the other point of view and is less likely to become aggressive due to misinterpreting another’s behavior. Aggression causes pain and distress and the observation of pain and distress should elicit empathic responses even if the child is the initiator of the aggressive act. It would be consistent to say that the development of empathetic skills, both in the cognitive abilities to assume another person’s perspective and correctly emotionally identify with another person, would decrease aggressive behavior in children and even adults. Furthermore, Euler et al. (2014) revealed that empathy is involved in the inhibition of aggressive behavior with proactive motivational underpinnings, but not with reactive aggression. Results indicated that cognitive but not affective empathy was negatively associated with proactive aggression while cognitive and affective empathy were unrelated to reactive aggression. Proactive aggression is related to lower levels of empathy and with studies that show that bullying is associated with lower levels of empathy. Thus, a higher ability to understand the cause and reason of emotions in others seems to reduce the amount of aggression 32 that is instrumental, organized, and motivated by the anticipation of reward. Affective empathy facets do not seem to be associated with lower proactive aggression. This finding is contrary to a number of studies that indicated that affective empathy is negatively related to antisocial behavior. In a consistent research finding in empathy and aggression which also concerns gender differences, Strayer & Roberts (2004) indicated that the more empathic children were less physically and verbally aggressive and also engaged in more prosocial behaviors. Results showed that girls engaged in more prosocial behavior than boys, and boys engaged in more physical aggression than girls. Boys also exhibited more anger than girls. This study enhanced previous research and underlined the negative association for empathy with anger and aggression. However, the study of Spellings (2007) indicates that empathy training program implemented by the researcher had no significant effect on either the children’s behavior or their empathy levels. However, one must take note that although the results are not statistically significant; the significance levels were in the predicted direction. Synthesis Synthesis explained how the study is related to other existing studies conducted. This shows how a certain topic often used. This would serve as a 33 baseline of the present study of the proponents. It compared the similarity and differences of both studies and methodology done. The reviewed related literature and studies cited by Feshbach, Freeman, Chalmers & Townsend, Minde, Bohart & Greenberg, Strayer and Roberts, Scrimgeour, Stanger et al., and Sohravardi et al. are different to the present study in terms of the chosen group of participants. The present study chose adolescents who are victims and, at the same time, perpetrators of bullying. Unlike the studies of Scrimgeour, Bohart & Greenberg, Minde and Strayer and Roberts, they examined the preschoolers’ behavior to assess aggression. Meanwhile, Feshbach and her team of researchers designed an empathy training program for elementary school children. The program was a ten-week course during which the child performs role-taking and affect-identification exercises with an empathy “trainer” three times a week. The researchers developed thirty hours of lessons that included problem-solving games, tape recordings, and storytelling. Similarly, the study of Spellings explored the relationship between the development of empathy and aggression in fifteen (15) elementary age children in an empathy training program. There were twenty-three exercises ranging in difficulty from easy to more demanding. To assess each child’s empathy and aggression levels, the researcher used two scales. With these studies, the present research is different in terms of the participants, length of duration in conducting the experiment and the interventions used to elicit and develop empathy. The 34 present study only used video clips to induce empathy and used one scale to assess the participants’ behavior. Likewise, the study of Sohravardi et al. held fifteen (15) training course sessions that lasted for two months due to existing limitations. The purpose of this study was to evaluate the effect of empathy teaching on aggression and compatibility in female students of elementary sixth grade in Yazd, Center of Iran. In addition to differences in terms of participants, duration of time and place of the study, the samples were 62 girl students that were put in two groups of control and experimental group randomly and equally. Whereas similar to the present study, data were collected by using questionnaires. Further difference to the present study, Stanger et al. aimed to investigate the effect of empathy on aggression in athletes and to examine whether provocation and gender moderate and emotions mediate. Also, the researchers aim to discover whether empathy inhibits athletes’ aggression during a competitive motor task under conditions of low and high provocation and whether these effects are moderated by gender. Moreover, two methods and measures of aggression were used in the study: unprovoked (or proactive) aggression, which was operationalized as the shock intensity chosen by the participant on the first trial, that is, before receiving any shocks; and provoked (or reactive) aggression, which was operationalized as the shock intensity chosen on subsequent trials. While 35 sharing the same main objective to the present study, researches varied in participants, method of the experiment, and instrumentation. Additional difference to the present study, Strayer and Roberts examined young children’s peer-group behavior to assess empathy and prosocial behavior. Their interactions, behaviors, and emotions were observed and videotaped. In order to specifically assess levels of empathy, the children individually viewed six emotionally evocative videotaped vignettes. Empathy was also measured by teacher, parent, best friend, and self-ratings. Strayer and Roberts also used direct observation of play time, but also incorporated story vignettes to assess levels of empathy in preschool children. The present study only used questionnaires to assess the self-reported aggressive behavior of the participants and used two (2) video clips to induce empathy on bully-victims. In comparison to Jolliffe &Farrington, Kemp et al., Lovett and Sheffield, and Euler et al., the present study is somehow similar to the participants and datagathering instrument used. In the study of Kemp et al. questionnaires were used to measure both empathy and aggression of early adolescence. Moreover, Lovett and Sheffield investigated affective empathy and aggression in mostly clinical and forensic samples of adolescents through the use of questionnaires also. They found a negative relationship between affective empathy and aggression in adolescents. In addition, Jolliffe and Farrington focused on forensic samples of adolescents and adults with empathy being measured by questionnaires. 36 The study of Euler et al. aimed to investigate the association between cognitive and affective empathy, and reactive and proactive aggression in a highrisk sample of adolescent girls and boys. A total of 184 adolescents living in socio-educational and juvenile justice institutions between the age of 12 and 22 years participated in this study. Three questionnaires were administered. Otherreport assessment was conducted by the caseworker. Caseworkers are used to working with adolescents with disruptive behavior and are trained in the evaluation of behaviors relative to a certain age group. In similar to the present study, both genders were considered and both focused on the two aspects of empathy, which were used as the manipulation in the present study. The differences between the studies are the number of participants, age range in adolescents, and number of instruments used. In present study, the researchers limited the participants to 12 with their age ranging from 15-20 years old. Also, a researcher-structured questionnaire was the only instrument used to measure aggressive behavior of the participants. In connection on the two (2) aspects of empathy, Freeman studied the development of empathy in young children (between 43 months and 67 months) and also focused on the cognitive and affective aspects of empathy. Meanwhile, Chalmers & Townsend only focused on social perspective taking on socially maladjusted girls. On the other hand, Minde examined the developmental changes of aggression in preschool children. Two standardized tests (Richman Graham 37 Behavior Screening Questionnaire and the Preschool Behavior Questionnaire) were completed for behavioral assessment. Mother and child direct observations were conducted, and direct observations at school took place. In addition, Scrimgeour investigated correlations between empathy and relational and physical aggression in preschoolers. Aggression was measured by direct observation and a teacher questionnaire. Different from the present study, no observations were conducted. The experimental study was done online due to limitations. Conceptual Framework This conceptual framework explains the aim of the research which is to determine the effect of empathy induction on aggressive behavior of bully-victims. The conceptual paradigm is illustrated below. The conceptual model of the study consists mainly of the input, process and output. The input of the modelconsists of the manipulations used in the experiment. Interventions conducted on experimental group were a 3-minutevideo clipinducing a cognitive empathy and a 9-minute animated short film inducing affective empathy. The process composed of the pre-test and post-test aimed to determine the significant effect of empathy induction on the aggressive behavior of bullyvictims. Meanwhile in selecting participants, the researchers used Bully-Victim Survey Form to assess their bullying experience. The researchers used a true 38 experimental design wherein the comparison of pre-test and post-test results was made. The instrument used in measuring the participants’ behavior is the 20-item Aggression Scale. Lastly, the output represents the implications of the result of the study and the proposed intervention and recommended activity that will reduce the aggressive behavior of the respondents. The expected output will be based on the result of the study. The researchers should come up with effective programs that’ll help people decrease and manage aggressive and violent behavior. The figure shows the research paradigm of the study: Figure 1 Conceptual Paradigm on The Effect of Empathy Induction on Aggressive Behavior of Bully-Victim INPUT PROCESS Independent Variable: Dependent Variable: Empathy INPUTInduction Aggressive Behavior PROCESS 1st level : Type of Inducing Films (Cognitive and Pre-test and Post Tests (Aggression Scale) Affective) 2nd level : Duration of films ( 1-5mins.&6-10mins. ) 1.1 Control Group 1.2 Experimental Group OUTPUT OUTPUT Intervention Program: “Empathy Activating Tasks” 39 Hypothesis of the Study The research hypothesis was presented in null form. Based on the nature of the study, the null hypothesis is made to test whether the result of the findings can be rejected or accepted. The null hypothesis stated that empathy induction has no significant effect on the aggressive behavior of bully-victims. Thus, the researchers want to disprove the claim through the creation of this study. Definition of Terms For better understanding of the study, the researchers felt it necessary to define the following terms conceptually and operationally. Aggressive Behavior. It can be conceptualized as the observable manifestation of aggression, which is defined as the act to cause harm, pain, injury in another. (Zirpoli, 2008) In this study, it is the dependent variable measured on bully-victims through questionnaire. Empathy. This is a complex and multidimensional phenomenon that includes the ability to notice, feel, and automatically respond to other people and, on the other, to understand their emotional states. (Hoffman, 2000) It is assumed to inhibit antisocial and aggressive behavior in this study. Empathy Induction. Empathy is normally induced by seeing, believing, or imagining that the person is experiencing an emotion or is in an emotion-inducing 40 situation. Maibom (2012) It is the independent variable in this study designed to elicit empathic feelings through video clips. Affective Empathy. It refers to an affective connection with another person's emotional state, and closely relates to another oriented reaction regarding the perceived welfare of other person (Batson et al., 1997). Affective empathy is elicited through a video clip which appeals to emotions. Cognitive Empathy. It is the capacity to take the mental perspective or mentally putting oneself in another person’s shoes in order to understand their thoughts and feelings. (Beadle & de la Vega, 2019). In this study, cognitive empathy is induced through a perspective-taking video clip. Adolescents. It is a phase of maturation: it is a transitional period of physical and psychological human development between childhood and adulthood, the cultural purpose of which involves preparation to assume adult roles. (iFightDepression, 2020) It refers to the age of participants included in the study ranging from 15-20 years old. Bully-Victims. Bully-victims are both a perpetrator and victim that experience worse outcomes than either bullies or victims, being at greater risk for anxiety, depression, low self-esteem, self harm, suicidal ideation, aggression, and delinquency. (Berkowitz & Benbenishty, 2012) They are the target respondents of the researchers who are both bullies and victims. 41 CHAPTER III RESEARCH METHODOLOGY This chapter presents the research methodology and procedures used by the researchers in the study. This chapter contains the method of research to be used, the respondents of the study, the instrument to be used, the administration of the instrument and the statistical treatment for the data that had been collected. Moreover, this chapter gives a detailed description of the experimental system and procedures used in data acquisition and data processing throughout this experiment. Research Design The main purpose of this study is to determine whether empathy induction had an effect on aggressive behavior of bully-victims. The participants’ behavior will be measured and compared so that the researchers can test the hypothesis about the effect of inductions on behavior. The researchers used true experimental design. Williams (2015) stated that a true experiment is a type of experimental design that is thought to be the most accurate type of experimental research because it supports or refutes a hypothesis using statistical analysis. The comparison of pre-test and post-test results was made in the experimental and control group of the experiment. The study will use quantitative research methods 42 through Aggression Scale, a researchers-structured questionnaire, as main data gathering instrument. Subjects of the Study The researchers aim to establish a study that will determine how aggressive behavior can be manifested by bully-victims, ages 15-20 years old.The researchers chose bully-victims or adolescents that are both bullies and victims as their respondents because they are more prone to emotional instability and aggression since they receive such an awful treatment among their friends or classmates. Sigurdson et al. (2015) proved that it is common among adolescents being involved in bullying. Prevalence rates of being victims of bullying globally from 6 to 35% and bullying others from 6 to 32% whereas a smaller group, from 1.6 to 13% has experience both as a bully and victim (bully-victim). The researchers used Purposive Sampling also known as judgemental, selective, or subjective sampling in which researchers rely on their own judgment when choosing members of the population to participate in their study. (Foley, 2018). With this, the researchers chose the respondents through a survey that will determine their experiences in bullying. They chose twelve (12) participants online which consist of six (6) men and six (6) women as the respondents in the study. 43 Data Gathering Instrument Questionnaires were utilized as the researchers’ main data gathering instrument. In selecting the respondents, the researchers used Bully-Victim Survey Form which assesses bullying and victimization experiences based on self-report. Moreover, the researchers used a 20-item Aggression Scale to assess the aggressive behavior of bully-victims. Aggression Scale is used to measure frequency of self-reported aggressive behaviors. It is a researcher-structured questionnaire, approved by our adviser in research, adapted from Aggression Scale of Orpinas & Frankowski (2001). Point values are assigned. All items are scored on the following 4-point scale. Responses of 3 “Very often”, 2 “Often”, 1 “Sometimes” and 0 “Never” were recoded. All scores are summed to derive a total. Higher scores indicate higher frequencies of aggressive behavior. Data Gathering Procedures The researchers first conducted an online survey through Bully-Victim Survey Form, assessing bullying and victimization experiences, to determine and verify the participants in the study. The questionnaires will be created and administered through Google Forms. After the survey, the researchers secured the respondents’ availability of internet connection for the experimental study to be possible. After seeking their approval, they were informed about the experimental research and assured the confidentiality of the information disclosed. 44 During the actual experiment, in the pre-test activity of both experimental and control group, the researchers will proceed on administering a 20-item Aggression Scale to measure the frequency of self-reported aggressive behaviors of the participants. After the respondents finished answering the pre-test, the experimental group will then be divided into two (2) groups for the manipulation. The researchers will use two (2) kinds of empathy inducing clips with different durations that might affect the aggressive behavior of the respondents. For the first (1 st) experimental group’s manipulation, a 3-minute cognitive empathy inducing clip entitled “Why I Bully You” will be shown. It is a short video clip showing reasons of bullying from different perspectives. Likewise, a 9-minute animated short film which appeals to emotions and encourages in constructing a more inclusive world, will be presented to activate the affective empathic feelings of the second (2 nd) experimental group. The link of the videos will be sent after the respondents finished answering the pre-test. After watching the video clips, the researchers will request the participants to message them for assurance. After (5) five-day interval on the pre-test, the researchers administered the post test to control and experimental group to further assess the aggressive behavior of the respondents on the past days and also to find out if there is a significant difference between the pre-test and post-test results of the participants after the experimental group’s intervention. The results of the questionnaires and 45 activities done were tallied, prepared in a tabular form, presented and analyzed in the text applying the appropriate statistical treatment. Analysis of Data The statistical treatments employed in this study were as follows: Mean. It was used to determine the pre-test and post test result of the control and experimental groups. Independent-Sample T-test. It was used to compare the mean scores of experimental and control groups’ pre-test and post test to test their difference. Paired-Sample T-Test. It was used to test the difference between the pretest and post test results of the means of control and experimental group. 46 CHAPTER IV PRESENTATION, ANALYSIS AND INTERPRETATION OF DATA This chapter manifests data gathered from the survey through questionnaires given to the respondents. They were presented in tabular form, analyzed, interpreted and complemented with inferences, implications and theoretical bases. The figures and interpretation presented represents only the data from where the researchers conducted their study. 1. Pre-Test Results of the Participants Pre-test were given to control and experimental groups to determine their aggressive behavior before the experiment. The test was composed of twenty (20) items to measure the frequency of self-reported aggressive behaviors. Table 1 presents the results of both control and experimental groups during their pre-test. It shows the mean scores of the participants followed by its corresponding interpretation. Table 1 Pre-Test Results of Control and Experimental Group Mean Result 1.08 SD 0.44 Interpretation Low Aggressive Behavior Experimental Group 1 1.18 0.54 Low Aggressive Behavior Experimental Group 2 1.35 0.78 Low Aggressive Behavior Control Group 47 As shown in the table, the control group got a mean score of 1.08 with a 0.44 standard deviation indicating a low aggressive behavior. Moreover, the mean score of experimental group 1 is 1.18 with a 0.54 standard deviation while experimental group 2 got a mean score of 1.35 and a 0.78 standard deviation that both indicate low aggressive behavior. Overall, the result implied that the participants in this study have low aggressive behavior. It means that there is a presence of aggression among the participants however, it was low. According to Sourander (2011), some kids occupy the middle of the bullying food chain. They get bullied by dominant individuals but they also perpetrate bullying themselves. Rejected, victimized, and aggressive, these “bullyvictims” tend to have more psychological problems than either “pure bullies” or “pure victims”. Both type may suffer from psychiatric disorders, and both are at increased risk for committing severe, violent offenses as they got older. Moreover, in some cases, kids who are bullied are also bullies themselves. Kochel (2015) stated that bully-victims demonstrate many of the same behaviors as do bullies and victims. A typical victim is likely to be aggressive, think negative thoughts and has a trouble with social interaction. The very sad part comes later, when they reach adulthood and experience long lasting psychological effects that are more severe than that experienced by either bullies or victims alone. 48 2. Post Test Results of the Participants The same test was administered to both control and experimental groups after the latter’s intervention. This test will determine if the intervention affects the behavior of the participants. Table 2 presents the results of both control and experimental groups during their post test. It shows the mean scores of the participants followed by its corresponding interpretation. Table 2 Post Test Results of Control and Experimental Group Mean Result SD Interpretation Control Group 0.96 0.39 Low Aggressive Behavior Experimental Group 1 0.43 0.34 Very Low Aggressive Behavior Experimental Group 2 0.88 0.54 Low Aggressive Behavior The above table revealed the post test result of both groups. The mean score of the control group is 0.96 with a standard deviation of 0.39 that indicates low aggressive behvior. Meanwhile, the experimental group 1 got a mean score of 0.43 with a 0.34 standard deviation indicating very low aggressive behavior. Moreover, the experimental group 2 got a mean score of 0.88 and a 0.54 standard deviation indicating low aggressive behavior. 49 The mean scores in post test revealed that the experimental group 1 (M = 0.43) decreased aggressive behavior more compared to experimental group 2 (M = 0.88) and control group (M = 0.96). The result implied that the aggressive behavior of the participants in experimental group 1 decreased from low (M = 1.18, SD = 0.54) to very low (M = 0.43, SD = 0.34) aggressive behavior. The result indicates that the intervention in experimental group 1 (cognitive empathy induction) is more effective than experimental group 2 (affective empathy induction). Results showed that a short cognitive empathy inducing film is more effective than a long affective empathy inducing film in reducing aggression. Jolliffe and Farrington (2004) claimed cognitive empathy to have a stronger relationship than affective empathy with some types of aggression, such as offending. The researchers emphasized that it is possible that bullies have cognitive empathy, but have reduced affective empathy. Probably the lack of affective empathy separated those who were prone to repeated, constant aggressive behavior. They found a significant positive relationship between empathy and antisocial behavior. Their findings suggested that the association was stronger for cognitive than for affective empathy, weaker for adults than adolescents, and moderated by intelligence and socioeconomic status (SES). In agreement with Jolliffe and Farrington (2004), Freeman (1984) concluded that cognitive empathy plays a crucial role in the development of empathy. Results of the studies strongly indicated that “empathy is a multifaceted construct 50 including both cognitive and affective dimensions.” Among preschoolers of both sexes, the cognitive expression of empathy seems to emerge prior to the affective one” Results from the 54 children indicated that cognitive empathy is easier for younger kids to express than affective empathy. Furthermore, Euler et al. (2014) indicated that cognitive but not affective empathy was negatively associated with proactive aggression and concluded that a higher ability to understand the cause and reason of emotions in others seems to reduce the amount of aggression that is instrumental, organized, and motivated by the anticipation of reward. In addition, Philippot (2010) noted that film clips have the ability to employ particularly complex stimuli in a short amount of time. Maffei (2019) also stated that shorter clips engage the attention of the viewer and making possible the process of “willing suspension of disbelief” which is critical for the ecology of the emotional experience achieved through film watching. Using longer clips (i.e. three minutes or more) may increase the difficulty to disengage from the stimulus, thus prompting carryover effects on the following excerpts within the experimental paradigm. 3. Significant Difference between the Pre-test Results of the Participants The researchers compared the mean scores of experimental and control groups’ pre-test through independent-sample t-test to find out if there is a significant difference between the pre-test results of the participants. 51 Table 3 presents the summary of computations to compare the pre-test results of the participants followed by its corresponding interpretation. Table 3 Summary of Computations on Test of Difference Between the Pre-test Results of the Control and Experimental Group Mean Control Group Experimental Group Computed Values P Value Decision Conclusion -0.98 0.166 Failed to Reject H0 Not Significant 1.08 1.24 DR: Reject H0 if p value is lesser than = 0.05. Table 3 disclosed the comparison between pre-test results of the participants. The researchers generated a probability value of 0.166 that is greater than alpha () at 0.05. Due to the result, the researchers failed to reject the null hypothesis. Therefore, there is no significant difference between the pre-test results of the participants. 4. Significant Difference between the Post Test Results of the Participants Using the independent sample t-test, the mean scores of the participants in their post test were evaluated to find out if there is a significant difference or not. Table 4 presents the summary of computations to compare the post test results of the participants followed by its corresponding interpretation. 52 Table 4 Summary of Computations on Test of Difference Between the Post Test Results of the Control and Experimental Group Mean Control Group Computed P Value Values Conclusion Reject H0 Significant 0.96 3.08 Experimental Group Decision 0.002 0.59 DR: Reject H0 if p value is lesser than = 0.05. In the above table, it revealed that since P value, which is 0.002, is lesser than 0.05, the null hypothesis is rejected at 5% level of significance. Hence, it is concluded that there is a significant difference between the post test results of control and experimental groups. Table 3 showed that the mean scores in experimental group became lower after the interventions compared to the control group. The table above implied that interventions had an effect on participants’ behavior. It means that empathy inductions lessen the aggressive behavior of the participants. Minde (1992) supported the idea that the development of empathy and prosocial behavior helps decrease levels of overall aggression. Also, the study of Scrimgeour (2007) entitled “Empathy and Aggression: A Study of the Interplay Between Empathy and Aggression in Preschoolers” found that high levels of empathy were significantly correlated with low levels of aggression. Conceivably, 53 the development of empathy reduced the presence of aggressive behavior in participants. These findings underline the importance of the development of empathy and prosocial behavior and their ability to inhibit aggressive acts towards others. He also concluded that if more children were encouraged to explore their feelings and to understand the feelings of their peers, then perhaps statistics of school violence could be reduced and school shootings could be a phenomenon of the past. In the study of Stanger et al. (2015), findings proved that empathy reduced participants’ aggression. Empathy has the potential to attenuate aggression in sport, a context in which individuals could be aggressive in their efforts to outperform others. Perspective-taking may strengthen one’s cognitive ability to counteract the arousal that could lead to aggression. 5. Significant Difference between the Pre-test and Post Test Rresults of the Participants To determine the significant difference of the pre-test and post test results of control and experimental group, the researchers used the paired-sample t-test to know if the intervention has an effect on the behavior of the participants. Table 5 presents the summary of computations to compare the pre- test and post test results of the participants followed by its corresponding interpretation. 54 Table 5 Summary of Computations on Test of Difference Between Pre-Test and Post Test Results Mean Pre-test Post test 0.96 Pre-test 1.24 Post test P Value Decision Conclusion 0.75 0.230 Failed to Reject H0 Not Significant 3.42 0.001 Reject H0 Significant 1.08 Control Group Experimental Group Computed Values 0.59 DR: Reject H0 if p value is lesser than = 0.05. Table 5 revealed the comparison between pre-test and post test results of the participants. In control group, the researchers generated a probability value of 0.230 that is greater than alpha () at 0.05. Due to the result, the researchers failed to reject the null hypothesis. This indicates that there is no significant difference between the pre-test and post test results of the control group. Although the mean score of control group decreased after the intervention (M = 0.96), it is not enough to claim that there is a significant difference between the pre-test and post test results of the control group (p > 0.05). Further analysis, the mean scores of experimental group on both pre-test and post test were compared. In experimental group, the researchers generated a probability value of 0.001 that is lesser than the alpha level () at 0.05. The test 55 was found to be significant, indicating that the aggressive behavior before (M = 1.24) was significantly different than the aggressive behavior after the intervention (M = 0.59). Hence, it is concluded that there is a significant difference on the aggressive behavior of participants before and after the intervention. The result indicates that empathy induction implemented by the researcher has a significant effect on the aggressive behavior of the participants. From a review of research, Dodaj et al. (2012) summarized that empathy plays an important role in reducing violent behavior. The higher the level of empathy, the lower the tendency towards violent behavior. During testing of the multidimensional construct of empathy, it was found that both the cognitive and affective components of empathy reduce aggressive behavior. Moreover, Feshbach (1983) pointed out that the overall findings suggest that empathy plays a significant role in the control of aggression and argued that empathy hinders both types of aggression. The researchers found that the children who participated in the training program showed an increase in prosocial activity during the course of the training and also showed somewhat of a decrease in their tendency to act aggressively. He explains that the empathetic person is able to understand the other point of view and is less likely to become aggressive due to misinterpreting another’s behavior. Aggression causes pain and distress and the observation of pain and distress should elicit empathic responses even if the child is the initiator of the aggressive act. It would be consistent to say that the 56 development of empathetic skills, both in the cognitive abilities to assume another person’s perspective and correctly emotionally identify with another person, would decrease aggressive behavior in children and even adults. Furthermore, Bohart & Greenberg (1997) cited that empathy serves as a learned lesson and as an inhibitor of further aggressive acts to a child who vicariously experienced pain through another child’s experience. As children advance cognitively, the more likely they are able to understand and control their emotions as well as take another’s perspective. This cognitive achievement allows for a reduction in potential aggressive conflicts and welcomes the probable occurrence of prosocial behaviors. He supported the notion that children from a young age experience empathy and that their empathic skills develop with age. Meanwhile, to support the effectiveness of empathy induction, Chalmers & Townsend (1990) supported the claim that perspective-taking has received empirical support with its relationship to empathy and its effects on aggression. Research on empathy trainings further supports these findings. After attending intervention programs on empathy-related learning participants behave less aggressively and show fewer social problems. As empathy training promotes the reduction of negative social behaviors to the benefit of prosocial interactions with others, (affective) empathy mediates social behaviors. 57 CHAPTER V SUMMARY, CONCLUSION AND RECOMMENDATION This chapter contains the summary of findings based on the result of the analysis and interpretation of the data gathered from the respondents that leads to establishment of conclusion and recommendation. Summary This study was conducted to determine the effect of empathy induction on aggressive behavior of bully-victims. Purposive sampling was used in choosing 12 participants’ ages 15-20 years old. Researchers used true experimental design with pre-test and post test design. Intervention was given to experimental group and “Aggression Scale” was administered to both groups to assess their behavior. Specifically, it answered to the following questions: 1. What are the pre-test results of the participants? 2. What are the post-test results of the participants? 3. Is there a significant difference between the pre-test results of the participants? 4. Is there a significant difference between the post test results of the participants? 5. Is there a significant difference between the pre-test and post-results of the participants? 6. What are the proposed intervention and activities based on the findings? 58 Findings After the analysis and the interpretation of the data, the researchers came up with the following findings: 1. Pre-Test Results of the Participants The result of the pre-test showed that participants in this study have low aggressive behavior wherein there is a presence of aggression among the participants however, it was low.The control group got a mean score of 1.08. Experimental group 1 got 1.18 while experimental group 2 got a mean score of 1.35 that all indicate low aggressive behavior. 2. Post Test Results of the Participants Post test results revealed that participants in this study still scored low in aggression; however experimental group 1 with a mean score of 0.43 decreased to very low aggressive behavior compared to experimental group 2 with a mean score of 0.88 and control group with 0.96 mean score indicating the greater effectiveness of the 1st intervention. 3. Significant Difference Between the Pre-Test Results of the Participants The comparison between pre-test results of the participants generated a probability value of 0.166 that is greater than alpha () at 0.05. Due to the result, the researchers failed to reject the null hypothesis. 59 4. Significant Difference Between the Post Test Results of the Participants The result in testing the difference between the post test of the participants showed that the mean scores in experimental group became lower after the interventions compared to the control group. It revealed that since P value, which is 0.002, is lesser than 0.05, the null hypothesis is rejected at 5% level of significance. 5. Significant Difference Between the Pre-Test and Post Test Results Of The Participants The comparison between pre-test and post test results of the participants generated a probability value of 0.230, in control group, that is greater than alpha () at 0.05. Due to the result, the researchers failed to reject the null hypothesis. Meanwhile, the researchers got a probability value that is lesser than the alpha level in experimental group which means the null hypothesis should be rejected. Conclusion Based on the findings, the following hereby concluded: 1. Victims of bullying have tendencies to be aggressive. 2. A short cognitive empathy inducing film is more effective than a long affective empathy inducing film in reducing aggression. 3. There is no significant difference between the pre-test results of the participants. 60 4. There is a significant difference between the post test results of control and experimental groups. Empathy inductions reduced the aggressive behavior of the participants. 5. Although the mean score of control group decreased after the intervention, it is not enough to claim that there is a significant difference between the pre-test and post test results of the control group. On the other hand, it is concluded that there is a significant difference on aggressive behavior of participants before and after experimental groups’ intervention. The result indicates that empathy induction implemented by the researcher has a significant effect on the aggressive behavior of participants. 6. The proposed intervention based on the findings are activities that will be helpful in recognizing and managing emotions as well as developing concern for others to reduce aggression. The suggested activities are the following: 61 Table 6 Proposed Program and Activities “Empathy Activating Task” EMOvies: Empathy Movies Crystals of Kaydor: Video Game If The Shoe Fits: A Group Activity Objective Brief Description EMOvies is a series of emotional films that’ll elicit emotions and compassion “To induce an affective among the viewers. This will state and foster be helpful in developing reflection.” empathic sharing or sharing of another’s emotional experience. It is a story-based game in “To activate and training empathy. It is an instrengthen perspectivegame training tool in which taking behavior and players gauge the intensity of encourage to practice emotions. Players will learn to emotion recognition recognize six basic emotions and empathic (anger, fear, happiness, responding” surprise, disgust and sadness). “To spend time understanding and feeling another person’s perspective and experiences.” A set of printed shoe prints with different conditions of living are laid and whoever it might fit, they will try to live and act as if they were on those situations for a day. Recommendation Based on the conclusion drawn, the following recommendations are hereby presented. 1. The researchers recommend the use of empathy induction in reducing and regulating aggressive behavior among individuals. 62 2. It is recommended to continue this study on different groups and ages of participants who are more likely to be aggressive like adult violent offenders. 3. A larger number or a more diverse participant sample should also be considered to provide more accurate and reliable results. 4. Due to time constraints, the researchers limited the duration of the experiment for only 5 days. Future studies should be conducted for a long period of time to observe long-term effects. 5. Future researchers should utilize different variety of methods to elicit empathy like the use of emotional imagery, presentation of visual stimuli (slides and videos), presentation of sound or music, recollection of autobiographical memories other than film clips. 6. In this study, the intervention and administration of questionnaires were conducted online due to several restrictions brought by the pandemic. It is suggested that future researchers should conduct face-to-face research methods to avoid uncertain and inaccurate result, and eliminate biases. 7. Similar research should be done with other instrument and assessment measures other than self-report. Future studies should also include observer reported behavior and peer reports. 8. It would also be of interest to determine other interventions except empathy elicitation that will decrease aggressive behaviour. 63 BIBLIOGRAPHY A. ELECTRONIC RESOURCES Beadle, J.& de la Vega, C. (2019) Impact of Aging on Empathy: Review of Psychological and Neural Mechanisms. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00331 Benedict, F.T, Vivier, P.M., & Gjelsvik, A. (2015). Mental health and bullying in the United States among children aged 6 to 17. Journal of Interpersonal Violence. Retrieved from https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/abs/10.1177/0886260514536279 Berkowitz, R., & Benbenishty, R. (2012). Perceptions of teachers’ support, safety, and absence from school because of fear among victims, bullies, and bullyvictims. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry. Retrieved from https:// www.apa.org/pubs/journals/releases/amp-a0038929.pdf Björkqvist, K., Lagerspetz, K. M. J., and Kaukiainen, A. (1992). Do girls manipulate and boys fight? Developmental trends in regard to direct and indirect aggression. Aggress. Behav. Retrieved from https://www.frontier sin.org/articles/10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00081/full Bohart, A.C., & Greenberg, L.S. (1997). Empathy reconsidered: New direction in psychotherapy. Washington, D.C.: American PsychologicalAssociation. Retrieved from https://digitalrepository.wheatoncollege.edu/bitstream/handle/1 1040/788/07_Scrimgeour.pdf?sequence=1 64 Buss, A.H. (1961). The psychology of aggression [Adobe Digital Editions version].doi:10.1037/11160-000. Retrieved from https://digitalcommons.unl.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=1053&context=c ehsdiss Chalmers, J. B., & Townsend, M. A. R. (1990). The effects of training in socialperspective taking on socially maladjusted girls. ChildDevelopment. Retrieved fromhttps://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/08862605119879229 Cherry, K.(2020) Factors that Lead to Aggression. https://www.verywellmind.com/what-is-aggression-2794818 Retrieved from Crick, N., & Grotpeter, J. (1995). Relational aggression, gender, and socialpsychological adjustment. Child Development. Psychological Bulletin. Retrieved from https://digitalcommons.brockport.edu/cgi/viewcontent. cgi? article=1043&context=edc_theses Crothers, L.M. et.al (2014). Proactive and Reactive Aggression and Cognitive and Affective Empathy among Students in Middle Childhood. InternationalJournal of School and Cognitive Psychology. Retrieved from https://www.longdom.org/open-access/proactive-and-reactive-aggressionand-cognitive-and-affective-empathy-among-students-in-middle-childhood2469-9837.1000105.pdf Dodaj, A., et.al. (2012). THE EFFECT OF EMPATHY ON INVOLVING IN BULLYING BEHAVIOR. Retrieved from https://pdfs.semanticscholar.org/8da0/c20c87b1d1c7c5b4f6dac0e0edf430e2e 4e3.pdf 65 Dodge, K. A., & Coie, J. D. (1987). Social-information processing factors in reactive and proactive aggression in children’s peer groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. Retrieved from https://core.ac.uk /download/pdf/81837689.pdf Eagle, M. & Wolitzky, D. (1997). Empathy: A psychoanalytic perspective. In A.C. Bohart & L.S. Greenberg (Eds.), Empathy Reconsidered: New Directions in Psychotherapy. Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association. Retrieved fromhttps://pdfs.semanticscholar.org /49d2/2500d6a963857cb32 da0e9b7f969f4d7d063.pdf Euler, F., Steinlin, C., Stadler, C. (2014) Cognitive and Affective Empathy: Associations with Aggressive and Prosocial Behavior. Retrieved from https://link.springer.com/article/10.1186/s13034-016-0141-4 Ferris CF, Grisso T. (1996) Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. Vol. 794. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences; New York: 1996. Understanding aggressive behavior in children. Retrieved from https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC1570125/ Feshbach, N. (1983). Learning to care: a positive approach to child training and discipline. Retrieved from https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/15374418309533142 Foley, B. (2018). Purposive Sampling 101. Surveygizmo. Retrieved from https://www.surveygizmo.com/resources/blog/purposive-sampling-101/ 66 Freeman, E.B. (1984). The development of empathy in young children: In search of a definition. Child Study Journal .Retrieved from https://psycnet.apa.org/record/1984-17457-001 Galen, B.R., & Underwood, M.K. (1997). A developmental investigation of social aggression among children .Developmental Psychology.Retrieved from https://www.researchgate.net/publication/7677457_An_Integrated_Review_o f_Indirect_Relational_and_Social_Aggression Gallese, V., & Goldman, A. (1998). Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind reading. Retrieved from https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21227300 Goldstein, A.P. & Michaels, G.Y. (1985). Empathy: Development, training, and consequences. Hillsdale, N.J. : L. Erlbaum Associates.Retrieved from https://etd.ohiolink.edu/!etd.send_file?accession=bgsu1180535095&dispositi on=inline Gopnik, A., & Meltzoff, A. N. (1998). Words, thoughts, and theories. Retrieved from https://mitpress.mit.edu/books/words-thoughts-and-theories Grodal, T. & Kramer, M. (2010). Empathy, Film, and the Brain. Recherches sémiotiques / Semiotic Inquiry. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.7202/1025921ar Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Rapson, R. L. (1994). Emotional contagion. Cambridge, United Kingdom: Cambridge University Press.Retrieved from https://dialnet.unirioja.es/descarga/articulo/5216092.pdf 67 Hay, D.F. (2005). The beginnings of aggression in infancy. Retrieved from https://psycnet.apa.org/record/2005-09268-006 Hoffman M. L. (2000). Empathy and Moral Development. Implication for Caring and Justice. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Retrieved from https://www.ncbi. nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5988850/ Hogan R (1969) Development of an empathy scale. J Consult Clin Psychol. Retrieved from https://www.longdom.org/open-access/proactive-andreactive-aggression-and-cognitive-and-affective-empathy-among-students-inmiddle-childhood-2469-9837.1000105.pdf Howard, A. L. (2014). Elicitation of Empathic Emotions Using Film: Development of a Stimulus Set. Retrieved from https://pdfs.semanticscholar.org/8f54/27d36afd825be57f8ddbd2aed7aed285e 056.pdf Ickes, W. (1997). Empathic accuracy. New York: Guilford Press. Retrieved from https://www.researchgate.net/publication/272202633_Empathy_A_ Review_of_the_Concept Jolliffe D, Farrington DP. (2004) Empathy and offending: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Aggression and Violent behavior. Retrieved from https://www.ncbi. nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3017348/ Kemp, R. A. T. de, Overbeek, G., Wied, M. de, Engels, R. C. M. E., & Scholte, R. H. J. (2007). Early adolescent empathy, parental support, and antisocial behavior. The Journal of GeneticPsychology. Retrieved from https://dspace. 68 library.uu.nl/bitstream/handle/1874/188513/masterthesisheerebeek,ecmvan3111342.pdf?sequence=1 Killen, M., & Smetana, J. G. (Eds.) (2006). Handbook of moral development. London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Retrieved from https://books.google.com.ph/books?id=PTVHAQAAQBAJ&pg=PA184&lpg =PA184&dq=Killen+and+Smetana+(2006)+defined+empathy+as+%E2%80 %9Can+affective+response+that+stems+from+the+apprehension+or+compre hension+of+another%E2%80%99s+emotional+state+or+condition+and+is+s imilar+to+what+the+other+person+is+feeling+or+would+be+expected+to+fe el+in+the+given+situation.%E2%80%9D&source=bl&ots=mfxdi_5k50&sig =ACfU3U20si_7p3wLZErWuCey6c7TyOr2Yg&hl=en&sa=X&ved=2ahUK EwiU75Wc9tjpAhVBy4sBHSlSBQsQ6AEwAHoECAMQAQ Kochel KP, Ladd GW, Bagwell CL, Yabko BA. 2015. Bully/Victim Profiles' Differential Risk for Worsening Peer Acceptance: The Role of Friendship. J Appl Dev Psychol. Retrieved from https://www.parenting science.com/bullyvictims.html Kochenderfer-Ladd, B., & Ladd, G.W. (2001). Variations in peer victimization. In J.Juvonen & S. Graham (Eds.), Peer harassment in school: The plight of thevulnerable and victimized. New York: Guilford. Retrieved from https://digitalcommons.unl.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=1053&context=c ehsdiss Krug, E. et. al,. (2202) World Health Organization. World report on violence and health.Geneva. Retrieved from https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3411865/ Lagerspetz KM, Bjorkvist K, Peltonen T. Is indirect aggression typical of females? Gender differences in aggressiveness in 11- to 12-year-old children. AggressiveBehavior. Retrieved from https://www.ncbi.nlm. nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3736589/ 69 Loeber, R., & Hay, D. (1997). Key issues in the development of aggression and violence from childhood to early adulthood. Annual Review of Psychology. Retrieved from https://www.counseling.org/resources/library/Selected%20Topics/Bullying/B ullying_%20Early_Adolescence.html Lovett, B. J., & Sheffield, R. A. (2007). Affective empathy deficits inaggressive children and adolescents: A critical review. ClinicalPsychology Review. Retrievedfrom https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/088260519879229 Maffei A, Angrilli A (2019) E-MOVIE Experimental MOViesfor Induction of Emotions in neuroscience: An innovative film database with normative data and sex differences. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1371/journal. pone.0223124 Maibom, H.L., (2012). THE MANY FACES OF EMPATHY AND THEIR RELATION TO PROSOCIAL ACTION AND AGGRESSION INHIBITION. Retrieved from https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26301398 Minde, K. (1992). Aggression in preschoolers: Its relation to socialization. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. Retrieved from https://books.google.com.ph/books?id=5V66-WRcGL8C&pg=PA206& dq=Minde,+K.+(1992).+Aggression+in+preschoolers:+Its+relation+to+social ization.+Journal+of+the+American+Academy+of+Child+and+Adolescent+P sychiatry&hl=en&sa=X&ved=0ahUKEwiwq42Dz9jpAhXPFogKHVHJAEU Q6AEIKDAA#v=onepage&q=Minde%2C%20K.%20(1992).%20Aggression %20in%20preschoolers%3A%20Its%20relation%20to%20socialization.%20 Journal%20of%20the%20American%20Academy%20of%20Child%20and% 20Adolescent%20Psychiatry&f=false 70 Nansel, R.R., Overpeck, M., Pilla, R.S., Ruan, W.J., Simon-Morton, B., & Scheidt, P. (2001). Bullying behaviors among U.S. youth: Prevalence and association with psychosocial adjustment. Journal of the American Medical Association. Retrieved from https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles /PMC3766526/ Olweus, D. (2001). “Peer harassment: A critical analysis and some important questions,” in J. Juvonen & S. Graham (eds.), Peer harassment inschool: The plight of the vulnerable and victimized. New York,NY: Guilford Press. Retrieved fromhttps://digitalcommons.unl.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=1174&conte xt=edpsychpapers Parke, R. & Slaby, R. (1983). The development of aggression. In P. Mussen & E. Hetherington (Eds.), Handbook of Child Psychology: Vol. 4. Socialization, personality, and social development.Retrieved from https://etd.ohiolink.edu /!etd.send_file?accession=osu1312893747&disposition=inline Pepler, D., Craig, W., & O’Connell, P. (2010). Peer processes in bullying:Informing prevention and intervention strategies. In S. R. Jimerson,S. M. Swearer, & D. L. Espelage (Eds.), Handbook of bullying inschools: An international perspective. New York, NY:Routledge. Retrieved from https://www.apa.org/pubs/journals/releases/amp-a0038929.pdf Preston, S. D., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2002). Empathy: Its ultimate and proximate bases. Retrieved from https://www.cambridge.org/core/journals/behavioraland-brain-sciences/article/empathy-its-ultimate-and-proximatebases/953E0D092176FEE351ED81E933FE646D 71 PREVNet (2019) Adolescents. Promoting Relationships & Eliminating Violence. Canada's Healthy Relationships Hub: Preventing Interpersonal Violence through Research and Practice. Retrieved from https://www.prevnet.ca/bullying/parents/parents-of-adolescents Scrimgeour, M. (2007). Empathy and Aggression: A Study of the Interplay Between Empathy and Aggression in Preschoolers. Retrieved from https://books.google.com.ph/books?id=q7RBQAAQBAJ&pg=PT82&lpg=PT82&dq=Scrimgeour,+M.+(2007).+Empath y+and+Aggression:+A+Study+of+the+Interplay+Between+Empathy+and+A ggression+in+Preschoolers&source=bl&ots=t3fEHL_xm&sig=ACfU3U097vhOWyTZRnz2OsTxbFO6QHuNRQ&hl=en&sa=X&v ed=2ahUKEwjo4T0zNjpAhWKLqYKHRP6BsoQ6AEwAXoECAoQAQ#v=onepage&q=Scr imgeour%2C%20M.%20(2007).%20Empathy%20and%20Aggression%3A %20A%20Study%20of%20the%20Interplay%20Between%20Empathy%20a nd%20Aggression%20in%20Preschoolers&f=false Sigurdson, J.F., et.al. (2015) The Long-Term Effects of Being Bullied or a Bully in Adolescence on Externalizimg Mental Health Problems In Adulthood. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1186/s13034-015-0075-2 Sohravardi, H., et al. (2015). The Effect of Empathy Training Programs on Aggression and Compatibility Students of Elementary Schools in Yazd, Center of Iran. International Journal of Pediatrics. Retrieved from https://www.sid.ir/en/journal/ViewPaper.aspx?id=458287 Sourander A, Brunstein Klomek A, Kumpulainen K, Puustjärvi A, Elonheimo H, Ristkari T, Tamminen T, Moilanen I, Piha J, Ronning JA. 2011. Bullying at age eight and criminality in adulthood: findings from the Finnish Nationwide 1981 Birth Cohort Study. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. Retrieved from https://www.parentingscience.com/bully-victims.html 72 Stanger, N. et al., (2015). Empathy Inhibits Aggression in Competition: The Role of Provocation, Emotion, and Gender. Retrieved from https://www.researchgate.net/publication/299475243_Empathy_Inhibits_Agg ression_in_Competition_The_Role_of_Provocation_Emotion_and_Gender Strayer, J., & Roberts, W. (2004). Empathy and observed anger and aggression in five year-olds. Social Development. Retrieved from https://books.google.com.ph/books?id=z0fIAgAAQBAJ&pg=PA96&lpg=PA 96&dq=Strayer,+J.,+%26+Roberts,+W.+(2004).+Empathy+and+observed+a nger+and+aggression+in+five+yearolds.+Social+Development&source=bl&ots=LWTEidNvBh&sig=ACfU3U1 3NGLWWiT624BP43tvybS3VomePQ&hl=en&sa=X&ved=2ahUKEwiUreP gx9jpAhW8L6YKHViHDu4Q6AEwBXoECAYQAQ#v=onepage&q=Straye r%2C%20J.%2C%20%26%20Roberts%2C%20W.%20(2004).%20Empathy %20and%20observed%20anger%20and%20aggression%20in%20five%20ye ar-olds.%20Social%20Development&f=false Susan M. Swearer, Shelley Hymel (2015). Understanding the Psychology of Bullying Moving Toward a Social-Ecological Diathesis–Stress. Retrieved from http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0038929 Tremblay, R.E., & Nagin, D.S. (2005). The developmental origins of physical aggression in humans. New York: The Guilford Press. Retrieved from https://www.guilford.com/books/Developmental-Origins-of-Aggression/Tre mblay-Hartup-Archer/9781593851101/contents Williams, Y. (2015). True Experiment: Definition & Examples. Retrieved from https://study.com/academy/lesson/true-experiment-definition-examples.html. 73 APPENDICES 74 APPENDIX A CONSENT FORM 75 REPULIC OF THE PHILIPPINES BATANGAS STATE UNIVERSITY LIPA CITY, BATANGAS COLLEGE OF ARTS AND SCIENCES Consent Form Good day! We, the researchers, are conducting an experimental study entitled "The Effect of Empathy Induction on Aggressive Behavior of Bully-Victims". The purpose of this study is to investigate if empathy induction may affect the bully-victim’ aggressive behavior. Thus, we are requesting for your permission to be our respondent. In this experiment, the researchers will use empathy inducing films for the experimental group's intervention. To assess the behavior of the respondents, a 20-item Aggression Scale will be used in measuring the frequency of self-reported aggressive behaviors of the participants. This scale will be administered for pre-test and post test. Post test will be given 5 days after answering the pre-test. We hope the information from this research will help us contribute in developing interventions that will help decrease and regulate aggressive behaviors that leads to violence. We look forward to your outmost cooperation. Rest assured that all your responses will be kept strictly confidential. You are free to decide not to participate in this study or to withdraw if you think this study will be harmful to you. If you check "Yes", it means you have decided to participate and have read everything in this form. Thank you for your time. Yes, I wil participate. No, I will not participate. Noted by: SHELYN S. EXTRA RPm Research Adviser 76 APPENDIX B INSTRUMENTATION 77 Bully-Victim Survey Form Bullying has been defined as a specific type of aggressive, interpersonal behavior that involves intent to cause harm, occurs repetitively, and involves an imbalance of power. Direction: In this survey, you will be asked to answer questions about aggression and victimization. Please check the box/ boxes which fit your answer. Remember that all answers will be treated confidentially. Name: ____________________ (optional) Age: _____ Gender: Male Female A. Victimization 1. Have you been bullied before? Yes 2. How often? Most days 3. No One or more times a week One or more times a month Where have you been bullied? (Check all possible answer) School Home Online Others (please specify): _________________ 4. What type of bullying did you experience? (Check all possible answer) Physical form- hit, kicked, tripped, and having things stolen Verbal form- called nasty “names”, shouted at, insulted and been threatened Indirect Form- having lies or rumours spread behind your back and being left out from social groups Other form (Please specify): ________________ 78 B. 1. Aggression Did you bully anyone before? Yes 2. How often? Most days 3. No One or more times a week One or more times a month Where did you bully him/ her? (Check all possible answer) School Home Online Others: (please specify): _________________ 4. How did you bully him/her? (Check all possible answer) Physical form- hitting, kicking, tripping, and hiding things Verbal form- call nasty “names”, shouted, insulted and threatened someone Indirect Form- spread lies or rumours behind their back and left someone I didn’t like. Other forms (Please specify): ________________ 79 Pre-Test Aggression Scale Name: __________________ (optional) Age:_____ Gender: ______ These items measure the frequency of the aggressive behavior of the participants. Directions: Please check the appropriate column that best represents how often you did the following behaviors. Please answer the questions as honestly as possible. Never 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. I mock or tease someone to piss them off. I get irritated over little things. I curse and swear a lot. I insult someone by calling them mean “names.” I yell or shout to release my frustration. I fight back when someone hit me first. I say things behind someone’s back. I call someone stupid or “bobo” when they did wrong. 9. I tell lies and make up stories that aren’t true. 10. I get mad easily. 11. I fight when I am provoked. 12. I poke, slap or push someone. 13. I blackmail or threaten someone. 14. I trip others just to make fun. 15. I hide someone’s belonging to trip him/her. 16. I broke things when I am mad. 17. I ignore people I don’t like anymore. 18. I give insulting or negative comments on someone’s posts. 19. I send or post embarrassing photos without permission. 20. I spread rumors and fake news online. Sometimes Often Very Often 80 Post Test Aggression Scale Name: __________________ (optional) Age:_____ Gender: ______ These items measure the frequency of the aggressive behavior of the participants. Directions: Please check the appropriate column that best represents how often you did the following behaviors in the past 5 days. Please answer the questions as honestly as possible. In the past 5 days, how often did.... Never 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. I yell or shout to release my frustration. I blackmail or threaten someone. I spread rumors and fake news online. I broke things when I am mad. I say things behind someone’s back. I trip others just to make fun. I call someone stupid or “bobo” when they did wrong. 8. I curse and swear a lot. 9. I mock or tease someone to piss them off. 10. I send or post embarrassing photos without permission. 11. I hide someone’s belonging to trip him/her. 12. I fight back when someone hit me first. 13. I get irritated over little things. 14. I poke, slap or push someone. 15. I ignore people I don’t like anymore. 16. I insult someone by calling them mean “names.” 17. I tell lies and make up stories that aren’t true. 18. I fight when I am provoked. 19. I give insulting or negative comments on someone’s posts. 20. I get mad easily. Sometimes Often Very Often 81 APPENDIX C STATISTICAL COMPUTATION 82 MEAN SCORES COMPUTATION Control Group (PRETEST) Very Often-3 Items 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 VO 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 Often-2 Sometimes-1 Never-0 O 1 1 1 1 3 3 0 1 0 2 1 2 1 4 3 2 3 0 0 0 S 5 5 5 5 3 2 5 3 4 4 3 3 0 2 3 2 2 3 4 1 N 0 0 0 0 0 1 1 1 2 0 2 1 5 0 0 2 1 3 2 5 OVERALL MEAN MEAN 1.17 1.17 1.17 1.17 1.50 1.50 0.83 1.33 0.67 1.33 0.83 1.67 0.33 1.67 1.50 1.00 1.33 0.50 0.67 0.17 1.08 O 2 0 1 1 0 0 2 2 1 1 1 0 4 0 3 2 0 0 1 4 S 4 1 2 3 3 5 4 1 5 4 5 3 2 5 2 3 4 4 4 2 N 0 5 3 2 3 1 0 3 0 1 0 2 0 1 1 1 2 2 1 0 OVERALL MEAN MEAN 1.33 0.17 0.67 0.83 0.50 0.83 1.33 0.83 0.50 1.00 1.17 1.00 1.67 0.83 1.33 1.17 0.67 0.67 1.00 1.67 0.96 Control Group (POST TEST) Items 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 VO 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 83 Experimental Group 1 (PRETEST) Items 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 VO 1 1 1 1 1 0 1 1 1 1 0 0 0 1 0 0 1 0 1 0 O 0 0 0 0 1 2 0 1 0 0 1 0 0 1 1 0 0 0 0 0 S 1 2 1 2 0 1 0 0 0 1 2 1 2 1 1 1 2 1 1 1 N 1 0 1 0 1 0 2 1 2 1 0 2 1 1 2 0 2 1 2 OVERALL MEAN Mean Score 1.33 1.67 1.33 1.67 1.67 1.67 1.00 1.67 1.00 1.33 1.33 0.33 0.67 2.00 1.00 0.33 1.67 0.33 1.33 0.33 1.18 N 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 0 1 0 1 1 3 0 1 2 0 0 1 3 OVERALL MEAN Mean Score 2.00 2.00 2.67 1.00 1.67 2.33 0.67 1.33 1.00 2.00 1.33 0.67 0.00 2.00 1.33 0.33 2.00 2.00 0.67 0.00 1.35 Experimental Group 2 (PRETEST) Items 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 VO 0 1 2 0 0 1 0 0 0 1 0 0 0 1 0 0 0 0 0 0 O 3 1 1 0 2 2 0 1 1 1 2 0 0 1 2 0 3 3 0 0 S 0 1 0 3 1 0 2 2 1 1 0 2 0 1 0 1 0 0 2 0 84 Experimental Group 1 (POST TEST) Items 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 VO 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 O 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 0 0 0 0 1 0 0 0 0 1 1 0 1 S 2 0 1 1 0 1 1 1 1 0 0 1 1 1 1 1 0 1 1 1 N 1 3 2 2 3 2 1 2 2 3 3 1 2 2 2 2 2 1 2 1 OVERALL MEAN MEAN 0.67 0.00 0.33 0.33 0.00 0.33 1.00 0.33 0.33 0.00 0.00 1.00 0.33 0.33 0.33 0.33 0.67 1.00 0.33 1.00 0.43 S 3 0 0 1 0 2 2 1 3 0 0 1 1 1 3 1 0 3 2 2 N 0 3 3 2 2 0 0 0 0 3 2 1 0 2 0 1 2 0 0 0 OVERALL MEAN Mean Score 1.00 0.00 0.00 0.33 0.67 1.67 1’.33 1.67 1.00 0.00 0.67 1.00 1.67 0.33 1.00 1.00 0.67 1.00 1.33 1.33 0.88 Experimental Group 2 (POST TEST) ITEMS 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 VO 0 0 0 0 0 1 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 O 0 0 0 0 1 0 1 2 0 0 1 1 2 0 0 1 1 0 1 1 85 The scores were interpreted using Likert’s 4-point scale. The researchers rated them using the numerical ratings and descriptions: Mean Ranges 2.26-3.00 1.51-2.25 0.76-1.50 0.00-0.75 Scale 3 2 1 0 Verbal Interpretation Very Often / Very high aggressive behavior Often / High aggressive behavior Sometimes / Low aggressive behavior Never / Very low aggressive behavior SIGNIFICANT DIFFERENCE PRE TEST CG Mean Standard Error Median Mode Standard Deviation Sample Variance Kurtosis Skewness Range Minimum Maximum Sum Count EG 1.0755 0.097865 1.17 1.17 0.437667 0.191552 -0.56166 -0.56377 1.5 0.17 1.67 21.51 20 Mean Standard Error Median Mode Standard Deviation Sample Variance Kurtosis Skewness Range Minimum Maximum Sum Count t-Test: Two-Sample Assuming Equal Variances Mean Variance Observations Pooled Variance Hypothesized Mean Difference Df t Stat P(T<=t) one-tail t Critical one-tail P(T<=t) two-tail t Critical two-tail CG EG 1.0755 1.2415 0.191552 0.378908 20 20 0.28523 0 38 -0.9829 0.165936 1.685954 0.331873 2.024394 1.2415 0.137642 1.33 1.67 0.615555 0.378908 -1.18741 -0.41952 1.83 0.17 2 24.83 20 86 POST TEST CG Mean Standard Error Median Mode Standard Deviation Sample Variance Kurtosis Skewness Range Minimum Maximum Sum Count EG 0.9585 0.087043 0.915 0.83 0.389268 0.151529 -0.17123 0.128672 1.5 0.17 1.67 19.17 20 Mean Standard Error Median Mode Standard Deviation Sample Variance Kurtosis Skewness Range Minimum Maximum Sum Count t-Test: Two-Sample Assuming Equal Variances Mean Variance Observations Pooled Variance Hypothesized Mean Difference df t Stat P(T<=t) one-tail t Critical one-tail P(T<=t) two-tail t Critical two-tail CG EG 0.9585 0.59105 0.151529 0.132979 20 20 0.142254 0 38 3.080817 0.001914 1.685954 0.003827 2.024394 0.59105 0.081541 0.665 0.33 0.364663 0.132979 -1.25282 -0.14986 1.17 0 1.17 11.821 20 87 PRE-TEST AND POST TEST t-Test: Paired Two Sample for Means CONTROL GROUP Mean Variance Observations Pearson Correlation Hypothesized Mean Difference Df t Stat P(T<=t) one-tail t Critical one-tail P(T<=t) two-tail t Critical two-tail PRE POST 1.0755 0.9585 0.191552368 0.151529 20 20 0.406619209 0 19 0.753950629 0.230060653 1.729132812 0.460121307 2.093024054 t-Test: Paired Two Sample for Means EXPERIMENTAL GROUP Mean Variance Observations Pearson Correlation Hypothesized Mean Difference Df t Stat P(T<=t) one-tail t Critical one-tail P(T<=t) two-tail t Critical two-tail PRE POST 1.2415 0.59105 0.378908 0.132979 20 20 -0.46841 0 19 3.423005 0.001426 1.729133 0.002852 2.093024 88 CURRICULUM VITAE 89 IRIS KATHLEEN S. DOMION Tibagan, Siranglupa Calamba City, Laguna Phone: 09611314822 iriskathleendomion@gmail.com PERSONAL INFORMATION Date of Birth Age Place of Birth Sex Civil Status Nationality Religion : : : : : : : January 19, 2000 20 Calamba, Laguna Female Single Filipino Roman Catholic EDUCATIONAL BACKGROUND Senior High School Laguna College of Business and Arts S.Y. 2016-2018 Junior High School Majada In National High School S.Y. 2014-2016 Little Jesus Learning Center S.Y. 2012-2014 Elementary Little Jesus Learning Center S.Y 2006-2012 90 CHRISTINE M. GLORIENE Brgy. Sampaguita, Lipa City Phone: 09758534394 christinegloriene@gmail.com PERSONAL INFORMATION Date of Birth Age Place of Birth Sex Civil Status Nationality Religion : January 20, 2000 : 20 : Sampaguita, Lipa City : Female : Single : Filipino : Roman Catholic EDUCATIONAL BACKGROUND Senior High School LCC Silvercrest Senior High School SY 2016 – 2018 Junior High School Bolbok National High School SY 2012 – 2016 Elementary Bolbok Elementary School SY 2006 – 2012 91 NIESHA MARIE L. LALIMAN Manggas, Padre Garcia, Batangas Phone: 09107673436 lalimannieshamarie@gmail.com PERSONAL INFORMATION Date of Birth Age Place of Birth Sex Civil Status Nationality Religion : September 16, 2000 : 19 : Manggas, Padre Garcia, Batangas : Female : Single : Filipino : Roman Catholic EDUCATIONAL BACKGROUND Senior High School Holy Family Academy S.Y. 2016-2018 Junior High School Pansol National High School S.Y. 2012-2016 Elementary Manggas-Tamak Elementary School S.Y. 2005-2012 92 JEANNEN L. ONA Brgy. Marawoy, Lipa City, Batangas Phone: 09551409201 onajeannen@gmail.com PERSONAL INFORMATION Date of birth Age Place of birth Sex Civil Status Nationality Religion : Janaury 27, 2000 : 20 : Bacolod, Negros Occidental : Female : Single : Filipino : Roman Catholic EDUCATIONAL BACKGROUND Senior High School LCC Silvercrest Senior High School SY 2016-2018 Junior High School Inosloban National High School SY 2012-2016 Elementary Inosloban Marawoy Elementary School SY 2006-2012 93 IAN PAOLO H. MANGOBOS Brgy.10 Lipa City Phone: 09058369160 imangobos@gmail.com PERSONAL INFORMATION Date of Birth Age Place of Birth Sex Civil Status Nationality Religion : March 12, 1998 : 22 : San Sebastian, Lipa City : Male : Single : Filipino : Roman Catholic EDUCATIONAL BACKGROUND Secondary Lodlod National High School SY 2010-2013 Primary G.B Lontok Memorial School SY 2004-2009