Aktobe Regional University named by K.Zhubanova Abstract On the topic: The main linguistic trends of the early XX century and text analysis. Structuralism V. Ya. Propp. Complex whole A. M. Peshkovsky. Discourse Analysis of 3. Harris. Performed by: Tazhden Aruzhan Checked by: Kushkarova G.K. The term structuralism was used as a slogan and rallying cry by a number of different schools of linguistics, and it is necessary to realize that it has somewhat different implications according to the context in which it is employed. It is convenient first to draw a broad distinction between European and American structuralism and then to treat them separately. In other words, structure in Propp's analysis comes before experience; it appears like a prior form, which constitutes the genre of fairy-tales. He believed in a "common structure," which could be expressed and formalised in a way similar to a chemical formula. While for Propp structure was something to be identified within texts, for his disciples structure becomes something illusory, even "absent" (as Umberto Eco posits) or an "empty cell" (as in Lévi Strauss's introduction to M. Mauss's Sociology and Anthropology), something that emerges a posteriori, after the experience, in a deferred action of analysis. That is why structure is, for these later theorists, not a totality of discourse, but rather a changeable effect of discourse. Lacan saw the analyst's aim in deconstruction of the totality of discourse, which he calls symptom, and in the discovery of the constant points of contact between the signifier and the real, which help (re)construct the matter of the patient's phantasm. Many articles have been written about Alexander Matveevich Peshkovsky (1878-1933), an outstanding linguist and teacher, and his methodological experiments, carried out at the dawn of the "linguistic age", have long become a philological tradition. Peshkovsky's legacy, overgrown over the years with sometimes bizarre methods, "newspeak" and all kinds of innovations, was not lost, but even more confirmed his name in the history of Russian philology. Among the endless vacillations, searches and ideological battles of the early 20th century, he was able to pave his way in science, contrary to the strained "concepts" of some contemporaries and followers, focusing on the study of the psychology of word perception, on the creation of a scientific base of linguistic knowledge in the learning process. A conscious experiment gave birth to his theories. He was equally good at a strict linguistic skill and at the same time subtly felt a completely different facet of linguistic creativity - poetry and prose. The views of A. M. Peshkovsky, which are, of course, somewhat outdated, but thereby showing the ultimate vulnerability of any hypothesis, are being actively discussed; the ideas that he developed, as well as the system of occupations created by him "from sound to meaning", "from meaning to form" turned out to be in demand today. The study of naturally occurring connected sentences, spoken or written, is one of the most promising and rapidly developing areas of modern linguistics. Traditional linguistics has concentrated on sentence-centred analysis. Now, linguists are much more concerned with the way language is 'used' than what its components are. One may ask how it is that language-users interpret what other language-users intend to convey. When is carried this investigation further and asked how it is that people, as language-users, make sense of what they read in texts, understand what speakers mean despite what they say, recognize connected as opposed to jumbled or incoherent discourse, and successfully take part in that complex activity called conversation, then one is undertaking what is known as discourse analysis. The first linguist to refer to discourse analysis was Zellig Harris. In 1952, he investigated the connectedness of sentences, naming his study 'discourse analysis.' Harris claimed explicitly that discourse is the next level in a hierarchy of morphemes, clauses and sentences. He viewed discourse analysis procedurally as a formal methodology, derived from structural methods of linguistic analysis: such a methodology could break a text down into relationships (such as equivalence, substitution) among its lower-level constituents. Structural was so central to Harris's view of discourse that he also argued that what opposes discourse to a random sequence of sentences is precisely the fact that it has structure: a pattern by which segments of the discourse occur (and recur) relative to each other. Michael Stubbs says, 'Any study which is not dealing with (a) single sentences, (b) contrived by the linguist, (c) out of context, may be called discourse analysis.' (Stubbs 1983:131). In other words, there is a shift of focus from sentences in isolation to utterances in context: to study language in use is to study it as discourse. This is a fact that 'knowledge of a language is more than knowledge of individual sentences.' (Leech 2008:76) The true meaning of a sentence can't be assigned by its only linguistic construction but it largely depends on reference (meaning in relation to exterior world), sense (meaning in relation to linguistic system) and force (meaning in relation to situational context). Let's take an example: I love you. Clearly the assigned meaning is different in different situations if the speaker is one's lover or beloved as opposed to one's parent or child. As Chomsky states, 'To understand a sentence we must know more than the analysis of this sentence on each linguistic level. We must also know the reference and meaning of the morphemes or words of which it is composed; naturally, grammar cannot be expected to be of much help here.' (Chomsky 2002:103-04). Widdowson, also criticizes the well familiar definition of discourse analysis that discourse is the study of language patterns above the sentence and states; If discourse analysis is defined as the study of language patterns above the sentence, this would seem to imply that discourse is sentence writ large: quantitatively different but qualitatively the same phenomenon. It would follow, too, of course, that you cannot have discourse below the sentence. (Widdowson, 2004: 3) In other words, the discourse information is crucial to a complete theory of language. Smith and Kurthen also argue that 'the existence of arbitrary and language-specific syntactic and referential options for conveying a proposition requires a level of linguistic competence beyond sentential syntax and semantics' (Smith and Kurthen 2007:455). Sentential models of linguistic competence are unequipped to explain the existence of and the difference between multiple sentence forms with the same semantic interpretation. Similarly, Prince argues, 'sentential grammars alone are not capable of constraining the use of definite and indefinite NPs' (Prince 2004:119). There are several additional reasons for assuming that linguistic competence must be modeled beyond the level of the sentence. First, sentential grammars rely on the artifactual boundaries of written language. In some respects, this is a (short-term) advantage. The boundaries may be too small but they nonetheless provide a well-defined range of linguistic phenomena for a model of language to explain. In fact, this approach has been taken by generative grammarians for years with a great deal of success. However, the long-term disadvantages are also obvious. When one starts with a particular definition of language, any phenomena that do not fit into that definition will generally be ignored. If that definition is too narrow, then crucial data may be lost. Also, choosing to define language in terms of sentences in particular automatically includes a bias towards the type of language that one has been trained to consider 'proper' as opposed to what one knows through the initial process of first language acquisition. This argument alone takes our views beyond sentential boundaries. Once we accept that a language is not confined to sentence boundaries, we are free to explore broader possibilities. Second, the phenomenon of language requires at least a limited extension of sentential grammars. For example, sentential grammars can not completely account for the determination of pronoun co-reference, the scope of quantifiers, or the use of discourse deixis. In addition, 'English null arguments provide more evidence that knowledge of a language consists of more than a grammar for producing and interpreting sentences' (Tracy 1995:215). It is obvious that null subjects play an active role in conversational English, though they have received little attention in the past due to their rarity in written or 'formal' English. It is also clear that the presence of implicit null objects in English may not be distinguishable from truly intransitive constructions without an examination of extra-sentential information. While defining discourse, three definitions have been discussed – one derived from formalist paradigm, other from functionalist paradigm and third that includes both formalist and functionalist paradigms. Discourse analysis also deals with these paradigms. Formalist or structural analysis of discourse describes '… discourse at several levels or dimensions of analysis and in terms of many different units, categories, schematic patterns or relations' (Dijk 1985:4). Structural analyses focus on the way different units function in relation to each other but they disregard 'the functional relations with the context of which discourse is a part' [Dijk 1985:4]. Structurally based analysis of discourse find 'constituents' (smaller linguistic units that have particular 'relationship' with one another and that can occur in a restricted number of (often ruledgoverned) 'arrangements'. Structural views of discourse analysis accept that discourse is comprised of 'units.' Harris's unit was the morpheme (and their combination into sentences) while Linde, Labov and many other linguists identified clause as unit. Many contemporary structural analysis of discourse view the sentence as the unit of which discourse is comprised. The structural view of discourse analysis places discourse in a hierarchy of language structures, thus fostering the view that one can describe language in a unitary way that continues unimpeded from morpheme to clause to sentence to discourse. But this kind of analysis does not pay attention to the purposes and functions for which so called 'units' are designed to serve in human affairs. Discourse analysis is necessarily the analysis of language in use. The functionalist view of discourse analysis asserts that 'the study of discourse is the study of any aspect of language use' (Fasold 1990:65). Discourse analysis can not be restricted to the description of linguistic forms independent of the purposes and functions which these forms perform. Functional analyses of discourse rely less upon the strictly grammatical characteristics of utterances as sentences, than upon the way utterances are situated in contexts.