See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/301743856 The concept of homophobia: A psychosocial perspective Article in Sexologies · April 2016 DOI: 10.1016/j.sexol.2016.02.002 CITATIONS READS 15 7,229 2 authors: Christèle Fraïssé Jaime Barrientos Université de Bretagne Occidentale Alberto Hurtado University 19 PUBLICATIONS 63 CITATIONS 115 PUBLICATIONS 1,267 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects: System Justification among Sexual Minorities in Chile View project Ações e políticas afirmativas universitárias para estudantes LGBTI+ : um estudo comparativo entre Brasil, Chile e Espanha View project All content following this page was uploaded by Christèle Fraïssé on 25 October 2017. The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file. +Model ARTICLE IN PRESS SEXOL-551; No. of Pages 5 Sexologies (2016) xxx, xxx.e1—xxx.e5 Disponible en ligne sur ScienceDirect www.sciencedirect.com ORIGINAL ARTICLE The concept of homophobia: A psychosocial perspective夽 C. Fraïssé a,∗, J. Barrientos b a EA1285, UFR lettres et sciences humaines, centre de recherche en psychologie, cognition et communication, université Bretagne occidentale, 20, rue Duquesne, CS93837, 29238 Brest cedex 3, France b Escuela de Psicología, Universidad Católica del Norte, Avenida Angamos, 0610 Antofagasta, Chile KEYWORDS Homophobia; Heterosexism; Psychosocial approach Summary The purpose of this article is to consider the concept of homophobia from a psychosocial perspective. After a brief history of the emergence of the concept of homophobia and other concepts that have emerged over time to clarify the idea, we will discuss what these concepts provide in terms of framing and defining the phenomenon, and the questions this framing and defining raise, in turn, about this concept. The various ways of measuring homophobia will also be examined, in order to identify the limitations of these tests and what they teach us about the phenomenon of homophobia. Using these as starting points, we will try to redefine homophobia as a complex system that brings together several concepts (heterosexism, sexual prejudice, heteronormativity, sexism and male dominance). This system-based definition of homophobia will lead to a more nuanced understanding of various situations involving discrimination, inequality and violence, and will uncover violent situations that remain hidden by the current understanding of homophobia. © 2016 Published by Elsevier Masson SAS. Since the 1970s, when psychology researchers began to work on homophobia rather than homosexuality, this concept has spread in the French and international literature (Fraïssé, DOI of original article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.sexol.2016.02.001. 夽 La version en français de cet article, publiée dans l’édition imprimée de la revue, est également disponible en ligne : http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.sexol.2016.02.001. ∗ Corresponding author. E-mail addresses: fraisse@univ-brest.fr (C. Fraïssé), jbarrien@ucn.cl (J. Barrientos). 2011; Barrientos, 2015). Nevertheless, the most common definition of homophobia — ‘‘an attitude of hostility toward male or female homosexuals’’ (Borillo, 2001, p. 3), indicates that this concept is relatively restricted and tends to individualise the process of discrimination and rejection. This is why the term has gradually become more qualified. A distinction has developed between general and specific homophobia (Welzer-Lang, 1994), between families of phobias of different target groups: lesbophobia, gayphobia, biphobia and transphobia (Chamberland and Lebreton, 2012), and between homophobia and sexual prejudice (Herek, 1984). Finally, the concept of heterosexism (Neisen, 1990; Herek, 1990) will be proposed as a way of http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.sexol.2016.02.002 1158-1360/© 2016 Published by Elsevier Masson SAS. Please cite this article in press as: Fraïssé C, Barrientos J. The concept of homophobia: A psychosocial perspective. Sexologies (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.sexol.2016.02.002 +Model SEXOL-551; No. of Pages 5 ARTICLE IN PRESS xxx.e2 describing the more collective aspect of this type of discrimination. Researchers have assumed that the concept of homophobia would vanish, as it seemed so inappropriate to the direction research was taking. The fact that the concept of homophobia has become so widespread therefore raises questions, and it seems useful to us to re-examine the psychosocial perspective (Moscovici, 1984), in other words considering that any relationship between an individual and a social object is mediated by an alter. In the beginning there was homophobia Weinberg’s 1972 book, at the same time as the article by Smith (1971), popularised the term homophobia and gradually overturned the approach of most psychologists, who still approached homosexuality as a mental illness. In his preface, Weinberg sets out his position, which is that homophobia, rather than homosexuality, is the diseased state. However, since 1972, the idea of fear has given way to hostility and aversion towards homosexuals, as can be seen in the definition given in the introduction. There have been two main criticisms of this usage of the concept (Barrientos and Cárdenas, 2013). The first is of the pathological connotation of the suffix ‘‘phobia’’. The second is of its individualising dimension (Chamberland and Lebreton, 2012), which makes it difficult to discern the collective aspects of such discrimination. In the 1990s, with the purpose of responding to these two criticisms, various authors suggested other concepts. Many concepts: just one phenomenon? According to Welzer-Lang (1994), homophobia can be considered to involve two phenomena that are not necessarily linked to sexual orientation; in his view, homophobia governs the relationship between men and women, by saving the state of masculinity from all feminisation. Assuming these two categories are kept entirely separate from one another and, moreover, are hierarchical with the masculine being the superior category, the concept of homophobia maintains this separateness and hierarchy. Homophobia is thus closely linked with sexism and male dominance. For this reason, he proposes specific homophobia, the familiar type, which relates to sexual orientation with ordinary hostility in the form of verbal and physical aggression. He also proposes general homophobia, which emerges in contexts that are independent of sexual orientation but that feature gender transgression. Individuals who present a gender identity that is not consistent with their apparent biological sex would under this definition be victims of general homophobia. Homophobia is therefore primarily a masculine phenomenon, as it is ‘‘the key, for men, to achieving dominance over women, by structuring relationships between men in the image of relationships between men and women’’ (Welzer-Lang, 1994, p. 62). Welzer-Lang suggests extending the concept of homophobia beyond the strictly individual, to include individual hostility or aversion that arises from societal and ideological factors, and, following Neisen (1990) and Herek (1990), introduces the concept of heterosexism. This idea C. Fraïssé, J. Barrientos complements homophobia, describing a process that is allied to homophobia but more complex, and just like sexism, heterosexism is, according to Welzer-Lang, the expression of a hierarchy of sexualities. The benefit of the concept of heterosexism is that it reintroduces the notion of power, which is absent from the term homophobia, and ‘‘provides an opportunity for closer examination of the connection between anti-homosexual sentiment and sex role stereotype’’ (Neisen, 1990, p. 25). Herek (1990, p. 316) defines heterosexism ‘‘as an ideological system that denies, denigrates, and stigmatizes any non-heterosexual form of behavior, identity, relationship, or community’’. According to Herek, there are two forms of heterosexism: one is cultural and relates to a heterosexual bias in lifestyles and institutions within society; the other is psychological, and relates to attitudes and behaviour of individuals that are directed directly at sexual minorities. The conceptual framework based on this notion of sexual stigma that was developed by Herek (2007, 2009) works in the same way. Herek (2009) identifies a structural sexual stigma — heterosexism — which he describes as an ideology that is translated into institutional practices such that it disadvantages sexual minorities. There are two processes at work here. The first relates to the assumption that all individuals are heterosexual, meaning that sexual minorities are made invisible. The second describes the fact that when sexual minorities become visible, they are automatically considered to be ‘‘diseased’’ and ‘‘unnatural’’, which means that explanations are required and these individuals are problematised. According to Herek (2007), beyond this cultural and collective dimension, there exists sexual stigma that is directly experienced and expressed by individuals, which has three elements: enacted sexual stigma, consisting of insults, attacks, marginalisation or rejection of sexual minorities; second, felt sexual stigma, which leads to behaviour change on the part of individuals seeking to avoid stigmatisation; and internalised sexual stigma, in which individuals incorporate negative evaluations of sexual minorities into their own value systems. If these individuals belong to these minority groups, Herek names this ‘‘self-stigma’’, and if these individuals are heterosexual, this is manifested as sexual prejudice. Herek’s model takes a unified approach to the various levels, from intra-individual to ideology, using a psychosocial approach (Doise, 1982); it maintains the fundamental power dimension of heterosexism and uses a disease-based approach to the concept of homophobia. By abandoning the distinction between cultural heterosexism and psychological heterosexism, it arrives at a wording that is consistent with the concept of heterosexism, which is necessarily structural and ideological in nature, and which is only individual in its expression. Finally, heterosexism is built into the process of heteronormativity, which, as it presupposes that heterosexuality is normal and superior, is part of the way society works, for both individuals and institutions. This normative regulation operates in daily life, and also in institutions, via ‘‘regulatory practices (. . .) that produce and constrain gender intelligibility’’ (Chambers, 2007, p. 666) permitting the expression of certain forms of gender and prohibiting others. Using the sex/gender matrix (Butler, 2006), this process maintains consistency between sex (a male or female being), gender (being masculine or feminine) and desire Please cite this article in press as: Fraïssé C, Barrientos J. The concept of homophobia: A psychosocial perspective. Sexologies (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.sexol.2016.02.002 +Model SEXOL-551; No. of Pages 5 ARTICLE IN PRESS The concept of homophobia: A psychosocial perspective (which is necessarily heterosexual) (Bastien Charlebois, 2011). Bringing these two processes (heterosexism and heteronormativity) together explains why homosexuality is invisible and excluded in all social spaces, and why it is stigmatised as a deviant and pathological sexual orientation. The various statements set out in this brief summary show that homophobia, as the term is currently used, corresponds to more than one phenomenon. It refers to phenomena that are very different in nature, from insults and aggression (inter-individual and position-based interaction), to the failure to make marriage available to homosexual people (ideological interaction), and shame and suicide (intra-individual), all of which takes place within a specific normative framework. These various phenomena have also been conceptualised as research into negative attitudes to homosexuality in social psychology has progressed, and as the various concepts that have been observed have been added to the canon. For all these authors, the concern was not to restrict the identification and analysis of situations that, at various levels, create, legitimise and maintain inequality, discrimination and violence. However, approaching homophobia in this way raises questions about how to measure the phenomenon. Measuring homophobia From the 1970s, research into homophobia moved towards measuring attitudes towards homosexuality or towards gay and lesbian people. Tracing attitudes: homophobia as an attitude Homophobia can be measured using several methods. Herek (1984) proposed the concept of ‘‘sexual prejudice’’, which describes negative attitudes towards homosexuality. He thus linked hostility towards homosexuality with a large body of theoretical work on prejudice. This redefinition of homophobia as a sexual prejudice enables us to go beyond the original idea behind the term homophobia, which is that attitudes and behaviour that are hostile to homosexual people primarily arise from fear and are treated as a disease. These are the tests that are most commonly used, in the form of self-evaluation measures, as seen, for example, in the scale created by Herek in the mid-1980s, the Attitudes towards Lesbians and Gay men (ATLG) Scale. This type of measure requires those surveyed to state to what extent they agree with a series of statements about homosexuality, and sometimes bisexuality, about individuals from sexual minorities. They may also require that those surveyed declare how comfortable they were with contact with homosexual people or gay and/or lesbian people in various social situations. However, individuals are now less willing to express negative attitudes openly, as a general norm of tolerance has developed in Western societies (Meertens and Pettigrew, 1997; Saucier et al., 2005). For this reason, researchers have created ‘‘modern’’ prejudice scales, in which the items are less overtly negative than those used in previous measures. Similarly, implicit testing methods have been developed. For instance, there are physiological tests such as facial electromyography (Vanman et al., 1997), measurement of xxx.e3 heart rate acceleration and MRI measurement of amygdala activation (Olson and Fazio, 2003). Reaction time measurement has also been used, as in the Implicit Association Test (IAT, Greenwald et al., 1998) and the Go/No go Association Task (GNAT, Nosek and Banaji, 2001), which test the strength of associations between concepts. These implicit tests complement explicit self-evaluation tests, and refine our understanding of the mechanisms involved, and enable the creation of different ways of engaging with the general public on this subject. A final comment: regardless of whether tests are implicit or explicit, they will be construed differently depending on the operational definition of homosexuality that the researchers use. What should we measure? Homophobia, gayphobia, lesbophobia? The content of the statements in the tests relate to a variety of phenomena that are rooted in a particular historical and political reality. They may, for example, examine the institutional aspects of homophobia, moral or religious aspects, deviance or pathology, or daily life. The content thus varies over the years, focusing on certain aspects in the light of the issues that are prominent at the time. Measures of homophobia therefore depend on how homosexuality and homophobia are seen in society and by the researchers at the time the test is devised. In particular, this content will be designed to look at either homosexuality in general, or at individuals of different sexualities: homosexual women or men. The first type of scale only measures attitudes towards homosexuality in general. However, continuing the work of Herek (1984), from the mid-1980s, researchers (Morrison and Morrison, 2003; Raja and Stokes, 1998) gradually incorporated the difference between homosexual men and women into their work, in order to measure sexual prejudice. In the light of this, two approaches to these scales arose in parallel, which affected the way in which the items were developed and how the phenomenon of homophobia itself was described. The first approach considered homosexual people to be a quasi-ethnic minority group (Herek, 2002), which led to items that measured attitudes towards homosexuality and homosexual people in general. In the light of this, respondents were not meant to draw a distinction between gay and lesbian people, as they were understood to share the same experience: that of occupying a minority position. The perceived hierarchy here is of heterosexuality and homosexuality. According to this approach, sexual prejudice or homophobia are measured as general concepts, which in principle is gender-neutral; this fails to address the discrimination that is linked to lesbianism and bisexuality. In addition, the situation of individuals presenting nonconforming gender, trans* people, transgender people and intersex people are made invisible by this concept. Thinking of and testing homophobia as a form of sexual prejudice would therefore appear to be a sexist concept (Chamberland and Lebreton, 2012). A second approach considers the issue using a system of beliefs about sexual roles that distinguishes between Please cite this article in press as: Fraïssé C, Barrientos J. The concept of homophobia: A psychosocial perspective. Sexologies (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.sexol.2016.02.002 +Model SEXOL-551; No. of Pages 5 ARTICLE IN PRESS xxx.e4 masculine and feminine, and the different sociological positions occupied by men and women, particularly in terms of male dominance. Using this approach, homosexuality is considered as a violation of sexual roles, and thus gay and lesbian people are not seen as perpetrating the same transgressions. Respondents can therefore express different attitudes to one or other category. In this approach, the relevant hierarchy involves men and women. Finally, regardless of the approach used, the question stems from the distinction between two essentialist categories: homosexual and heterosexual, or man and woman. The use of categories entails setting in stone the processes that are being described, and also making invisible those individuals who do not conform, such as lesbians, transgender people, intersex people and bisexual people. Reflecting on complexity and change: the homophobic system Understanding homophobia generally starts from categories of sex or sexual orientation. Such an approach tends to provide a static, fixed representation of the process of homophobia, in that every individual is assigned to one and one category alone, in a binary or indeed unitary process; in other words, this approach considers just one dimension of the process (Baschetta, 2009). In terms of sex, for example, an individual can only be a man or a woman, with the gender characteristics associated with their sex. This assignment takes no account of transgender or intersex people, or anyone else who presents a non-conforming gender identity. Similarly, sexual orientation involves the opposition between heterosexual and homosexual, implying that sexuality must fall into one of these two categories. Sexuality is thus confined to a specific type of practice, and no attention is paid to the diversity of individuals’ sexual practices (Bozon, 2013), or to the dynamic nature of identity, where the way in which individuals see themselves over time can change. In addition, those who fall into neither of these two categories vanish, in favour of ‘‘a universalising interpretation of the subject’s formation, which has the effect of erasing subordinate subjects’’ (Baschetta, 2009, p. 3). It can be seen that this dominant approach to understanding homophobia neglects dominance effects, and does not truly give an account of the dynamic nature of human action. For example, for Tomsen (2009), sexual prejudice is not a static and fixed phenomenon; rather, it is fluid and shifting, depending on the situation at hand. In his research into homophobic violence, Tomsen (2006) suggests that attackers’ motivations primarily concern the protection of their masculine identity, rather than an irrational or pathological fear of homosexual people. Such violence is permitted because of a conception of the other which is based on an assumption of natural supremacy of some identities over others, which produces and reproduces a ‘‘natural’’ order in which non-heterosexual sexualities occupy a subordinate position (Mason, 2001). This heterosexist conception is reminiscent of interiorised sexual stigma (Herek, 2007). However, this concept, which considers homophobia to be sexual prejudice or self-stigma, takes a somewhat static view of the process, as it is confined by categorisation, while C. Fraïssé, J. Barrientos Mason (2001) presents this as a dynamic concept. Heterosexism, using this explanation, is just as rife within sexual minorities as among heterosexual people, and affects everyone’s lives. Resorting to categorisation or binary or unitary approaches (Baschetta, 2009) obscures in part society’s heterosexist and heteronormative way of working, while it ought to reveal this in full. This is because it conceals a whole facet of society behind the idea of a universal man or woman. For this reason, it seems useful to consider homophobia as a complex system, in which heterosexism, sexual prejudice, heteronormativity, sexism and male dominance interact with one another. The idea of the homophobic system, as it part of a psychosocial approach will, in our view, promote a more nuanced and comprehensive understanding of the discrimination, inequality and violence that are experienced by some citizens, but which also affect the whole of society. This idea will also uncover violent situations, which have hitherto been invisible. Conclusion This article acts as an invitation to develop psychosocial research into what we have called the ‘‘homophobic system’’, and to recognise and incorporate, as researchers and/or practitioners, the historical character of categorisations that have been produced by the field of psychology. We should not forget or deny the sociohistorical dimension of category creation, regardless of whether these categories are pathological. Nor should we forget or deny the effects of being assigned to such categories; if we remember and acknowledge this, all practitioners and researchers can position themselves as participants and can inform their professional practice. Furthermore, this positioning reminds psychologists that they take part in and function as part of this ‘‘homophobic system’’, which should lead them to reflect upon their practice, when caring for patients and when doing research. We also suggest that such reflective processes should be combined with a system of ‘‘ethics of consideration for consequences’’ (Piron, 1996), which positions a researcher who takes responsibility for what he/she writes, both in terms of the content and of the effects on others. For psychologists, considered and ethical positioning should make it easier to identify the ‘‘homophobic system’’ of which they are a part in their various interactions, in order to avoid pitfalls such as the assumption of heterosexuality, and the failure to understand the wish that LGBTI people have to be recognised, which is linked to their subordinate position (for examples, see Bastien Charlebois, 2011). Disclosure of interest The authors declare that they have no competing interest. References Barrientos J. Violencia Homofóbica en América Latina y Chile. Santiago de Chile: Ediciones y Publicaciones El Buen Aire S.A; 2015. Please cite this article in press as: Fraïssé C, Barrientos J. The concept of homophobia: A psychosocial perspective. Sexologies (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.sexol.2016.02.002 +Model SEXOL-551; No. of Pages 5 ARTICLE IN PRESS The concept of homophobia: A psychosocial perspective Barrientos J, Cárdenas M. Homofobia y Calidad de Vida de Gay y Lesbianas: Una Mirada Psicosocial. Psykhe 2013;22(1):3—14. Baschetta P. Co-formations : des spatialités de résistance décoloniales chez les lesbiennes « of color » en France. Genre Sex Soc 2009:1, Récupéré de http://gss.revues.org/index810.html. Bastien Charlebois J. Au-delà de la phobie de l’homo : quand le concept d’homophobie porte ombrage à la lutte contre l’hétérosexisme et l’hétéronormativité. Reflets 2011;17(1): 112—49. Borillo D. L’homophobie. Paris: PUF; 2001. Bozon M. Sociologie de la sexualité. Domaines et approches. Paris: Armand Colin; 2013. Butler J. Défaire le genre. Paris: Éditions Amsterdam; 2006. Chamberland L, Lebreton C. Réflexions autour de la notion d’homophobie : succès politique, malaises conceptuels et application empirique. Nouv Questions Feministes 2012;31(1):27—43. Chambers SA. ‘‘An incalculable effect’’: subversions of heteronormativity. Pol Stud 2007;55(3):656—79. Doise W. L’explication en psychologie sociale. Paris: PUF; 1982. Fraïssé C. L’homophobie et les expressions de l’ordre hétérosexiste. Rennes: Presses universitaires de Rennes; 2011. Greenwald AG, McGhee DE, Schwartz JL. Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the implicit association test. J Pers Soc Psychol 1998;74(6):1464—80. Herek GM. Attitudes toward lesbians and gay men: a factor-analytic study. J Homosex 1984;10(½):39—51. Herek GM. The context of anti-gay violence: notes on cultural and psychological heterosexism. J Interpers Violence 1990;5(3):316—33. Herek GM. Gender gaps in public opinion about lesbians and gay men. Public Opin Q 2002;66(1):40—66. Herek GM. Confronting sexual stigma and prejudice: theory and practice. J Soc Issues 2007;63(4):905—25. Herek GM. Hate crimes and stigma-related experiences among sexual minority adults in the United States: prevalence estimates from a national probability sample. J Interpers Violence 2009;24:54—74. xxx.e5 Mason G. The spectacle of violence: homophobia, gender and knowledge. London: Routledge; 2001. Meertens RW, Pettigrew TF. Is subtle prejudice really prejudice? Public Opin Q 1997;61(1):54—71. Morrison MA, Morrison TG. Development and validation of a scale measuring modern prejudice toward gay men and lesbian women. J Homosex 2003;43(2):15—37. Moscovici S. Psychologie sociale. Paris: PUF; 1984. Neisen JH. Heterosexism: redefining homophobia for the 1990s. J Gay Lesbian Psychother 1990;1(3):21—35. Nosek BA, Banaji MR. The go/no-go association task. Soc Cognit 2001;19(6):625—64. Olson MA, Fazio RH. Relations between implicit measures of prejudice. What are we measuring. Psychol Sci 2003;14(6):636—9. Piron F. Écriture et responsabilité. Trois figures de l’anthropologue. Anthropol Soc 1996;20(1):125—48. Raja S, Stokes J. Assessing attitudes toward lesbians and gay men: the modern homophobia scale. Int J Sex Gender Stud 1998;3(2):113—34. Saucier DA, Miller CT, Doucet N. Differences in helping whites and blacks: a meta-analysis. Pers Soc Psychol Rev 2005;9(1): 2—16. Smith K. Homophobia: a tentative personality profile. Psychol Rep 1971;29:1091—4. Tomsen S. Homophobic violence, cultural essentialism and shifting sexual identities. Soc Leg Stud 2006;15(3):389—407. Tomsen S. Violence, prejudice and sexuality. London & New York: Routledge; 2009. Vanman EJ, Paul BY, Ito TA, Miller N. The modern face of prejudice and structural features that moderate the effect of cooperation on affect. J Pers Soc Psychol 1997;73(5):941—59. Weinberg G. Society and the healthy homosexual. New York: Saint Martin’s Press; 1972. Welzer-Lang D. L’homophobie : la face cachée du masculin. In: Welzer-Lang D, Dutey P, Dorais M, editors. La peur de l’autre en soi. Du sexisme à l’homophobie. Montréal: vlb éditeur; 1994. p. 13—91. Please cite this article in press as: Fraïssé C, Barrientos J. The concept of homophobia: A psychosocial perspective. Sexologies (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.sexol.2016.02.002 View publication stats