What is Pseudoscience? Leah Murphy Psych 4850 Prof. Louise Barrett As this semester progresses, the more I realize how messy ‘science’ is and that there is no easy solution to the issues we face in our pursuit for answers. With science being a discipline based on trust (in researchers, peer reviewers, publishers etc.), how do we distinguish real, bona fide science from pseudoscience? The trouble with determining pseudoscience from science is the superficial resemblance of each other, making it sometimes difficult to separate real information from fake. One of my favorite examples of this is astrology, which is the practice of studying planets and star patterns with the belief that they have a powerful influence on our behaviors and future. Though astrology uses outwardly ‘scientific’ terms and derives from aspects in the study of astronomy, it is not actually based on any real scientific evidence or knowledge and instead is based on ancient beliefs. Like astrology, many forms of pseudoscience have existed since the emergence of science itself. Pseudoscience comes in many ways, shapes and forms. From bogus claims such as Count Arthur Gobineau and his theory on The Inequality of Races (1854), to outright manipulating or faking data such as the Piltdown Man fiasco (Marks, 2009). Science has always been based on a ‘gentleman’s code of honor’, a system of trust in the methods, data etc. used in the process of acquiring answers. Mark’s makes an important point in how when the shift of science from a hobby into a profession began, this ‘gentleman’s honor code’ somewhat weakened with this shift. Stresses in publication deadlines, grant money and maintaining a reputation in the field, pressured researchers in ways that compromised the trust system. Pseudoscience resembles real science in many ways, making it that much more problematic in knowing when a red flag comes up, and when to investigate. There is no policing in science, only peer reviewers who double-check findings if they ‘seem’ valid. Mark’s asks if a person is caught as a falsifier, does this mean all of their other work is at risk for this as well? While I would personally like to believe everyone is honest in his or her work, once you have been labeled as a cheat, it would be hard to regain your reputation back as an authentic scientist. To offer a thorough outlook of what is pseudoscience, I will shift from the brief history and factors that make up a pseudoscience, into why people are prone to believing pseudoscience. Boudry, Blancke and Pigliucci (2014) attempt to create and understanding on why Pseudoscience still thrives today in their article, What makes weird beliefs thrive? The epidemiology of Pseudoscience. They make a genuine comparison between the struggles in the demands of bona fide science, and how Pseudoscience manages to slip through these principles in our pursuit for a ‘perfect’ science; Science, on the one hand, by bowing to the demands of evidence, tends to become hard to swallow for laypeople and even scientists themselves. Without its undeniable technological success, its close interconnectedness, and the institutional support it receives (partly in virtue of those technological rewards), no beliefs as unpalatable as those of modern science would stand a chance in the struggle for influence over human minds. Given the way the human mind works, science is a frail creature indeed. The ultimate source of its remarkable cultural dominance is the world out there, which tends to be pretty stable. (p. 1193) This suggestion of ‘science being a frail creature indeed’ certainly resonates with my thought processes throughout this term, and I have began questioning everything I read about science ever since. In my last paper, I too discussed the critical construction of science being a cultural practice. However, contrary to what is suggested in the above passage, I believe the world is rather unstable which continuously challenges and reinvents what we know as science. In comparison, Boudry et al., (2014) suggests Pseudoscience’s gain their popularity by delving into the comfortable intuitive representations of the world. Therefore their intuitive appeal is a major source for their continued success in today’s world, despite their shortcomings in evidence, “Pseudoscience has traded intellectual respectability for intuitive allure”. (p. 1194) The question still remains, how do we stop this from happening and is it possible to imagine a world where all scientific information published and found can be honest and truthful (to the best of our abilities)? Hoaxing, forging, trimming and cooking up (Babbage, 1830) data is an easy way out to create a false perception that what you’re trying to suggest to the world is more impressive than actual results you’ve found. To some, this is an alluring risk that is better than facing subpar or disappointing results. Boudry suggests that is also the case for people believing these alluring claims. It is a tricky cycle to get out of until they get caught! A better filter between the raw data being found, and what is presented to the public would help with Pseudoscience slipping through the cracks of scientific methods. However, despite efforts for nipping fraudulence at the source, people still love a grand story that appeals to the intuitions. Take Jenny McCarthy’s infamous claims on vaccinations causing autism; despite the countless scientific evidence claiming this to not be true, people still believe it! This is troubling and there is no easy solution to how we can discontinue false information being spread. Hopefully in the future, we will have a better way of filtering through inaccurate information, and have a more honest work of knowledge of the world. References Boudry, M., Blancke, S., & Pigliucci, M. (2014). What makes weird beliefs thrive? The epidemiology of pseudoscience. Philosophical Psychology, 28:8, 1177-1198. Marks, J. (2009). Why I am not a scientist: Anthropology and modern knowledge. University of California Press.