The current issue and full text archive of this journal is available on Emerald Insight at: www.emeraldinsight.com/1066-2243.htm Confusion in internet retailing: causes and consequences Confusion in internet retailing Marion Garaus Department of Marketing, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria Abstract Purpose – The purpose of this paper is to introduce the new construct online shopper confusion and to identify online confusion causes and consequences. Design/methodology/approach – Data obtained from a projective technique and a quantitative study were analyzed to identify online shopper confusion causes. Two experiments employing different stimulus materials tested the conceptualized consequences of online shopper confusion. Findings – Confusing online store elements are classified into three online confusion causes. Data yielded from two experiments using fictitious and real shopping scenarios as stimulus material show that a confusing internet retail process leads to negative consumer reactions. Research limitations/implications – The resulting taxonomy of confusing online store elements offers guidance on the creation of non-confusing online shopping trips, and highlights the relevance of a non-confusing internet retail process. Online shopper confusion is linked to negative behavioral reactions. Consequently, this research offers an explanation for undesirable consumer reactions in internet retailing. Practical implications – The findings provide practitioners with concrete insights into how the internet retail process confuses shoppers which help to assess the confusion potential of their existing online stores and consider confusion issues in the development of new online stores. Originality/value – This research is the first to explore confusion during the internet retail process. The multi-method approach offers highly valid insights into the causes and consequences of online shopper confusion. Keywords Internet retail process, Online shopper confusion, Online store design, Shopping cart abandonment Paper type Research paper Introduction Current figures affirm the importance of online shopping for every industry. In 2013, 191.1 million US citizens shopped or browsed products, compared prices, and purchased goods online at least once. Business-to-consumer customers spent US$ 322 billion online in 2013, with the majority of this figure accounting for retail e-commerce sales (US$ 210.6 billion) (Statista.com, 2014). In general, companies strive to generate additional profit through online stores; however, many e-shops fail in this objective (Hausman and Siekpe, 2009). According to the website analytic provider Fireclick.com (2014), conversion rates are 3.8 percent, with a cart abandonment rate of 70 percent. One possible way to improve order conversion rates is a pleasant online store design (Mummalaneni, 2005). The design of an appealing website that encourages users to stay and surf represents one of the main challenges for internet retailers. Indeed, website appeal has been identified as a reliable predictor of purchase intention (Liu et al., 2017). Nevertheless, research revealing insights into the website design of online stores is scarce. Hausman and Siekpe (2009, p. 11) state that “the absence of theoretically-based guidance for internet design has resulted in over-blown technologies that may irritate or frustrate the online user rather than aiding the user in undertaking shopping tasks.” Not only do advanced technologies overstrain and confuse shoppers, but the increasing amount of information provided by websites and complex website structures do as well (Chen et al., 2009; Cho et al., 2006; Mai et al., 2014). In line with these theoretical considerations, confusion represents a state that likely occurs in online shopping situations. Thus, studies have investigated the phenomenon of confusion only from a product-related perspective, arguing that too much product information on a 477 Received 24 November 2016 Revised 18 March 2017 21 June 2017 28 July 2017 Accepted 30 July 2017 Internet Research Vol. 28 No. 2, 2018 pp. 477-499 © Emerald Publishing Limited 1066-2243 DOI 10.1108/IntR-11-2016-0356 INTR 28,2 478 website overloads and confuses shoppers (Lee and Lee, 2004). However, recent research in stationary retailing reveals that not only products but also physical surroundings confuse shoppers (Garaus et al., 2015). Although studies have not examined confusion in an online setting, they reveal that online store elements trigger emotions that result in either positive or negative reactions (Hsieh et al., 2014; Shen and Khalifa, 2012). With confusion representing a negative state of mind, the central premise of the current research is that shoppers react to confusing online store elements with negative behavioral intentions. Under this assumption, the confusion triggered by online store elements is of serious concern for e-retailers. The relevance of understanding drivers of confusion during internet retailing is even more pronounced by evidence suggesting that negative information outweighs positive information ( for a review, see Mizerski, 1982) and that negative experiences influence behavior more than positive experiences (Baumeister et al., 2001). Despite the growing amount of research on negative incidents in internet retailing, these studies have largely focused on negative online shopping incidents in general, without examining what consumers actually feel during negative online shopping trips (e.g. Holloway and Beatty, 2003). However, because different emotions cause different consequences (Gelbrich, 2010; Laros and Steenkamp, 2005), the knowledge of particular mental states experienced during the internet retail process would provide a deeper understanding of online shoppers’ behavior. Moreover, the extant literature points to the potential of the online store environment to evoke emotions (Lim, 2015; Manganari et al., 2011; Jeong et al., 2009). In addition, this stream of research does not link internet retail failures to outcomes of high interest for e-retailers, such as shopping cart abandonment or word of mouth (WOM). Scholars have criticized that reasons for this undesirable behavior are scarcely researched (Kukar-Kinney and Close, 2010). Against this background, the current research addresses the following two research questions: RQ1. What are the causes of online shopper confusion in internet retailing? RQ2. How do shoppers react to a confusing internet retailing process? Three objectives guide the research. First, this study aims to transfer the confusion construct to an internet retailing context; in doing so, it conceptualizes online shopper confusion. In addition, the study presents a research framework by tying online shopper confusion to its causes and consequences while taking the particularities of online retailing into consideration. Second, drawing from the extant literature in the field of online store design, this research theoretically identifies various online store elements that confuse shoppers during the internet retail process. Both qualitative and quantitative data collection (Study 1) verify the classification of online confusion causes. Third, this research aims to tie online shopper confusion to consumer reactions. Every construct gains theoretical importance, and relevance from a practical standpoint, when it is related to specific consequences. Accordingly, two experiments theoretically identify and validate consumer responses that are likely occurring during a confusing internet retail process. The findings of this research contribute to the literature in several important ways. The findings highlight the risk of online shopper confusion during the internet retail process. The results offer detailed insights into the confusion potential of various online store elements. The knowledge of consumers’ negative responses to a confusing internet retailing process contributes to explain their online shopper behavior. From a practical perspective, the taxonomy of online confusion causes helps retailers avoid online shopper confusion during the internet retailing process. In addition, the findings reveal which online store elements bear the highest confusion potential. From a more general perspective, the research finds support for the notion that not only products but also the (online) store environment possesses confusion potential. However, stationary and online confusion causes differ in some important respects due to the particularities of internet retailing. Literature review and model development Ample studies in internet retailing have explored the role of attitude in predicting online shopping usage. Most of these studies draw on the technological acceptance model (TAM) (Davis, 1986), which represents an advancement of the theory of reasoned action (Fishbein and Ajzen, 1975). The two main constructs of the TAM are perceived ease of use and perceived usefulness, both of which predict attitude toward a new technology and, in turn, usage intentions and actual use (Davis, 1986). For example, Childers et al. (2001) report that online store characteristics affect ease of use, perceived usefulness, and enjoyment. The importance of these constructs for determining attitude toward interactive online shopping varies depending on the shopping context (hedonic vs utilitarian). Another study (Ha and Stoel, 2009) confirms the importance of these three constructs in determining attitude toward online shopping and, in turn, online shopping intention. In addition, the authors emphasize the relevance of website design as a predictor of e-shopping quality, which reflects an antecedent of trust, ease of use, and enjoyment. In confirmation of this result, Al-Debei et al. (2015) reveal that website quality (defined as “the extent to which the web site design and processes are simple, smooth, reliable and effective,” p. 710) determines the attitude toward online shopping. Accordingly, research has recognized the importance of website design in determining online shopping acceptance. More recently, studies exploring consumers’ online shopping behavior have successfully employed Mehrabian and Russell’s (1974) stimulus-organism-response (S-O-R) model as a theoretical framework (Koo and Ju, 2010; Li et al., 2012). In contrast with the TAM, the S-O-R model does not explore consumers’ acceptance of new technologies in early adoption stages (Al-Debei et al., 2015), but rather explains why shoppers react with approach or avoidance behavior to certain internet environments. Thus, Mehrabian and Russell’s environment psychological model offers an appropriate theoretical foundation to explore the construct online shopper confusion. Following the S-O-R paradigm, Mehrabian and Russell (1974) postulate that various environmental stimuli induce approach or avoidance behavior and that emotional states mediate this relationship. Donovan and Rossiter (1982) were the first researchers to validate the model in a consumer behavior setting. They demonstrate that store environmental stimuli induce the three emotional states of arousal, pleasure, and dominance, which, in turn, result in either approach or avoidance behavior. Since then, the model has served as a theoretical basis in studies exploring shoppers’ responses to store environmental stimuli (e.g. Baker et al., 1992; Bitner, 1992). In line with these extant studies, the current research treats confusing online store elements as stimulus (S). Online shopper confusion represents the organism variable (O) that mediates the relationship between confusing online store elements and responses (R). As online shopper confusion reflects a negative mental state, consumers react with avoidance behavior. In particular, shoppers experiencing online shopper confusion are likely to have lower revisit intentions, higher shopping cart abandonment rates, lower spending time, lower unplanned expenditures, and spread less positive WOM than non-confused shoppers (see Figure 1). While confusion in internet retailing has not attracted research attention so far, studies on physical retailing offer profound conceptualizations of the confusion construct. Extant definitions conceptualize confusion either through its causes (e.g. Walsh et al., 2007) or as a mental state (e.g. Garaus and Wagner, 2016). The current research implements Confusion in internet retailing 479 INTR 28,2 Stimulus (S) Figure 1. Conceptual research framework: online shopper confusion Consequences Online confusion causes Design and navigation 480 Response (R) Organism (O) Informational content Website functionalities Study 1 Online shopper confusion Cognitive Affective Conative – Revisit intention + Shopping cart abandonment – Spending time – Low revisit Unplanned Intentions expenditures – WOM Study 2 and Study 3 both perspectives by identifying the causes (i.e. confusing online store elements) that evoke the mental state online shopper confusion. For the sake of clarity, the terminology used throughout the paper is specified as follows: online confusion causes (e.g. design and navigation) comprise various confusing online store elements (e.g. unstructured layout, unclear layout) that result in the mental state of mind online shopper confusion during the internet retail process. Accordingly, confusion causes/elements represent objective confusion characteristics of online stores, while online shopper confusion reflects a subjectively experienced state of mind, which constitutes a part of the shopping experience. The following subsections elaborate on each variable of the theoretical model in detail. The mental state online shopper confusion Garaus et al. (2015) define (stationary) retail shopper confusion as a mental state that likely occurs during shopping situations. In line with this definition then, online shopper confusion reflects a mental state that occurs during the internet retail process and therefore constitutes a part of the shopping experience. In line with Blackwell et al.’s (2006) consumer decision model, the internet retail process consists of five stages: need recognition, information phase, evaluation of alternatives, purchase decision, and post-evaluation. Studies agree that shoppers generally perceive confusion as negatively and respond with negative consumer responses. While other cognitive challenging states of mind, such as distraction, might result in positive consumer reactions (e.g. decrease of counter arguments) (Petty et al., 1976), confusion reflects a mental state that is perceived as unpleasant (Garaus and Wagner, 2016; Mitchell et al., 2005). However, a much-debated question is whether confusion reflects a cognitive or an emotional construct, or both. For many years, the cognitive perspective represented the prevailing view in the consumer behavior literature (e.g. Mitchell and Papavassiliou, 1999; Mitchell et al., 2005). During the state of confusion, information processing is negatively affected (Turnbull et al., 2000) because the cognitive resources required exceed the cognitive resources available (Anand and Sternthal, 1989). Consumers therefore feel overloaded and inefficient ( Jacoby, 1984; Jacoby et al., 1974). More recently, literature has emerged that considers not only a cognitive but also an emotional and a conative component of the confusion construct (Garaus and Wagner, 2016). This three-dimensional approach also appears in the psychological literature, which proposes that confusion is “a feeling state characterized by bewilderment and uncertainty [cognition], associated with a general failure to control attention [conation] and emotions [affect]” (Terry et al., 2003, p. 127). Neglecting one dimension when exploring a construct can lead to inconsistent results among studies and failure to capture the whole construct under investigation (see Bagozzi et al., 1979; Mayer, 2001). In line with this perspective, well-established constructs have recognized the importance of considering the cognitive, affective, and conative dimensions of the human mind for construct development (e.g. attitude; Fishbein and Ajzen, 1975). Informed by this evidence and in line with extant confusion conceptualizations (Garaus and Wagner, 2016), the current study defines online shopper confusion as a mental state evoked during the internet retail process, capturing a cognitive dimension, an affective dimension, and a conative dimension. Similar to stationary retail shopper confusion (Garaus and Wagner, 2016), online shopper confusion is a reflective, second-order construct (Netemeyer et al., 2003). Accordingly, during the mental state of confusion, changes in these three dimensions, which are operationalized by feelings (Garaus and Wagner, 2016), occur simultaneously. In particular, the cognitive dimension captures negative feelings caused by the exceedance of consumers’ cognitive processing abilities during the mental state of confusion (Schweizer et al., 2006). Confused consumers experience emotions such as anger and frustration (Walsh et al., 2007), representing the affective confusion dimension. Finally, negative feelings (e.g. feeling helpless and lost) associated with an inhibition of shopping task fulfillment reflect the conative dimension (Dogu and Erkip, 2000). Transferring the confusion construct to an online context requires differentiating similar constructs. The construct information overload and online shopper confusion share several characteristics. Information overload occurs when “consumers are provided with too much information at a given time, such that it exceeds their processing limits” (Malhotra, 1982, p. 419). Chen et al. (2009, p. 50) offer a more profound definition of perceived information overload in an online shopping context: “a perception of having too much product information to deal with while making a buying decision.” Accordingly, similar to the mental state of mind online shopper confusion, information overload occurs when cognitive processing abilities are exhausted. However, both constructs differ in three fundamental respects. First, these constructs differ in terms of the number of subsystems involved. While online shopper confusion captures a cognitive, an affective, and a conative component, information overload represents an entirely cognitive phenomenon. Second, online shopper confusion reflects a negative state of mind and therefore lacks positive consumer reactions, while information overload can also lead to positive consumer reactions (Huang, 2003; Jacoby et al., 1974). Third, the extant literature ties information overload to product-related factors, such as the number of alternatives available or the amount of attributes (Lee and Lee, 2004). In contrast, online shopper confusion is not limited in scope to product-related information but captures information provided during the whole internet retail process (e.g. payment and delivery terms). E-satisfaction reflects another state of mind that has attracted research attention in internet retailing research. In contrast with online shopper confusion, satisfaction judgment requires a comparison referent. Thus, studies tie satisfaction with a given online shopping experience to prior online shopping experiences (Evanschitzky et al., 2004) or to brick-and-mortar shopping experiences (Anderson and Srinivasan, 2003). Accordingly, e-satisfaction represents a deliberate evaluation process of the whole shopping experience, which includes both favorable and unfavorable incidents. Thus, in accordance with evidence in stationary retailing (see Foxman et al., 1990; Mitchell and Papavassiliou, 1999; Wang and Shukla, 2013; Walsh and Mitchell, 2010), confusion likely contributes to e-satisfaction, in addition to all other positive and negative experiences during a particular online shopping trip. Compared with e-satisfaction, the exploration of online shopper confusion offers a more focused picture of and detailed insights into consumers’ feelings during negative shopping experiences. Confusion in internet retailing 481 INTR 28,2 482 Confusing online store elements Recent studies in internet retailing have highlighted the importance of understanding consumers’ interactions with and perceptions of a website and the influence on their behavior for the internet retailing success (Cho, 2014; Montoya-Weiss et al., 2003). However, no study so far has explored confusion during the internet retail process, though some studies have theoretically postulated the potential of specific online store elements to evoke negative and confusing shopping experiences. From this initial evidence, three major confusion causes (design and navigation, informational content, and website functionalities) emerged, each comprising various confusing online store elements. Design and navigation. Specific website features might stimulate need recognition (Alhudaithy and Kitchen, 2009); however, they can also create confusion (Hoque and Lohse, 1999; Lohse, 1993). Animations, various color schemes, and innovative elements constitute website design and might entertain shoppers, but they also might distract them from their ultimate shopping goal. If these elements delay completion of the shopping task, shoppers easily become annoyed (Eroglu et al., 2001). A navigational structure describes the organization and hierarchical layout of the website’s content. It reflects consumers’ movement through the website, as characterized by the number of clicks it takes to go through the content (Dailey, 2004). Prior research has found that complex websites negatively affect pleasure and inhibit consumers’ shopping tasks (Mai et al., 2014; Wang et al., 2011). An unclear and disorganized store structure evokes confusion (Hausman and Siekpe, 2009; Koo, 2006). In addition, a poor structure or non-working navigation cues (e.g. buttons, search function) can result in irritation and frustration (Dailey, 2004), both of which are associated with confusion (Garaus and Wagner, 2016); as such, confusion is likely to arise from poorly structured online stores with non-working navigation cues. Informational content. Website informational content comprises the sum of communicated content on the website and consists of information utility (the extent to which consumers perceive the information as useful), information accuracy (the extent of perceived correctness of information), information timeliness (consumers’ up-to-date perceptions) (Montoya-Weiss et al., 2003), and information format (Hong et al., 2004). Several researchers have stressed the confusion potential of information on websites (Chen and Dibb, 2010; Everad and Galletta, 2006; Konradt et al., 2003). Cho et al. (2006) argue that internet retailing has more confusion potential than brick-and-mortar stores because of the unlimited space of websites to provide information. The wealth of information likely overloads shoppers (Lee and Lee, 2004). Because human beings’ cognitive processing abilities are limited, individuals process information efficiently to a certain threshold (Sicilia and Ruiz 2010); however, when the amount of information exceeds this threshold, consumers engage in dysfunctional behavior and make poorer decisions ( Jacoby et al., 1974). If consumers deem information as useless, inaccurate, or outdated, they need to acquire further information. This search task requires additional cognitive abilities and, in turn, raises the risk of confusion. Moreover, inappropriate terminology causes confusion (Everad and Galletta, 2006). Similarly, hard-to-find information or information that is not available entails enhanced cognitive processing abilities and inhibits shopping goal achievement (Griffith, 2005). As a consequence, useless, inaccurate, or outdated information, as well as poorly organized information, are assumed to confuse shoppers during the internet retail process. Website functionalities. Website functionalities comprise all elements related to operational procedures during the internet retail process, including website links, order forms, and customer support. Consumers use website links and might contact the internet retailer in different stages of the shopping process (e.g. when requiring additional information in the information phase or support for product use in the post-purchase stage). Prior research has identified a confusing transaction process as retailer failure (Holloway and Beatty, 2003) and as a driver of online shopping hesitation (Cho et al., 2006). In addition, some specific features that differentiate the online shopping process from the physical retail process possess confusion potential. For example, the lack of physical presence of products and personal advice largely distinguishes online shopping from purchases made in brick-and-mortar stores (e.g. Holloway and Beatty, 2003). Informed by this theoretical evidence, poor website functionalities are likely to confuse shoppers during the internet retail process. Consequences of online shopper confusion In contrast with an interesting or a complex website design (which might result in positive responses at moderate levels; Mai et al., 2014), confusion implies nothing positive about a website, causing negative consumer reactions (see Figure 1). Consumers tend to dislike confusing physical service or retail encounters (Bitner, 1992), resulting in low revisit intentions (Garaus and Wagner, 2016). Revisit intentions are related to actual purchase behavior, and hence reflect an important determinant of internet retailing (Zheng et al., 2017). Negative experiences influence behavior more strongly than positive experiences (Baumeister et al., 2001), so shoppers will likely not return to a confusing online store because they neither enjoy the online shopping experience nor fulfill their shopping trip efficiently. A major consequence of consumer confusion in physical retail environments is shopping abandonment (Mitchell and Papavassiliou, 1999). Shopping cart abandonment is also an undesirable behavioral reaction in online retailing. It describes “consumers’ placement of item(s) in their online shopping cart without making a purchase of any item(s) during that online shopping session” (Kukar-Kinney and Close, 2010, p. 240). Confused shoppers feel angry and helpless to pursue their shopping goals, so they give up and do not complete their shopping trip. Indeed, prior research provides empirical evidence that confusion leads to online shopping hesitation (Cho et al., 2006). In a confusing online store environment, shoppers intend to escape this confusing situation as soon as possible. Research demonstrates that online store perception affects the desire to stay in an online store and patronage intention (Kim et al., 2007). In stationary retailing, confusion negatively affects spending time (Garaus and Wagner, 2016). Shen and Khalifa (2012) point to the relevance of impulse buying in an online retail context. The overall online shopping experience correlates with the number of purchases (Anderson and Srinivasan, 2003). Shoppers experience confusion as a negative state and thus do not enjoy their shopping drip and do not spend more money than intended. While a favorable online store design leads to higher purchase intention (e.g. Shen and Khalifa, 2012; Richard, 2005), a confusing store design is assumed to negatively influence unplanned purchases. Finally, López-López et al. (2014) show that consumers tend to share negative emotions caused by unsatisfactory service experiences with other people. While positive online shopping experiences evoke positive WOM (Anderson and Srinivasan, 2003), a confusing shopping experience is assumed to reduce positive WOM. Methodology From an epistemological perspective, this research follows a positivist research approach. In line with this paradigm, the overall objective of the empirical studies is to generate reliable and valid results. Particular effort is devoted to the minimization of the subjective Confusion in internet retailing 483 INTR 28,2 484 influence of the researcher (Becker and Niehaves, 2007). To achieve this aim and avoid inherent method biases (Davis et al., 2011), the methodological part of the research employs a sequential mixed-method design using within- (Study 1) and across-method triangulation (Study 2 and Study 3) (see Jick, 1979). Such a research design benefits from strong and robust results with a high degree of external validity (Davis et al., 2011). In addition, the mixed-method design offers a holistic understanding of the phenomenon of online shopper confusion ( Jick, 1979). Four data collections constitute three studies, each of which builds on the other. Thus, the data of each study are analyzed and interpreted before implementation of another method. This procedure allows for informing subsequent studies and expanding insights into the research problem (Davis et al., 2011) and is particularly suitable for the exploration of novel constructs (Creswell, 2009). The objective of Study 1, which consists of an exploratory and a descriptive data collection, is to identify confusing online store elements. Study 2 and Study 3 test the proposed consequences of online shopper confusion with causal research designs. Study 1: identification of online shopper confusion causes Study design. Premised on the notions and statements of the extant literature, the theoretical part of this research identifies three online confusion causes, each consisting of confusing online store elements. Nevertheless, as no study has specifically explored confusion in internet retailing, the three theoretically derived online confusion causes may not fully capture the nature of online shopper confusion. These causes represent an explicit theoretical perspective that guides the empirical identification of online confusion causes and confusing online store elements (Creswell, 2009). The exploratory nature of Study 1 qualifies for a sequential exploratory design (Creswell, 2009) that employs across-methods triangulation ( Jick, 1979). In the first phase, qualitative data are collected, followed by a second phase in which quantitative descriptive data are collected; the data collection of the second phase builds on the results of the first phase (Davis et al., 2011). Such an approach is particularly useful for the initial exploration of a phenomenon or an emergent theory (Creswell, 2009) and thus is suitable for the exploration of the new construct online shopper confusion. Considering the scope of the research, an online survey was considered an appropriate data collection method. Moreover, in line with the positivism research paradigm applied herein, an online survey benefits from the elimination of interviewer bias and therefore fulfills the requirement of minimizing the subjective influence of the researcher. Respondents might feel uncomfortable admitting confusion in shopping situations; therefore, a projective technique (see Chang et al., 2013) served as the data collection instrument. Research indicates that projective questions provide valid data that are less biased by socially desirable responses (Fisher, 1993). The stimulus was drawn by means of an open source software with effort to create a realistic talking situation. A cartoon showed two people chatting on a street. The first person said, “I have recently ordered something at an online shop.” The second person answered, “I experience online shops confusing, because […].” The subsequent open-ended question asked respondents to complete the sentence; they were allowed to provide as many statements as they wanted. Respondents were recruited by students in marketing classes, who received course credit for participant acquisition. Students forwarded the link to family members, other students, and work colleagues, resulting in a convenience sample of 100 respondents (47 women, mean age: 37 years). The sampling procedure of the qualitative data collection limits the generalizability of the findings. To overcome this drawback, a large-scale descriptive data collection based on a pre-defined quota representing the population under investigation (in age and gender) aimed to validate online shopper confusion consequences. The 23 confusing online store elements obtained through the projective technique constituted the items for the questionnaire. Respondents were asked to indicate on a five-point Likert scale (1 ¼ do not agree at all, 5 ¼ totally agree) the extent to which they agreed that the respective elements evoke confusion. The questionnaire proceeded with items assessing demographic data, internet and online shopping experience, and online shopping motivation. A personal survey was chosen as data collection method. In total, 26 business administration students received course credit for interviewing 30 individuals each based on an individual quota. This procedure resulted in a final sample of 733 respondents. The sample represents online shoppers in the country under investigation in terms of age and gender; however, the 25-34 age group was slightly over-presented, while the oldest age group (55-75) was underrepresented, which is likely due to the ease/difficulty in reaching these target groups (see Table I). Results. The data of the qualitative data collection were evaluated with content analysis in line with the procedure and quality criteria of Kassarjian (1977) and Keaveney (1995). Each statement (i.e. sentence completion) reflected a unit of analysis. After elimination of meaningless entries, 100 respondents provided 114 statements. In a first step, one coder reduced the statements into basic units (unitizing). For example, statements such “There is not sufficient information regarding shipping costs” were unitized into “limited Study 1 (n ¼ 733) 38 Study 2 (n ¼ 185) 36 Gender (%) Men Women 50.7 49.3 41.2 58.8 50 50 Education (%) University High school Vocational school Apprenticeship Compulsory schooling 38.5 32.1 11.3 11.6 6.4 38.4 46.3 7.3 6.2 1.7 37 61.1 0 0.9 0.9 1.8 4.2 4.5 5.3 12.7 71.4 Mean (SD) 3.78 (1.12) 3.32 (1.19) – 4.9 3.8 5.4 15.2 70.7 Mean (SD) 3.82 (1.10) 3.25 (1.22) – 0 0.9 0 2.8 96.3 Mean (SD) 4.21 (0.84) 3.90 (1.00) Mean age Frequency of internet usage (%) Nevera Every few weeks Once or twice a week Three for five times a week Once a day Several times a day Internet experience Online shopping experience Confusion in internet retailing 485 Study 3 (n ¼ 107) 24 Online shopping motivation Hedonic beliefs 3.40 (0.95) 3.30 (1.06) 3.89 (1.00) Utilitarian beliefs 3.48 (0.89) 3.45 (0.96) 3.73 (0.90) Notes: aAnswer category “Never” was only available in Study 1; Study 2 use an online survey and Study 3 a student sample, making this answer category irrelevant; internet experience, online shopping experience, Table I. and online shopping motivation were measured on five-point Likert scales (1 ¼ do not agree at all, Sample characteristics 5 ¼ totally agree) (study 1-study 3) INTR 28,2 486 Table II. Classification system for online confusion causes information regarding shipping costs,” and statements such as “They all have a different structure” were unitized into “different structure.” The purpose of this step was to eliminate unnecessary words in each statement to facilitate further analysis. In the second step, three coders independently coded the content material. The theoretically derived online confusion causes (design and navigation, informational content, and website functionalities) served as a preliminary category system. Thus, a deductive approach guided the content analysis (see Table II). Individual variables that cause confusion during the online shopping process (e.g. experience with online shopping) did not receive consideration in the categorization process, as individual traits do not fulfill the requirement of objective confusion characteristics. The judges were instructed to add additional confusion categories if necessary; however, no new categories emerged. Cohen’s κ served as an indicator of intercoder reliability (or interjudge reliability), which measures “whether different judges classify the same phenomena into the same categories” (Keaveney, 1995, p. 73). Values above 80 percent constitute a good result (Kassarjian, 1977). High agreement among the three judges resulted in an average intercoder reliability of 0.82 (Cohen’s κ), representing an acceptable result. Statements related to the first category design and navigation included “The offers are unclearly arranged,” “There is no clear structure,” “One cannot conduct a search,” and “I can’t find items quickly.” The second confusion category reflects misleading informational content. As proposed in the theoretical part, respondents indicated that a missing cost overview, unclear product information, and too little or too much information confused them. These statements seem to reflect the utility dimension of information, such that information is provided in a way that diminishes its usefulness and confuses shoppers. Information accuracy was represented by statements such as “Ambiguous information as regards to delivery costs” and “Sometimes, items are not clearly marked.” Several respondents mentioned information formatting issues as a confusion driver: “There is [too] much information printed in small font” and “Contract terms are often hidden.” Finally, the third online confusion cause comprises website functionalities. Answers included payment issues, such as “Payment is always very complicated,” “Shops accept only debit cards,” and “Terms of payment are explained at the very end.” The lack of personal advice was identified as another confusion driver within this category: “There is no qualified personnel available for advice” and “There is no contact person.” Statements indicating problems with reclamation were also allocated to the website functionalities category (see Table II). A descriptive analysis evaluated the confusion potential of each online store element as obtained through the second data collection. The average internet experience was 3.78, and the average online shopping experience was 3.32. Respondents shopped for both hedonic and utilitarian reasons ( for sample characteristics, see Table I). Table III presents the means and standard deviations of each item arranged according to the three online confusion causes. On an item level, all means exceeded the scale mid-point of 3.00, with the exception of the item “too much information.” Problems with reclamation exhibited the most confusion potential, followed by hidden information and complicated payment. Online confusion causes Descriptions Design and navigation Informational content Website functionalities Organization of content and navigation through the website Format, content, and timeliness of information Payment, order procurement, and reclamation issues Online confusion causes Confusing online store elements Design and navigation Unstructured layout Unclear layout Overloaded design Poor orientation Long search time Overall mean 3.31 3.30 3.20 3.19 3.11 3.22 Informational content Hidden information Unspecific information Unclear product information Too little information Information not up-to-date No overview of costs Too much information Overall mean 3.57 3.45 3.43 3.26 3.21 3.15 2.84 3.27 Website functionalities Problems with reclamation Complicated payment process Order process interrupted Payment not working Payment options differ among various stores No contact person Payment options not clear Limited payment options No advice Unwieldy order process Product out of stock Overall mean Note: For each online confusion cause, confusing online store elements are sorted according to confusion potential (measured on a five-point Likert scale (1 ¼ do not agree at all, 5 ¼ totally agree)) M 3.59 3.48 3.47 3.46 3.29 3.29 3.28 3.26 3.24 3.22 3.13 3.34 their The results of this study provide first empirical evidence of the confusion potential of the internet retail process. The findings confirm the category system developed from extant literature. The study employed a projective technique to avoid any social desirability bias at this early stage of research. The answers obtained are relatively consistent among respondents and thus affirm the stability of the conceptual online confusion causes framework. Data from the second data collection validated the results with a representative sample and offer insights into the confusion potential of each specific online store element. Study 2: online shopper confusion consequences: fictitious shopping scenarios Study design. From both a practical and a scientific point of view, it is of interest to test whether confusion is linked to specific outcome variables. Prior research on stationary retailing has demonstrated that shoppers react with negative responses to confusing shopping situations. Drawing on these notions, the online shopper confusion framework suggests that online shopper confusion negatively affects revisit intentions, shopping cart abandonment, spending time, unplanned expenditures, and WOM (see Figure 1). Study 2 aims to test these proposed consequences of online shopper confusion with a causal research design. A one-factor, between-subjects design was employed, with confusion as the manipulated variable. An online survey employed fictitious written online shopping scenarios, with one scenario describing a confusing internet retail process and the other a non-confusing internet retail process. Respondents were randomly allocated to one of the Confusion in internet retailing 487 Table III. Categorization of confusing online store elements into online confusion causes INTR 28,2 488 experimental conditions. The first page of the questionnaire instructed respondents to read the scenario carefully and to put themselves into the described shopping situation. Then, a short text of four paragraphs described a confusing/non-confusing internet retailing process. The results of Study 1 served as basis for creating fictitious shopping scenarios. In the confusing shopping scenario, the online store had an unusual and unclear structure, making the orientation difficult. Many products and information were presented, the search function was underdeveloped, and illustrations of the products had poor quality. In addition, the preferred payment option was not available, and the shopping cart was missing. In the non-confusing shopping scenario, these elements were presented in a favorable way (i.e. the online store had a usual and clear structure, a comfortable amount of information was presented, the search function worked, the preferred payment option was available, and the ordering process went smoothly). After stimulus exposure, respondents answered questions about what they would feel and how they would react in such a shopping situation. The 13-item retail shopper confusion scale developed by Garaus and Wagner (2016) was used to measure confusion. The consequences shopping cart abandonment and unplanned expenditures reflect concrete and singular constructs (Donovan and Rossiter, 1982; Rajamma et al., 2009), justifying the assessment by a single item (Bergkvist and Rossiter, 2007; Rossiter, 2002). Items assessing revisit intentions (Hausman and Siekpe, 2009), spending time (Kim et al., 2007), and reduced WOM ( Janda et al., 2002) measured the proposed consequences (see Appendix). All measures consisted of five-point Likert scales (1 ¼ low agreement, 5 ¼ high agreement). The questionnaire concluded with questions about age, gender, educational background, internet and online shopping experience, and online shopping motivation. The online survey produced 185 completely answered questionnaires (41 percent men, mean age: 36 years, mean internet experience: 3.82, mean online shopping experience: 3.25; for sample characteristics, see Table I). The scales exhibited satisfactory psychometric properties (Cronbach’s α above 0.7, significant correlations among items), justifying the calculation of composite scores ( for item reliabilities, see Appendix). Results. An analysis of variance with the average confusion score as the dependent variable and the experimental setting as the independent variable revealed that respondents perceived the confusing scenario as significantly more confusing (M ¼ 3.72, SD ¼ 0.69) than the non-confusing scenario (M ¼ 1.89, SD ¼ 0.75, F(1,183) ¼ 296.68, po0.01). A multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) tested the influence of online shopper confusion on the theoretically derived consequences. The analysis yielded a significant model (Pillai’s trace V ¼ 0.75, F(5, 162) ¼ 95.42, p o 0.01). The results confirmed all proposed consequences (see Table IV ). The results confirmed the assumption that confusion drives low revisit intentions (3.87 vs 1.43, F(1, 183) ¼ 322.89, p o0.01, η2 ¼ 0.64). In the confusing retail process condition, respondents indicated that they would abandon their shopping trip (1.61 vs 4.37, F(1, 183) ¼ 262.30, p o0.01, η2 ¼ 0.59). Furthermore, confused shoppers tried to escape a confusing online shopping situation as soon as possible, leading to reduced spending time (2.64 vs 1.25, F(1, 183) ¼ 126.15, p o0.01, η2 ¼ 0.41). In the confusing internet retail process, respondents indicated that they would have less unplanned expenditures than in the non-confusing online shopping situation (2.62 vs 1.33, F(1, 183) ¼ 74.10, p o0.01, η2 ¼ 0.29). Finally, confusion hindered shoppers from spreading positive WOM (3.40 vs 1.28, F(1, 185) ¼ 255.96, p o0.01, η2 ¼ 0.58). Study 2 aimed to test the negative consequences of online shopper confusion. The analysis revealed that consumers respond negatively to confusion during the internet Consequence Non-confusing M SD Confusing M SD 2 Difference F p Effect size (η ) Study 2: fictitious online shopping scenarios Revisit intention 3.87 (1.14) Shopping cart abandonment 1.61 (1.13) Spending time 2.64 (1.15) Unplanned expenditures 2.62 (1.27) WOM 3.40 (1.15) 1.43 4.37 1.25 1.33 1.28 (0.69) (1.15) (0.49) (0.75) (0.63) 2.44 2.76 1.39 1.29 2.12 322.89 262.30 126.15 74.10 255.96 o0.01 o0.01 o0.01 o0.01 o0.01 0.64 0.59 0.41 0.29 0.58 Study 3: real online shopping experiences Revisit intention 4.37 (0.82) Shopping cart abandonment 1.73 (1.17) Spending time 3.50 (0.92) Unplanned expenditures 3.65 (1.44) WOM 3.98 (0.74) 3.11 2.64 2.63 2.52 2.56 (1.15) (1.71) (1.29) (1.59) (1.16) 1.26 0.91 0.87 1.13 1.42 44.05 10.61 16.92 14.59 60.06 o0.01 o0.01 o0.01 o0.01 o0.01 0.30 0.09 0.14 0.12 0.36 retail process. Medium-to-large effect sizes provide empirical evidence of the relevance of the confusion construct in explaining consumers’ negative responses to online shopping situations. All identified negative consumer reactions are of high relevance for e-retailers. Study 3: online shopper confusion consequences: real shopping experiences Study design. While the results of Study 2 offer important insights into online shopper confusion consequences, the findings should be interpreted with caution in terms of their generalizability. The employment of fictitious shopping scenarios benefits from high internal validity but suffers from limited external validity. To account for this limitation, Study 3 uses the recall of real shopping experiences as a stimulus for testing the postulated consequences (see Figure 1) and employs a one-factor (confusing vs non-confusing real online shopping experiences) between-subjects design. This approach is based on the critical incident technique, which aims to collect data on extremely positive or negative incidents (Flanagan, 1954). Internet research studies have successfully applied this technique (e.g. Holloway and Beatty, 2003, 2008). Moreover, past shopping experiences have served as a stimulus when exploring emotions in consumption situations (e.g. Machleit and Eroglu, 2000; Richins, 1997). Business administration students were randomly allocated to one of the two groups. Students are more likely to experience online shopping trips negatively (see Cho et al., 2006) and therefore represent a reasonable target group for examining online shopper confusion. The first page of a paper-and-pencil questionnaire informed respondents about the subject of the study (i.e. exploring what consumers feel during confusing online shopping experiences). In the confusing group, few examples of a confusing online shopping experience were provided (e.g. unstructured layout, hidden information, problems with reclamation complicated payment process); in the control group, these examples were provided in a non-confusing way. The provision of the examples helped respondents understand the confusion construct. Afterward, respondents were asked to recall an online shopping situation they experienced as confusing (non-confusing). To ensure that they recalled and elaborated on a confusing shopping situation, they were asked why they considered the shopping situation confusing (non-confusing) and to describe the situation in as much detail as possible. The questionnaire proceeded with an assessment of online shopper confusion. Subsequently, respondents were asked how they behaved in the shopping situation. For all constructs, the same items as in Study 1 were used (the Study 1 items were slightly adapted to reflect real and not anticipated behavior). The questionnaire Confusion in internet retailing 489 Table IV. Comparison of mean scores for online shopper confusion consequences INTR 28,2 490 ended with items assessing demographic data, internet and online shopping experience, and online shopping motivation. The data collection resulted in 107 questionnaires (50 percent men, mean age: 24 years, mean internet experience: 4.21, mean online shopping experience: 3.90; for sample characteristics, see Table I). The reliability analysis exhibited satisfying results for all constructs ( for item reliabilities, see Appendix). Composite scores were calculated for the subsequent analysis. Results. Descriptions of confusing online shopping experiences are mainly consistent with the confusing online store elements identified in Studies 1 and 2, and therefore require no further elaboration. Detailed descriptions of confusing/non-confusing shopping experiences among all respondents confirm their ability to recall past shopping experiences. The majority of respondents (60 percent) experienced confusion when shopping online for clothes, followed by shopping for general merchandise (25 percent, e.g. Amazon.com). The remaining respondents recalled confusing shopping experiences in food stores, jewelers, opticians, ticket services, and travel operators. In the non-confusing shopping condition, 43.1 percent of respondents recalled an online shopping trip in a clothing store, and 34.5 percent described online shopping in a store selling general merchandise (e.g. Amazon.com). The remaining respondents recalled shopping experiences in stores selling cosmetics, food, games, jewelry, travel, leisure activities, music, and sports equipment. An analysis of variance (F(1,105) ¼ 142.04, p o 0.01, η2 ¼ 0.58) tested whether the confusion manipulation worked as intended. Respondents in the confusing condition experienced the shopping situation as more confusing (3.44) than respondents in the non-confusing condition (1.79). A MANOVA involving the condition (confusing vs non-confusing) as a between-subjects factor and the proposed consequences (see Figure 1) as the dependent variables yielded a main effect of online shopper confusion on all consequences (Pillai’s trace V ¼ 0.42, F(5,101) ¼ 14.61, p o 0.01) (see Table IV ). As predicted, respondents in the confusing condition reported lower intentions to revisit the store (4.37 vs 3.11, F(1,105) ¼ 44.05, p o 0.01, η2 ¼ 0.30). Respondents also had a higher shopping cart abandonment rate in the confusing internet retail process (1.73 vs 2.64, F(1,105) ¼ 10.61, p o 0.01, η2 ¼ 0.09). Moreover, a comparison of time spent between the two conditions showed that confused shoppers spent less time in the store (3.50 vs 2.63, F(1,105) ¼ 16.92, p o 0.01, η2 ¼ 0.14). In the confusing condition, respondents also had less unplanned expenditures (3.65 vs 2.52, F(1,105) ¼ 14.59, p o 0.01, η2 ¼ 0.12). Finally, confused shoppers were less likely to spread positive WOM than non-confused shoppers (3.98 vs 2.56; F(1,105) ¼ 60.06, p o 0.01, η2 ¼ 0.36). Overall, the results validate the findings of Study 2 and confirm that shoppers react negatively to confusing online shopping experiences. Discussion While various studies have demonstrated the effect of physical surroundings in brick-andmortar stores, the research on the influence of online store design on consumer behavior is still at the beginning stage. Considering that online retailers have operated only for 20 years (e.g. Amazon.com), compared with traditional retailing, which has existed for more than 100 years, the lack of knowledge on online shoppers’ responses to specific online store elements is not surprising. The current study attempts to contribute to a better understanding of consumers’ mental states and behavioral responses during the internet retail process. Recent research has examined popular consumer behavior constructs under an online retailing perspective (e.g. information overload (Chen et al., 2009); e-satisfaction (Evanschitzky et al., 2004)). Although constructs developed under the premise of traditional shopping are applicable to online services to some extent, the internet retail process is characterized by particular characteristics (e.g. lack of possibility to evaluate products or services before the transaction; Zheng et al., 2017) that justify the amendment of constructs to an online context. In line with such an approach, the current research adapts the confusion construct (Garaus et al., 2015; Walsh and Mitchell, 2010) to the internet retail process. Confusion is experienced as a negative mental state that consumers tend to avoid. Drawing from theoretically derived conceptualizations of confusion, this research identified 23 confusing online store elements that constitute three online confusion causes. Online shopper confusion results in undesirable consumer behavior. Theoretical implications This research explores confusion in an internet retailing context. In doing so, it adds a new construct of high relevance to the internet retailing literature. This study not only develops a theoretically well-founded conceptualization of the mental state online shopper confusion but also integrates online shopper confusion into a comprehensive research framework. On the one hand, the development of this research framework benefits by considering context-specific causes and consequences of online shopper confusion; on the other hand, it affords researchers a holistic perspective on the online shopper confusion construct. Furthermore, by exploring online shopper confusion, this research answers the call to explore a specific mental state occurring during negative online shopping incidents (e.g. Holloway and Beatty, 2003). The findings provide theoretical and empirical evidence of the risk of confusion during the internet retail process. The identification of confusing online store elements offers insight into the confusion potential of specific elements of the internet retail process. As expected, online shopper confusion causes vary considerably from stationary consumer confusion causes. The inclusion of various elements particular to internet retailing justifies the necessity of differentiating online shopper confusion from extant confusion constructs (e.g. Garaus and Wagner, 2016; Walsh and Mitchell, 2010). In stationary retailing, confusing elements include ambient, design, and social factors (Garaus et al., 2015), whereas the three major confusion factors design and navigation, information content, and website functionalities emerge in online shopping situations. These differences are mainly due to the lack (or limitation) of ambient and social factors in retailing. Nevertheless, for both online shopper confusion and stationary retail shopper confusion, the design and navigation category exhibits some similarities. A poor orientation and an unclear customer flow lead to confusion in brick-and-mortar retailing (Garaus et al., 2015); similarly, poor orientation and an unstructured or unclear online store layout result in confusion in online retailing. Aggressive visual merchandising evokes confusion in stationary retailing, which is comparable to an overloaded design in online retailing. In addition, in both retail formats, different payment methods confuse shoppers. Given these similarities and reasonable differences among stationary and online shopper confusion, the findings are face valid. Overall, a comparison of stationary and online shopper confusion causes leads to the conclusion that confusion in internet retailing is stronger with regard to the shopping process itself (e.g. problems with reclamation, complicated payment process, order process interrupted). The online shopper confusion cause website functionalities has the highest number of confusing online store elements, and the average confusion potential in this category was the highest among the three online confusion causes. As expected, online shopper confusion exerts a strong negative influence on consumer responses. In particular, the results indicate that confusion negatively affects revisit intentions. As with many other consumer behavior constructs, consumer loyalty has Confusion in internet retailing 491 INTR 28,2 492 attracted recent research attention in the context of online shopping. Researchers have acknowledged the importance of retaining customers for success because online customer acquisition costs are often higher than the average lifetime value of customers (Hoffman and Novak, 2000). In addition, reduced spending time reflects another negative behavioral reaction to confusion. Moreover, confusion evoked by the internet retail process results in shopping cart abandonment. Still, the reasons for shopping cart abandonment are largely unexplored (e.g. Cho et al., 2006). By identifying a confusing store environment as a driver of shopping cart abandonment, the current research contributes to the understanding of this undesirable online shopper behavior. Online retailers need to expend more effort in prompting impulse purchases without any physical evidence. Brick-and-mortar stores rely on environmental stimulation and personal advice to increase unplanned expenditures; both stimuli are not available in virtual stores. Therefore, e-retailers have a comparative disadvantage in this context and should invest effort to trigger impulse purchases. Study 2 and Study 3 demonstrate that a confusing internet retail process explains low impulse purchase rates; thus, retailers should try to counteract these low rates by providing a clear and non-confusing retail process. Finally, with confusion identified as a factor that reduces WOM, retailers should pay special attention to avoid confusion so as to create favorable WOM. Although Study 3 validates the findings of Study 2, the effect sizes of the tested consequences vary considerably between the two studies. The smaller effect sizes of Study 3 were expected to some extent because real confusing online shopping experiences might not reach the same confusion potential as confusion manipulated in fictitious online shopping scenarios. In Study 2, revisit intentions, shopping cart abandonment, and WOM exhibit the largest effect sizes, indicating the high relevance of both consumer reactions. While revisit intentions and WOM exhibit the largest effect sizes in Study 3 as well, anticipated shopping cart abandonment differs from real shopping cart abandonment. This finding helps guide further research on shopping cart abandonment by underscoring the importance of assessing real behavior. Managerial implications Holloway and Beatty (2003) report that online retailers are not adequately recovering from failures and stress the importance of avoiding negative consumer shopping experiences. Some research effort has already been devoted to internet retail failures. However, little is known about the reasons for negative online shopping experiences. By exploring the specific mental state online shopper confusion, the current research offers important insights into shoppers’ online behavior from a practical perspective. First, the conceptualization of the online shopper confusion construct helps managers understand why shoppers might experience online shopping negatively and react with undesirable responses. This knowledge might help marketers create recovery strategies tailored to a confusing online store environment by giving consumers some indication of what went wrong and why. Second, the negative consumer responses associated with confusing online store elements highlight the importance of a non-confusing store design. Confusion negatively affects not only short-term consequences (i.e. shopping cart abandonment, spending time, and unplanned expenditures) but also long-term consequences (i.e. revisit intention and WOM). Despite the relevance of shopping cart abandonment from both a theoretical and practical perspective, research on the mental states that trigger such behavior is scarce. Retailers confronted with the undesirable consumer responses explored in this research should examine the confusion potential of the online store. Third, the findings provide practitioners with concrete insights into how the internet retail process confuses shoppers. The taxonomy of confusion causes should help managers assess the confusion potential of their existing online stores and consider confusion issues in the development of new online stores. Especially, clothes stores and stores selling general merchandise bear the risk of confusing shoppers. From a more general perspective, the results reveal the importance of focusing not only on the creation of positive experiences but also on the avoidance of negative experiences. This implication is even more pronounced when considering that negative information outweighs positive information (Mizerski, 1982) and that recovery strategies do not always turn out to be successful (Holloway and Beatty, 2003). Finally, the taxonomy of confusing online store elements can be used by policy makers who review consumer complaints about a confusing and misleading online purchase process. For example, they could use the knowledge of confusion drivers in the online retail environment to set a standard among different industries. Limitations and further research The findings reveal several areas that further research might extend. The study set out to determine confusion causes and consequences among various industries. Consequently, data collection was not limited to a specific internet retail sector. Related research indicates that not all facets of website appeal can be generalized to all sectors. In contrast, differences exist among various service and product domains (Blake et al., 2017). Further research could test whether specific sectors have a higher risk of confusion. In a similar vein, personality characteristics might influence the extent of online shopper confusion experienced in online retailing. Yan et al. (2017) report that individuals with high levels of self-efficacy seem to be better able to cope with the plenty of information available in internet services. In line with this finding, it could be argued that self-efficacy in online shopping might reduce online shopper confusion. Online shopper confusion might be of relevance not only in a retailing environment but also in e-service industries (e.g. online banking), though this assumption requires further empirical evidence. The identified confusion causes might serve as an initial step in the development of an index to assess online shopper confusion. Although this study shows severe consequences of online shopper confusion, not all possible consequences have been examined. For example, online shopper confusion might have a negative impact not only on unplanned purchases but on planned purchases as well. It would be worthwhile to test consumers’ reactions to different levels of confusion in real online shopping situations with additional outcome variables of interest, such as sales data. In addition, linking confusion at the different stages of the internet retail process to particular outcome variables would be useful. Finally, the segmentation of shoppers prone to online shopper confusion is another rich field for study. Central to such research would be the question whether some consumer groups bear a higher risk of experiencing online shopper confusion than other groups. Such a classification of online shoppers prone to confusion would help retailers target their web store design to their main customers. In addition, the knowledge of consumer characteristics that foster confusion would help shed light on the personality of online shoppers. References Al-Debei, M.M., Akroush, M.N. and Ashouri, M.I. (2015), “Consumer attitudes towards online shopping”, Internet Research, Vol. 25 No. 5, pp. 707-733. Alhudaithy, A.I. and Kitchen, P.J. (2009), “Rethinking models of technology adoption for internet banking: the role of website features”, Journal of Financial Services Marketing, Vol. 14 No. 1, pp. 56-69. Confusion in internet retailing 493 INTR 28,2 Anand, P. and Sternthal, B. (1989), “Strategies for designing persuasive messages: deductions from the resource matching hypothesis”, in Cafferata, P. and Tybout, A.M. (Eds), Cognitive and Affective Responses to Advertising, Lexington Books/D.C. Heath, Lexington, MA, pp. 135-159. Anderson, R.E. and Srinivasan, S.S. (2003), “E-satisfaction and e-loyalty: a contingency framework”, Psychology & Marketing, Vol. 20 No. 2, pp. 123-138. 494 Bagozzi, R.P., Tybout, A.M., Craig, C.S. and Sternthal, B. (1979), “The construct validity of the tripartite classification of attitudes”, Journal of Marketing Research, Vol. 16 No. 1, pp. 88-95. Baker, J., Levy, M. and Grewal, D. (1992), “An experimental approach to making retail store environmental decisions”, Journal of Retailing, Vol. 68 No. 4, pp. 445-460. Baumeister, R.F., Bratslavsky, E., Finkenauer, C. and Vohs, K.D. (2001), “Bad is stronger than good”, Review of General Psychology, Vol. 5 No. 4, pp. 323-370. Becker, J. and Niehaves, B. (2007), “Epistemological perspectives on IS research: a framework for analysing and systematizing epistemological assumptions”, Information Systems Journal, Vol. 17 No. 2, pp. 197-214. Bergkvist, L. and Rossiter, J.R. (2007), “The predictive validity of multiple-item versus single-item measures of the same constructs”, Journal of Marketing Research, Vol. 44 No. 2, pp. 175-184. Bitner, M.J. (1992), “Servicescapes: the impact of physical surroundings on customers and employees”, Journal of Marketing, Vol. 56 No. 2, pp. 57-71. Blackwell, R.D., Miniard, P.W. and Engel, J.F. (2006), Consumer Behavior, 10th international student ed., Thomson/South-Western, Mason, OH. Blake, B.F., Given, S., Neuendorf, K.A. and Horvath, M. (2017), “Generalizing the appeal of B2C site features across domains”, Internet Research, Vol. 27 No. 4, pp. 730-751. Chang, H.H., Rizal, H. and Amin, H. (2013), “The determinants of consumer behavior towards email advertisement”, Internet Research, Vol. 23 No. 3, pp. 316-337. Chen, J. and Dibb, S. (2010), “Consumer trust in the online retail context: exploring the antecedents and consequences”, Psychology & Marketing, Vol. 27 No. 4, pp. 323-346. Chen, Y.C., Shang, R.A. and Kao, C.Y. (2009), “The effects of information overload on consumers’ subjective state towards buying decision in the internet shopping environment”, Electronic Commerce Research and Applications, Vol. 8 No. 1, pp. 48-58. Childers, T.L., Carr, C.L., Peck, J. and Carson, S. (2001), “Hedonic and utilitarian motivations for online retail shopping behavior”, Journal of Retailing, Vol. 77 No. 4, pp. 511-535. Cho, C., Kang, J. and Cheon, H. (2006), “Online shopping hesitation”, Cyber Psychology & Behavior, Vol. 9 No. 3, pp. 261-274. Cho, Y.K. (2014), “Service quality and price perceptions by internet retail customers: linking the three stages of service interaction”, Journal of Service Research, Vol. 17 No. 4, pp. 432-445. Creswell, J.W. (2009), Research Design: Qualitative, Quantitative, and Mixed Methods Approaches, 3rd ed., Sage Publications, Thousand Oaks, CA. Dailey, L. (2004), “Navigational web atmospherics: explaining the influence of restrictive navigation cues”, Journal of Business Research, Vol. 57 No. 7, pp. 795-803. Davis, D.F., Golicic, S.L. and Boerstler, C.N. (2011), “Benefits and challenges of conducting multiple methods research in marketing”, Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, Vol. 39 No. 3, pp. 467-479. Davis, F.D. (1986), “A technology acceptance model for empirically testing new end-user information systems: theory and results”, doctoral thesis, Sloan School of Management, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA. Dogu, U. and Erkip, F. (2000), “Spatial factors affecting wayfinding and orientation: a case study in a shopping mall”, Environment and Behavior, Vol. 32 No. 6, pp. 731-755. Donovan, R.J. and Rossiter, J.R. (1982), “Store atmosphere: an environmental psychology approach”, Journal of Retailing, Vol. 58 No. 1, pp. 34-57. Eroglu, S.A., Machleit, K.A. and Davis, L.M. (2001), “Atmospheric qualities of online retailing: a conceptual model and implications”, Journal of Business Research, Vol. 54 No. 2, pp. 177-184. Evanschitzky, H., Iyer, G.R., Hesse, J. and Ahlert, D. (2004), “E-satisfaction: a re-examination”, Journal of Retailing, Vol. 80 No. 3, pp. 239-247. Everad, A. and Galletta, D.F. (2006), “How presentation flaws affect perceived site quality, trust, and intention to purchase from an online store”, Journal of Management Information Systems, Vol. 22 No. 3, pp. 56-95. Fireclick.com (2014), “Global conversion rate”, available at: http://index.fireclick.com/ (accessed July 25 2014). Fishbein, M. and Ajzen, I. (1975), Belief, Attitude, Intention, and Behavior: An Introduction to Theory and Research, Addison-Wesley, Reading, MA. Fisher, R.J. (1993), “Social desirability bias and the validity of indirect questioning”, Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 20 No. 2, pp. 303-315. Flanagan, J.C. (1954), “The critical incidental technique”, Psychological Bulletin, Vol. 51 No. 4, pp. 327-358. Foxman, E.R., Muehling, D.D. and Berger, P.W. (1990), “An investigation of factors contributing to consumer brand confusion”, Journal of Consumer Affairs, Vol. 24 No. 1, pp. 170-189. Garaus, M. and Wagner, U. (2016), “Retail shopper confusion: conceptualization, scale development, and consequences”, Journal of Business Research, Vol. 69 No. 9, pp. 3459-3467. Garaus, M., Wagner, U. and Kummer, C. (2015), “Cognitive fit, retail shopper confusion, and shopping value: empirical investigation”, Journal of Business Research, Vol. 68 No. 5, pp. 1003-1011. Gelbrich, K. (2010), “Anger, frustration, and helplessness after service failure: coping strategies and effective informational support”, Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, Vol. 38 No. 5, pp. 567-585. Griffith, D.A. (2005), “An examination of the influences of store layout in online retailing”, Journal of Business Research, Vol. 58 No. 10, pp. 1391-1396. Ha, S. and Stoel, L. (2009), “Consumer e-shopping acceptance: antecedents in a technology acceptance model”, Journal of Business Research, Vol. 62 No. 5, pp. 565-571. Hausman, A.V. and Siekpe, J.S. (2009), “The effect of web interface features on consumer online purchase intentions”, Journal of Business Research, Vol. 62 No. 1, pp. 5-13. Hoffman, D.L. and Novak, T.P. (2000), “How to acquire customers on the web”, Harvard Business Review, Vol. 78 No. 3, pp. 3-8. Holloway, B.B. and Beatty, S.E. (2003), “Service failure in online retailing: a recovery opportunity”, Journal of Service Research, Vol. 6 No. 1, pp. 92-105. Holloway, B.B. and Beatty, S.E. (2008), “Satisfiers and dissatisfiers in the online environment”, Journal of Service Research, Vol. 10 No. 4, pp. 347-364. Hong, W., Thong, J.Y. and Tam, K.Y. (2004), “The effects of information format and shopping task on consumers’ online shopping behavior: a cognitive fit perspective”, Journal of Management Information Systems, Vol. 21 No. 3, pp. 149-184. Hoque, A.Y. and Lohse, G.L. (1999), “An information search cost perspective for designing interfaces for electronic commerce”, Journal of Marketing Research, Vol. 36 No. 3, pp. 387-394. Hsieh, J.K., Hsieh, Y.C., Chiu, H.C. and Yang, Y.R. (2014), “Customer response to web site atmospherics: task-relevant cues, situational involvement and PAD”, Journal of Interactive Marketing, Vol. 28 No. 3, pp. 225-236. Huang, M.-H. (2003), “Modeling virtual exploratory and shopping dynamics: an environmental psychology approach”, Information and Management, Vol. 41 No. 1, pp. 39-47. Jacoby, J. (1984), “Perspectives on information overload”, Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 10 No. 4, pp. 432-435. Confusion in internet retailing 495 INTR 28,2 Jacoby, J., Speller, D.E. and Berning, C.K. (1974), “Brand choice behavior as a function of information load: replication and extension”, Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 1 No. 1, pp. 33-42. Janda, S., Trocchia, P.J. and Gwinner, K.P. (2002), “Consumer perceptions of internet retail service quality”, International Journal of Service Industry Management, Vol. 13 No. 5, pp. 412-431. Jeong, W.S., Fiore, A.M., Niehm, L.S. and Lorenz, F.O. (2009), “The role of experiential value in online shopping”, Internet Research, Vol. 19 No. 1, pp. 105-124. 496 Jick, T.D. (1979), “Mixing qualitative and quantitative methods: triangulation in action”, Administrative Science Quarterly, Vol. 24 No. 4, pp. 602-611. Kassarjian, H.H. (1977), “Content analysis in consumer research”, Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 4 No. 1, pp. 8-18. Keaveney, S.M. (1995), “Customer switching behavior in service industries: an explorative study”, Journal of Marketing, Vol. 59 No. 2, pp. 71-82. Kim, J., Fiore, A.M. and Lee, H.H. (2007), “Influences of online store perception, shopping enjoyment, and shopping involvement on consumer patronage behavior towards an online retailer”, Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services, Vol. 14 No. 2, pp. 95-107. Konradt, U., Wandke, H., Balazs, B. and Christophersen, T. (2003), “Usability in online shops: scale construction, validation and the influence on the buyers’ intention and decision”, Behaviour & Information Technology, Vol. 22 No. 3, pp. 165-174. Koo, D.-M. (2006), “The fundamental reasons of e-consumers’ loyalty to an online store”, Electronic Commerce Research and Applications, Vol. 5 No. 2, pp. 117-130. Koo, D.-M. and Ju, S.-H. (2010), “The interactional effects of atmospherics and perceptual curiosity on emotions and online shopping intention”, Computers in Human Behavior, Vol. 26 No. 3, pp. 377-388. Kukar-Kinney, M. and Close, A.G. (2010), “The determinants of consumers’ online shopping cart abandonment”, Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, Vol. 38 No. 2, pp. 240-250. Laros, F.J. and Steenkamp, J.B.E. (2005), “Emotions in consumer behavior: a hierarchical approach”, Journal of Business Research, Vol. 58 No. 10, pp. 1437-1445. Lee, B.K. and Lee, W.N. (2004), “The effect of information overload on consumer choice quality in an online environment”, Psychology & Marketing, Vol. 21 No. 3, pp. 159-183. Li, M., Dong, Z.Y. and Chen, X. (2012), “Factors influencing consumption experience of mobile commerce”, Internet Research, Vol. 22 No. 2, pp. 120-141. Lim, W.M. (2015), “Antecedents and consequences of e-shopping: an integrated model”, Internet Research, Vol. 25 No. 2, pp. 184-217. Liu, F., Xiao, B., Lim, E.T.K. and Tan, C.-W. (2017), “The art of appeal in electronic commerce: understanding the impact of product and website quality on online purchases”, Internet Research, Vol. 27 No. 4, pp. 752-771. Lohse, G.L. (1993), “A cognitive model for understanding graphical perception”, Human-Computer Interaction, Vol. 8 No. 4, pp. 353-388. López-López, I., Ruiz-de-Maya, S. and Warlop, L. (2014), “When sharing consumption emotions with strangers is more satisfying than sharing them with friends”, Journal of Service Research, Vol. 17 No. 4, pp. 475-488. Machleit, K.A. and Eroglu, S.A. (2000), “Describing and measuring emotional response to shopping experience”, Journal of Business Research, Vol. 49 No. 2, pp. 101-111. Mai, R., Hoffmann, S., Schwarz, U., Niemand, T. and Seidel, J. (2014), “The shifting range of optimal web site complexity”, Journal of Interactive Marketing, Vol. 28 No. 2, pp. 101-116. Malhotra, N.K. (1982), “Information load and consumer decision making”, Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 8 No. 4, pp. 419-430. Manganari, E.E., Siomkos, G.J., Rigopoulou, I.D. and Vrechopoulos, A.P. (2011), “Virtual store layout effects on consumer behaviour: applying an environmental psychology approach in the online travel industry”, Internet Research, Vol. 21 No. 3, pp. 326-346. Mayer, J.D. (2001), “Emotion, intelligence, and emotional intelligence”, in Forgas, J.P. (Ed.), Handbook of Affect and Social Cognition, Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ, pp. 410-431. Mehrabian, A. and Russell, J.A. (1974), An Approach to Environmental Psychology, MIT Press, Cambridge, MA. Confusion in internet retailing Mitchell, V.W. and Papavassiliou, V. (1999), “Marketing causes and implications of consumer confusion”, Journal of Product & Brand Management, Vol. 8 No. 4, pp. 319-339. Mitchell, V.W., Walsh, G. and Yamin, Y. (2005), “Towards a conceptual model of consumer confusion”, in Menon, G. and Rao, A.R. (Eds), Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 32, Association for Consumer Research, Duluth, MN, pp. 143-150. Mizerski, R.W. (1982), “An attribution explanation of the disproportionate influence of unfavorable information”, Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 9 No. 3, pp. 301-310. Montoya-Weiss, M.M., Voss, G.B. and Grewal, D. (2003), “Determinants of online channel use and overall satisfaction with a relational, multichannel service provider”, Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, Vol. 31 No. 4, pp. 448-458. Mummalaneni, V. (2005), “An empirical investigation of web site characteristics, consumer emotional states and on-line shopping behaviors”, Journal of Business Research, Vol. 58 No. 4, pp. 526-532. Netemeyer, R.G., Bearden, W.O. and Sharma, S. (2003), Scaling Procedures: Issues and Applications, Sage Publications, Thousand Oaks, CA. Petty, R.E., Wells, G.L. and Brock, T.C. (1976), “Distraction can enhance or reduce yielding to propaganda”, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 34 No. 5, pp. 874-884. Punj, G. (2011), “Effect of consumer beliefs on online purchase behavior: the influence of demographic characteristics and consumption values”, Journal of Interactive Marketing, Vol. 25 No. 3, pp. 134-144. Rajamma, R.K., Paswan, A.K. and Hossain, M.M. (2009), “Why do shoppers abandon shopping cart? Perceived waiting time, risk, and transaction inconvenience”, Journal of Product & Brand Management, Vol. 18 No. 3, pp. 188-197. Richard, M.O. (2005), “Modeling the impact of internet atmospherics on surfer behavior”, Journal of Business Research, Vol. 58 No. 12, pp. 1632-1642. Richins, M.L. (1997), “Measuring emotions in the consumption experience”, Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 24 No. 2, pp. 127-146. Rossiter, J.R. (2002), “The C-OAR-SE procedure for scale development in marketing”, International Journal of Research in Marketing, Vol. 19 No. 4, pp. 305-335. Scarpi, D. (2012), “Work and fun on the Internet: the effects of utilitarianism and hedonism online”, Journal of Interactive Marketing, Vol. 26 No. 1, pp. 53-67. Schweizer, M., Kotouc, A. and Rudolph, T. (2006), “Scale development for consumer confusion”, in Pechmann, C. and Price, L. (Eds), Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 33, Association for Consumer Research, Duluth, MN, pp. 184-190. Shen, N.K. and Khalifa, M. (2012), “System design effects on online impulse buying”, Internet Research, Vol. 22 No. 4, pp. 396-425. Sicilia, M. and Ruiz, S. (2010), “The effect of web-based information availability on consumers’ processing and attitudes”, Journal of Interactive Marketing, Vol. 24 No. 1, pp. 31-41. Statista.com (2014), “E-Commerce”, available at: http://de.statista.com/statistik/daten/studie/191135/ umfrage/us-e-commerce-sales-figures-in-retail-since-2002/ (accessed July 25, 2014). Terry, P.C., Lane, A.M. and Fogarty, G.J. (2003), “Construct validity of the profile of mood states – adolescents for use with adults”, Psychology of Sport and Exercise, Vol. 4 No. 2, pp. 125-139. Turnbull, P.W., Leek, S. and Ying, G. (2000), “Customer confusion: the mobile phone market”, Journal of Marketing Management, Vol. 16 Nos 1-3, pp. 143-163. 497 INTR 28,2 Walsh, G. and Mitchell, V.W. (2010), “The effect of consumer confusion proneness on word of mouth, trust, and customer satisfaction”, European Journal of Marketing, Vol. 44 No. 6, pp. 838-859. Walsh, G., Hennig-Thurau, T. and Mitchell, V.W. (2007), “Consumer confusion proneness: scale development, validation and application”, Journal of Marketing Management, Vol. 23 Nos 7-8, pp. 697-721. 498 Wang, Q. and Shukla, P. (2013), “Linking sources of consumer confusion to decision satisfaction: the role of choice goals”, Psychology & Marketing, Vol. 30 No. 4, pp. 295-304. Wang, Y.J., Minor, M.S. and Wei, J. (2011), “Aesthetics and the online shopping environment: understanding consumer responses”, Journal of Retailing, Vol. 87 No. 1, pp. 46-58. Yan, Y., Zhang, X., Zha, X., Jiang, T., Qin, L. and Li, Z. (2017), “Decision quality and satisfaction: the effects of online information sources and self-efficacy”, Internet Research, Vol. 27 No. 4, pp. 885-904. Zheng, X., Lee, M. and Cheung, C.M.K. (2017), “Examining e-loyalty towards online shopping platforms: the role of coupon proneness and value consciousness”, Internet Research, Vol. 27 No. 3, pp. 709-726. Appendix. Constructs, items, and Cronbach’s α values of studies 1-3 Online shopper confusion (Garaus and Wagner, 2016), αstudy3 ¼ 0.94 Helplessness Helpless Lost Awkward Baffled Weak Overstrained Irritation Annoyed Irritated Nerved Inefficiency Efficient* Careful* Productive* High-performing* Revisit intention (Hausman and Siekpe, 2009; Kim et al., 2007), αStudy2 ¼ 0.96, αStudy3 ¼ 0.89: I am encouraged to revisit this site in the near future. I would visit this online retailer again. In the future, I would very probably shop at this online retailer. Spending time (Kim et al., 2007), αStudy2 ¼ 0.80, αStudy3 ¼ 0.58: I would like to stay at this online store for as long as possible. I enjoyed spending time at this online store. Shopping cart abandonment (Rajamma et al., 2009): In such a shopping situation, I would abandon the shopping process. Unplanned expenditures (Donovan and Rossiter, 1982): I would spend more money than initially planned in this store. WOM ( Janda et al., 2002), αStudy2 ¼ 0.94, αStudy3 ¼ 0.82: I would say positive things about this online retailer to other people. I would recommend this online retailer to someone who seeks my advice. I would encourage friends and relatives to do business with this online retailer. Hedonic consumer beliefs (Punj, 2011), αStudy1 ¼ 0.71, αStudy2 ¼ 0.78, αStudy3 ¼ 0.78: I enjoy online shopping. I like to have many choices while shopping. I like to research products in online shops. Utilitarian consumer beliefs (Punj, 2011), αStudy1 ¼ 0.66, αStudy2 ¼ 0.65, αStudy3 ¼ 0.50: Shopping online saves me time. Shopping online saves me money. Shopping online helps to find the right product. Internet experience (Scarpi, 2012): How expert would you rate yourself with the internet? Frequency of internet usage (Punj, 2011): How often do you use the internet? Online shopping experience (Scarpi, 2012): How expert would you rate yourself with online shopping? “*” indicates reversed item. About the author Marion Garaus is an Assistant Professor in Marketing at the Institute of Business Administration, University of Vienna, Austria. Her research interests include consumer behavior, environmental aesthetics, and store design. Her research appears in Journal of Business Research, Journal of Consumer Behaviour, Psychology & Marketing, and Technological Forecasting and Social Change. She is the co-author of the book Store Design and Visual Merchandising: Creating Store Space That Encourages Buying. Marion Garaus can be contacted at: marion.garaus@univie.ac.at For instructions on how to order reprints of this article, please visit our website: www.emeraldgrouppublishing.com/licensing/reprints.htm Or contact us for further details: permissions@emeraldinsight.com Confusion in internet retailing 499