EDUCATIONAL AND PSYCHOLOGICAL MEASUREMENT 1980,40 THE RELATIONSHIP OF AN EARLY MEASURE OF INTELLIGENCE TO THE ABILITY TO LEARN SIGHT VOCABULARY WORDS AND TO LATER READING ACHIEVEMENT RALPH CIANFLONE Seton Hill THOMAS G. ZULLO College University of Pittsburgh The Slosson Intelligence Test (SIT) was administered to a group of kindergarten children. Scores were correlated with a measure of Sight Vocabulary achievement in kindergarten and reading achievement (Stanford Achievement Test) scores at the end of the first and second grade. Correlation coefficients involving the SIT ranged from a low of .61 to a high of .87. THE Slosson Intelligence Test (SIT) (Slosson, 1963) was developed provide a valid and reliable measure that can be administered in a short period (approximately 10 to 20 minutes) by individuals not trained in giving individualized tests of general intelligence or scholastic aptitude. Requiring no reading, the test is designed to be used with young children who cannot as yet read. Slosson (1963) reported a meto dian correlation of .92 between the SIT and the Stanford-Binet In- telligence Scale (SBIS) (Terman and Merrill, 1960) for subjects ranging in age from four years to eighteen and above. Those findings were reinforced by Armstrong and Mooney ( 1971 ) who obtained a correlation of .94 between the SBIS given by personnel qualified to administer it and the SIT administered by classroom teachers. Employing a population of subjects with emotional impairments, Kaufman and Ivanoff (1969) cited a correlation of .93 between the SIT and the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS) (Wechsler, 1955). Martin and Kidwell (1977) reported the correlation between the SIT and the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised (WISC-R) (Wechsler, 1974) to be .79, .82 and .50 for the Full, Verbal, and Performance scales, respectively. There appears to be a paucity of literature on the relationship be1197 Downloaded from epm.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 10, 2016 1198 EDUCATIONAL AND PSYCHOLOGICAL MEASUREMENT tween measures of intelligence administered very early in childhood and reading achievement. Lesiak (1977) found a correlation of .54 between the Cognitive Abilities Test, Primary I, Form I (IQ) (Thorndike, Hagen, and Lorge, 1974) administered at the beginning of first grade and the total score from Lee-Clark Reading Tests, First Reader, Form A (Lee and Clark, 1958) given at the end of first grade. Hartlage and Steele (1977) administered the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC) (Wechsler, 1949) in mid-second grade and the Wide Range Achievement Test (WRAT) (Jastak and Bijou, 1946) six months later and obtained correlations of .59 and .44 between the full scale WISC and the (a) WRAT Reading and (b) WRAT Spelling subtest, respectively. In an extensive study involving pre-school children, Durkin (1970) found the correlation between the SBIS and the (a) number of letters and (b) number of words children could identify at the end of a Head Start Program to be .44 and .42, respectively. The correlation of the SBIS with the same measures at the end of kindergarten were reported as .26 and .49, respectively. The purpose of this paper was to determine the degree to which a relatively easy-to-administer measure of intelligence (SIT) taken at the kindergarten level correlated with (a) the acquisition of sight vocabulary and (b) later measures of reading achievement as measured by the Stanford Achievement Test, Form B (SAT) (Madden, Gardner, Rudman, Karlsen, and Merwin, 1974). Procedure The subjects who comprised the original sample consisted of 108 children from two schools in an urban school district outside of Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania. The SIT was administered to all pupils during the first three weeks of their second semester in kindergarten. Immediately after the children had completed the (SIT), they were asked individually whether they were able to identify any of 63 sight vocabulary words that had been selected from the Comprehensive Reading Vocabulary-Primary Level (CRV-PL) (Sartain, 1972). None of the children was able to identify any of the words. The pupils were then &dquo;taught&dquo; the sight vocabulary words by a method that combined the features of the phonic, kinesthetic, and meaningful context methods (Cianflone, 1976). Twenty-four hours after two separate exposures to the teaching procedure, children were asked to identify the vocabulary words. The number of words correctly identified constituted the score for the sight vocabulary. SAT scores were obtained for the subtests of Reading, Vocabulary, and Word Study Skills at the end of Downloaded from epm.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 10, 2016 1199 CIANFLONE AND ZULLO (SAT-1) and second (SAT-2) grades. able for 51of the original subjects. first Complete data were avail- { ,Results and ~~~~.~,~ion Pearson coefficients computed bethe SIT and scores on the SAT Reading, Vocabulary and Work Study Skills subtests at the end of first grade were .77, .87 and .78 respectively. These coefficients exceeded the values reported in the literature between measures of reading achievement and either reading readiness tests or individualized intelligence tests. Correlations between the SIT scores and the corresponding SAT subtests at the end of the second grade were .76, .79 and .61. Again these magnitudes, which represented a nearly two and one-half year time span, exceeded most values previously reported that involved shorter time periods. The correlation between the SIT and sight vocabulary scores of .70 exceeded that found by Durkin (1970) who employed the SBIS. (It must be noted, however, that Durkin measured Word Identincation over a longer period than that involved in this study). All correlation coefficients reported in the present study were statistically signincant at the .001 level. The results of this investigation would tend to indicate the utility of further investigations regarding the use of the SIT as a potential predictor of general reading achievement. Obviously, replication of this product-moment correlation tween scores on study with other samples would provide needed cross-validation data. REFERENCES Armstrong, R. J. and Mooney R. R. The Slosson Intelligence Test: Implications for reading specialists. The Reading Teacher, 1971, 24, 336-40. Cianflone, R. Comparative Difficulty of Beginning Reading Vocabulary: Set V (Nouns). Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Pittsburgh, Durkin, D. 1976. arts program for pre-first grade children: two year achievement report. Reading Research Quarterly, 1970, 5, 534-565. Hartlage, L. C. and Steele, C. T. WISC and WISC-R correlates of academic achievement. Psychology in the Schools, 1977, 14, 15-18. Jastak, J. and Bijou, S. The Wide Range Achievement Test. New York: A language Psychological Corporation, 1946. Kaufman, H. and Ivanoff, J. The Slosson Intelligence Test as a screening instrument with a rehabilitation population. Exceptional Children, 1969, 35, 745-51. Lee, J. M. and Clark, W. W. Lee-Clark Reading Test. Monterey: CTB/McGraw-Hill, 1958. Downloaded from epm.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 10, 2016 1200 EDUCATIONAL AND PSYCHOLOGICAL MEASUREMENT Lesiak, J. The Gates-MacGinitie Readiness Skills Test and Illinois Test of Psycholinguistic Abilities as predictors of first grade reading. Psychology in the Schools, 1977, 14, 4-10. Madden, R., Gardner, E. F., Rudman, H. C., Karlsen, B. and Merwin, J. Stanford Achievement Test, Primary Level I Battery, Form B. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, 1974. Martin, J. D. and Kidwell, J. C. Intercorrelations of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised, the Slosson Intelligence Test, and the National Educational Developmental Test. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 1977, 37, 1117-1120. Sartain, H. W. The Comprehensive Reading Vocabulary-Primary Level (Unpublished vocabulary studies, Falk Laboratory Schools) Pittsburgh : University of Pittsburgh, 1972. Slosson, R. L. Slosson Intelligence Test for Children and Adults. New York: Slosson Educational Publication, 1963. Terman, L. and Merrill, J. Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale: Manual for the Third Revision, Form L-M. Boston: Houghton Mifflin, 1960. Thorndike, R. L., Hagen, E. and Lorge, I. Cognitive Abilities Test, Primary 1 Boston: Houghton Mifflin, 1974. , Wechsler, D. Manual for the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children. New York: Psychological Corporation, 1949. Wechsler, D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Manual. New York: Psychological Corporation, 1955. Wechsler, D. Manual for the Wechsler Intelligence Scalefor ChildrenRevised. New York: Psychological Corporation, 1974. Downloaded from epm.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 10, 2016