ZFCJ8 - ANTH0060 UG - 17/12/2018 15:00 - Volker Sommer - 1419 words Female orgasm. Do non-human primates experience them too, and why do they exist? Female sexuality is fascinating to discover not only from the male perspective, for obvious reasons, but also as a female writer. From the evolutionary standpoint, while the male orgasm concludes with an ejaculation with the clear purpose of ‘spreading its seeds’, the female orgasm has remained largely a mystery, with the case even more murky for non-human primates. Fortunately, with science research suggesting measurable cues such as increase in heart rate and blood pressure, as well as vaginal, anal and uterine contraction as indications of sexual climax in women, primatologists are able to study and question the possibility of orgasm in female non-human primates by observing the presence of these characteristics (Carosi & Troisi, 1998). Indeed, most arguments supporting female non-human primates’ orgasms are built on evidence of the above physiological changes and the observable display of sudden behavioural responses during sexual intercourse (Chevalier-Skolnikoff, 1974), including muscular body spasms, vocalisations in certain species, and the well-researched clutching reaction where the female turns back to look at her partner before grasping him (Carosi & Troisi, 1998). If these phenomena were taken to be homologous with orgasm, then it is in my opinion that while female non-human primates consider sexual sensations pleasant, the existence of their orgasm remains more of a possibility than a fact, for its definition is ambiguous and its occurrence is highly variable. Theories for its existence could be categorised into adaptationist and nonadaptationist perspectives, which will be demonstrated in the second half of the essay. Observations from the laboratory Page 1 of 7 ZFCJ8 - ANTH0060 UG - 17/12/2018 15:00 - Volker Sommer - 1419 words Despite a general paucity of data recording the sexual responses of non-human primates, there is nonetheless enough information to at least conclude that they do, certainly, enjoy genital stimulation (Lemmon & Allen, 1978). Allen (1977), for instance, conducted a research on female chimpanzees through manually stimulating their circumclitoral area and vagina. The majority allowed stimulation to proceed to sexual arousal. One even allowed stimulation to vaginal contractions on ten independent occasions, always terminating stimulations and leaving quadrupedally after the vaginal contractions had stopped (ibid., 1977). In an interesting case, a female turned back and attempted to push the experimenter’s thrusting hand deeper into her vagina (ibid., 1977). The same female did not discontinue stimulation while she was experiencing vaginal contractions on two separate occasions as her infant tried to intervene the process, in comparison to similar occurrences of meddling during earlier stages where she initiated the termination of stimulation, indicating that she was experiencing some form of exceptional pleasure which could be an indication of orgasm (ibid., 1977). In addition to enjoyment, other physiological changes observed across the chimpanzees in this study included hyperventilation, clitoral tumescence, reflexive spasms in the limbs with muscle tension, or changes in facial expressions, which are also typical reactions found in women during orgasm, suggesting a possibility of orgasm in female non-human primates (ibid., 1977). Chevalier-Skolnikoff (1974), too, in a study of female stumptails found behaviours homologous to the orgasmic phenomenon of male stumptails, which he argues are responses fundamentally identical to those reported by Masters and John (1966) in women and hence supports the argument for female non-human primates’ orgasm. Common observations across these stumptails included the clutching reaction and the postejaculatory phase where the sexual partners engaged in mutual grooming and close physical contact (ibid., 1974). Among female homosexual interactions, it was noticed on three independent events that the mounter displayed all the behavioural signals found in males during sexual climax: body rigidity and muscular Page 2 of 7 ZFCJ8 - ANTH0060 UG - 17/12/2018 15:00 - Volker Sommer - 1419 words body spasms were accompanied with a pause and the distinctive “frowning round-mouthed stare expression” (ibid., 1974: 109) and rhythmic vocalisation. Although these evidence — consisting mainly of physiological behavioural responses — reflect that female non-human primates find genital stimulation pleasurable, and show characteristics comparable to the orgasmic experiences of male primates and human female, there are limitations to what can be interpreted from this data. Firstly, they are limited to artificial social situations, often in laboratories, which make theorising difficult (Phoebus, 1982). Secondly, there is no definite proof that these physiological changes can be equated to a subjective experience of orgasm as found in those of women’s sexual climaxes (Carosi & Troisi, 1998). For example, few emotional responses, such as blatant vocalisation which are common among women, could be noticed during intense vaginal contractions and strong palpitation (Allen, 1977). Hence, I hold that the existence of orgasm in female non-human primates remains debatable, albeit the case for it is strong. Adaptationist versus Anti-adaptationist More significantly, the female non-human primates’ orgasm — or the lack thereof — is tied to a broader narrative of its raison d’être. Combining theories from the evolution of the orgasm in human females, explanations for its existence can be categorised as adaptationist (Lemmon & Allen, 1981; Morris, 1967; Hrdy, 1981; Carosi & Troisi, 1998) or nonadaptationist (Symons, 1979). Lemmon and Allen (1981) contends that the orgasmic response of females in mammals has evolved to trigger the orgasmic response in males, through observation that contractions of the perivaginal muscles productively grip and stimulate the penis which encourages ejaculation. Also from the perspective in relation to the male counterpart, Morris argues the Page 3 of 7 ZFCJ8 - ANTH0060 UG - 17/12/2018 15:00 - Volker Sommer - 1419 words female orgasm “[makes] sex sexier” (1967: 65). It functions as a behavioural reward to condition the female into availing herself whenever her partner solicited sex, enhancing the “pair-bond” (ibid., 1967: 38) between them and thus enabling females to secure from their partners the greater parental investment needed as a consequence of modern humans’ longer life history and longer juvenile period. For Morris, orgasms facilitate monogamy (ibid., 1967). On the other hand, while Hrdy (1981) likewise construes the female orgasm as a behavioural reward, for him it facilitates promiscuity instead. A ‘promiscuous’ female primate that strives to copulate with as many partners enjoy a selective advantage for two reasons (ibid., 1981). Firstly, it allows the female to assess the “capabilities” (ibid., 1981: 150) of each and so strategise to mate with the strongest and fittest when it ovulates. Secondly, it is beneficial to its offspring as it could protect them from infanticide and possibly lead to multiple partners involving themselves in their care since the paternity of its babies are ambiguous (ibid., 1981). Similar to Hrdy (1981), Carosi and Troisi (1998) makes the link between female orgasm and mate choice, arguing for the former as an adaptation which evolutionary purpose is the latter. Their finding that male rank influences the likelihood of orgasmic-like responses in female suggests that physical stimulation is not the only variable affecting female sexual arousal: social stimuli is a non-negligible factor too (ibid., 1998). Supporting their claim is previous research that female macaque occasionally displays keen sexual liking for unacquainted males who are new to the community, counteracting preferences for fitter and high-status resident males (Takahata, 1982). Of the four adaptationist theories attempting to account for the female orgasm’s raison d’être, the first two presented (Lemmon & Allen, 1981; Morris, 1967) reduce to the ontological claim that female orgasm is a natural phenomenon. Subsequently, the next two arguments (Hrdy, 1981; Carosi & Troisi, 1998), which associates female orgasm with mate choice, see the female orgasm as belonging to the domain of culture instead. Page 4 of 7 ZFCJ8 - ANTH0060 UG - 17/12/2018 15:00 - Volker Sommer - 1419 words Symons (1979), who proposes that the female orgasm is a byproduct of the evolutionary advantage orgasms have on males, recognise that orgasms are as cultural as “agriculture, the wheel, and indoor plumbing” (1979: 95). Her non-adaptationist view, unlike in the previous reasonings presented, sees the female orgasm as serving no adaptive function, similar to the clitoris which has no known purpose apart from a source of great sensation that is “presumably pleasure” (ibid., 1979: 88). She supports this by drawing an analogy with the widely-cited example of male nipples (ibid., 1979). Female representation; Female orgasm Notably, there is no one agreed conclusion to the purpose of the female orgasm, whether in humans or non-human primates. Theories can be as diverse as ranging from adaptationists versus non-adaptationists as explained above, to anthropocentrists versus antianthropocentrists, which is a debate embedded in the broader feminist movement (Bosley, 2010). What can be agreed on, however, is that the female is not passive in sex: in Lemmon and Allen (1981), it suggests the male orgasm follows the lead of the female orgasm. In Morris (1967), it shows females know how to fight for greater parental investment. In both Hrdy (1981) and Carosi and Troisi (1998), similarly, mate choice reflects that women (or female primates) are not powerless in the game of relationships. And, ultimately, with more women power and female representation, maybe then can we someday find out if female non-human primates indeed experience orgasms. Page 5 of 7 ZFCJ8 - ANTH0060 UG - 17/12/2018 15:00 - Volker Sommer - 1419 words Bibliography Allen, M. L. & Lemmon, W. B. 1978. Continual sexual receptivity in the female chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). In: Folia Primatologica, 30: 80-88. Allen, M. L. & Lemmon, W. B. 1981. Orgasm in female primates. In: American Journal of Primatology, 1: 15-34. Bosley, J. 2010. Monkey facts to human ideologies: Theorising female orgasm in human and nonhuman primates, 1967-1983. In: The University of Chicago Press Journals, 35(3): 647671. Carosi, M. & Troisi, A. 1998. Female orgasm rate increases with male dominance in Japanese macaques. In: Animal Behaviour, 56: 1261-1266. Chevalier-Skolnikoff, S. 1974. Male-female, female-female, and male-male sexual behaviour in the stumptail monkey, with special attention to the female orgasm. In: Archives of Sexual Behaviour, 3(2): 95-116. Hrdy, S. B. 1981. The woman that never evolved. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Morris, D. 1967. The naked ape: A zoologist’s study of the human animal. New York: McGraw-Hill. Page 6 of 7 ZFCJ8 - ANTH0060 UG - 17/12/2018 15:00 - Volker Sommer - 1419 words Phoebus, E. C. 1982. Letter: Primate female orgasm. In: American Journal of Primatology, 2: 223-224. Symons, D. 1979. The evolution of human sexuality. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Takahata, Y. 1982. The socio-sexual behaviour of Japanese monkeys. In: Zeitschrift fu ̈r Tierpsychologie, 59: 89-108. Page 7 of 7