Texas Students’ College Expectations: Does High School Racial Composition Matter? Michelle Bellessa Frost Princeton University Office of Population Research Preliminary Draft, not for citation Revised August 17, 2004 This research was supported by the Hewlett Foundation and Ford Foundation. Institutional support was provided by the Office of Population Research. Special thanks go to Marta Tienda, Mario Small, Germán Rodriguez, and Meredith Kleykamp for helpful comments at various stages in this research. A version of this paper was presented at the Notestein Seminar Series at Princeton University in May 2004. Please direct comments to Michelle Bellessa Frost, Office of Population Research, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ, 08544, mbfrost@princeton.edu. 1 Introduction Although 50 years have passed since the landmark Brown vs. Board of Education case, public schools remain in large part segregated by race and ethnicity. Furthermore, the recent removal of court mandates to desegregate schools and challenges to other race based assignment policies have led to an increase in segregation. Across the nation, 66 percent of blacks and 73 percent of Hispanics attend schools in which at least half the students are not white (Orfield and Yun 1999). In Texas, a state with a large and rapidly growing minority school age population, school segregation is widespread. For example, in 2002-03 twothirds of Hispanic and half of black students attended schools with at least 70 percent other minority students (Texas Education Agency 2003)1. Yet, despite the prevalence of segregation, few recent studies have examined how school racial composition influences students’ educational outcomes. Among these important educational outcomes, the goal to attend and complete college is an essential first step toward an eventual university degree. Early work in the status attainment tradition established the importance of educational expectations on academic attainment and other school outcomes, both within and across racial groups (Sewell, Haller et al. 1969; Sewell, Haller et al. 1970; Campbell 1983; Hanson 1994; Hao and Bonstead-Bruns 1998; Buchmann and Dalton 2002). Expanding on studies that investigated only the effects of individual level attributes on expectation formation, other sociologists turned to contextual models to search for school influences on individual students’ educational expectations, including an examination of how desegregation efforts were impacting black students’ outcomes (Meyer 1970; Alexander and Eckland 1975; St. John 1975; Hauser, Sewell et al. 1976; Alwin and Otto 1977). However, by the late 2 1970’s, this research was largely abandoned because of the lack of results showing any real effects of schools, as well as a changing political climate. Combining the importance of understanding the impacts of continued school segregation in a state with widely heterogeneous schools with recent advances in multilevel statistical models and computing ability with, I examine whether and how school racial composition influences Texas high school students’ educational expectations. Specifically, three possible scenarios linking school racial composition to educational expectations are considered. First, school racial composition could act merely as a proxy for individual characteristics that directly influence expectations to complete college, such as family socioeconomic status. This suggests that no school effects on expectations exist, and is the default position of most previous research. Or, it is possible that school effects are indeed present, but that the distribution of students by race/ethnicity within school reflects other school characteristics, such as academic climate, that are related to the level of educational expectations within a school. Finally, the prospect exists that something fundamental about racial composition in school directly influences the educational goals held by students there. In order to analyze the relative explanatory power of these three alternatives, I employ multilevel models to answer the following research questions: Does the level of educational expectations vary between schools? What is the relationship between school racial composition and educational expectations? Do student characteristics and other school characteristics explain why school racial composition is related to educational expectations? Which factors are most important in understanding between school differences in students’ educational goals? 1 Data from the Texas Education Agency analyzed by author. 3 So as to investigate these questions, the paper proceeds as follows. Grounded in a comprehensive literature review, I detail three hypotheses that explain how and why school racial composition can influence students’ expectations to graduate from college. Following a discussion of a unique survey suited to test these propositions and why Texas is an apt location for the study of school effects, I outline an analysis plan. Results from hierarchical logistic regression models suggest the counterintuitive finding that when comparing similar schools, greater concentrations of minority students are associated with an increased likelihood of students in those schools expecting to attain a 4-year college degree. The final section discusses the implications of these findings. Influences of School Racial/Ethnic Composition on Educational Expectations A central hypothesis of this research is that school characteristics, and specifically a school’s racial/ethnic composition, exert an independent impact on students’ educational expectations. A simple way to conceptualize school effects is to envision a single student attending a given school, and then alternatively attending a different school; here, a school with a different racial composition. My analysis addresses how this hypothetical student’s educational expectations would change by attending a substitute school in the absence of other changes in the student’s personal and family circumstances. In this section, I examine evidence to formulate three separate but interrelated hypotheses regarding how the racial and ethnic makeup of a school could impact the school level of educational expectation. Relationship to Student Characteristics 4 First, the school racial composition could act, in part, as a proxy for the individual characteristics of the enrolled students. Thus, differences in average educational expectations across schools might reflect compositional differences in student bodies that are associated with racial composition. For example, a high minority school often includes larger shares of students from poor families and with prior educational deficiencies. On the whole, these students have lower levels of expectation to attend college, but this is partly a function of individual students’ characteristics that are associated with the school racial composition and not because of any school characteristic in and of itself. In the strongest version of this hypothesis, educational expectations for two statistically identical students who attend vastly different schools would be similar. In other words, this hypothesis suggests that apparent school effects are attributable in part to correlated individual student characteristics. In order to locate and isolate true school effects, then, individual level correlates of educational expectations must first be accounted for. Additionally, most research examining correlates of educational expectations has focused on individual level explanations. The next section, thus, briefly reviews literature focusing on student factors that influence educational goals. Starting with the Wisconsin status attainment model, Sewell and others elaborated on Blau and Duncan’s (1967) basic stratification model that introduced social psychological processes, including educational expectations, to explain educational and occupational attainment (Sewell and Shah 1968; Sewell, Haller et al. 1969; Sewell, Haller et al. 1970). Initially, they focused on how the encouragement and expectations of people important to students, including parents, teachers, and friends explained variation in 5 students’ educational expectations and aspirations (Sewell and Hauser 1992).2 Additionally, Sewell et al.(1969) theorized of the influence of both academic performance and social origins on students’ educational expectations. In extensions of Sewell’s original model of educational expectations (Sewell and Shah 1968; Sewell, Haller et al. 1969; Sewell, Haller et al. 1970), others have focused more exclusively on how socioeconomic factors—including the direct and opportunity costs of attaining higher education—shape educational ambitions (Jencks, Crouse et al. 1983; Morgan 1998). For example, Morgan’s (1998) rational choice framework concentrates attention on how family resource differences to pay for education and expected benefits of educational credentials shape educational expectations. Researchers interested in the influences of race and ethnicity on educational ambitions find that minority students, especially blacks, have higher educational aspirations and expectations than whites of comparable socioeconomic status and early educational experiences (Kao and Tienda 1998; Qian and Blair 1999; Hirschman, Lee et al. 2003). Goldsmith (2004) links higher minority educational goals to other ambitions as a representation of a generalized pattern of optimism among minority youth. Examinations of immigrant expectations further confirm that first and second generation immigrants are highly optimistic about their children’s futures, often expressing college ambitions for their offspring (Kao and Tienda 1995; Cheng and Starks 2002) Thus, at the student level, prior studies in the Wisconsin status attainment tradition identify three types of covariates that explain differentials in educational expectations: significant others’ influence, student socioeconomic background, and academic ability and 2 Here, significant others’ expectations can by conveyed indirectly through modeling, as in the case of admired peers or educated adults, or through specific encouragement of college attendance, often given by 6 experiences. Furthermore, other lines of research suggest that minority and immigrant status are associated with college expectations. These student characteristics are not randomly distributed across schools—students are often clustered in schools according to these factors. Therefore, the impact of racial composition on college expectations partly derives from differences in the student body composition of schools. Specifically, I hypothesize that once relevant student characteristics are accounted for, between school differences in educational expectations will shrink. Furthermore, an initial negative relationship between the proportion of minority students and the average level of educational expectations in a school will lessen, weakening the link between the two. I label this the null hypothesis, since it suggests no independent effects of school racial composition or other school characteristics. Table 1 describes the substantive and analytic components of this and other hypotheses. Relationship to Other School Characteristics Second, racial composition can act as a proxy for other school characteristics that impact student educational expectations. I examine three possibilities, including school socioeconomic status, school average achievement level, and school academic environment. First, schools with high proportions of minorities are also schools that are likely to have a high concentration of economically disadvantaged children. Thus, with a high correlation between race and socioeconomic status, average educational expectations of high minority schools might derive from the school socioeconomic composition. Research findings in the 1960’s and early 1970’s, emanating from a flurry of work focusing on the effects of school context on educational expectations, showed that high school students from high SES schools had higher aspirations than those who attended low SES influential adults, including parents and teachers (Sewell, Haller, et al, 1969; Haller, 1982). 7 schools, even after controlling for their individual SES (Michael 1961; Turner 1964; Meyer 1970). Researchers theorized that the aspirations and attainment of a low SES, working class student might be raised by informal contact with higher SES peers in middle class schools because of exposure to middle class values, aspirations, and knowledge (Thrupp 1997). Presumably, the cultural norms found in a school with few poor students valued educational attainment and identified college attendance as desirable and even inevitable (Jencks and Mayer 1990). The normative role of peers, both providing and reinforcing standards, is central to this perspective. According to the Coleman report, transmission of middle class values from socioeconomically privileged White students to poor minority children was an important benefit of desegregation (Coleman 1966). This suggests that independent of a student’s own family socioeconomic status, there are accrued benefits from attendance in a non-poor school: thus, the socioeconomic composition of peers is an important school characteristic to consider in examination of students’ educational goals. Additionally, this implies that average educational expectations in schools with large shares of minorities are depressed because high minority schools are also schools of lower socioeconomic status. By comparing schools of the same poverty level, the overall relationship between school minority composition and the average level of educational expectations will become less negative. In other words, equalizing schools by school poverty and school socioeconomic status should increase the adjusted level of educational expectations in high minority school should increase. (See Table 1.) A second school characteristic of likely importance also emerged from early research. High minority schools are also more likely than predominately white schools to 8 have lower levels of overall achievement. Perhaps, then, school racial composition is partly an indicator of scholastic attainment. In addition to considering the socioeconomic composition of a school, Davis (1966) examined the role of achievement context on student expectations. He suggested that peers supply a means of reference for self-examination, with a high ability student body providing a more competitive scholastic environment. In a challenging scholastic environment, with academic rewards distributed primarily within schools, students suffer by comparison to more able students. Thus, a student of a given ability level in a highly scholastic student body has a lower academic self-perception than a student of comparable ability in a less talented school. Furthermore, students in high ability schools stand out less in comparison to their bright counterparts and receive less attention and encouragement to attend college from teachers and counselors (Meyer 1970). Davis called this the frog-pond effect. Other research examined the impact of desegregation on black students’ aspirations. In a review article, St John (1975) reports that most studies find black students’ movement to desegregated White schools to be associated with a corresponding drop in their educational ambitions. This is consistent with Davis’s frog-pond model: as black students entered desegregated schools with more skilled and competitive student bodies, not only did their academic self perception drop, but so too did their expectations to attend college, mainly because “realistic possibilities” were reconceptualized (Falk 1978). The frog-pond view implies a negative relationship between average levels of school achievement and educational expectations, and suggests that predominately minority schools have higher average expectations due to the lower average achievement levels of their student bodies. Thus, the theorized effects of school SES and school 9 achievement on educational expectations are in opposite directions. Equalizing schools by achievement composition should decrease the adjusted average expectation level in high minority schools, if Davis’s frog pond theory is correct. (See Table 1.) A third school characteristic related to both minority composition and educational expectations is the academic climate, the scholastic atmosphere of a school and the general amount of educational engagement and interest on the part of the students. Because of fewer resources, large student-counselor ratios, less rigorous curriculums, and lowered teacher expectations, school with large shares of minority students are likely to have less academic environments (Rumberger and Willms 1992; Caldas and Bankston 1998; Ferguson 1998; Corwin, Venegas et al. 2004). Additionally, the school socioeconomic and achievement composition are interrelated with a school’s overall academic climate, making it difficult to completely separate these factors analytically. However, direct measures of school climate suggest that schools with greater academic and collegiate foci are also schools with higher average educational goals and lower shares of minority students. Thus, predominately minority schools might have lower levels of expectations due to the correlation with schools’ less academic atmospheres. By equalizing school on academic environment, increasing proportions of minority students should be associated with more positive levels of educational expectations. (See Table 1.) My second hypothesis with its three variants suggests that the effect of school racial composition on educational expectations can be explained in part by the relationship between school racial composition and other school characteristics, including school socioeconomic status, school achievement, and academic environment. I list the explicit substantive and analytic hypotheses for each school mechanism in Table 1. 10 Racial Composition’s Independent Association As an alternative to hypotheses one and two, including its three variants, there is the ever present possibility that something fundamental about racial composition exists to influence the school level of educational expectations. Supporters of affirmative action in higher education have based recent legal arguments on this notion: the compelling need for racial and ethnic diversity derives from important and socially desirable educational outcomes (Gurin, Dey et al. 2002). This view presumes that there is some optimal racial and ethnic composition of student bodies that enhances educational outcomes for students of all racial and ethnic groups. Likewise, it is possible that high school racial composition could independently influence high school students’ educational expectations, with similar effects for students of all racial groups. However, there are also theories that suggest a differential impact of racial composition on minority students. John Ogbu theorized the development of an oppositional culture when members of involuntary minority groups, defined as groups historically forced to incorporate within the majority society by colonization, enslavement, or conquest, are concentrated in geographical areas, such as schools (Fordham and Ogbu 1986; Ogbu 1991). Because of their shared experience of exclusion and discrimination, involuntary minorities develop a group identity, whose main characteristic is direct opposition to the majority culture. Presumably, black or Hispanic students who desire to excel academically and pursue higher education are betraying their group identities by “acting White” and hence are marginalized from their groups. Ogbu’s premise implies that there is a statistical interaction between students’ ethnicity and the racial composition of their school. Specifically, when black and Hispanic students attend schools with critical 11 masses of their own groups, the development of an oppositional culture leads to a devaluation of academic goals and lowers educational ambitions. In concrete terms, higher proportions of minority students should be associated with lower average expectations, but only for these minority students. Other studies find that minority self esteem and academic self perception is lower in racially balanced schools than in high minority schools (Drury 1980; Gray-Little and Carels 1997). Thus, if self esteem shapes educational expectations, in part, then minority students who attend schools where their own race is more highly represented will have higher expectations to attend and graduate from college. This is reminiscent of Davis’ (1966) earlier frog-pond hypothesis. Based on this proposition, black school administrators in districts with large black populations have created schools solely for black children, with an Afro-centric curriculum, because they believe it will be of greater educational benefit than a mixed race school. This view contradicts Ogbu’s claims about oppositional culture: Ogbu implies that high minority composition produces poor educational outcomes for minority students, while the self esteem hypothesis suggests that minority students might have more ambitious educational goals when they are clustered together. Finally, a recent paper offers another possible explanation for an independent effect of racial composition. Goldsmith (2004) found that black and Hispanic students have higher educational expectations when attending schools with more minority students and minority teachers, while white beliefs are largely independent of the amount of school segregation by race. He suggests that this is because black and Hispanic students have more positive beliefs about their future prospects and more pro-school attitudes, and that their concentration in school improves the normative climate. He also claims that minority 12 teachers are better able to raise minority students’ beliefs than are white teachers. He concludes that when clustering of minority students within schools occurs, although disadvantageous in some regards, optimism and pro school attitudes increase among these minority students. Although I won’t directly test these three theories in this paper, I shed some light on their validity by an examination of the empirical evidence of an independent influence of racial composition on expectations, as well a differential impact of racial composition on students of differing races. (See Table 1.) DATA Data for this study are primarily taken from the Texas Higher Education Opportunity Project (THEOP), an ongoing project designed to understand the consequences of Texas’ replacement of a race sensitive college admission regime with a percent plan on minority students’ college enrollment3. The sampling design of this study makes the study of school effects possible. The survey was based on a stratified random sample of 108 Texas public schools with a student body consisting of at least 10 enrolled seniors, and which was further stratified on the basis of metropolitan area status and school racial/ethnic composition. Of the eligible schools selected, 93% participated in the study. Thus, 13,803 seniors are clustered in 96 high schools, and 19,969 sophomores are clustered in 97 high schools. The sample is representative of students enrolled in Texas public high schools during the spring of 2002. 3 Under the 1996 5th Circuit Court Hopwood v. University of Texas Law School decision, race sensitive affirmative action for college admissions was banned. In its place, the Texas legislature passed HB 558, popularly known as the top 10% Law, which allows the top 10% of students from any Texas high school to attend any public university in Texas. 13 During the spring of 2002, baseline data were collected within sampled schools from high school sophomores and seniors using a paper and pencil survey. A random sample of the original senior cohort is being followed for a planned total of six years as these students continue from high school on to college and other post high school activities. The first follow up of the senior cohort took place during spring and summer of 2003. Additionally, the sophomore cohort were reinterviewed during their senior year, in the spring of 2004. For the purposes of this study, I use baseline data only from the senior cohort. The survey asked respondents about their grades and class rank, course taking, extra-curricular activities, and knowledge and perceptions of college admissions, including the top 10% law. Additionally, seniors were asked about their future plans, college applications, and their university preferences. Essential for the purposes of our study, students were asked how much education they would like to attain, a series of questions about their educational experiences, and how much encouragement they received from teachers, counselors, and parents regarding college attendance, as well as standard background questions, including immigration status and language ability. To assess contextual determinants of educational expectations, I merged data from the US Department of Education and the Texas Education Agency (TEA) with the individual level data files using a school identifier. The Common Core of Data, a program of the United States Department of Education, is an annual collection of school and district level information, and I use this data to obtain information on school racial composition, the key independent variable, as well as on school poverty, measuring the proportion of students eligible for free and reduced price lunches. The TEA provides school-level 14 information about a variety of outcomes, including high schools’ achievement and test scores, AP course availability and enrollment, and school size. Finally, by aggregating survey data by school, I generated additional contextual variables, including school parental education level and the proportion of students within a school knowing that the top ten percent of students from each school are automatically granted admission to any public Texas university. Although I only use the senior survey data for statistical analyses, I aggregate both senior and sophomore data together by school in order to obtain greater precision in contextual measures. I imposed two constraints on my study sample. First, I omit all cases that lack valid responses in my main dependent variable, educational expectations. This excludes 7.6% of the sample cases4. Second, because individual variable measuring racial status, I include only those students identifying themselves as White, black, Hispanic, and Asian. Other ethnic groups had small sample sizes, and thus I omit all students who report that they are Native American, “other”, or multi-racial. Only a small proportion of the sample is dropped for this reason, 1.7%. Thus, the final study sample is made up of 12,526 senior students clustered in 96 schools. To address the other individual level missing data in my study sample, I used a form of hotdeck imputation. In order from the most missing to the least missing, I regressed each variable with missing values on all the other individual level variables used in the analysis, and then sorted the data based on predicted values for the variable of 4 In the survey, the question from which I draw my dependent variable of educational expectations, “Realistically, how far do you think you will go in school?” immediately follows the question, “How far would you like to go in school?” measuring academic aspirations. After examining the missing patterns in the two questions, it seems highly likely that those who were missing on the second question (expectations) but not the first (aspirations) assumed that the questions were identical and believed answering the second was replicating their previous answer. Thus, for the small portion of students who were missing on 15 interest. I then divided my sample up into bins of 50 respondents each to locate donors for missing values. Within each bin, I randomly selected a non-missing value to impute a value for missing cases. I repeated this process for each of the variables with missing data and flagged all instances where data were imputed. Texas as a research context Texas, like other immigrant receiving states, has witnessed rapid change in its racial and ethnic diversity, especially among the school age population. This makes Texas a propitious location to study school effects. Between 1980 and 2000, as the total school age population increased, the white school age population declined as a proportion of the total from 56% to 43%, while the Hispanic share of the total rose from 28% to 40% (Murdock, Hoque et al. 1997). This trend is expected to continue. If the school age population was distributed equally throughout the state of Texas by race, this demography would not give reason to investigate the effects of school racial composition on educational outcomes. However, this is not the case: the state is racially segregated by region, within cities, and by school district. Figure 1 shows how the racial distribution of students within schools varies by race across the state of Texas. The bar furthest on the left shows the overall school racial composition experienced by the average student in Texas in 2002. As a trait of individual students, these differences indicate that the average Texas student attends school with about 50% white students, one-third Hispanic, 12% black, and 2% Asian. Then, each additional bar to the right represents the school racial composition for the typical black, white, Hispanic, and Asian student. Because of housing patterns, residential segregation, and expectations, but not aspirations, I replaced their response for aspirations on the expectations question. This included .72% of the sample. 16 geographic clustering, students of differing races attend school with very difference racial compositions. Thus, Figure 1 shows that the average Hispanic student attends a school that is more than 60% Hispanic, and 29% White, 8% black, and 2% Asian. Yet, the average white students attends schools with two-thirds white students and only 20% Hispanics. All in all, a clear pattern of racial clustering within schools emerges. Given this racial separation of students in Texas, it is important to understand how school racial composition impacts educational outcomes, and specifically, educational expectations. Measures and Descriptive Results Educational expectations was obtained from a closed-choice question that asked high school students how much education they realistically expect to attain. Choices range from high school graduation to Ph.D., M.D., or other professional degree. I convert these responses into a dichotomous variable measuring whether survey respondents expect to attain a 4-year college degree. Table 2 shows that on average, 67% of students expect to attain a 4-year college degree. Overall expectations are high, given current statistics suggesting that only 32% of recent high school graduates actually attained a four year college degree5. However, because I focus on senior students, high school dropouts are excluded from the sample, likely inflating somewhat the proportion of the senior cohort that expect to complete college. At the same time, other studies have reported similar levels of educational expectations among high school youth (Kao and Tienda 1998; Qian and Blair 1999; Hirschman, Lee et al. 2003). Table 2 also shows differences in average expectation by the racial composition of schools. Here, low and high minority schools are defined to be schools in the first and last quartile, respectively, of the school proportion of 17 total non-Asian minority students. Thus, 60% of students in high minority schools, compared to almost 70% of students in low minority schools, expect to attain a college degree. The key independent variables in my analysis are school racial/ethnic composition, measured as the proportion of black and Hispanic students within each school6. The average school in the sample contains slightly more than 50% white students, one-third Hispanic students, and 12% black students, with the remaining 2% student population consisting primarily of Asian students. In the schools with the lowest shares of minority students, almost 90% of students, on average, are white, compared to only 8% in schools with high proportions of minorities. I measure two aspects of school composition by generating indices through factor analysis to operationalize school socioeconomic status and school academic climate. My third measure of school composition, school achievement, is based on only one variable. All variables measuring school characteristics are described below. Table 3 shows factor loadings and Cronbach’s alphas for variables included in the indices. One measure of the school’s socioeconomic status is the school average of attained parental education, which captures both parental income differences and values about education. Presumably, a greater proportion of college educated parents within a school will translate to strong school support for higher education. A critical mass of college educated parents can impact the quality of instruction because they are more likely to be a collective agent for high standards and an academic curriculum in a school. Because I am 5 Author’s tabulations from March 2002 CPS. Although the composition of Asian students within schools suggests a unique and interesting impact on the level of expectations to complete a 4-year college degree, the limited variance in this variable produced unstable estimates. Thus, I focus here on only disadvantaged minority groups. 6 18 interested in student’s college expectations, I measure parental education at the school level as the proportion of parents who have obtained at least a four year college degree. The school average for the sample is one-third. However, this varies dramatically between high- and low- minority schools. In low minority schools, 44% of parents have attained at least a 4-year college degree, while the comparable proportion in high minority schools is half as many parents, only 21%. As an additional measures of school socioeconomic status, I use the proportion of students qualifying for free and reduced price lunches. Only 23% of students in low minority schools, compared with 61% in high minority schools, qualify, while the sample average is 37%. Using factor analysis, I combine these two measures to generate an overall measure of school socioeconomic status, with positive scores representing schools with wealthier students. Comparison of high and low minority school shows that schools with many minority students are, as expected, poorer. I measure school achievement by using data from the Texas Education Agency regarding the proportion of the schools’ 10th graders who met state standards in all required state testing areas on the Texas Assessment of Knowledge and Skills (TAKS)7. Because of state requirements, seniors are not tested, and data is only available for 10th graders. Although my sample consists of 12th graders, this measure reflects the achievement composition of the school they attended. Because lower ability students are more likely to withdraw from school between 10th and 12th grade, measurement of academic achievement in the 10th grade provides a better index of school achievement than a comparable measure of senior students. On average, about 50% of sampled students passed Texas state tests in all required testing areas. As expected, low minority schools have higher levels of 19 achievement than high minority schools, with 58% compared with 37%, respectively, of students meeting state standards in all tested areas. I use three measures of a school’s academic environment: feeder high school status, AP courses taken per senior, and the proportion of students reporting knowledge of the top 10% law. Feeder high schools are affluent schools with strong histories of sending students to the two Texas flagship universities: University of Texas—Austin and Texas A&M. Thus, for each university, the top 20 high schools, based on the numbers of students admitted to UT-Austin and A&M, are designated as feeder schools. In total, only 4 schools in our sample are feeder schools, but three of these have low minority shares, while there are no feeder schools among the highest minority schools in our sample. By school, the average number of AP courses taken per senior taken is slightly lower in high minority schools: .145 vs. .211 in low minority school. In additional, only 30% of students within schools, on average, report knowing about the top 10% law, a key feature of Texas higher education, and this varies little between low and high minority schools. Using factor analysis, I combine these three variables into one overall measure of school academic climate, and as expected, high minority schools have less scholastic environments, on average. In order to isolate the impacts of school level characteristics on educational expectations, I include several categories of individual covariates in the multilevel analysis that are well established correlates of educational expectations, including students’ socioeconomic background, educational background and experience, significant others’ influence, and language and immigration8. All student characteristics are taken from the 7 8 Includes English language arts, math, science, and social studies. Hispanic status, language, and immigration are not highly correlated. 20 THEOP 2000 wave 1 survey. Table 1 shows that approximately half of Texas high school students are White, and one-third are Hispanic, which is consistent with state reports. The remainder of the student population is divided between blacks, around 11%, and Asians, 4%. Thus, the school age population in Texas is about equally split between white and minority students. Table 1 also shows how the other independent variables vary according to the school racial composition. Higher shares of parents of students who attend low minority schools completed college. For the most part, students from higher minority schools are disadvantaged with respect to their educational experiences and background, but there are a couple of notable exceptions. Students in high minority schools are more likely to be enrolled in a college preparatory curriculum, according to their self reports9. Additionally, these students are more likely to report positive attitudes towards school and express educational goals. As anticipated, higher proportions of students in high minority schools are immigrants and speak languages other than English with friends, which suggests some deficiency in English language ability. Estimating School Effects on Educational Expectations I use a multilevel modeling strategy to estimate how school racial composition and other school characteristics impact educational expectations. In multilevel parlance, level-1 refers to the micro-level unit of analysis, here the individual student, and level-2 refers to the contexts where level-1 units are clustered, in this instance the school (Raudenbush and Bryk 2002). Multilevel regression has increasingly been used to estimate class, school, and other context effects within multiple level data structures, especially when individuals 21 observations are clustered within these higher level units. Because students clustered within schools are not statistically independent observations, traditional linear and binary regression models produce downwardly biased standard errors, which can lead to incorrect inferences about the statistical and substantive importance of school context variables (Guo and Zhao 2000). However, multilevel models explicitly adjust for the nonindependence of sample members who share a level-2 context, such as a school (Raudenbush and Bryk 2002). Two general types of multilevel models are used to estimate school effects on a specific student level outcome, namely random slope and random intercept models. With random slope models, the effects of individual student attributes are allowed to vary across schools. Thus, these models explicitly test for differences in slopes across schools. Random-intercept models examine questions about differences in the level of a student outcome across schools. When the outcome variable differs across schools, random intercept models can be used to identify what school characteristics are responsible for different outcomes. Because my research focuses on the extent to which school racial composition and other explanatory variables explain variance in educational expectations across schools, I fix all slopes and estimate random intercept models to answer the following questions: Does the level of educational expectations vary across schools? Do student and school characteristics explain the relationship between school racial composition and educational expectations? Do student characteristics, school racial composition, and other school level characteristics account for variation in educational expectations between schools? Specifically, I use hierarchical logistic random intercept 9 However, I have no ability to control for the quality of the courses students have taken. 22 models estimated with sixth order approximation to the likelihood based on a Laplace transform for Bernoulli models. To begin, I first examine a simple histogram of the proportion of students by school who expect to complete a four-year college degree. This is the school level distribution of my dependent variable and provides a visual description of how much variation in educational expectations exists between schools. Following up with this descriptive analysis, I formally test whether there is significant between school variation in expectation to complete a university education by estimating a simple unconditional model without any predictors. This provides a baseline estimate of τ00, the between school variance, and its standard error, information to analyze the underlying premise of this paper that educational expectation do in fact vary between schools. The initial estimate of τ00 serves as a point of comparison for subsequent models. Thus, I can examine how much τ00 is reduced—or how much of the between school variance in educational expectations is explained—by the inclusion of specific groups of variables. Subsequently, I model the effect of school racial/ethnic composition on educational expectations before any controls, providing an estimate of the observed relationship between them. Because of the continuous measure of school racial composition, coefficients represent the estimated change in log odds of expecting a 4-year college degree that are associated with a unit change in the percent of Hispanic or black students in a school. Next, I introduce student characteristics into my models to examine how much of the between school variance in educational expectations is due to school differences in student composition, and how the association between school racial composition and 23 educational expectations changes with these additions, as discussed in the null hypothesis. Table 1 provides analytic details for this and the other hypotheses I test. I also examine in brief how students’ individual level characteristics impact educational expectations. In these regressions, I centered the values of each student variable around the grand mean by subtracting the state mean from each one. Although grand mean centering does not change the estimated values of the regression slopes, it does shift the value of the intercept. With this transformation, the intercept provides an estimate of a school’s mean college expectation level with the statewide average on all included variables. Because grand mean centering provides more stable estimates (Raudenbush and Bryk 2002), I use this transformation throughout the analysis. In the next step of analysis, I test hypothesis 1 by adding in the other school characteristics, one group at a time, to a model that includes both student racial composition and individual student characteristics. Because a school’s socioeconomic status, achievement, and academic environment could be related to school racial composition and students’ expectation to complete a university degree, I use these model to examine how the estimates of the school racial composition variables change in the presence of these additional school characteristics. (See Table 1.) Additionally, I can observe if the other school variables exert an independent effect on educational expectations. I include school size as a control variable. In the final analytic portion designed to evaluate hypothesis 2, I look for evidence of independent effects of the school minority composition on seniors’ college expectations by analyzing a full model with all school level and student level characteristics. I additionally include interaction effects between individual students’ race and their schools’ 24 racial composition to ascertain whether the effect of racial composition on educational expectations varies by student race. (See Table 1.) Results The central question of my analysis is how school racial composition impacts high school students’ expectations to complete a 4-year college degree. Beginning my analysis, I examine between school differences in educational expectations with a histogram shown in Figure 2. This data is reported at the school level with a total sample size of 96, and so the x-axis measures the school average of the proportion of students who expect to attain a 4-year college degree. For example, there are 23 schools in the sample where between 46% and 55% of the students in these schools expect to complete college, as shown by the 4th bar. For seniors, the school average proportion of students who expect to complete a 4year college degree ranges from 20% to 90%, with a clustering of schools falling between 50% and 80%. This figure demonstrates substantial heterogeneity of educational expectations across Texas high schools. Substantively, a school where only 30% of students expect to complete college is quite different from one where 80% of students expect to complete college. In order to formally test whether there is significant variation in the level of educational expectations among schools, as suggested by Figure 2, I test an unconditional model, which estimates only τ00, the between school variance component, and an intercept describing the mean level of educational expectations, and these results are presented in Model 1 of Table 3. Although hierarchical logistic models do not allow comparisons of the between- and within-school variance components of expectations to complete college, the 25 initial estimate of the between school variance component, or τ00, provides a benchmark for examining the sources of reduction in the between-school variance. Because the estimates of τ00 and its standard error confirm that the level of expectation to complete a 4- year college degree does vary significantly among schools10, I next turn to multivariate analysis to examine the sources of this variation and analyze the role of school racial composition on educational expectations. Model 2 in Table 3 shows the effects of the percentage of black and Hispanic students on plans to complete college before any other covariates are added. While the distribution of black students is not related to educational expectations, the proportion of Hispanic students in a school is negatively associated with the proportion of students in a school who expect to complete a 4-year college degree. In substantive terms, a 10% increase in the share of Hispanic students lowers the school odds of expecting a 4-year college degree by 5% (1-exp(-.005*10)). Model 3 in Table 3 shows results from a model testing the null hypothesis to determine if racial composition acts as a proxy for the school composition of student characteristics. By adjusting for individual socioeconomic status, educational experiences and achievement, significant others’ influence, race, and immigration status, the negative impact of the proportion of Hispanic students on educational expectations disappears, suggesting that as schools increase the number of Hispanics enrolled, these students average less of the resources and educational experiences associated with college graduation expectations. Furthermore, the 26% reduction in τ00 from Model 2 and Model 3 suggests that the collection of student level characteristics related to educational 10 HLM does not provide significance tests of the Laplace transform approximations because of disagreement over exactly how to estimate p-values, but using a rough rule of thumb, parameter estimates larger than 1.96 26 expectations at the school level is responsible for a substantial portion of the between school variance in educational expectations. The weakened relationship between school racial composition and educational goals lends some support to the null hypothesis proposing that school racial composition acts as an indicator of individual student characteristics that influence their educational expectations. (See Table 1.) However, examination of τ00 and its standard error for Model 3 show that the between school variance in educational expectations is still significant, not entirely accounted for by differences in school composition. For the most part, the student level results from Model 3 are consistent with previous research and in the expected directions. Parental socioeconomic status, educational background and ability, and significant others’ influences are strongly associated with students’ educational expectations. Speaking a language other than English with friends, suggesting limited English proficiency, is associated with lower expectations, while individual immigration status is also associated with lower expectations, even when adjusting for socioeconomic status and educational background. I find no racial/ethnic differences in expectations to complete college once I also adjust for socioeconomic status and educational background, in contrast to other research that finds that blacks, and sometimes Hispanics, have significantly higher expectations than whites of comparable socioeconomic status (Qian and Blair 1999; Hirschman, Lee et al. 2003; Goldsmith 2004). To evaluate the second hypothesis that racial composition is a proxy for other school characteristics that influence educational expectations, I examine models that add the school characteristics one group at a time to Model 3 in Table 3, just reported. Table 4 reports the results from these models, showing only the coefficients of the school level times their standard error are significant at the .05 level, and this estimate of τ00 clearly meets this criteria. 27 variables, even though all individual level variables are also included.11 The first model includes the factored variable measuring the socioeconomic composition of the school and shows that schools with more college educated parents and fewer students qualifying for free/reduced price lunches are strongly associated with students’ college expectations. Thus, when the socioeconomic composition of the school increases by one standard deviation on the factored scale, the odds of expecting a college degree increase by 50%12. Once school are equalized by their socioeconomic composition, the proportion of both black and Hispanic students becomes positively and significantly related to college expectations. Substantively, among schools of comparable socioeconomic status, a 10% increase in Hispanic students raises the odds of expecting to complete a 4-year college degree by 8%, while the comparable figure for a 10% increase in black students is 9%. As predicted by hypothesis 1a, because higher educational expectations are associated with both schools of higher socioeconomic status and lower proportions of minority students, adjustment for the socioeconomic composition of schools results in a positive association between higher proportions of minority students and higher overall levels of expectations. (See Table 1.) With the second group of measures, I include the achievement composition of the school. The effect on college expectations of the school average pass rate on the state standardized tests is positive and significant: a 10% increase in a school’s pass rate is associated with a 12% increase in the odds of students expecting to graduate with a 4-year 11 In each of these models, estimates of individual level coefficients and standard errors do not change in any substantial fashion from those reported in Model 3, Table 3. Full student level results are available upon request. 12 A similar analysis using individual variables rather than a factor show similar results and suggests that the proportion of college educated parents in a school is especially important in explaining between school variation in educational expectations. 28 college degree. Unfortunately, comparable data is not available for individual student achievement, and although I have adjusted for other student factors that are associated with achievement, I can not eliminate the possibility that this school variable simply reflects variation in individual achievement. At best, it is likely that the effect of school achievement shown here is overstated. However, given these caveats, the positive effect of achievement on expectations is the opposite from what the Davis frog pond model suggested. In fact, as school average scholastic achievement increases, individual college expectations increase as well. Similar to the socioeconomic composition of schools, equalizing schools based on their achievement composition results in an association of higher proportions of black students with rising expectations. An increase of 10% in the black proportion of students within a school yields a 9% increase in the school level odds of expecting a 4-year college degree. Thus, greater educational expectations are associated with higher levels of school achievement, but high minority schools are more likely to have low average levels of scholastic achievement. Once adjustment is made for the achievement composition of schools, higher proportions of minority students result in increased expectation levels, contrary to the predictions of hypothesis 2b. (See Table 1.) In the next model, I examine the influence of academic environment on both educational expectations and the effect of racial composition on expectations. Results in Table 4 show both that academic environment is unrelated to expectations and does not mediate the impact of racial composition on expectations, contrary to the prediction of hypothesis 1c. (See Table 1.) 29 I include all school variables in a complete full model, shown in Model 4 of Table 3. Results show, as suggested before, the positive effects of school socioeconomic composition and achievement composition on educational expectations, although the effect of school SES is reduced somewhat from the earlier model shown in Table 4. Additionally, in this model, the proportions of black and Hispanic students are positively related to educational expectations. Thus, equalizing school achievement and socioeconomic status, an increase of 10% in the proportion of black or Hispanic students in a school is associated on average with a 8-13% increase in the odds of expecting a 4-year college degree. This finding suggests an independent effect of school racial composition, consistent with my third hypothesis. (See Table 1.) The between school variance for the full model of .082 represents a 65% decrease in τ00 from the previous model that controlled only for individual level characteristics and school racial composition.13 Thus, a substantial proportion of the school level differences in educational expectations is accounted for with the school variables included in my analysis. Other analysis shows that this reduction is largely due to the addition of school parental education composition included in the school socioeconomic status factor: thus, a primary reason for the between school heterogeneity of educational expectations derives from school differences in average parental education level. However, comparison of τ00 and its standard error in Model 4 still show that the variation in educational expectations between schools remains statistically significant, 13 Because of the sequential ordering of my models, it is impossible to compare τ00 from a model with only student characteristics to one with both student and school characteristics. Other analysis, not shown here, estimates τ00 to be .242 when only level-1 characteristics are included, a 29% reduction from the unconditional model. Adding level-2 characteristics to this results in a τ00 of .082, leading to the conclusion that level-2 characteristics are responsible for a reduction of 66% in the between school variance not accounted for by level-1 characteristics. 30 indicating that there are other undetermined factors that influence differences between schools in students’ educational goals. Finally, I consider if the effect of school racial proportion is the same for students of differing racial and ethnic groups by including interaction terms between individual student race and school racial distribution and these results are shown in Model 5 of Table 3. Both the main effects of the variables measuring the school proportion of black and Hispanic students their statistical significance suggesting that for white students, attending schools with black or Hispanic students is associated with higher educational expectations. Additionally, black and Hispanic students’ expectations improve in schools with larger shares of black students, contradicting Ogbu’s suggestion of a negative relationship between the individual minority students and the proportion of minority students. This finding could be related to the self esteem or optimism hypotheses presented earlier, although it is unclear why only the proportion of black students and not Hispanic students is significantly associated with higher college expectations for minority students. Discussion Unlike most studies that consider school effects on educational outcomes, this study finds several school characteristics that are associated with higher educational expectations, including greater proportions of minority students. The observed negative relationship between the proportion of minority students and educational expectations is reversed by comparing schools with similar kinds of students, socioeconomic levels, and scholastic achievement. Moreover, the analysis suggests that although greater concentrations of minority students positively influence students of all racial backgrounds, greater 31 concentrations of black students have an even larger influence on black and Hispanic students. How robust is the finding that students who attend school with large proportions of minority students are more likely to expect to graduate from a 4-year university? I tested various specifications of the variables measuring school racial composition to examine whether the same results emerge from predominately white schools, mixed racial schools, and predominately minority schools. These analyses (based on dichotomous measures to represent segments of the school minority composition) reaffirmed my basic result: all other things equal, college expectations are higher among student, who attend schools with greater shares of minority students. I also considered whether there is an optimal mix of students associated with higher educational expectation, and that any segregated school— either highly white or highly minority—would not exhibit the same level of expectations by including higher-order racial composition terms. These results further support the positive monotonic influence of schools’ minority composition on educational expectations. Whether higher proportions of minority students actually raise students’ goals to complete a university degree or whether more segregated schools concentrate students with more ambitious educational goals is unclear. Because of the cross-sectional nature of the survey data used and the nonrandom assignment of students to schools, this question of causality cannot be definitively answered. It is possible that students differ across schools in ways that are related to their educational expectations, but were not accounted for in this analysis. Nevertheless, this study showed that net of individual and school differences, students attending more minority segregated schools, and black and Hispanic students 32 attending more black schools are more likely to have ambitious educational goals to complete college. What are possible reasons for the counterintuitive finding that in similar school contexts, students in schools with greater shares of minority students are more likely to expect to graduate from a 4-year college? The evidence from this analysis does not square with either the oppositional culture or the self-esteem hypothesis. Ogbu’s oppositional culture theory suggested that concentrated shares of minority students result in lowered educational ambitions, but only for minority students. My findings refute this claim: I find that increased proportions of minority students are associated with higher educational expectations, for all students. My findings are closer to those proposed by the self-esteem hypothesis, which suggests that segregated schools benefit minority students’academic self image, leading to higher educational goals. Contrary to my findings, this effect is only expected for minority students, but the additional importance of greater school concentrations of blacks for black and Hispanic students’ educational goals provides some support for this hypothesis. Perhaps, as suggested by the self-esteem hypothesis, the reason that black and Hispanic students have higher educational expectations when attending schools with more black students is because of the greater school academic self-esteem in segregated schools due to a shielding from real levels of competition. There are two problems, though, with this explanation. There is no theoretical reason to assume that the concentration of Hispanic students would not have an influence similar to black concentration on educational expectations. But, I find no differential influence of the proportion of Hispanic students on the educational goals of black and Hispanic students. Additionally, the self-esteem hypothesis shares some similarities with Davis frog pond 33 theory: both suggest that students are shielded from a realistic and complete view of academic competition by school segregation, although the mechanisms are different. I find evidence in this analysis that school achievement levels are associated with higher educational expectations, contrary to Davis’ frog pond theory. If achievement is positively linked with self esteem, then it is not clear that self esteem will be higher in schools with high concentrations of minority students. With direct self-esteem measures, a more complete investigation of this theory would be possible. However, self-esteem in THEOP is measured only at wave 2, available only for a sub-sample of the original 13,803 students. Because the original school level sample is not used for wave 2 sampling, it is impossible to link student self-esteem with school effects. In a variation of this theme, Goldsmith (2004) reports that minority students attending segregated minority schools with many minority teachers are more optimistic and express more pro-school attitudes than similar minority students attending white schools. He suggests that the racial composition of the teaching faculty is key to understanding this effect, and that minority teachers are better able to encourage and influence minority students. Despite differences in our data14, I attempted to replicated his results, but found no evidence that schools’ teacher racial composition influenced students’ educational expectations. Goldsmith’s study focused on very high educational goals (graduate or professional school) of younger, 8th grade students, whose expectations are not fully formed and who may have more unrealistic educational goals. Perhaps these younger more impressionable students are more influenced by their teachers than older students who are more seasoned by life’s reality checks. Whatever the reasons for this difference, I 14 Goldsmith uses NELS 1988 national data. 34 find no evidence that minority teachers in schools with large proportions of minority students are associated with high educational expectations. Goldsmith (2004) also claims that school concentration of black and Hispanic students in schools improves their normative climate because they have more optimistic and pro-school attitudes, which could lead, in turn, to more ambitious educational goals. “Concentrating students with high beliefs generally will raise all students’ beliefs” (p. 141). In subsequent analysis, I find that black and Hispanic students do have more pro-school and positive attitudes than do white students, and this seems to be one likely explanation, and would account for the reason why even for white students, schools with more minority students are associated with higher educational expectations. My findings also square with research by Ainsworth-Darnell and Downey (1998), who find that black students report more pro-school attitudes than do whites, which enhances their academic success. However, in further analyses, I find that the overall school attitudinal climate is unrelated to educational expectations and does not mediate the association between school racial composition and educational expectations. It is possible that these measures do not completely capture optimism and pro-school attitudes within a school. More research is needed to examine the association between school racial composition and school optimism. Although school racial composition, the focus of this paper, is associated with college expectations, it is noteworthy that other school factors, especially the school parental education composition, are more important in explaining why schools differ in their levels of student educational expectations. Thus, although student in high minority schools do have the advantages of higher than expected educational goals, they are also in school contexts disadvantaged by low socioeconomic status and low achievement, in 35 addition to greater levels of individual socioeconomic and educational disadvantage. Furthermore, educational expectations are only one of many important academic outcomes that accrue to students during the period of their high school enrollment and only one of the first of many steps to an eventual university degree. Further work is needed to consider how high schools influence other stages of the college preparation and attainment process, including academic preparation, application, and initial and continued university enrollment. 36 Agency, T. E. (2003). Academic Excellence Indicator System Report, 2002-2003. Austin, TX. Ainsworth-Darnell, J. W. and D. B. Downey (1998). "Assessing the Oppositional Culture Explanation for Racial/Ethnic Differences in School Performance." American Sociological Review 63(4): 536-553. Alexander, K. and B. K. Eckland (1975). "Contextual Effects in the High School Attainment Process." American Sociological Review 40(June): 402-416. Alwin, D. F. and L. B. Otto (1977). "High School Context Effects on Aspirations." Sociology of Education 50(October): 259-273. Blau, P. M. and O. D. Duncan (1967). The American Occupational Structure. New York, New York, Wiley. Buchmann, C. and B. Dalton (2002). "Interpersonal Influences and Educational Aspirations in 12 Countries: The Importance of Institutional Context." Sociology of Education 75(April): 99-122. Caldas, S. J. and C. I. Bankston (1998). "The Inequality of Separation: Racial Composition of Schools and Academic Achievement." Educational Administration Quarterly 34(4): 533557. Campbell, R. T. (1983). "Status Attainment Research: End of the Beginning or Beginning of the End." Sociology of Education 56(1): 47-62. Cheng, S. and B. Starks (2002). "Racial Differences in the Effects of Significant Others on Students' Educational Expectations." Sociology of Education 75(October): 306-327. Coleman, J. (1966). Equality of Educational Opportunity. Washington, D.C., U. S. Government Printing Office. Corwin, Z. B., K. M. Venegas, et al. (2004). "School Counsel: How Apppropriate Guidance Affects Educational Equity." Urban Education 39(4): 442-457. Davis, J. A. (1966). "The Campus as a Frog Pond: An Application of the Theory of Relative Deprivation to Career Decisions of College Men." American Journal of Sociology 72: 17-31. Drury, D. W. (1980). "Black Self-Esteem and Desegregated Schools." Sociology of Education 53(2): 88-103. Falk, W. W. (1978). "School Desegregation and the Educational Attainment Process: Some Results from Rural Texas School." Sociology of Education 51(4): 282-288. 37 Ferguson, R. (1998). Teachers' Perceptions and Expectations and the Black-White Test Score Gap. The Black-White Test Score Gap. C. Jencks and M. Phillips. Washington, D.C., Brookings Institution Press. Fordham, S. and J. U. Ogbu (1986). "Black Students' School Success: Coping with the Burden of 'Acting White'"." The Urban Review 18(3): 176-206. Goldsmith, P. A. (2004). "Schools' Racial Mix, Students' Optimism, and the Black-White and Latino-White Acheivement Gaps." Sociology of Education 77(April): 121-147. Gray-Little, B. and R. A. Carels (1997). "The Effect of Racial Dissonance on Academic Self-Esteem and Achievement in Elementary, Junior High, and High School Students." Journal of Research on Adolescence 7(2): 109-131. Guo, G. and H. X. Zhao (2000). "Multilevel modeling for binary data." Annual Review of Sociology 26: 441-462. Gurin, P., E. L. Dey, et al. (2002). "Diversity and higher education: Theory and impact on educational outcomes." Harvard Educational Review 72(3): 330-366. Hanson, S. L. (1994). "Lost Talent: Unrealized Educational Aspirations and Expectations Among US Youths." Sociology of Education 67(3): 159-183. Hao, L. and M. Bonstead-Bruns (1998). "Parent-Child DIfferences in Educational Expectations and the Academic Achievement of Immigrant and Native Students." Sociology of Education 71: 175-198. Hauser, R. M., W. H. Sewell, et al. (1976). High School Effects on Achievement. Schooling and Achievement in American Society. W. H. Sewell, R. M. Hauser and D. L. Featherman. New York, Academic Press. Hirschman, C., J. C. Lee, et al. (2003). Explaining Race and Ethnic Disparities in Educational Ambitions. The UW Beyond High School in Washington State Project, Seattle, Washington. Jencks, C., J. Crouse, et al. (1983). "The Wisconsin Model of Status Attainment: A National Replication with Improved Measures of Ability and Aspiration." Sociology of Education 56(1): 3-19. Jencks, C. and S. E. Mayer (1990). The Social Consequences of Growing Up in a Poor Neighborhood. Inner City Poverty in the United States. L. E. Lynn and M. G. H. McGeary. Washington, D. C., National Academy Press. Kao, G. and M. Tienda (1995). "Optimism and Achievement: The Educational Performance of Immigrant Youth." Social Science Quarterly 76(2): 1-19. 38 Kao, G. and M. Tienda (1998). "Educational Aspirations of Minority Youth." American Journal of Education 106(3): 349-384. Meyer, J. W. (1970). "High School Effects on College Intentions." American Journal of Sociology 76(1): 59-70. Michael, J. A. (1961). "High School Climates and Plans for Entering College." Public Opinion Quarterly 25(585-595). Morgan, S. L. (1998). "Adolescent Educational Expectations: Rationalized, Fantasized, or Both?" Rationality and Society 10(2): 131-162. Murdock, S. H., N. Hoque, et al. (1997). The Texas Challenge: Population Change and the Future of Texas. College Station, TX, Texas A&M University Press. Ogbu, J. U. (1991). Low School Performance as an Adaptation: The Case of Blacks in Stockton, California. Minority Status and Schooling: A Comparative Study of Immigrant and Involuntary Immigrants. M. A. Gibson and J. U. Ogbu. New York, New York, Garland Publishing, Inc: 249-285. Orfield, G. and J. T. Yun (1999). Resegration in American Schools. The Civil Rights Project, Harvard University. Cambridge, MA. Qian, Z. and S. L. Blair (1999). "Racial/Ethnic Differences in Educational Aspirations of High School Seniors." Sociological Perspectives 42(4): 605-625. Raudenbush, S. W. and A. S. Bryk (2002). Hierarchical Linear Models: Applications and Data Analysis Methods. Thousand Oaks, CA, Sage Publications. Rumberger, R. W. and J. D. Willms (1992). "The Impact of Racial and Ethnic Segregation on the Achievement Gap in California High Schools." Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis 14q(4): 377-396. Sewell, W. H., A. O. Haller, et al. (1970). "The Educational and Early Occupational Attainment Process: Replication and Revision." American Sociological Review 35(6): 1014-1027. Sewell, W. H., A. O. Haller, et al. (1969). "The Educational and Early Occupational Attainment Process." American Sociological Review 34: 82-92. Sewell, W. H. and R. M. Hauser (1992). A Review of the Wisconsin Longitudinal Study of Social and Psychological Factors in Aspirations and Achievements 1963-1992, Center for Demography and Ecology University of Wisconsin, Madison. 39 Sewell, W. H. and V. P. Shah (1968). "Social Class, Parental Encouragement, and Educational Aspirations." American Journal of Sociology 73(6): 559-572. St. John, N. H. (1975). School Desegregation Outcomes for Children. New York, John Wiley and Sons. Thrupp, M. (1997). The School Mix Effect: How the Social Class Composition of School Intakes Shapes School Processes and Student Achievement. American Education Research Association, Chicago, IL. Turner, R. A. (1964). The Social Context of Ambition. San Francisco, Chandler. 40 41 Figure 1. School Racial Composition by Individual Student Race 100% 90% 80% 70% 60% Asian Black 50% Hispanic White 40% 30% 20% 10% 0% All Students White Hispanic Black Asian Student Race 42 Figure 2: Average expectation by school, Seniors. 43 Table 1. Hypotheses regarding the association between school racial composition and educational expectations (SRC=School racial composition, EE=Educational Expectations) Substantive Hypothesis Null Between school differences in EE and the impact of SRC on EE derives in part from individual differences in the students attending schools, rather than from any independent school effects. H1 Analytical Hypothesis Results I hypothesize a negative baseline relationship between SRC and EE. By adjusting for student characteristics, including family socioeconomic background, students’ educational experience and ability, significant others’ influence, and race and immigration status, the between school variance in EE will be reduced substantially and the effect of SRC on EE will become less negative. Confirmed: 23% of the between school variance is associated with students’ individual characteristics, and the observed negative relationship between SRC and EE disappears, leaving no net relationship. See Table 4, Model 3. Effect of SRC on EE SRC is correlated with other school characteristics that influence EE. But derives from the adjusting for these factors, the relationship between SRC and EE can be better relationship of SRC to other 1a. School socioeconomic status: Because school SES is negatively related school characteristics that to the SRC and positively related to EE, by adjusting for school SES, the are associated with student effect of SRC on EE becomes more positive. EE, including school SES, 1b. School achievement: Because school achievement is negatively related school achievement, and to both the SRC and EE (frog pond theory), by adjusting for school school academic achievement, the effect of SRC on EE becomes more negative. environment. 1c. Academic environment: Because more academic school environments are negatively related to the SRC and positively related to EE, by adjusting for school academic environment, the effect of SRC on EE becomes more positive. H2 SRC independent influences EE, and might differentially influence black and Hispanic students. However, there is no consensus on the existence or direction of a differential effect. 1a, Confirmed: See Table 5 1b, Refuted: See Table 5. School achievement is positively related to EE, and by adjusting for it, the effect of SRT on EE becomes more positive 1c, Refuted: See Table 5. School environment was not related to EE, and did not mediate the effect between EE and SRC. 2a. Independent effect of school racial composition : even after adjusting for student and school characteristics associated with EE, a statistically significant relationship between SRC and EE remains. 2a, Confirmed. See Table 4, Model 4. 2b. Interaction effect : I do not hypothesize a specific interaction effect between individual student race and SRC, but examine empirical evidence to see what it exists, and if so, what form it takes. 2b. The proportion of black students is positively associated with higher EE for both black and Hispanic students than for similar white students. 44 Table 2. Descriptive Statistics by School Racial Composition* (Standard deviations) Dependent Variable Expect to complete a 4 year college degree School Level Variables Racial/Ethnic Composition Percentage Black students Percentage Hispanic students Percentage White students Socioeconomic Status Percentage qualifying for free/reduced priced lunches Percentage of parents with 4-year college degree School SES factor (two preceding variables factored together) Achievement Percentage students meeting state standards, all tests Academic Environment Feeder high school Average Low Minority Schools High Minority Source of Schools Data 0.666 0.685 0.597 [96] [24] [24] 12.4 (17.8) 32.0 (30.1) 53.1 (31.0) 2.9 (04.1) 7.9 (5.3) 86.9 (6.7) 18.6 (29.2) 72.2 (29.3) 8.0 (6.5) 36.6 (22.1) 34.3 (17.4) 0 (.852) 22.7 (15.3) 43.9 (19.6) 0.542 (.755) 61.0 (15.7) 21.4 (9.8) -0.845 (.520) 51.7 (18.3) 58.2 (19.7) 37.0 (12.8) 0.042 0.0125 0 Survey CCD CCD CCD CCD Survey TEA THEOP project data Number of AP courses taken per senior 0.191 (.199) 29.5 (14.5) 0 (.755) 1305 (1123) 0.211 (.271) 28.7 (20.8) 0.188 (1.30) 804 (1093) 0.145 (.115) 26.8 (7.8) -0.205 (.214) 1818 (977) [12526] [2868] [3019] Black 0.104 0.041 0.146 Survey Hispanic 0.338 0.109 0.788 Survey White 0.518 0.801 0.056 Survey 0.396 0.518 0.196 Survey 0.834 0.876 0.787 Survey Percentage seniors knowing about Top Ten Percent Plan Academic environment factor (three preceding variables factored together) School size Student Level Race/Ethnicity Parental Socioeconomic Status Parent college graduate Parents own home TEA Survey TEA Table 2 continues on next page 45 Table 2 continued Average Low Minority Schools Educational Experiences and Background Enrolled in college prep track 0.633 0.594 0.663 Survey Had early expectations to attend college 0.556 0.614 0.451 Survey GPA 3.17 (.017) 0.434 3.3 (.036) 0.466 3.01 (.034) 0.391 Survey 2.7 (.011) 1.07 (.054) 2.64 (.020) 1.16 (.124) 2.85 (.017) 1.08 (.085) Survey 0.937 0.946 0.927 Survey 0.86 0.859 0.892 Survey 0.043 0.011 0.138 Survey 0.12 0.088 0.209 Survey Know about Top 10% Law Attitudes towards education Number of AP courses Significant Others' Influence Parent encourage college attendance Teacher encourage college attendance Immigration and Language Speak language other than English with friends Foreign-born High Minority Source of Schools Data Survey Survey *Appropriate school variabls are measured as percentages to ease interpretation at the multivariate stage of analysis and are labeled as such in this table. Other variables are measured as proportions. Student level statistics are weighted Low minority schools are schools in the first quartile of the distribution of combined black and hispanic students High minority schools are schools in the last quartile of the distribution of combined black and hispanic students 46 Table 3: Factor loadings and Cronbach's Alpha Description of Variable School Socioeconomic Status Proportion of parents with college degree Proportion of students qualifying for free/reduced price Factor Loading α 0.792 0.792 0.828 School Academic Climate Feeder High School Proportion of students knowing about Top Ten Percent Plan Number of AP courses taken per senior student 0.680 0.567 0.555 0.669 47 Table 4. Hierarchical Logistic Regressions of College Expectations (Log Odds) (Laplace Estimation) Intercept School Level Percentage Black Students Percentage Hispanic Students 1 0.637 *** (0.071) 2 3 4 0.647 *** 0.778 *** 0.847 *** (0.072) (0.082) (0.051) 5 0.771 *** (0.065) -0.005 (0.006) -0.005 * (0.002) 0.010 (0.004) 0.019 (0.004) 0.592 (0.137) 0.015 (0.005) -0.155 (0.129) 0.012 (0.006) 0.003 (0.005) 0.000 (0.003) 0.012 (0.004) 0.008 (0.003) 0.295 (0.117) 0.009 (0.004) -0.088 (0.110) 0.031 (0.006) School socioeconomic status factor Percentage students meeting state standards, all tests Academic climate factor School size ** * * * *** Interaction Terms Percentage Black*Black Percentage Hispanic*Black Percentage Hispanic*Hispanic Hispanic Asian Parental Socioeconomic Status Parent College Home ownership Educational Experiences and Background Enrolled in college prep track Had early expectations to attend college GPA Know about Top 10% Law Attitudes towards education Number of AP courses Significant Others' Influence Parent encourage college attendance Teacher encourage college attendance Immigration and Language Speak language other than English with friends Foreign born *** *** *** 0.020 ** (0.006) 0.020 *** (0.006) 0.005 (0.004) -0.006 (0.004) Percentage Black*Hispanic Individual Level Black * 0.062 (0.068) -0.137 (0.080) -0.101 (0.119) -0.011 (0.073) -0.039 (0.086) -0.412 *** (0.122) -0.083 (0.102) -0.122 (0.096) -0.020 (0.164) 0.644 *** 0.661 *** (0.070) (0.081) 0.258 ** 0.274 *** (0.091) (0.082) 0.625 *** (0.078) 0.320 ** (0.105) 0.596 (0.066) 0.860 (0.065) 0.508 (0.047) 0.751 (0.064) 0.421 (0.051) 0.175 (0.022) 0.553 (0.076) 0.821 (0.069) 0.531 (0.051) 0.680 (0.066) 0.498 (0.060) 0.189 (0.024) *** *** *** *** *** *** 0.608 (0.068) 0.903 (0.064) 0.557 (0.051) 0.657 (0.062) 0.459 (0.056) 0.187 (0.023) *** *** *** *** *** *** *** *** *** *** *** *** 0.831 *** 0.818 *** (0.118) (0.117) 0.318 *** 0.314 *** (0.072) (0.076) 0.854 *** (0.121) 0.320 *** (0.077) -0.392 *** -0.556 ** (0.096) (0.171) -0.435 ** -0.431 *** (0.145) (0.097) -0.656 ** (0.186) -0.392 *** (0.099) τ00 0.341 0.304 0.233 0.083 0.067 0.064 0.059 (0.069) (0.019) (0.024) SOURCE: 2002 Texas Higher Educational Opportunity Study, 12526 students clustered in 96 schools *** p<.001; **p<.01; * p<.05 All variables are grand mean centered 48 Table 5. Hierarchical Logistic Regressions of College Expectations on School Characteristics1 (Log Odds) Intercept Model 3, Table 1 0.778 *** (0.082) + School +School +Academic SES Achievement Environment 0.828 *** 0.873 *** 0.849 *** (0.050) (0.054) (0.053) Racial Composition Percentage Black Students Percentage Hispanic Students 0.003 (0.005) 0.000 (0.003) 0.009 * (0.004) 0.008 * (0.003) 0.009 ** (0.003) 0.002 (0.002) 0.003 (0.006) -0.002 (0.002) Socioeconomic Status School socioeconomic status factor 0.406 *** (0.117) Achievement Percentage students meeting state standards, all tests Academic Environment Academic climate factor School Size 0.011 ** (0.004) 0.104 (0.091) 0.039 *** (0.006) 0.120 (0.026) 0.028 *** 0.036 *** (0.006) (0.005) τ00 0.233 0.106 0.114 (0.069) (0.026) (0.023) 1 Models here include all controls for students' individual characteristics included in Table 4. *** p<.001; **p<.01; * p<.05 SOURCE: 2002 Texas Higher Educational Opportunity Study, 12526 students clustered in 96 schools All variables are grand mean centered 49