J Am Acad Audiol 2 :91-98 (1991) Perceptual Features for Normal Listeners' Phoneme Recognition in a Reverberant Lecture Hall Jeffrey L. Danhauer* Carole E . Johnsont Abstract Johnson et al (1988) found that normal-hearing listeners' consonant recognition scores on a nonsense syllable test were significantly better when they were seated within versus beyond the critical distance of a typical lecture hall . The present study used a Sequential Information Analysis (SINFA) to document a priori perceptual features for Johnson et al's subjects' confusion errors to the initial and medial consonant portions of the stimuli . The features and the percent of information transmitted for them differed as a function of consonant position in the stimulus and whether the subjects were seated within or beyond the critical distance of the room . Generally, more information was transmitted within rather than beyond the critical distance for both initial and medial consonants . However, less information was transmitted for features for the medial than for the initial consonant position . Beyond the critical distance, high-frequency features (e .g ., sibilant) were transmitted somewhat better than those based more on low-frequency energy (e .g ., voicing) . Place of articulation features were not transmitted as well for the initial as for the medial position and they suffered in both positions as distance increased. The effects of reverberation on the subjects' perception are discussed and compared to results of other studies. Key Words: Consonant recognition, critical distance, information transmission, lecture hall, normal-hearing, perceptual features, reverberation his study evaluated perceptual features for normal-hearing listeners' consonant T recognition in a typical reverberant lecture hall. Several studies (e.g., Finitzo-Hieber and Tillman, 1978 ; Gelfand and Silman, 1979 ; Nabelek and Mason, 1981) have found that reverberation adversely affects listeners' speech recognition abilities. These studies have also found that the longer the reverberation time of a room, the more adverse are the effects on speech understanding and that consonants in 'Department of Speech and Hearing Sciences, University of California Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, California tDepartment of Communicative Disorders, University of Oklahoma Health Sciences Center, Oklahoma City, Oklahoma Reprint requests : Jeffrey L . Danhauer, Department of Speech and Hearing Sciences, Snidecor Hall, University of California Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, CA 93106 the initial position of words are less affected by reverberation than those appearing in the middle or at the end of words. The effects of reverberation also depend on speaker-to-listener distance. Peutz (1971) investigated the effects of loudspeaker-to-listener distance on the phoneme recognition abilities of normal-hearing listeners in rooms differing in volume and reverberation time. He found that while speech understanding was essentially unaffected in front of a loudspeaker, it gradually decreased with increasing distance from the source up to the critical distance (point where direct and reflected energy is equal) of the room. However, beyond the critical distance, speech recognition remained essentially unchanged with further increases in loudspeaker-to-listener distance. Johnson et al, (1988) evaluated normal-hearing listeners' nonsense syllable test (Edgerton Journal of the American Academy of Audiology/Volume 2, Number 2, April 1991 and Danhauer, 1979) scores in a reverberant lecture hall having a reverberation time of 1 .9 s. They found that vowel scores (M = 89.9%) were not significantly affected by distance. However, consonant scores (M = 61 .8%) were significantly better (68% correct) within the critical distance of the room than at two distances (59.5% correct) that were beyond the critical distance. The scores beyond the critical distance were not significantly different from each other. The findings of Peutz (1971) and Johnson et al (1988) demonstrated that beyond the critical distance, the full effect of masking by reverberation occurs and remains constant independent of loudspeaker-to-listener distance . However, even within the critical distance, consonant recognition was difficult for normal-hearing listeners. Edgerton and Danhauer (1979) reported that the mean correct consonant score on these stimuli for normal-hearing subjects (listening diotically under earphones at 55 dB sensation level re SRT) was only 85 .8 percent (SD = 10 .1). An analysis of perceptual features from subjects' consonant confusion errors (Johnson et al, 1988) may provide insight as to why their consonant recognition performance was poor in these conditions . Related studies (Miller and Nicely, 1955 ; Wang and Bilger, 1973 ; Gelfand and Silman, 1979) have used information transmission analyses to evaluate sets of a priori perceptual features for normal-hearing subjects' consonant confusions in filtering, noise, and/or reverberation. Other studies (e.g., Doyle et al, 1981 ; Danhauer et al, 1986) have used a multidimensional scaling procedure to analyze a posteriori features for normal-hearing listeners' initial and medial consonant confusion errors to nonsense syllable stimuli. There is a major difference between the a posteriori and the a priori types of analyses. In the a posteriori methods, a set of features is derived from the data via empirically predicted relationships among the stimulus items. However, considerable interpretation of the results is usually required on the part of the experimenters. The a priori methods determine the subjects' levels of performance (amount of transmitted information) for a set of predetermined features, which the a posteriori methods cannot do. Thus, the a posteriori methods are useful in arriving at a set of features for a particular body of data when the features for it are not known in advance. However, the a priori methods are more useful in determining subjects' performance on specific features once the set is known. We may assume that the set of features has been identified for the stimuli used in this study (e .g., Doyle et al, 1981), and that it is appropriate to use the a priori method to evaluate the effects of reverberation on these normal-hearing listeners' perception of certain features . Thus, the present study analyzed subjects' consonant confusion errors (Johnson et al, 1988) using an a priori. method to determine the effects of loudspeaker-to-listener distance (i .e ., within versus beyond the critical distance of the room) on their perceptual features for initial and medial consonants . METHOD Subjects The subjects were 12 women between 19 and 35 (M = 25) years of age. They were all speech and hearing majors in college and had normal hearing (pure-tone thresholds of 15 dB HTL or better for the octave frequencies between 250 and 8000 Hz and no history of hearing problems). Stimuli The stimulus was an Auditec of St. Louis audio cassette tape recording of List A of the Edgerton and Danhauer (1979) Nonsense Syllable 'Ibst (NST) as shown in Figure 1. The NST is a 25-item consonant-vowel-consonantvowel (CVCV) test of phoneme recognition. Six randomizations of List A are available on the tape. Edgerton et al (1981) found no practice effects for the NST when up to seven administrations of List A were presented to normal-hearing listeners in a single test session. The NST items are presented in an open-response format following the carrier word "say." The subject's task is to repeat what is heard while the examiner phonemically transcribes any errors to the stimuli on special response forms. There are 50 consonants (25 initial and 25 medial) and 50 vowels (25 medial and 25 final) on NST List A. Test Environment Each subject was tested individually in a college lecture hall that contained furniture, the Reverberant Room Features/Danhauer and Johnson FORM A 1 a~j o 14 C) 15 2 QI 3 b~ ,~O 4 fLJel 16 17 5 fIe~0 18 6 f*1Sa2 19 7 VUVI 20 s d3 afe b uma 10 S 11 12 JE feed6 T [~e 13 bntI 21 22 23 ba,na baebae bUmas f i t fa Pnd U tfo e a-0Pits d3AbU eA f I habo 24 daea 25 ni9U subject, experimenters, and equipment. The room had a volume (V) of 415 m3 and a reverberation time (T) of 1 .9 s. Using a sound absorption value of 0.2, the critical distance (CD) of the room was calculated to be 3 m according to Peutz's (1971) equation : CD = 0.2 (V/T). Subjects were tested at three loudspeaker-tolistener distances : at 1 m (within the critical distance of the room) and at 4 m and at 8 m (both beyond the critical distance). The reverberation time for the room was measured by firing a cap pistol in one corner of the room and recording it with a high quality reel-to-reel tape recorder (Sony APR-2003) positioned in the opposite diagonal corner. The output of the tape recorder was connected to a one-third octave band filter that was connected to a graphic level recorder (Bruel & Kjaer 2203). Decay of the impulse noise was measured as a function of time using a reverberation time protractor (Bruel & Kjaer SC2361) at a paper speed of 100 mm/s . Reverberation times for the onethird octave bands centered at 250, 500, 1000, 2000, 4000, and 8000 Hz were 1 .9 s, 2.0 s, 1 .9 s, 1.8 s, 1.4 s, and .9 s, respectively. The average reverberation time (500, 1000, and 2000 Hz onethird octave bands) was 1 .9 s. The NST stimuli were presented from a tape player through a loudspeaker to the subject who sat in a desk facing (at zero degrees azimuth) the loudspeaker. The stimuli peaked at about 75 dB A on a sound level meter at the 1 m distance and at about 71 dB A at the 4 m and 8 m distances. The ambient noise in the room peaked at about 54 dB A. Thus, the signal-to-noise ratio (S/N) ranged from about +21 to +17 dB . Danhauer, Doyle, and Lucks (1986) found no significant difference between +20 and +10 dB S/Ns for normal listeners' performance on the NST. Thus, the difference for the within versus beyond the critical distance conditions should not have been a factor in this study. Measurements were also made of ambient noise levels for octave-band filters with center frequencies from 250 to 8000 Hz using a precision sound level meter (Bruel & Kjaer 2203) and a slow meter setting. Measurements made at all three test locations were within ±1 dB of each other. Ambient noise measurements (i.e ., without the NST signal present) were: 58 dB (250 Hz), 55 dB (500 Hz), 47 dB (1000 Hz), 40 dB (2000 Hz), 35 dB (4000 Hz), and 29 .5 dB (8000 Hz). Measurements made with the NST signal present for the 4 m and 8 m locations were the same: 68 dB (250 Hz), 74 dB (500 Hz), 66 dB (1000 Hz), 56 dB (2000 Hz), 42 dB (4000 Hz), and 29 dB (8000 Hz). Thus, the resulting S/Ns of +10 dB (250 Hz), +19 dB (500 Hz), +19 dB (1000 Hz), +16 dB (2000 Hz), +7 dB (4000 Hz), and -0 .5 dB (8000 Hz) were most favorable in the lower frequency bands . Procedure The subjects were told that they would hear nonsense syllables, to repeat them aloud, and guess if unsure of a response . Each subject received one randomized form (from the six available) of NST List A at each of the three distances. The randomizations and distances were counterbalanced across subjects . The entire procedure required approximately 20 min per subject. Two experimenters experienced with the NST and phoneme transcription sat 2 m from and facing perpendicular to the listener. One experimenter sat on each side of the subject so that visual cues were available to them in tran- Journal of the American Academy of Audiology/Volume 2, Number 2, April 1991 scribing responses. The 2 m subject-toexperimenter distance was selected so that the experimenters would be within the critical distance of the room to see and hear the subject clearly, but not be so close that they would interfere with the room acoustics. The subjects all repeated their responses so that they were loud and clear to the experimenters. Also, the use of visual cues by the experimenters in transcribing the subjects' responses should have compensated for any problems due to subjectto-experimenter distance. Both experimenters were also present in the room while all the reverberation measurements were made to account for their presence during the test sessions . Thus, because the experimenters were present at all three test locations, their effects on room acoustics should not have been a factor in the subjects' performance at the three distances. The experimenters independently phonemically transcribed each subject's responses to the stimuli. Inter-judge reliability was 96 percent. Phoneme scoring was used in which each consonant or vowel recognized correctly in each of the 25 CVCV items was awarded 1 percent toward a possible 100 percent score. Separate consonant scores and vowel scores were also derived by awarding 2 percent for each consonant or vowel component of each item recognized correctly. Data Preparation The subjects' consonant recognition scores were significantly better when they were within (1 m) versus beyond (4 m and 8 m) the critical Table 1 distance for the room . However, the scores for the 4 m and 8 m locations were not significantly different from each other. Therefore, the subjects' errors were converted to two separate confusion matrices (one for the 1 m location within and one pooling both the 4 m and 8 m locations beyond the critical distance). Further, because the stimuli comprising the initial and medial consonants of the NST differ, separate confusion matrices were also constructed for each consonant position . Inspection of each subject's confusion matrices produced no apparent individual differences across the subjects. Thus, all the subjects' data were pooled for each of the conditions noted above. This resulted in 300 observations per matrix (25 consonants X 12 subjects X 1 distance, 1 m) for the within condition and 600 (25 consonants X 12 subjects X 2 distances, 4 m and 8 m) for the beyond, which are fewer than those used in some other studies (e.g., Miller and Nicely, 1955 ; Wang and Bilger, 1973 ; Gelfand and Silman, 1979). The matrices are not provided here, but are available from the authors. The subjects' consonant confusion matrices were submitted to a Sequential Information Analysis (SINFA) program (Wang and Bilger, 1973) to determine the amounts of information transmitted for selected a priori perceptual features. SINFA performed a multivariate uncertainty analysis on the confusion matrices . The features used in this analysis included those derived from earlier a posteriori analyses of other normal-hearing listeners' responses to consonants of the NST (Danhauer et al, 1986 ; Doyle et al, 1981) . The features used in the present analyses are shown in Table 1 for the initial and the medial consonants . Features Used in the SINFA for the Initial and Medial Consonants Medial Consonants Initial Consonants Feature Uncond. Info. ' Feature Uncond . Info . Stop 943 Dental Affricative .971 529 971 904 401 528 794 942 401 Fricative Front/back Voiceless-stop Nasal Sonorant Dental Sibilant Fricative Front/back Voicing Nasal Sibilant Voicing 242 855 971 'Unconditional information is shown in bits . 903 528 999 Reverberant Room Features/Danhauer and Johnson Table 2 Summary of SINFA for Initial Consonants Beyond Critical Distance Within Critical Distance Feature Ranks Info . Transmitted (In %) (In Bits) Sibilant Affricative Voicing Stop Dental Front/back Nasal Fricative Tot. bits info . tra ns . For matrix = Obtained = Prop , info . trans. Info . Transmitted (In Bits) (In %) Feature Ranks 829 97 631 88 Sibilant Affricative Nasal 59 53 Voicing Front/back 478 .171 Stop 147 254 491 191 161 037 .037 2 .904 2 .566 92 76 76 53 Fricative Dental 793 206 106 93 75 73 197 26 131 67 42 26 25 2 .323 1 .902 88 RESULTS B riefly, the SINFA program used in the present analyses first estimated the unconditional transmitted information for each feature in bits. As seen in Table 1, the stimulus information for each feature varied considerably across the features . The percentage of information transmitted for each feature is also shown and was used to normalize the features for inequalities in stimulus feature information. Thus, the subjects' relative performance on the features is presented as the percentage of information transmitted for each feature. The program then partialed out the effects of one feature on another by completing a series of iterations in which the percent of conditional information transmitted for each of the features was determined. The feature identified as having the highest performance on iteration 82 one (i .e ., the unconditional transmitted information discussed above) was partialed out for iteration two and the conditional information for the remaining features was determined independent of that for the feature identified in iteration one. That feature identified as having the highest performance in iteration two was then also partialed out for iteration three, and so on . This process continued to partial out, in each subsequent iteration, the effects of features identified in previous iterations until all the features were accounted for. For a complete discussion of the SINFA procedure, see Wang and Bilger (1973) . The results of the SINFA are summarized for each distance in Tables 2 and 3 for the initial and medial consonants, respectively. The tables show a ranking of the features in bits and according to the percent of information transmitted for each. The tables also show the total information transmitted for each matrix and the Table 3 Summary of SINFA for Medial Consonants Beyond Critical Distance Within Critical Distance Feature Ranks Info. Transmitted (In Bits) (In %) Feature Ranks Info . Transmitted (In Bits) (In %) Voicing Voiceless-stop Front/back Nasal 928 .282 347 320 121 93 91 88 86 65 Voiceless-stop Sibilant Front/back Nasal 313 388 319 090 89 88 76 61 111 111 65 65 Dental Fricative Voicing 243 243 113 40 40 26 Sibilant Sonorant Dental Fricative Tot. bits info . trans . For matrix = Obtained = Prop . Info . trans . = 111 2 .443 2 .157 65 88 Sonorant 243 2 .165 1 .566 40 72 Journal of the American Academy of Audiology/Volume 2, Number 2, April 1991 amount of information obtained in bits, and the proportion of information transmitted in percent . The results on all three categories were better when the subjects were within rather than beyond the critical distance . This was true for both initial and medial consonants, although the scores for the medial consonants were somewhat lower. Sibilant and affricative features were transmitted well at both distances for the initial consonants. Interestingly, sibilant was only transmitted well beyond the critical distance for the medial consonants. Voicing was transmitted much better within the critical distance than beyond it for both consonant positions. With the exception of front/back in the medial position, features relating to place of articulation were not transmitted very well in either position for either distance . Tables 2 and 3 show that over 50 percent of the information was transmitted for each of the features analyzed for the initial and medial consonants when presented within the critical distance. Considerably less information was transmitted for most of the features when presented beyond the critical distance. Thus, the alterations in the rankings of the features noted across distances and consonant positions relate to the reverberant characteristics of this particular room and to whether or not a vowel preceded the consonant in the stimulus . DISCUSSION Effects of Loudspeaker-to-Listener Distances and Noise on Features The rankings and percentages of information transmitted for most of the features in this analysis were consistent with the results of Gelfand and Silman (1979) who evaluated normalhearing listeners' recognition of consonants in monosyllabic words recorded in a small room having a 0.8 s reverberation time . For example, the sibilant feature (characterized by highfrequency energy and present in the phonemes /s, z, J , ,t J , d3 /) was transmitted well in all conditions . Gelfand and Silman (1979) found that the information transmitted for sibilant was barely affected by reverberation. Our sibilant feature is also similar to Miller and Nicely's (1955) duration feature that included the phonemes /s, z, (, 3 /. Their duration feature was salient for their normal-hearing lis- teners only in relatively quiet conditions or when the stimuli were presented through highpass filters; it was perceived poorly in conditions of noise or low-pass filtering. However, Miller and Nicely (1955) used much poorer S/Ns and their white noise probably had different spectral characteristics than the ambient noise in our study. While white noise has equal intensity at all frequencies, the spectrum of the ambient noise in our lecture hall had greater amplitude in the lower frequencies than in the higher frequencies . Our results for affricative (Its and d3 /), another high-frequency feature that is somewhat redundant with the sibilant feature, showed that it was transmitted well at both distances for the initial consonants . This feature was not analyzed for the medial consonants because /v and d3 / do not occur in that position of the NST stimuli. Inspection of Gelfand and Silman's (1979) matrices also reveals that all of their subjects always perceived /tV' and d3 / correctly whether in quiet or under reverberation and regardless of consonant position in the stimulus. Although affricative only ranked third overall of the five features analyzed by Miller and Nicely (1955), they included the fricative phonemes in their affricative category. Interestingly, the fricative feature was also not transmitted well in any conditions of the present study nor in that of Gelfand and Silman. Thus, the results of these studies are in fair agreement. These findings probably relate to the lowfrequency reverberant characteristics of our particular lecture hall that may have masked many of the cues available to the listeners when they were situated beyond the critical distance. The 1.9 s reverberation time for this room was considerably longer than that for typical living rooms or small offices having reverberation times between 0.4 and 0.8 s (Nabelek and Nabelek, 1985). Normal-hearing listeners can tolerate up to about 1 .2 s of reverberation in favorable S/Ns without speech recognition being adversely affected (Nabelek, 1980). Our 1 .9 s reverberation time is similar to that found in small auditoriums and churches (Nabelek and Nabelek, 1985). The 1.9 s time also falls between two (1 .6 s and 2.3 s) of the four times that Moncur and Dirks (1967) used to evaluate the effects of reverberation on binaural speech intelligibility. They considered these times to be relatively long and they produced word recognition scores between approximately 67 and 60 percent, respectively. Reverberant Room Features/Danhauer and Johnson Thus, as would be predicted by Moncur and Dirks (1967), our subjects had difficulty perceiving the stimuli within and beyond the critical distance of the lecture hall. This was reflected both in their percent correct scores and in the information transmitted by the perceptual features. Features characterized mainly by highfrequency energy (e.g., sibilant and affricative) were less affected by the predominantly lowfrequency reverberant noise in the room than were features that are more low-frequency based (e.g., voicing and nasality) . For example, voicing was transmitted better within than beyond the critical distance. The finding that voicing, sonorant, and nasal were not transmitted as well as some of the other features was somewhat unexpected based on Miller and Nicely's (1955) data that showed that these features were among the most salient in their various noise and filtering conditions. Features relating to place of articulation were transmitted somewhat better than expected from Gelfand and Silman's (1979) results which showed that place was degraded more than any other feature in reverberation. Our place features were generally perceived better within than beyond the critical distance. This follows from Miller and Nicely (1955) who noted that those sounds that are easiest to see are often hardest to hear. Initial Versus Medial Feature Differences Some interesting differences were observed among the features for the two consonant positions of the NST They employ different stimulus sets, and the medial consonants are preceded by a vowel while the initial consonants are not. Generally, the features for the initial consonants had higher percentages of information transmitted than those for the medial consonants . As seen in Tables 2 and 3, the total information transmitted in bits was also greater for the initial (2 .566 within and 1.902 beyond the critical distance) than for the medial (2 .157 within and 1.566 beyond the critical distance) consonants. Further, the proportion of information transmitted for the confusion matrices was: (a) initial consonants within the critical distance, 88 percent ; (b) initial-beyond, 82 percent; (c) medialwithin, 88 percent ; and (d) medial-beyond, 72 percent. These results are in fair agreement with the subjects' mean percent recognition scores and seem to indicate that the initial consonants were less affected by the reverberation of the room than those in the medial position. These results are in agreement with those of Gelfand and Silman (1979) and Nabelek and Mason (1981) who found that initial consonants were less affected by reverberation than final consonants . Nabelek and Nabelek (1985) noted that reverberation causes sounds to overlap one another such that the prolonged acoustic energy of vowels masks consonants following vowels more than those preceding vowels . Thus, our results make sense considering that the medial consonants of the CVCV stimuli could have been masked by the prolongation of the preceding vowel due to reverberation, while the initial consonants were not influenced by a preceding vowel. Our findings regarding the importance of certain features relative to the position of the consonant within the nonsense syllable are also in agreement with those of Wang and Bilger (1973) . They tested listeners with CUs and VCs in quiet and at six S/Ns from -10 dB to +15 dB at 5 dB intervals. Wang and Bilger found that voicing, fricative, sibilant, duration, and place (features from their study that were common to ours) were all perceived better in CUs than in VCs in noise. They found that voicing was transmitted best in noise (at all S/Ns except +15 dB), but as the S/N improved, the differences in the information transmitted by the features in each position began to diminish. The differences were negligible in quiet, except that the importance of duration, sibilant, fricative, and place switched (i .e., they had more information transmitted in the VC position than in the CV). Unfortunately, both reverberation and noise are present in real classroom listening situations and interact synergistically to degrade speech recognition to a degree that can not be predicted by summing the effects of each factor separately (Finitzo-Hieber and Tillman, 1978 ; Nabelek and Mason, 1981). Another difference noted between the results for the two consonant positions related to the relative order of the feature rankings according to the percentage of information transmitted. The order was fairly well maintained for the initial consonants at both distances (sibilant, affricative, and voicing were among the first four features for each distance). However, there were several alterations in the feature rankings for the medial consonants across the two Journal of the American Academy of Audiology/Volume 2, Number 2, April 1991 distances. For example, voicing (93%) and sibilant (65%) were ranked first and fifth, respectively within the critical distance and last (voicing 26%) and first (sibilant 88%), respectively beyond the critical distance. For the medial consonants, the voicelessstop feature (which only includes two phonemes, /t and tf / and is also characterized by highfrequency energy) was perceived well at both distances suggesting again that the room acoustics had less effect on high-frequency sounds than on low-frequency sounds. As noted earlier, the S/N was more favorable in the lower than in the higher octave bands and the reverberation time was less favorable for the lower frequency one-third octave bands. Thus, because the lowfrequency features were more affected by reverberation than the higher-frequency features, it is doubtful that the S/N caused the reduction in the percentage of information transmitted for medial consonant features beyond the critical distance. SUMMARY T his study offers insight about the subjects' perception of the consonants of the NST when presented in a reverberant lecture hall. These results go beyond the information provided by the simple percent correct consonant recognition scores that showed that the subjects' performance was poorer when they were situated beyond rather than within the critical distance of the room, but failed to demonstrate how they were poorer. This analysis of the subjects' error data determined their performance for each of the perceptual features investigated . Generally, the subjects performed better within than beyond the critical distance of the room, but alterations were noted for the salience of certain features depending on whether the consonants were in the initial or medial position of the stimuli. The fact that high-frequency features (e.g., sibilant) were transmitted somewhat better than those based more on low-frequency energy (e.g., voicing), suggests that the reverberant characteristics of the room affected the subjects' perception of the features. These results help show why preferential seating aids the perception of certain aspects of a stimulus and not others . Our results are in agreement with other studies that have used more observations and different stimuli. Further research is war- ranted to substantiate the stability of our findings with a larger number of observations . Acknowledgments. Part of this research was supported by a University of California General Research Grant (!l 8587529-19900-7) . REFERENCES Danhauer JL, Abdala C, Johnson CE, Asp CW (1986). Perceptual features from normal-hearing and hearingimpaired children's errors on the NST Ear Hear 7:318-322 . Danhauer JL, Doyle PC, Lucks LE . (1986) . Effects of signal-to-noise ratio on the Nonsense Syllable Tbst. Ear Hear 7 :323-324 . Doyle KJ, Danhauer JL, Edgerton BJ . (1981) . Features from normal and sensorineural listeners' nonsense syllable test errors. Ear Hear 2:117-121 . Edgerton BJ, Danhauer JL . (1979). Clinical Implications of Speech Discrimination Testing Using Nonsense Stimuli. Baltimore: University Park Press. Edgerton BJ, Danhauer JL, Rizzo S. (1981). Practice effects for normal listeners' performance on a nonsense syllable test. J Aud Res 21 :125-131 . Finitzo-Hieber T, Tillman TW (1978) . Room acoustics effects on monosyllabic word discrimination ability for normal and hearing-impaired children . J Speech Hear Res 21 :440-458 . Gelfand SA, Silman S. (1979) . Effects of small room reverberation upon the recognition of some consonant features. J Acoust Soc Am 66 :22-29 . Johnson CE, Nabelek AK, Asp CW, Danhauer JL. (1988) . Loudspeaker- to-listener distance and phoneme recognition in a reverberant lecture hall. Paper presented at the Annual Convention of the American Speech-LanguageHearing Association, Boston. Miller G, Nicely PE . (1955) . An analysis of perceptual confusions among English consonants . JAcoust Soc Am 27:338-352 . Moncur JP, Dirks D. (1967). Binaural and monaural speech intelligibility in reverberation . JSpeech Hear Res 10:186-195. Nabelek AK . (1980) . Effects of room acoustics on speech perception through hearing aids . In: Libby R, ed . Binaural Hearing and Amplification. Chicago: Zenetron Inc. Nabelek AK, Mason D . (1981) . Effect of noise and reverberation on binaural and monaural word identification by subjects with various audiograms . JSpeech HearRes 24:375-383 . Nabelek AK, Nabelek IV (1985). Room acoustics and speech perception . In : Katz J, ed . Clinical Audiology. 3rd ed. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins, 834-846. Peutz VMA . (1971). Articulation loss of consonants as a criterion for speech transmission in a room . J Audio Eng Soc 19 :915-919 . Wang MD, Bilger RC. (1973) . Consonant confusions in noise: a study of perceptual features. J Acoust Soc Am 54:1248-1266.