10.1177/1077727X03255900 FAMILY Jung, Lennon AND/CONSUMER BODY IMAGE SCIENCES AND MEDIA RESEARCH IMAGES JOURNAL ARTICLE Body Image, Appearance Self-Schema, and Media Images Jaehee Jung University of Delaware Sharron J. Lennon Ohio State University This study examines the effects of women’s appearance self-schemas and exposure to attractive media images on body image, self-esteem, and mood. Women’s response to media images was expected to vary according to an individual difference variable, appearance self-schema, or cognitive representations of organized information about the self in relation to appearance. College women volunteers (N = 168) were divided into two groups (schematic and aschematic) on the basis of appearance self-schema; half of each group was exposed to photos of attractive images, whereas the other half was not exposed to any images before completing measures of body image, self-esteem, and mood. Women who are schematic on appearance exhibited lower body image, lower self-esteem, and more negative mood than did those who are aschematic on appearance. Neither exposure to media images in conjunction with appearance self-schema nor exposure alone affected response on body image, self-esteem, and mood. Keywords: appearance self-schema; body image; mood; self-esteem; media images Body image is a mental image of one’s body (Garner & Garfinkel, 1981) and simply refers to how individuals perceive their own bodies. Because society places a greater emphasis on the appearance of women than that of men in establishing their identities, women are often evaluated by their physical attractiveness (Locher, Unger, Sociedade, & Wahl, 1993; Timko, Striegel-Moore, Silberstein, & Rodin, 1987) rather than by their abilities or achievements (Drogosz & Levy, 1996). This emphasis is evidenced by heightened interest in diet industries and the increased use of advanced medical technologies to improve physical appearance. Thus, researchers have documented women’s dissatisfaction with their bodies (Cash & Henry, 1995; Rodin, Silberstein, & Striegel-Moore, 1985) and an increasing Family and Consumer Sciences Research Journal, Vol. 32, No. 1, September 2003 27-51 DOI: 10.1177/1077727X03255900 © 2003 American Association of Family and Consumer Sciences 27 28 FAMILY AND CONSUMER SCIENCES RESEARCH JOURNAL tendency among women to diet (Mellin, Irwin, & Scully, 1992; Polivy, Garner, & Garfinkel, 1986; Rosen, 1990). In contemporary U.S. society, media images reflect cultural standards and emphasize unrealistically thin body shapes and attractive physical characteristics. Because most women are not granted the ideal body size and shape from nature, approximating the cultural ideal of female beauty is difficult (Brownell, 1991). If women consider appearance important in self-perceptions and perceptions about self made by others, their body images may be affected by failure to measure up to the cultural ideal. Fallon (1990) has articulated a social-cultural explanation of body image that suggests that social and cultural factors (e.g., media images) have a powerful influence on the development and maintenance of body image. However, there have been mixed results for the effects of media images on body image and selfesteem, some possibly due to methodological differences. In Richins’s (1991) study, female college students exhibited lower satisfaction with their own physical attractiveness when they were exposed to advertisements containing idealized images. Thornton and Moore (1993) found similar results for the effects of media images on physical attractiveness and social anxiety but not on self-esteem. On the other hand, Cusumano and Thompson (1997) found no relationship between simple exposure and indices of body image, eating dysfunction, and self-esteem. Although sociocultural messages about women’s bodies typically glamorize an unrealistically slender body, only some women are adversely affected by those messages. Some women are satisfied with their bodies even when they deviate from the ideal (Rosen, 1996), some women are more susceptible to media images than are others (e.g., depressed women or bulimic women) (Mori & Morey, 1991; Peterson, 1987), and some women are dissatisfied with their bodies even when they do approximate the ideal (Hesse-Biber, 1996). Thus, any explanation of body image must accommodate the heterogeneity of body image experiences. A few studies identified “self-schema” (Markus, 1977) or cognitive representations of organized information about the self as an individual difference variable that may moderate the relationships between sociocultural pressure and eating disorders (Vitousek & Hollon, 1990) and between self-concept and eating disorders (Stein, 1996). Selfschemas are available in a variety of domains such as independence (Markus, 1977), masculinity (Markus, Smith, & Moreland, 1985), and body weight (Markus, Hamill, & Sentis, 1987). For example, Jung, Lennon / BODY IMAGE AND MEDIA IMAGES 29 masculine schematics were those individuals who rated themselves as extreme on most trait adjectives concerning masculinity (e.g., aggressive, dominant, and acts as a leader) and who indicated that those characteristics were important to their self-evaluation (Markus et al., 1985). Similarly, when individuals are highly invested in their appearance (i.e., appearance is important, self-relevant, and central to the individuals) and are actively concerned with it, they are said to be appearance schematic or schematic on appearance. On the other hand, when appearance is not important, self-relevant, and central to the individuals, they are said to be appearance aschematic or aschematic on appearance. Cash and Labarge (1996) found that college women who are schematic on appearance are more self-conscious in public and more socially anxious, express more symptoms of depression and eating disorders, and have poorer social selfesteem than those who are aschematic on appearance. Presumably, women’s responses on measures of body image (e.g., body satisfaction or dissatisfaction, appearance satisfaction or dissatisfaction, appearance evaluation, and appearance orientation) can be predicted by whether they are schematic or aschematic with respect to appearance. Also, the extent to which media images affect women’s body image, self-esteem, and mood may depend on whether they are schematic or aschematic on appearance. Therefore, the purpose of this research was to examine the effect of appearance self-schema and exposure to attractive media images on body image, self-esteem, and mood. THE RELATED LITERATURE Body Image, Self-Esteem, and Mood Many researchers have found that body image (Jackson, Sullivan, & Rostker, 1988; Lennon, Rudd, Sloan, & Kim, 1999) or body satisfaction (Lerner, Orlos, & Knapp, 1976; Simmons & Rosenberg, 1975) is positively related to self-esteem. For example, Jackson et al. (1988) found that high self-esteem in women explained favorable evaluations of appearance and more appearance-directed behaviors, both of which are measures of body image. Lower self-esteem, on the other hand, was associated with abnormal eating behavior (Button, Loan, Davies, & Sonuga-Barke, 1997). Although most of the previously mentioned studies focused on European Americans, the same results 30 FAMILY AND CONSUMER SCIENCES RESEARCH JOURNAL have been found with African American women (Akan & Grilo, 1994; Thomas, 1989), Asian women (Lee & Lee, 1996), and Asian American women (Akan & Grilo, 1994). Because a positive relationship exists between body image and self-esteem, it is reasonable to expect that a poor body image would be associated with other indicators of poor psychological health along with low self-esteem. For example, as low self-esteem has been found to be related to depressive symptomatology in women (Russo, Green, & Knight, 1993), it might be expected that women with low scores on body image variables would exhibit more symptoms of depression than would women with higher scores on body image variables. Koenig and Wasserman (1995) recently found that depression was associated with a poor body image in their sample of male and female college students. In a sample of 174 college women, Silverstein, Caceres, Perdue, and Cimarolli (1995) found that depressive symptomatology was associated with anxiety and poor body image. These and other studies (Fabian & Thompson, 1989; Thompson & Psaltis, 1988) seem to suggest relationships among poor body image, depression, low self-esteem, anxiety, and other measures of negative mood. Much of the research on body image has studied either clinical populations (e.g., Garner & Kearney-Cooke, 1996) or college students (e.g., Keeton, Cash, & Brown, 1990). Although the importance of appearance among women decreases somewhat with age (Pliner, Chaiken, & Flett, 1990), concern with appearance and body image remains important to women across the life span (Gupta & Schork, 1993; Pliner et al., 1990). Indeed, Pliner et al. (1990) found that the importance of appearance to women of college age (M = 3.9) and women aged 60 and older (M = 3.8) are quite similar. Likewise, in a large national survey, Cash and Henry (1995) found no significant difference between college-age women and other women up to the age of 70 on one measure of body image (body areas satisfaction). Media Influences on Body Image Media images seem to play a critical role in shaping the cultural ideal of beauty and attractiveness through advertising, retailing, and the entertainment industry. Although there are no perfect criteria that people can use to judge their own or another’s attractiveness, a uniform standard of beauty was made possible by the rise of mass media (Mazur, 1986). Previous studies suggested that the constructed Jung, Lennon / BODY IMAGE AND MEDIA IMAGES 31 images of attractive models in the omnipresent media may influence the general criteria for evaluating attractiveness to a considerable degree and elevate our standards for attractiveness (Martin & Kennedy, 1993; Richins, 1991). Beauty industries such as cosmetics, diet foods, plastic surgery, and the fashion business have expanded their economic investments and emphasized the need for appearance-enhancing products and behaviors. Perhaps this is because there is increasing dissatisfaction with their bodies among women due to increasing demand for a thin body size. For example, female college students exhibited increased negative feelings and body dissatisfaction after exposure to ultra-thin models (Stice & Shaw, 1994). The fashion and beauty industries appeal to female consumers to use their products by presenting attractive images (e.g., young, slim, and attractive models with flawless skin) through advertisements. According to Shaw and Waller (1995), comparisons made with media images through indirect contact such as looking at magazine photos are likely to encourage women to use those images as reference points in evaluating their own bodies. For example, the extent to which girls reported magazines as an important source of information for their appearances was correlated with the degree of investment in thinness, weight management behavior, and disturbed eating (Levine, Smolak, & Hayden, 1994). Therefore, it would be important to examine the extent to which exposure to media images can trigger negative responses on women’s body image, self-esteem, and mood, especially for those who are schematic on appearance. Schema Theory Self-schemas resulting from the process of acquiring knowledge about the self (e.g., categorization and evaluation of behavior by oneself and others) influence information processing about the self in a variety of domains, with more consistent judgments in one’s schematic domains. Self-schemas allow one to attend selectively to information of greater relative importance while ignoring other information. For example, in a study by Markus et al. (1987), body weight schematics and body weight aschematics were identified depending on whether the term overweight was self-descriptive and was an important dimension to individuals’ self-concept among overweight people. Overweight people who were schematic on body weight were more likely to perceive themselves as overweight than were overweight people who were aschematic on body weight; the latter 32 FAMILY AND CONSUMER SCIENCES RESEARCH JOURNAL were more likely to perceive themselves as of normal body weight. Thus, people with well-defined appearance self-schemas are likely to attend to appearance information (e.g., physical attractiveness and thinness), yet those without well-defined appearance self-schemas are likely to ignore or pay less attention to such information. Attributes related to self-schemas are always in people’s minds as well-developed self-structures and are ready to be activated in information processing as chronically accessible constructs. Unlike temporarily activated constructs that are not expected to last long, chronically accessible constructs are those that are cognitively available due to persistent activation over time (Higgins, King, & Mavin, 1982). Thus, exposure to attractive media images might temporarily activate appearance-related information for most of the people, but the exposure can activate chronically accessible appearance-related information for those who have appearance self-schemas. For those who have appearance self-schemas, appearance-related information can be used in their self-evaluations and their evaluations of others. For example, for those who are self-schematic on appearance, a variety of stimuli can be evaluated with reference to their own appearances (e.g., Does this hair style make me look younger? Is she more attractive than I am? Does this dress make me look slim?). Thus, appearance may be an important part of schematics’ thoughts, emotions, and behaviors, and appearance self-schema may be an important variable explaining individual differences in body image, self-esteem, and mood. Also, individuals who are temporarily exposed to attractive media images may have negative evaluations of their bodies and themselves, especially for those who are highly invested in their appearances, as those images reflect cultural standards with unrealistically thin body sizes and attractive physical characteristics. Research Hypotheses It was expected that individuals who are schematic on appearance are more likely to be dissatisfied with their bodies and self in general and are more likely to devote their time and energy to taking actions to improve their appearances than are those who are aschematic on appearance. Similarly, individuals who are schematic on appearance were expected to exhibit more negative mood than were those who are aschematic on appearance. Finally, exposure to attractive media images was expected to influence individual differences in body Jung, Lennon / BODY IMAGE AND MEDIA IMAGES 33 image, self-esteem, and mood, especially for appearance schematic individuals; individuals who are schematic on appearance were expected to exhibit the most negative mood, self-esteem, and body image after being exposed to attractive media images. Therefore, based on the literature reviewed, the following hypotheses were proposed in this study. Hypothesis 1: Women who are schematic on appearance will exhibit lower self-esteem and body image scores than will those who are aschematic on appearance. Hypothesis 2: Women who are schematic on appearance will exhibit higher scores on negative mood and lower scores on positive mood than will those who are aschematic on appearance. Hypothesis 3: Women who are exposed to attractive media images will exhibit lower body image scores, lower self-esteem scores, and greater negative mood scores than will those who are not exposed to media images. Hypothesis 4: Appearance self-schema will mediate effects of exposure to attractive media images on body image, self-esteem, and mood; as a result, women who are schematic on appearance and are exposed to media images will exhibit the most negative mood, self-esteem, and body image scores. METHOD Instrument Development To test research hypotheses, the following four main variables were considered in this study: appearance self-schema, mood, selfesteem, and body image. The four variables were measured using 12 instruments. Participant characteristics were assessed for preliminary analyses as well as descriptive purposes. The Appearance Schemas Inventory was used to assess appearance self-schema, that is, the importance, self-relevance, and centrality of appearance in individuals’ lives (Cash & Labarge, 1996). This measure consists of 14 items in a Likert-type rating scale format (1 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree). Higher scores indicate that an individual is more schematic toward appearance than are others. Example items are “What I look like is an important part of who I am” and “If I could look just as I wish, my life would be much happier.” The internal consistency of the Appearance Schemas Inventory has been established as .82 for college women (Cash, 1992). Its 1-month 34 FAMILY AND CONSUMER SCIENCES RESEARCH JOURNAL test-retest stability for female college students has been reported to be .71 (Cash, 1992). Two subscales (Joy and Distress) of the Differential Emotions Scale (Izard, 1972) were used to examine the level of mood distress after being exposed to attractive media images. These measures are composed of rating scale items on which people indicate how they are feeling regarding each of the items at that time (1 = very slightly or not at all to 5 = very strongly). For the Joy scale, ratings of the adjectives happy, joyful, and delighted are summed and used as the measure of joy. For the Distress scale, ratings of the adjectives discouraged, sad, and downhearted are summed and used as the measure of distress. Measures of internal consistency for the Joy and Distress measures have been found to be .80 and .90, respectively (Izard, 1972). Heinberg and Thompson’s (1992, 1995) visual analogue scales (VAS) were used to measure immediate response in mood and body satisfaction (an affective measure of body image) following exposure to attractive images. Participants were asked to indicate their disturbance level on a 100-mm line by placing a short vertical slash to reflect their current mood state. The anchors of the 100-mm lines were no anxiety or extreme anxiety, no depression or extreme depression, no anger or extreme anger, no body dissatisfaction or extreme body dissatisfaction, and no overall appearance dissatisfaction or extreme overall appearance dissatisfaction. Thus, each of these measures was a 101-point scale, scored such that higher scores indicated greater disturbance. All measures of mood (VAS-anxiety, VAS-depression, and VAS-anger) and body satisfaction (VAS-body dissatisfaction and VAS-overall appearance dissatisfaction) have demonstrated convergent validity (Heinberg & Thompson, 1995). The revised Janis-Field Self-Esteem Scale (Eagly, 1967; Janis & Field, 1959) was used to measure participants’ social self-esteem following exposure to attractive images. The instrument contains 20 items, 10 of which are keyed in a positive direction and 10 of which are keyed in a negative direction to balance for response-bias (Robinson & Shaver, 1973). Participants rated (1 = very often to 5 = never) their comfort level in various social situations in which higher scores are indicative of high social self-esteem. Sample items are “How often do you feel that you have handled yourself well at a social gathering?” and “How often do you worry about whether other people like to be with you?” This scale has reported split-half reliabilities of .72 and .88 (Robinson & Shaver, 1973) and a test-retest reliability of .92 (Campbell, Chew, & Scratchley, 1991). Jung, Lennon / BODY IMAGE AND MEDIA IMAGES 35 Rosenberg’s (1965) Self-Esteem Scale is a reliable (.85) and valid instrument that measures several areas of global self-worth (Silber & Tippett, 1965). It is composed of 10 items presented in a Likert-type format (1 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree). Half of the items are keyed in a positive direction, and the other half are keyed in a negative direction, which should be reverse scored so that higher scores indicate higher levels of global self-esteem. Example items are “I feel that I have a number of good qualities” and “I am able to do things as well as most other people.” Two subscales (Appearance Evaluation and Appearance Orientation) of the Multidimensional Body-Self Relations Questionnaire, a well-validated body-image measure (Keeton et al., 1990), were used to measure participants’ body images in relation to appearance. Appearance Evaluation assesses the extent to which individuals like and are satisfied with their physical appearances. This subscale is composed of seven statements concerning appearances. Example items are “Most people would consider me good-looking” and “I like my looks just the way they are.” Appearance Evaluation has a reported internal consistency of .88 and a 1-month stability of .91 (Cash & Szymanski, 1995). The 12 statements in Appearance Orientation assess cognitive importance of and behavioral improvement or maintenance of one’s appearance. Thus, Appearance Orientation measures the time, mental energy, and behavioral activity devoted to one’s appearance. This subscale has an internal consistency of .85 and a stability of .90 (Cash & Szymanski, 1995). Sample items are “I am always trying to improve my physical appearance” and “I check my appearance in a mirror whenever I can.” Stimulus Materials Photo slides were used to trigger participants’ cognitive and emotional vulnerabilities to attractive media images. Slides featured images of relatively unknown female models scanned from fashion magazines and catalogs (e.g., Glamour, Marie Claire, Harper’s Bazaar). White models were chosen for this study as they are dominantly featured in the media. As the result of a pilot test of female clothing and textiles graduate students, 16 photos were selected for use as stimuli. Photos used received average ratings between 6 and 7 on a 1 to 7 scale on the dimension of physical attractiveness. The 16 photos consisted of head-and-torso images and full-length images. All the models in the photos were judged to be thin by the pilot sample. 36 FAMILY AND CONSUMER SCIENCES RESEARCH JOURNAL Procedure Participants were recruited through (a) advertisements posted on bulletin boards of different university buildings or (b) oral solicitation in several classes. Participants were given incentives of cash prize drawings for their participation. Participants responded twice, once during an initial questionnaire session and then during a follow-up experimental session, with an interval of 2 weeks between sessions to prevent any carry-over effects from the initial session. Tasks were completed in small groups of 5 to 10. In the initial session, participants were given an informed consent form and asked to complete a questionnaire packet containing the Appearance Schemas Inventory and items assessing demographic information (age, major, height, weight, and ethnicity). Participants’ level of appearance self-schemas was based on a median split of scores; they were blocked into two groups (high appearance self-schema scores versus low appearance selfschema scores). For convenience, participants with high appearance self-schema scores are referred to as schematics on appearance, whereas participants with low appearance self-schema scores are referred to as aschematics on appearance. Half of each group was randomly assigned to exposure to the media images experimental condition, and the other half of each group was randomly assigned to the no exposure control condition for the follow-up session. After 2 weeks, participants were asked to return for a follow-up session, which did not require a parallel task. Separate sessions of small groups were held for those who were assigned to the exposure condition and for those who were assigned to the control condition. Those who were assigned to the exposure condition were presented with 16 media images in slide photos and given a filler task by which they assessed the formality and fashionability of models’ clothes. Participants were exposed to each slide for about 8 seconds. After exposure to the attractive images of stimulus photos, participants completed dependent measures of mood (joy, distress, depression, anxiety, and anger), self-esteem (social and global), and body image (body dissatisfaction, overall appearance dissatisfaction, appearance evaluation, and appearance orientation). Participants completed questionnaire packets within 25 to 30 minutes. Participants who were assigned to the control condition completed dependent measures without being exposed to media images. Jung, Lennon / BODY IMAGE AND MEDIA IMAGES 37 RESULTS Participant Characteristics This study used a college student sample for convenience; it was also reasonable given the nature of the association between age and measures of body image. A convenience sample of 175 college women enrolled in various majors at two large Midwestern universities volunteered for this study. Missing values resulted in usable data from a final sample of 168 women. Participants’ ages ranged from 18 to 28, with a mean of 21. The sample included 134 European Americans, 13 African Americans, 15 Asian Americans, 3 Hispanic Americans, and 3 other ethnicities. The majority of the participants (n = 104) were clothing-related majors such as textiles and clothing and apparel merchandising; other participants (n = 64) came from a variety of other majors such as business, nutrition, and the arts. Preliminary Analyses Cronbach’s coefficient alpha measured the internal consistency of the instruments used in this study (see Table 1). Reliabilities resulted for the Appearance Schemas Inventory, both social (Janis-Field SelfEsteem) and global (Rosenberg’s Self-Esteem) measures of selfesteem, two subscales (Appearance Evaluation and Appearance Orientation) of the Multidimensional Body-Self Relations Questionnaire, and two subscales (Joy and Distress) of the Differential Emotions Scale. Reliabilities of the VAS could not be calculated due to the nature of the measures, which required participants to mark a slash on a horizontal line rather than use a numerical value to indicate disturbance level. Because appearance is often important to clothing and merchandising students, an analysis of variance (ANOVA) determined if major (apparel major or non–apparel major) was related to level of appearance self-schemas with major as the independent variable and appearance schema scores as the dependent variable. Major was unrelated to level of appearance schemas in this sample, F(1, 166) = .123, p > .05. In an attempt to rule out the possible effects of major on the dependent variables of interest in this study, a multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was calculated. Results revealed that the 38 TABLE 1: FAMILY AND CONSUMER SCIENCES RESEARCH JOURNAL Means, Standard Deviations, and Minima and Maxima for All Variables Used in the Analyses Measures Appearance self-schema Appearance Schemas Inventory Mood Joy scale Distress scale VAS-anxiety VAS-depression VAS-anger Self-esteem Janis-Field SelfEsteem Scale Rosenberg’s SelfEsteem Scale Body image Appearance evaluation Appearance orientation VAS-body dissatisfaction VAS-overall appearance dissatisfaction Scale Mean SD Minimum Maximum Reliability 14 to 70 37.99 7.97 18 55 .81 3 to 15 8.39 3.03 3 to 15 5.74 2.58 0 to 100 44.43 28.73 0 to 100 25.59 22.65 0 to 100 16.82 20.22 3 3 0 0 0 15 15 100 85 100 .89 .80 20 to 100 68.93 10.81 36 97 .89 10 to 50 7.13 17 50 .89 7 to 35 23.33 5.17 12 to 60 44.57 8.41 0 to 100 43.01 29.20 10 20 0 35 60 98 .84 .89 0 to 100 36.69 24.20 0 92 39.39 overall multivariate effect for major on the dependent variables was not significant, F(11, 156) = .265, p > .05. Therefore, in this sample, major was unrelated to both the independent variable of interest and to all of the dependent variables. In a similar way, measures of body fat (body mass index [BMI]) were examined for their relationships to body image, mood, and appearance self-schema. There is a common perception that body fat is tied to body image such that smaller women have more positive body images than do larger women. Research results are mixed on the subject. Research with a variety of populations and age groups demonstrates that BMI or weight is sometimes negatively related to body image in the United States (Faith, Leone, & Allison, 1997; McCabe & Ricciardelli, 2001), particularly among women (Cash & Hicks, 1990). However, losing weight does not necessarily improve body image (Cash, Counts, & Huffine, 1990), and sometimes body image improves without a change in weight (Rosen, 1996). Because of these mixed results, it is possible that BMI could explain relationships among the variables of interest in this research. Based on a regression Jung, Lennon / BODY IMAGE AND MEDIA IMAGES 39 analysis with BMI as the independent variable and scores on appearance self-schema as the dependent variable, these two variables were found to be unrelated, F(1, 166) = .001, p > .05. The effects of BMI on the dependent variables in this study were assessed using multivariate analysis of variance using the standardized categorization scheme of BMI: A BMI of less than 20 as underweight (N = 49; 29%), a BMI between 20 and 25 as normal weight (N = 105; 63%), a BMI of more than 25 as overweight (N = 14; 8%). The overall effect of BMI on the dependent variables was not significant, F(11, 156) = 1.22, p > .05. Thus, results from preliminary analyses revealed that significant effects on the variables of interest in this study can be attributed neither to students’ choice of major nor to their BMI. Multivariate Analysis of Variance Research hypotheses were tested by a 2 (appearance self-schema) × 2 (exposure condition) between-subjects multivariate analysis of variance. Appearance self-schema (appearance schematic versus appearance aschematic) and exposure condition (exposure to attractive media images versus no exposure) served as independent variables. Based on a median split of scores, 83 respondents were classified as schematic on appearance and 85 were classified as aschematic on appearance. In all, 38 participants were classified as appearance schematic with exposure, 45 participants were classified as appearance schematic with no exposure, 50 participants were classified as appearance aschematic with exposure, and 35 participants were identified as appearance aschematic with no exposure. Thus, 88 participants received the experimental condition, and 80 received the control condition. Dependent variables were responses to the measures of body image (VAS-body dissatisfaction, VAS-overall appearance dissatisfaction, Appearance Orientation, and Appearance Evaluation), self-esteem scales (global self-esteem and social self-esteem), and the measures of mood (Joy scale, Distress scale, VAS-depression, VAS-anger, and VAS-anxiety). Variable means and standard deviations as well as their minimum and maximum scores are presented in Table 1. MANOVA revealed that neither the overall interaction between appearance self-schema and exposure nor the overall effect for exposure was significant. Contrary to predictions, appearance self-schema did not mediate effects of exposure to attractive media images on body image, self-esteem, and mood. In addition, no significant 40 FAMILY AND CONSUMER SCIENCES RESEARCH JOURNAL differences were found between women who are exposed to and those not exposed to attractive media images in terms of their body image, self-esteem, and mood scores. Thus, neither Hypothesis 3 nor Hypothesis 4 was supported. However, the MANOVA detected significant effects for appearance self-schema on the dependent variables, F(11, 156) = 9.68, p < .0001. To assess the cause of the significant overall effect, univariate analyses were performed for appearance self-schema on the dependent variables. Univariate Analyses of Variance Separate univariate analyses of variance for each of the dependent variables revealed that the effect for each of the dependent variables as a function of appearance self-schema significantly contributed to the multivariate main effect for appearance self-schema (ps < .01; see Table 2). Results revealed that women who subscribed to schematic beliefs about the importance, meaning, and effects of appearance in their lives received lower scores on social self-esteem, global selfesteem, and appearance evaluation than did those who did not hold such beliefs (see Table 2 for means and standard deviations). Similarly, the former group exhibited higher scores on body dissatisfaction, overall appearance dissatisfaction, and appearance orientation than did the latter group. As expected, women who are schematic on appearance had lower self-esteem and body image than did those who are aschematic on appearance. Thus, Hypothesis 1 was supported. It was also predicted that women who are schematic on appearance would exhibit greater negative mood and lower positive mood than would those who are aschematic on appearance. According to the analyses of variance, participants who held schematic beliefs regarding the importance, meaning, and effects of appearance in their lives expressed significantly greater mood distress, depression, anxiety, anger, and lower scores in positive mood (joy) than did those who did not hold such beliefs (see Table 2 for means and standard deviations). Thus, Hypothesis 2 was also supported. Multiple Regression Analyses Multiple regression analyses were used to examine relationships among appearance self-schema, body image, self-esteem, and mood. Jung, Lennon / BODY IMAGE AND MEDIA IMAGES TABLE 2: 41 Means and Standard Deviations for Each of the Dependent Variables as a Function of Appearance Self-Schema Dependent Variable Joy Distress Anxiety Depression Anger Body dissatisfaction Overall appearance dissatisfaction Social self-esteem Global self-esteem Appearance evaluation Appearance orientation Low Appearance Schema (N = 85) High Appearance Schema (N = 83) F(1, 166) 8.99 5.01 38.34 16.76 12.76 29.16 (2.97) (2.15) (29.14) (19.47) (16.92) (24.37) 7.77 6.49 50.66 34.63 20.96 57.19 (2.97) (2.78) (27.09) (22.21) (22.46) (26.95) 7.66** 13.92*** 7.90** 29.29*** 6.98** 50.76*** 25.93 72.98 42.80 25.11 41.18 (20.53) (9.23) (5.43) (4.68) (8.84) 47.71 (22.76) 64.78 (10.80) 35.90 (7.02) 21.51 (5.04) 48.04 (6.31) 41.16*** 28.18*** 52.37*** 25.59*** 31.05*** *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001. For instance, is attention to appearance information directly related to mood and body image outcomes, or does self-esteem mediate the relationship between appearance self-schema and mood and between appearance self-schema and body image? To assess possible relationships between appearance self-schema and measures of body image and mood mediated by self-esteem, nine multiple regression analyses were conducted, one for each dependent measure of body image (i.e., appearance evaluation, appearance orientation, body dissatisfaction, and overall appearance dissatisfaction) and for each dependent measure of mood (joy, distress, anxiety, depression, and anger). Independent variables were appearance self-schema and both social and global self-esteem. Appearance self-schema accounted for variance in all measures of body image except appearance evaluation (see Table 3 for regression statistics). Although there was a significant correlation between social and global self-esteem, r(168) = .73, p < .01, more body image variables were accounted for by global self-esteem (i.e., appearance evaluation, body dissatisfaction, and overall appearance dissatisfaction) than by social self-esteem (i.e., appearance evaluation and overall appearance dissatisfaction) (see partial R2s to estimate the percentage of variance accounted for by each of the three independent variables). Both 42 TABLE 3: Multiple Regressions for Appearance Self-Schema and Social and Global Self-Esteem on Measures of Body Image and Mood Dependent Variable Body image AE df F R R2 Independent Variable ASS SSE GSE ASS SSE GSE ASS SSE GSE ASS SSE GSE ASS SSE GSE ASS SSE GSE ASS SSE GSE ASS SSE GSE ASS SSE GSE 3,164 28.49*** .59 .34 AO 3,164 22.54*** .54 .29 BD 3,164 20.87*** .53 .28 OAD 3,164 25.89*** .57 .32 3,164 10.25*** .40 .16 Distress 3,164 13.95*** .45 .20 Anxiety 3,164 3.56* .25 .06 Depression 3,164 17.47*** .49 .24 Anger 3,164 4.59** .28 .08 Mood Joy Beta t Partial R Partial R2 –.04 .16 .18 .61 –.02 .12 1.10 –.15 –1.04 .57 –.44 –.94 –.05 .34 .25 .58 –.02 .10 .30 –.06 –.25 .19 –.20 –.28 –.71 3.65*** 2.63** 7.34*** –.20 1.02 3.77*** –.58 –2.51* 2.42* –2.08* –2.83* –.06 .27 .20 .50 –.02 .08 .28 –.05 –.19 .19 –.16 –.22 .0036 .0729 .04 .25 .0004 .0064 .0784 .0025 .0361 .0361 .0256 .0484 –.01 .06 .08 .03 –.06 –.07 .74 –.31 .22 .82 –.62 .05 .65 –.15 .13 –.03 .22 .19 .10 –.23 –.19 .21 –.12 .06 .29 –.29 .02 .26 –.08 .05 –.29 2.10* 1.74 1.23 –2.23* –1.75 2.26* –1.03 .48 3.52** –2.92** .14 2.85** –.74 .40 –.02 .16 .14 .10 –.17 –.14 .17 –.08 .04 .27 –.22 .01 .22 –.06 .03 .0004 .0256 .0196 .01 .0289 .0196 .0289 .0064 .0016 .0729 .0484 .0001 .0484 .0036 .0009 B NOTE: AE = appearance evaluation; AO = appearance orientation; BD = body dissatisfaction; OAD = overall appearance dissatisfaction; ASS = appearance self-schema; SSE = social self-esteem, GSE = global self-esteem. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001. Jung, Lennon / BODY IMAGE AND MEDIA IMAGES 43 appearance self-schema and social self-esteem accounted for variance in three out of five measures of mood (i.e., anxiety, depression, and anger by appearance self-schema and joy, distress, and depression by social self-esteem). Global self-esteem did not explain any variance in the mood variables. Pearson correlations revealed that appearance self-schema was significantly related to both social, r(168) = –.50, p < .01, and global self-esteem, r(168) = –.53, p < .01, in the negative direction. Higher scores on appearance self-schema were associated with lower scores on social as well as global self-esteem. Although correlated, the three measures did not always predict body image and mood (see Table 3). Therefore, appearance self-schema is a significant individual variable but is not entirely responsible for explaining participants’ responses on measures of body image and mood. Participants’ self-esteem is also a mediating factor on measures of body image and mood. DISCUSSION The purpose of this research was to examine the effects of women’s appearance self-schemas and exposure to media images on their body images, self-esteem, and mood. First, it was hypothesized that women who are schematic on appearance would exhibit lower selfesteem and body image scores than would those who are aschematic on appearance. The self-esteem scores of women schematic on appearance were lower than were the self-esteem scores of women aschematic on appearance for both social and global self-esteem. Analyses also revealed significant main effects for appearance selfschema on measures of body image. In each case, women schematic on appearance exhibited lower appearance evaluation scores, higher appearance orientation scores, higher body dissatisfaction scores, and higher overall appearance dissatisfaction scores than did those aschematic on appearance. Body image is a multidimensional construct for which four different measures were used (appearance evaluation, appearance orientation, body dissatisfaction, and overall appearance dissatisfaction). In general, body image is thought to be “negative” when individuals indicate dissatisfaction with their appearances and bodies and when they evaluate their appearances unfavorably. Results indicate that the more participants considered appearance important, central, and self-relevant in their cognitions (appearance self-schema), the lower they evaluated their own 44 FAMILY AND CONSUMER SCIENCES RESEARCH JOURNAL physical appearances (appearance evaluation). Similarly, the more participants considered appearance important, central, and self-relevant in their cognitions, the less satisfied they were with their bodies (body dissatisfaction) and overall appearances (overall appearance dissatisfaction), with much time spent on improving their appearances (appearance orientation). According to Cash (1995), appearance orientation (i.e., the devotion of time, mental energy, and behavioral activity to appearance) amplifies affective body image. Thus, if people are dissatisfied with their overall appearances and bodies or if they evaluate their appearances unfavorably, then higher scores in appearance orientation indicate a more negative body image. This is precisely how the appearance schematic women scored. Thus, women who were schematic on appearance had more negative body images than did women who were aschematic on appearance. It was also hypothesized that women who are schematic on appearance would exhibit higher scores on negative mood and lower scores on positive mood than would those who are aschematic on appearance. The analyses revealed significant effects for appearance self-schema on all five measures of mood (joy, distress, depression, anxiety, and anger). In each case, those who were appearance schematic exhibited higher scores on negative mood (distress, depression, anxiety, and anger) and lower scores on positive mood (joy) than did those who were appearance aschematic. Perhaps, young women who define themselves (in part) in terms of appearance (i.e., self-schematic on appearance) feel greater pressure to emulate the cultural ideal of beauty and are more emotionally distressed than those who define themselves otherwise. As a result, more negative mood responses were assessed from those who consider appearance an important domain of self-relevance and central to the self. As mentioned earlier, poor psychological health such as depression is associated with a poor body image, and thus, women who are schematic on appearance may be more likely to develop and maintain negative body images than are those who are aschematic on appearance. The third hypothesis suggested that women exposed to attractive media images would exhibit lower body image scores, lower selfesteem scores, and greater negative mood scores than would those not exposed to media images. However, findings revealed no statistically significant effects for exposure to media images on body image, self-esteem, and mood. Presumably, participants did not compare themselves to media images (as they evaluated mood and body image) or if they did, it had no significant effects on them. Participants Jung, Lennon / BODY IMAGE AND MEDIA IMAGES 45 were asked to assess the formality and fashionability of models’ clothes by the filler task given along with the stimulus photos. It is also possible that the nature of the filler task elicited a search for information from the images rather than comparison with them. In such a case, exposure to media images may not trigger cognitions about appearance. Finally, it was hypothesized that appearance self-schema would mediate effects of exposure to attractive media images on body image, self-esteem, and mood scores. The overall interaction effect between appearance self-schema and exposure condition on measures of mood, body image, and self-esteem was not significant. Thus, women who are schematic on appearance and were exposed to media images did not exhibit the most negative body image, self-esteem, and mood scores. Nevertheless, appearance self-schema has proven to be a critical individual difference variable moderating the relationship between variables such as body image, self-esteem, and mood. Multiple regression analyses revealed that self-esteem might be an important variable mediating the relationships between appearance self-schema and body image as well as between appearance selfschema and mood. Self-esteem accounted for variance in most of the measures of body image (more so by global self-esteem than by social self-esteem) and mood (by social self-esteem only) when the effects of appearance self-schema were controlled. However, further studies are necessary to clarify the relationships among appearance selfschema, body image, self-esteem, and mood. CONCLUSION AND IMPLICATIONS In conclusion, appearance self-schema was a significant factor in assessments of women’s body image, self-esteem, and mood. According to this study, women who placed high cognitive importance on appearance exhibited lower body image, lower self-esteem, and more negative mood than did those who placed low cognitive importance on appearance. Although this study examined whether appearance self-schema alone or appearance self-schema in conjunction with exposure to media images is responsible for the negative effects on women, it may be a moot point in that people are daily bombarded with a multitude of idealized images in the media. Exposing individuals briefly to a relatively small number of media images and 46 FAMILY AND CONSUMER SCIENCES RESEARCH JOURNAL monitoring any changes in body image satisfaction or dissatisfaction and mood may be an unreasonable approach in revealing the media’s effect, which takes place over time and is influenced by individual variables such as appearance self-schema as shown in this study. Given that this is the case, it seems reasonable to suspect methodological limitations in designing the exposure procedure employed in this study. Future researchers may find it necessary to use a more intense presentation of images that has external validity. As another limitation of this study, we were not able to measure ethnic differences on body image and self-esteem (e.g., higher levels of self-esteem and a more positive body image being reported by African American women than by Caucasian women; Molloy & Herzberger, 1998) upon their exposure to attractive images. Because only Caucasian models were used in this study, it is possible that women of color would have been less distressed by the images featuring ideals for Caucasian women. Studies in the future should work to refine the current experiment to compare ethnic groups for the effect of media images on their body images and self-esteem. Some may consider appearance to be an important dimension of self-evaluation, whereas others may place cognitive emphasis on other dimensions (e.g., sports or politics). Focusing on other schematic domains rather than appearance has been suggested as a coping strategy to overcome negative effects of appearance (Cash, 1995). To put it more simply and in the words of Cash (1995), “Negative body image has little to do with outward appearance; it’s a state of mind” (p. 27). Because appearance self-schema was significantly related to measures of body image, self-esteem, and mood in this study, appearance self-schema is demonstrated to be an important individual difference variable moderating the extent of individuals’ satisfaction with their bodies and selves in general. Studies found that attractiveness in the media sells products (Chaiken & Eagly, 1983; Kamins, 1990); however, attractive images seen may also reinforce viewers’ unrealistic expectations for how they should look, especially for young children. Fortunately, these results suggest that not everyone is affected by cultural pressure to approximate ideal female beauty, and efforts to counteract the effects of unrealistic images may be successful. For example, the need to educate children about media images is important because they are exposed to media images from an early age. Children may not realize that media images are artificially manipulated by makeup or computer graphics. To ground expectations in reality, children should be Jung, Lennon / BODY IMAGE AND MEDIA IMAGES 47 educated about unrealistic media images through class discussions and other activities. They should also be encouraged to develop nonappearance-related self-defining cognitive areas, such as athletic prowess, intellectual achievement, or musical ability. For example, female high school students who participated in a media literacy program teaching skills for challenging the media have reported less internalization of the thin beauty standard and lower perceived realism of media images (Irving, DuPen, & Berel, 1998). Educators, particularly family and consumer science educators, should help students to develop and maintain positive physical and mental health, including body image. Results of this study suggest that when young women define themselves in terms of their aesthetic value, they may experience more negative mood and lower self-esteem and may be dissatisfied with their appearances. Therefore, teaching children to value themselves and others for their unique capabilities (e.g., goalie, band member, or community volunteer) and attributes (e.g., perseverance, thoughtfulness, or friendliness) might be a useful strategy to encourage the development of self-schemas with a nonappearance focus. REFERENCES Akan, G. E., & Grilo, C. M. (1994). Sociocultural influences on eating attitudes and behaviors, body image, and psychological functioning: A comparison of AfricanAmerican, Asian-American, and Caucasian college women. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 18, 181-187. Brownell, K. D. (1991). Personal responsibility and control over our bodies: When expectation exceeds reality. Health Psychology, 10, 303-310. Button, E., Loan, P., Davies, J., & Sonuga-Barke, E. (1997). Self-esteem, eating problems, and psychological well-being in a cohort of schoolgirls aged 15-16: A questionnaire and interview study. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 21, 39-47. Campbell, J. D., Chew, B., & Scratchley, L. S. (1991). Cognitive and emotional reactions to daily events: The effects of self-esteem and self-complexity. Journal of Personality, 59, 473-505. Cash, T. F. (1992). Initial development of the Appearance Schemas Inventory. Unpublished research, Old Dominion University, Norfolk, Virginia. Cash, T. F. (1995). What do you see when you look in the mirror? Helping yourself to a positive body image. New York: Bantam Books. Cash, T. F., Counts, B., & Huffine, C. E. (1990). Current and vestigial effects of overweight among women: Fear of fat, attitudinal body image, and eating behaviors. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 12, 157-167. Cash, T. F., & Henry, P. (1995). Women’s body images: The results of a national survey in the U.S.A. Sex Roles, 33, 19-28. 48 FAMILY AND CONSUMER SCIENCES RESEARCH JOURNAL Cash, T. F., & Hicks, K. L. (1990). Being fat versus thinking fat: Relationships with body image, eating behaviors, and well-being. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 14, 327-341. Cash, T. F., & Labarge, A. S. (1996). Development of the Appearance Schemas Inventory: A new cognitive body-image assessment. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 20, 37-50. Cash, T. F., & Szymanski, M. L. (1995). The development and validation of the BodyImage Ideals Questionnaire. Journal of Personality Assessment, 64, 466-477. Chaiken, S., & Eagly, A. (1983). Communication modality as a determinant of persuasion: The role of communicator salience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 241-256. Cusumano, D., & Thompson, J. K. (1997). Body image and body shape ideals in magazines: Exposure, awareness, and internalization. Sex Roles, 37, 701-721. Drogosz, L. M., & Levy, P. E. (1996). Another look at the effects of appearance, gender, and job type on performance-based decisions. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 20, 437-445. Eagly, A. (1967). Involvement as a determinant of response to favorable information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 7, 1-15. Fabian, L. J., & Thompson, J. K. (1989). Body image and eating disturbance in young females. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 8, 63-74. Faith, M. S., Leone, M. A., & Allison, D. B. (1997). The effects of self-generated comparison targets, BMI, and social comparison tendencies on body image appraisal. Eating Disorders: The Journal of Treatment & Prevention, 5, 128-140. Fallon, A. (1990). Culture in the mirror: Sociocultural determinants of body image. In T. F. Cash & T. Pruzinsky (Eds.), Body images: Development, deviance, and change (pp. 80-109). New York: Guilford. Garner, D. M., & Garfinkel, P. E. (1981). Body image in anorexia nervosa: Measurement, theory and clinical implications. International Journal of Psychiatry in Medicine, 11, 263-284. Garner, D. M., & Kearney-Cooke, A. (1996, March/April). Psychology Today, pp. 55-56. Gupta, M. A., & Schork, N. J. (1993). Aging-related concerns and body image: Possible future implications for eating disorders. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 14, 481-486. Heinberg, L. J., & Thompson, J. K. (1992). The effects of figure size feedback (positive vs. negative) and target comparison group (particularistic vs. universalistic) on body image disturbance. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 12, 441-448. Heinberg, L. J., & Thompson, J. K. (1995). Body image and televised images of thinness and attractiveness: A controlled laboratory investigation. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 14, 325-338. Hesse-Biber, S. (1996). Am I thin enough yet? The cult of thinness and the commercialization of identity. New York: Oxford University Press. Higgins, E. T., King, G., & Mavin, G. (1982). Individual construct accessibility and subjective impressions and recall. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 35-47. Irving, L., DuPen, J., & Berel, S. (1998). A media literacy program for high school females. Eating Disorders, 6, 119-132. Izard, C. E. (1972). Patterns of emotions. New York: Academic Press. Jackson, L., Sullivan, L., & Rostker, R. (1988). Gender, gender role, and body image. Sex Roles, 19, 429-443. Jung, Lennon / BODY IMAGE AND MEDIA IMAGES 49 Janis, I. L., & Field, P. B. (1959). Sex differences and personality factors related to persuasibility. In C. I. Hovland, I. L. Janis, P. B. Field, H. Linton, E. Graham, & A. R. Cohen, et al. (Eds.), Personality and persuasibility (pp. 55-68). New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Kamins, M. A. (1990). An investigation into the “match-up” hypothesis in celebrity advertising: When beauty may be only skin deep. Journal of Advertising, 19, 4-13. Keeton, W. P., Cash, T. F., & Brown, T. A. (1990). Body image or body images?: Comparative, multidimensional assessment among college students. Journal of Personality Assessment, 54, 213-230. Koenig, L. J., & Wasserman, E. L. (1995). Body image and dieting failure in college men and women: Examining links between depression and eating problems. Sex Roles, 32, 225-249. Lee, A. M., & Lee, S. (1996). Disordered eating and its psychosocial correlates among Chinese adolescent females in Hong Kong. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 20, 177-183. Lennon, S., Rudd, N., Sloan, B., & Kim, J. (1999). Attitudes toward gender roles, selfesteem, and body image: Application of a model. Clothing and Textiles Research Journal, 17, 191-202. Lerner, R. M., Orlos, J., & Knapp, J. (1976). Physical attractiveness, physical effectiveness, and self-concept in late adolescents. Adolescence, 11, 313-325. Levine, M., Smolak, L., & Hayden, H. (1994). The relation of sociocultural factors to eating attitudes and behaviors among middle school girls. Journal of Early Adolescence, 14, 471-490. Locher, P., Unger, R., Sociedade, P., & Wahl, J. (1993). At first glance: Assessibility of the physical attractiveness stereotype. Sex Roles, 28, 729-743. Markus, H. (1977). Self-schemata and processing information about the self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 63-78. Markus, H., Hamill, R., & Sentis, K. P. (1987). Thinking fat: Self-schemas for body weight and the processing of weight relevant information. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 17, 50-71. Markus, H., Smith, J., & Moreland, R. (1985). Role of the self-concept in the perception of others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 1494-1512. Martin, M., & Kennedy, P. (1993). Advertising and social comparison: Consequences for female preadolescents and adolescents. Psychology & Marketing, 10, 513-530. Mazur, A. (1986). U.S. trends in feminine beauty and overadaptation. Journal of Sex Research, 22, 281-303. McCabe, M. P., & Ricciardelli, L. A. (2001). Parent, peer, and media influences on body image and strategies to both increase and decrease body size among adolescent boys and girls. Adolescence, 36(142), 225-240. Mellin, L., Irwin, C., & Scully, S. (1992). Prevalence of disordered eating in girls: A survey of middle-class children. Journal of the American Dietetic Association, 92, 851853. Molloy, B., & Herzberger, S. (1998). Body image and self-esteem: A comparison of African-American and Caucasian women. Sex Roles, 38, 631-643. Mori, D., & Morey, L. (1991). The vulnerable body image of females with feelings of depression. Journal of Research in Personality, 25, 343-354. Peterson, R. T. (1987). Bulimia and anorexia in an advertising context. Journal of Business Ethics, 6, 495-504. 50 FAMILY AND CONSUMER SCIENCES RESEARCH JOURNAL Pliner, P., Chaiken, S., & Flett, G. L. (1990). Gender differences in concern with body weight and physical appearance over the life span. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 16, 263-273. Polivy, J., Garner, D. M., & Garfinkel, P. E. (1986). Causes and consequences of the current preference for thin female physiques. In C. P. Zanna & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Physical appearance, stigma and social behavior: The Third Ontario Symposium in Personality and Social Psychology (pp. 89-112). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Richins, M. L. (1991). Social comparison and the idealized images of advertising. Journal of Consumer Research, 18, 71-83. Robinson, J. P., & Shaver, P. R. (1973). Measures of social psychological attitudes (2nd ed.). Ann Arbor, MI: Institute for Social Research. Rodin, J., Silberstein, L., & Striegel-Moore, R. (1985). Women and weight: A normative discontent. In T. B. Sonderegger (Ed)., Psychology and gender: Nebraska symposium on motivation (Vol. 32, pp. 267-307). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Rosen, J. (1990). Body-image disturbances in eating disorders. In T. F. Cash & T. Pruzinsky (Eds.), Body images: Development, deviance and change (pp. 190-214). New York: Guilford. Rosen, J. (1996). Improving body image in obesity. In J. K. Thompson (Ed.), Body image, eating disorders, and obesity: An integrative guide for assessment and treatment (pp. 425440). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Russo, N. F., Green, B. L., & Knight, G. (1993). The relationship of gender, self-esteem, and instrumentality to depressive symptomatology. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 12, 218-236. Shaw, J., & Waller, G. (1995). The media’s impact on body image: Implications for prevention and treatment. Eating Disorders: The Journal of Treatment & Prevention, 3, 115123. Silber, E., & Tippett, J. (1965). Self-esteem: Clinical assessment and measurement validation. Psychological Reports, 16, 1017-1071. Silverstein, B., Caceres, J., Perdue, L., & Cimarolli, V. (1995). Gender differences in depressive symptomatology: The role played by “anxious somatic depression” associated with gender-related achievement. Sex Roles, 33, 621-636. Simmons, R., & Rosenberg, F. (1975). Sex, sex roles, and self-image. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 4, 229-258. Stein, K. (1996). The self-schema model: A theoretical approach to the self-concept in eating disorders. Archives of Psychiatric Nursing, 10, 96-109. Stice, E., & Shaw, H. E. (1994). Adverse effects of the media portrayed thin-ideal on women and linkages to bulimic symptomatology. Journal of Social & Clinical Psychology, 13, 288-308. Thomas, V. G. (1989). Body-image satisfaction among Black women. Journal of Social Psychology, 129, 107-112. Thompson, J. K., & Psaltis, K. (1988). Multiple aspects and correlates of body figure ratings: A replication and extension of Fallon and Rozin (1985). International Journal of Eating Disorders, 7, 813-817. Thornton, B., & Moore, S. (1993). Physical attractiveness contrast effect: Implications for self-esteem and evaluations of the social self. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19, 474-480. Jung, Lennon / BODY IMAGE AND MEDIA IMAGES 51 Timko, C., Striegel-Moore, R. H., Silberstein, L. R., & Rodin, J. (1987). Femininity/ masculinity and disordered eating in women: How are they related? International Journal of Eating Disorders, 6, 701-712. Vitousek, K., & Hollon, S. (1990). The investigation of schematic content and processing in eating disorders. Cognitive Therapy & Research, 14, 191-214.