Reprinted from HUMAN ORGANIZATION, Vol. 23, No.1, Spring 1964 Still Photography in the Systematic Recording and Analysis of Behavioral Data Paul Byers* Every anthropologist goes into the field with at least a notebook and a still camera. He will have had yeal'S of training in the special literacy with which he will handle the observational, conceptual, and interpretive aspects of his discipline in the words he will write. But he may have . had little, if any, systematic training in seeing, under­ standing, taking, or u~ing the materials he will get with his camera. The camera ha's become an important part of the an­ thropologist's instrumentation, but there is, as yet, al­ most no photographic equivalent of a literacy with which to handle photographic observational materials syste­ matically and communicatively.. There have been numer­ ous more or less successful attempts to handle ,this prob­ lem but each time photography ,is used in behavioral science beyond simple illustrations-with-captions, a new invention is required ,to deal with the material meaning­ fully and communicatively.1 • Paul Byers teaches photography at Columbia University and is a 'professional ·photographer specializing in photographic work in behavioral science. The author is indebted to Dr. Ray L. Birdwhistel1 et al., at Temple Research Division, Eastern Pennsylvania Psychiatric Insti­ tute, Philadelphia, for insights into human communication and micro-cultural 'analysis and for training in linguistic-kinesic analy­ sis of behavior on sound-film. 1. Probably the most important of these inventions for the an­ thropological use of still photographs, including discussions of the procedures used, are: Gregory Bateson .and Margaret Mead, Balin~1I: Characttr, A Photoflraphic Analysis, New York Academy of Sciences, 1942; and Margaret Mead and Frances C. Macgregor, Growlh and Culture, A Photoflraphic Study of Balinese Childhood, G. P. Putnam's, New York, 1951. Discussions of still photography in anthropology may be found In: Margaret Mead, "Some Uses of Still Photography in Culture and Personality," in D. G. Haring (cd.), Personal Character and Cultural Milieu, Syracuse University Press, Syracuse, 1956, pp. 79-105; and Margaret Mead, "Anthropology Among the Sciences," American Anthropoloflist, LXIII (1961) 475-432.' If photographic information is to become systematic­ ally useful to behavioral scienti&tS, special training must be acquired not only in photographic tedmology but also in under&tanding the potentialities of the tool as much more than a recording or illustrating device. There is a vast world of visible but "unseen" information that can be accessible to photography and it is possible to develop a literacy or sy.stematization by which photographs can be taken and used in behavioral observation and in the currency of communication among scholars. In order ,to understand the kind of useful information that can be handled with some precision it is necessary, first, to understand something of the relation of photog­ raphy to the several bumanbehaviors involved in pro­ ducing photographs. As a starting point there is a useful parallel in lingui<stics. H. A. Gleason begins his Introduc­ tion to Descriptive Linguistics2 with the paragraph: As you listen to an unfamiliar Iangua~e you ~et the impression of a torrent of disorgamzed nOises carrying no sense whatever. To the native speaker it is qUite otherwise. He pays little attention to the sounds, but concerns himself instead with some situa­ tion which lies behind the act of speech and is, for him, somehow reflected in it. Both you and he have failed to grasp the nature of the phenomenon. Neither the casual observer nor the usual native speaker can give any real information about a lan­ guage. To be sure, some people, Americans perhaps more than most others, have decided notions about language. But the ideas held and discussed come far short of giving a complete picture of the language and sometimes have very lIttle relationship to the 2. H. A. Gleason, An Introduction to Descripti'fll! Linguistics, Holt, Rinehart and Winston, New York, 1961. HUMAN ORGANIZATION facts. Even people with considerable education are often wholly unable to answer certain quite simple questions about their language. For most people language is primarily a tool to be used, rather than a subject for close critical attention. 79 As a professional photographer who has worked with anthropologists for many years, I have been obliged to explore the tools I use and photography as a whole in terms of its human uses, in terms of current under­ standing of human communication, and as a cultural in­ vention. I am repeatedly surprised to find that whereas writers (professional or scientific) do not ,suppose that the value of their writing is significantly related to pen­ cils, alphabets, or typewriters or their technological skills with these instruments, they do behave as though photo­ graphic skill is somehow importantly related to cameras and photographic technology. So at the outset of this paper I must suggest that photography is not the product essentially a matter of technology (and cannot be suc­ cessfully taught as one) than speech is a matter of physiology. For the past few years, both for the Institute of In­ tercultural Studies and at Columbia University, I have taught behavioral science photography to graduate an­ thropology students in New YQrk City. This article is partly a product of that experience. In scanning the available literature and current prac­ tices in the teaching of photography one finds that the inherent aim in such teaching, and consequently the methods used, are determined by an historical two­ headed view of still photography as 1) an art and 2) a precise machine-made record of a scene or a subject. In the first view the primary concern is the vision of the phot~grapher-artist who uses the technology to produce a creative photograph of which the photographer is the " source. "1 n t he secon dview ' ' . t he th e primary concern IS accuracy with which the subject is recorded on film in which case the subject is the "source." This second view, however, presupposes that we have some acceptable criterion with which we can measure the relationship be­ tween 1) a three-dimensional, colored (and perhaps smell and sound-producing) subject moving in real time and 2) a photographic translation as a two-dimensional 8 X 10 inch, black-and-white representation of a l/loOth of a second's worth of subject/time. Furthermore when we show .almost any photograph to several different people and ~Iscover that they see different things and meanings, we fmd we have no real criterion with which we can even. iden tify what is meant by "the subject" except in the Simplest orders of classification. The truism that the same photograph has non-meas­ urable, different meanings to different viewers implies a semantic impasse which has led most observer,s to discard the photograph as a possible object of scientific study or analysi,s or as a useful vehicle for precise and/or com­ municable information. It is often clearly understood what cannot be done with photography because of the semantic problem. But it is the task of the photographer working in behavioral science to discover what observa­ tional and communication purposes photography can serve in behavioral science. At the beginning of a behavioral science photography course the class is asked to discard the concept of "ob­ jects" or "people" as photographic subjects. When, for example, a simple exercise-assignment in camera handling and light asks for pictures of a brick, the ,students are asked to decide, individually, what hypothetical signifi­ cance they chose to assign to the brick. Since "brick" is a generalized word wherea,s a brick itself may be conceived in a variety of ways depending on its con­ ceptual context, each student is required to describe a of a technology but i" the product of the -various hutnan "ign'i'GanGe; that he i" photographing. There are innu­ interactions involved: people being photographed, people taking photographs, people looking at photo­ graphs. Pencils and paper do not write and cameras do not take pictures. Behavioral photography is no more merable ways to photograph a brick artistically. But, if one conceives that a brick is significant, for example, be­ cause of its relation to the weight-bearing structural value of a wall, then this quality or relationship must Inherent in this point of view of the descriptive linguist is the recognition that there is more information in hu­ man speech than meets the untrained ear. There is also more information in photographs than meets the un­ trained eye. We can increase the information available and thus the useful value of still photography in be­ havioral disciplines if we understand photography in terms of the human behavior of which it is a product and in terms of the information which these behaviors con­ tribute to the photograph. I would paraphrase the above paragraph of Gleason in relation to photography: As you look at a series of unfamiliar photographs you may get the impression that what you are seeing is simple and evident on the face of the photographs. To the photographer who took them it is often quite different. He pays little attention to the light rays or the minute details but concerns himself instead with recording some scene in terms of the meaning it has for him and which is somehow reflected in the photo­ graphs. Both you and he have failed to grasp the full informational nature of photography. Neither the casual observer nor the usual photographer is likely to give much scientifically 'significant infor­ mation about the behavioral content of a photo­ graph. . . . The ideas held and discussed come far short of giving a complete picture of photography­ as-information and sometimes have very little rela­ tion to the significant behavioral information in photographs. Even people with considerable educa­ tion are often wholly unable to see quite simple in­ formational elements in a photograph. For most people photography is primarily a tool to record or illustrate concrete objects or convey abstract feel­ ings, rather than a subject for close and critical attention. 80 HUMAN ORGANIZATION somehow be evident in the photograph. Other possible to deal explicitly with useful kinds of information in brick-significances might be: the shape (as related to photographs. other shapes used, to the completed structure, or to the From another point of view all this serves to teach available manufacturing tools), or the substance (as re­ students to translate (and, indeed, teaches them what flecting the use of raw materials), non-utilitarian design, can and cannot be translated) from event to photograph. etc. The order of significance is required in words and the They begin to learn how to see like a camera but, in this significance itself has to be available in the photographs. course, like an anthropological camera. This is not the Photographs are criticised and informally valued in terms same as "seeing like an anthropologist." Information go­ of their success in showing specifically stated signifi­ ing to a brain i-s not recorded or "processed" the same cances. This produces a useful separation between that way as information going through a lens onto a film. The information that can be handled verbally (the students limiting factors are different. are required to write their orders-of-significance before In the past photographs have commonly been used to taking pictures) and that which is most effectively record or illustrate what the observer has already seen handled photographically. and already knows. Photographs are usually taken and/or When in the course we proceed to the photography of selected because they support an idea or prior image of human interaction, we do not think of people-as-subjects the observer. But the camera can also, and perhaps but, instead, of some aspect of the interaction that has most usefully, be used to find out what we may not some possible hypothetical significance. Each student is know and have little visual access to. To do this, how­ free to imagine a project to which some aspect of be­ ever, we must learn how to see what we do not know havior might contribute significantly: how people shake :ve a:-e going to see in photographs. It is more important hands, how children of three years touch each other, how In thIS aspect of photography to learn to see information mothers feed babies, etc. When this order of significance in photographs than to be able to see the same order of is stated in writing, the class as a whole can evaluate the information at an everit. Our culture provides consider­ photographs. In practice such significances are likely to able training in seeing what we already know in photo­ be of a kind that requires multiple pictures in series or graphs. We tend to use pictures as experiential re­ juxtaposition. For human interaction we commonly work rundancy. For example, we reject photographs of our­ in terms of shooting one 36-exposure roll of film per as­ selves or our children if those pictures do not support signment. our own theories about the subject-ourselves or our Introducing the concept of significance to replace sub­ chi~dren. We see actions in photographs in terms of ject serves several purposes. It removes the common actIOns we know about and are already familiar with un­ temptation to take "good" pictures that reflect one's own less we are ~ontr.adicted by. ~ caption. \Ve have no easy pictorial or artistic cleverness. Art does not count. It way to ratIOnalIze unfamIliar actions to make them ' shifts the photographer's attention to levels of informa­ predictable to us. tion that are appropriate to photography while providing One. goal in the cour-se is to exploit the capacities of the necessary verbal cross-referencing that enables the the stl~1 camera to see more than our eyes alone will. viewer to know exactly wha t to expect to see. It serves Early In the course students make event-discoveries in the purpose, also, of training the students to see the P?otographs that are important to them but which they world in front of their cameras in increasingly subtle and dId not see at the event they photographed. When it refined patterns. Human behavior is patterns of patterns ?ecomes . clear th.at photography can record significant of patterns, decreasing in size beyond what we normally mformatlOn that IS not seen with eyes, students can begin use in social seeing. As the students move back and forth to use photography to search for information about hu­ between looking at the material in front of the cameras man interaction that is unavailable to their eyes. in terms of anthropological significance and looking at We tend to forget that the photograph represents great numbers of photographs,. they gai~ a considerable a kind of visual infonnation that must not be confused measure of increased observattonal acuIty. Except for with what eyes normally see. We may look at a photo­ those few students who may be unable to accept the idea graph and see only the u<sual eye-available information. of seeing on new levels, the students can learn to see new But there is always more. If the telescope and the micro­ kinds of information on new levels in both human inter­ scope gave us new kinds of visual information (which action and in photographs. we had to learn to see with these instruments) based on The significance concept is also u<seful because it di­ new orders of magnification, the still camera gives us a vides our materials into a dichotomy appropriate for the new order of seeing based on a change in time. Seeing course. Anthropological thinking (i.e., the significance) is learned and culturally influenced. We learn what to see is left to the anthropology students and that which is and what not to see. We see in human time and in some photographic per se is the province of the teacher-photog­ relation to human rhythms. For this reason we find rapher. The class as a ""hole judges the communicati,:e movies easier to see than still pictures. Movies are pro­ value (and, to wherever extent we become systematIc jected, normally, at a human-time rate. But the still in a project-context, the data value) of the photographs. photograph holds a scene motionless for our continuous Thus the class simultaneou<sly learns and teaches itself involvement-in-time with a non-time representation. We HUMAN ORGANIZATION see relationships frozen that are, in life, too fleeting for our eyes. There is, therefore, more information available to us in a still photograph of a scene than was available at that moment at the scene itself. In the photograph we can examine the complexities of a single exposure-moment as long as we like. At the scene itself we had only 1/100th of a second in which to experience that 1/100th of a second. If 1/100ths of a second are not critical in behavioral analysis with our present analytic techniques, it can be demonstrated that 1/10ths of a second some­ times are. The important point for the behavioral scientist is not that the photograph has more information but that it has different information. This different information is available to the person trained to use photography to this end and who is trained to find and analyze this informa­ tion in a collection of photographs. In order to describe more meaningfully what new orders of significance or information we learn to deal with in the photography class, it is necessary to describe briefly some of the demonstration-experiments we carry out. The equipment we use is minimal. We use 35 mm (Ieica-size) cameras, medium price lenses, drugstore film, available room light sometimes with newspapers a's re­ flectors. We use light meters carefully and keep records of procedures. Film processing is carried out in kitchens or bathrooms-as is the making of contact sheets of all rolls. We begin by exploring the inherent limits of the materials to undel'Stand the possibilities of present day photo­ graphic technology and materials. Few students know, for example, that it is possible under these "field" conditions to photograph a page of a newspaper from the normal minimum focussing distance of about three feet with ordinary fast film (Kodak Tri-X) and produce a print in which all the text is readable. (They also discovered that carelessness at any of several point,s in the process can degrade or destroy the newsprint "information" in their photograph.) Students do not themselves make en­ largements in the course but enlargements are made else­ where to illustrate the degree of detail that could be handled photographically with our equipment. For the first human-subject demonstration we pose­ or simply sit-a person in front of a camera on a tripod. We use a lens (90 or 135 mm which enables us to fill each frame with a head-and-shoulders. The camera is focussed and the shutter and diaphragm settings are de­ termined and set. The ,subject is given no instructions except to keep in range of the camera in both lateral and focus distance directions. The subject, a student, is usual­ ly embarrassed and uncomfortable, but willing. The class may try either to sooth or needle the subject but this is not important to the results. One student is instructed to step to the camera and, watching through the view­ finder. take a half-roll of pictures of the subject. The student-photographer gives no instructions to his subject who is mostly engaged in interchanges with others in the class. Photographs are taken fairly quickly-perhaps as rapidly as one-per-second or as slowly at one-per-ten 81 seconds. When the first student has taken half a roll, another steps to the camera and takes another half-rolL (Although the students are encouraged to bring cameras to class and photograph each other and class proceedings casually, it is important not to use class members as subjects in demonstration-experiments. Analysis and discussion of a set of photographs can seem uncomfortably like analysis and discussion of the subject person. This either inhibits analysis and discussion or pro­ duces possible embarrassment. Subjects should be people outside the class.) The class also discovers that whereas they can often recognize and handle -small "informational" differences, they can usually not talk in other than loose and sub­ jective-interpretive language about what they are seeing. As we proceed, individual students are able to derive "meaning" from these comparisons or contrasts but it is always "meaning" in terms of some particular conceptual framework. It is not a simple inventory of observations that is equally meaningful to others. Sometimes several students can agree among themselves on what they see but none can be precisely verbal about it. We have experimented with father-vs.-mother pictures of a child, man-vs.-woman pictures of a man, man-vs.­ woman pictures of a woman, etc. We have even intro­ duced series taken by a "machine" in which the shutter was repeatedly set .to operate by self-timer (the photog­ rapher or operator released the self-timer and the ex­ posure follows about 15 seconds later). This produces a series of pictures for all intents and purposes humanly unselected as to exact moment. Eventually most of the students can examine two or three sets of photographs of the same subject (same "pose," exposure, angle, etc.) and analyze differences to the point of saying This series was taken by the woman and that by the man, [or] These are the machine shots and these are the child's shots. I t is less important that the student learn to identify the photographer than that he learns to see minimal but significant differences and that he can find pattern and meaning in these differences. The question then arises: are the differences those of the subject or the photographer? When we can establish informational difference in two sets of photographs, what is the source of the information? One can say that the information must be introduced by the photographer since the subject was the same for both with only "ran­ dom" possible differences. It is probably not important to answer the question in this form, but it is important to understand with some explicitness that photography is a human transaction and not" simple record. The par­ ticular fraction of a second one selects to push a shutter button is partly culturally conditioned and partly the product of an idiosyncratic personality. Even if two skill­ ful and intelligent photographers are instructed to take 82 HUMAN ORGANIZATION precisely the same kind of photographs of some inter­ action, the results will be different to a skillful analyst. This suggests not only that refined analytic techniques can be applied to still photographs but that the nature of the photographing or being-photographed process can offer a new possible use for still photography in behavioral science. If an adequately capable member of a group under study (whether an African Bushman, a city youth­ gang member, a child, or an upper-middle-class parent) is given a camera with instructions to take an ample series of photographs of a given subject (subject, here, need not be very specific), the trained photograph-analyst will be able to make inferences of the views, images, or theories that the photographer is wittingly or unwitting­ ly trying to support. What of all the possible time-slices and angles does he select by pushing the shutter button at one moment as against another? Americans, for ex­ ample tend to take "American" pictures of people look­ , . B ing like Americans with American expressIOns. ut we then attempt to get a pictorial representation of, for ex­ ample, African Bushmen by filtering the record through the cultural conditioning of the non-Bushman photogra­ pher. This has meant that we have had to limit the uses of still photography in anthropology to those kinds of pictorial record that are not significantly influenced by the special cultural filters of the photographer. (Without, for the most part, any systematic way to determine whether the record has or has not been significantly changed by the photographer.) And for those kinds of recording in behavioral research that involve micro­ analysis or analysis in terms of more subtle or smaller bits of behavior, still photography has been almost useless because the special bias of the photographer is known to skew the results. If a student or researcher has a more precise understanding of photography in terms of the nature of the human transactions involved and has ac­ quired skill at sorting for comparable data in multiple still photographs, he will be able to design useful and specific data-gathering procedures by planning in advance to analyze the differences by shifting the relations of control and variable and thus changing "skewing" to possible data. All this is to say that the city youth-gang member or the African Bushman may offer the researcher a more precisely structured and accessible interpretation of his culture through the photographs he will take of it than through the words he may talk about it or, perhaps, even through the inferences the observer may make from his behavior. At least it is possible to investigate new kinds of cultural or sub-cultural material in this way. .The class "experiments" are not usually carried out in strict experimental style. Instead they are designed to help the students discover that: 1) Small, minimal and often obscure differences may be significant. 2) One can learn to "see" these differences. 3) These differences are not significant per se but become significant when they can be related to a pattern. 4) All photographs are products of an interaction between photographer and subject and all view­ ing is interactional between viewer and photo­ graph. S) Information reported by any single viewer may have been introduced by anyone or more of the several interactional entities involved (subject, photographer, viewer, or even anyone outside this trio who exerts an influence indirectly such as an editor, the requests of a colleague, the effort to please a subject, etc.). 6) The information in a still photograph exists on many levels but training and the systematic relating of photographic information to a careful analytic technique can keep the levels separated and the information available and useful. In the class we usually avoid discussions of meaning and interpretation but sometimes these orders of state­ ment are offered and there is often disagreement. Often it is possible to point out that they are not disagreeing about the information but are reporting discoveries on different levels or relating them to different conceptual frameworks. Finding information in a photograph is not a matter of finding objects but rather in finding mean­ ingful, patterned reia tionships. Since different viewers, in the absence of a common hypothesis or framework may see relationships of differ­ ent shapes, it becomes necessary to evolve some syste­ matic way to cross-reference the informational material so that a photograph is not a problematic assortment of information but becomes specific in meaningful scientific communication. Photography, to the class, becomes more than a recording or illustrating tool. It becomes observation itself. When one has learned to use a camera to abstract from an interaction particular moments that are relevant to some analysis of that interaction, the -still-photography technique can be expanded to embrace a more or less broad spectrum of levels-events, photographically speak­ ing, that are taking place simultaneously or are over­ lapping. That is, the anthropological photographer can approach a scene with a more generalized sensitivity and take a relatively large number of only semi-specific photographs for later sorting and analysis. With this body of still photographs (perhaps several hundred) of such a thing, for example, as brief courtship encounters on street corners, it becomes possible to sort them for any number of particular aspects such as gestural echoing, postural parallelism or complementarity, location of events, stance, behavior of passerby or onlookers, etc. This is a matter of relatively quick shooting geared to a photographic response that is not quite calculated at each shot but geared to affect or movement rhythms. One will discover that fingers on a shutter-button will record HUMAN ORGANIZATION small significant fragments of time that culturally con­ Jitioned eyes may avoid or censor. It is sometimes said that entire continuous events can most easily be swept into the record with a moving-pic­ ture camera. But this will be a record in the same time­ dimension as the original event against which, on viewing it, one must again match his culturally conditioned see­ ing. And the movie record is not one that can easily be chopped up to re-sort single frames in search of new in­ sights or to compare non-contiguous bits. While single­ frames can be viewed by stopping a projector, it is diffi­ cult to arrange to view several single frames simultaneous­ ly for comparison or contrast. The movie record is used for a different kind of data recording. All this is, perhaps, a far cry from the point of view of the simple field "illustration" and is in contradiction to some of the mythology about photography. In an excellent survey-report of common attitudes toward and uses of photography in anthropology made by Oswald Werner for a masters degree thesis,3 he says that The aim of good field photography is an undis­ torted view, as close as pos'sible to the undisturbed "reality" of the people under study. This presupposes that there is an undistorted view. It is probably more useful to regard behavior as an infinite succession of views among which "distortion" is simply a moment that is atypical to the informant viewer. There are, of course, no real atypical moments unless one uses some statistical criterion and mistakes behavior-in-time with the learned way we usually see behavior. The nodal points in human rhythms that we pull out to "see" are what we call typical. For behavioral analysis it is often the in-hetw('('n mom('nts that we would find most useful. Margaret Mead, in this regard, has cautioned photog­ raphers to remember that the camera pointed at the rooms and contrivances built to demonstrate human perceptual distortions will record the same effects that eyes see: distortions. The camera will itself neither "dis­ tort" nor "correct distortion." Pictures and photographs for publication or communi­ cation are often selected or rejected, called typical or atypical, good or bad in terms of some criteria of the investigator, not in terms of the behavior itself. Typical! atypical are more often statements about the people who use them, or about contexts, than about behavior or photographs of behavior. If/when we study "seeing" cross-culturally, we are almost certain to find that the nodal points of behavior that are "seen" are not only somewhat different individually but very different cross­ ,culturally. As every professional photographer knows, the so-called accidental photographs taken at the "wrong moments" are often more revealing about subjects than the "right" ones. One may think in terms of distorted­ 3. Oswald Werner, Ethllograpllic Photography, (abstract of M. A. thesis), Syracuse University, 1961. 83 un~istorte~, right-wrong, typical-atypical for purposes of Iilustrauon. But for the analysis of behavior we can profitably find a way to study the in-between moments. The anthro~ol.ogical pho~og~aphe.r may be trained to get them even If It means lInkIng hIs camera to a machine or giving his camera to one of his subjects of study. 'Verner also reports that Good. re~?rding techniques should enable the anthro­ pologIst to brIng part of the field home" for further study by him as well as by other scientists. This is certainly true, particularly in relation to movie records. But to integrate direct observation with photo­ graphic observation in the field one needs to go beyond the "bring it home" use. It is astonishing to find that many field anthropologists do not process their own still photographs to the point of seeing them (making contact prints) in the field and are, therefore, unable to relate their photography to their personal experience of the photographed event more closely than the weeks or m~nths it may take to send film away to a lab and get prInts. back. If one uses photography only for illustrating what IS already seen and recorded, it is not essential to have the print immediately.. But insofar as photography becomes ?bservation itself, it is a great disadvantage to have avaIlable only part of one's observations, the other part having been seot away unseen until months later. To bring part of the field home is one thing. To send an important part of one's observations home unseen is another. A third common vIew of photography IS reflected In this sen tence: Tt ta kes an extremely skillful person (anthropologist) to record an event photographically without serious disturbance of the natural rhythms and smooth flow of events. This problem and the nature of the skill involved is greatly misunderstood. We tend to suppose that cameras are inherently frightening or interfering instruments whereas notebooks, pencils, and observing anthropologists are not. It is not the camera itself that interferes or dis­ turbs, it is the photographer using one who has not learned to put his camera activity into social situations easily and comfortably. If the photographer regards his camera as a mechanical instrument that exists outside himself, he must interpose it between himself and his subjects. With this interposition he can interrupt, change, or destroy situations. He can ridicule, irritate, or em­ barras people. Or he can admire, support, and reassure his subjects. What the photographer does with his camera is not essentially a matter of his technical facility, it is a matter of his own feeling about the photographic process and how he communicates this to the subject. If the photographer feels that the camera is a privacy-invading machine, the subjects will respond accordingly. Anthro­ pologists could, as some journalists do, frighten people or 84 HUMAN ORGANIZATION embarrass them with only notebooks and pencils. But they learn to present these things as extensions of them­ selves, and to the extent to which the anthropologist succeeds in relating himself to his subjects, his instru­ mentation can become related, too. But if the anthropolo­ gist (or any camera-user) feels awkward about the use of his intrument, the instrument will relate badly to the subjects. Photographers do not always realize the effect they may have on their subjects. In the photography course an effort is made to sensitize the students to the manner in which they are relating to subjects and sometimes the students take pictures of each other taking pictures in group situations by way of discovering how they are re­ lating to their subjects. 4 In the course students are urged to carry their cameras constantly from the beginning. They are forbidden to "sneak" pictures, but at the end of the course many of them are taking pictures in libraries, on subways, and in crowds with some ease. But it is not a simple matter to acquire the ease and learn the subtle signals that make subjects comfortable and "deliberately unaware." The problems are essentially psychological, not technological, 4. For a discussion of the sensItive use of the camera in the photographer-subject relationship see: Paul Byers, "Ken Heyman," in Tom Maloney (ed.), U. S. Camera '62, U. S. Camera Publishing Corp., New York, 1961, p. 88. and they are problems of the camera-user, not the sub­ jects. It is my opinion that there is much symbolic be­ havior involved in using cameras and that learning to use one in a useful anthropological way requires the in­ dividual guidance of a teacher-observer who can recognize what a student is doing to his subjects with his camera. Conclusion Training in photography for the behavioral scientist, then, should include competence in the technology, learn­ ing to see as a visual acuity related to the photographic search for information, understanding the interactional involvements of subject-photographer-viewer, translating visual images into their photographic counterparts, seeing new kinds of information in photographs, learning to search photographs for comparable pieces of data and to handle them systematically, understanding what others see as part of the semantic problem in photographic com­ munication, and relating to cameras and thence to sub­ jects in the process of taking photographs. When the tools and results of still photography arc handled in these ways, it will be possible to deal syste­ matically and communicatively with behavioral taxo­ nomies beyond the reach of words alone with a validity that can ultimately be expressed in the verbal literacy of the discipline. The photograph will become an event to be acquired and analyzed for its own kind of information and data.