PERCEPTIONS OF AN ACQUANTANCE RAPE: THE RAMIFICATIONS OF VICTIM’S WEIGHT, SEXUAL HISTORY AND CLOTHING A Thesis Presented to the faculty of the Department of Psychology California State University, Sacramento Submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS in Psychology (Counseling Psychology) by Mary Dorene Bell SPRING 2013 PERCEPTIONS OF AN ACQUANTANCE RAPE: THE RAMIFICATIONS OF VICTIM’S WEIGHT, SEXUAL HISTORY AND CLOTHING A Thesis by Mary Dorene Bell Approved by: __________________________________, Committee Chair Lisa Harrison, Ph.D. __________________________________, Second Reader Rebecca Cameron, Ph.D. __________________________________, Third Reader Kelly Cotter, Ph.D. _____________________________ Date ii Student: Mary Dorene Bell I certify that this student has met the requirements for format contained in the University format manual, and that this thesis is suitable for shelving in the Library and credit is to be awarded for the thesis. , Graduate Coordinator JianJian Qin, Ph.D. Date Department of Psychology iii Abstract of PERCEPTIONS OF AN ACQUANTANCE RAPE: THE RAMIFICATIONS OF VICTIM’S WEIGHT, SEXUAL HISTORY AND CLOTHING by Mary Dorene Bell College students (N = 168) from northern California examined the influence of victim weight, clothing, and sexual history on empathy, blame, and rape myth acceptance toward a perpetrator and victim in an acquaintance rape. Participants read a fictional interview of a rape victim describing events prior to the rape and completed questionnaires. Victim weight (thin versus obese), clothing (revealing versus modest), and sexual history (virgin versus promiscuous) were manipulated within the vignettes. While participants believed the interview described a rape, no significant difference for blame, empathy, or rape myth acceptance was found for the victim. For the perpetrator, participants placed more blame and responsibility on the perpetrator of the obese victim, and had higher empathy toward the perpetrator of the virgin victim. Results are discussed in terms of blame attributions. Future research should continue to analyze the effect of weight in cases of rape and increase awareness of rape biases. , Committee Chair Lisa Harrison, Ph.D. Date iv DEDICATION I would like to dedicate this thesis to my son, Samuel. He makes every day a joy. I want him to grow up in a world where everyone is equal and has the same opportunities. I want him to grow up in a safe world. I hope that by continuing to do studies like this, the world will become a better place, because we can learn what triggers blame and improve our treatment of victims. I never want anyone to be afraid to seek out help. While I hope my son never experiences any type of abuse or assault, I want him to feel safe to come forward, if he were to. I would also like to dedicate this thesis to all past, current, and future victims of rape. You are never at fault or to blame. Only perpetrators are to blame. It does not matter how one dresses or acts, rape is never the victims fault. Please seek out help. And to all victims, “I believe you.” v ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I would like to thank Dr. Lisa Harrison for all her help as my thesis chair. She gave me encouragement when I needed it, and supported me the whole way through. I would like to thank Dr. Rebecca Cameron for being my second reader and Dr. Kelly Cotter for being my third reader. I would also like to think my mother, Brenda, for her help and encouragement. She always believed in me and helped push me to finish my degree. I am eternally grateful to all of you for your support. vi TABLE OF CONTENTS Page Dedication ..................................................................................................................... v Acknowledgements ..................................................................................................... vi List of Tables ................................................................................................................ x List of Figures ............................................................................................................. xi Chapter 1. INTRODUCTION………………………………………………………………….1 Prevalence of Rape ........................................................................................... 3 Brief History of Rape and Laws ....................................................................... 6 Factors that Influence Rape Judgments .......................................................... 10 Rape Myths ..................................................................................................... 16 Rape Empathy ................................................................................................. 18 Present Study .................................................................................................. 20 2. METHOD ............................................................................................................. 24 Participants ...................................................................................................... 24 Design ............................................................................................................ 24 Materials ......................................................................................................... 25 Procedure ....................................................................................................... 28 3. RESULTS ............................................................................................................. 30 Effects of Victim Clothing on Victim Judgment ............................................ 30 Effects of Victim Clothing on Perpetrator Judgment ..................................... 31 vii Effects of Victim Weight on Victim Judgment .............................................. 32 Effects of Victim Weight on Perpetrator Judgment ....................................... 34 Effects of Victim Sexual History on Victim Judgment .................................. 35 Effects of Victim Sexual History on Perpetrator Judgment ............................ 37 Interaction Effects of Weight, Clothing, and Sexual History on Victim Judgment ......................................................................................................... 38 Interaction Effects of Weight, Clothing, and Sexual History on Perpetrator Judgment ......................................................................................................... 40 Vignette Describing Rape .............................................................................. 41 4. DISCUSSION ....................................................................................................... 42 Effects of Victim Clothing on Victim Judgment ............................................ 42 Effects of Victim Clothing on Perpetrator Judgment ..................................... 44 Effects of Victim Weight on Victim Judgment .............................................. 44 Effects of Victim Weight on Perpetrator Judgment ....................................... 45 Effects of Victim Sexual History on Victim Judgment .................................. 47 Effects of Victim Sexual History on Perpetrator Judgment ............................ 47 Effects of Weight, Clothing, and Sexual History on Victim Judgment ......................................................................................................... 48 Effects of Weight, Clothing, and Sexual History on Perpetrator Judgment ......................................................................................................... 50 Vignette Describing Rape .............................................................................. 51 viii Limitations ...................................................................................................... 52 Future Research ............................................................................................. 53 Conclusions and Implications ........................................................................ 55 Appendix A. Participant Characteristics ................................................................... 57 Appendix B. Vignettes .............................................................................................. 59 Appendix C. Blame Index ...................................................................................... 103 Appendix D. Empathy Scale ................................................................................... 105 Appendix E. Illinois Rape Myth Acceptance Scale ................................................ 108 Appendix F. Consent Form ..................................................................................... 111 Appendix G. Debriefing.......................................................................................... 112 References ................................................................................................................. 114 ix LIST OF TABLES Tables Page 1. Mean Victim Judgments as a Function of Victim Clothing ........................... 31 2. Mean Perpetrator Judgments as a Function of Victim Clothing ..................... 32 3. Mean Victim Judgments as a Function of Victim Weight .............................. 33 4. Mean Perpetrator Judgments as a Function of Victim Weight ....................... 35 5. Mean Victim Judgments as a Function of Victim Sexual History.................. 36 6. Mean Perpetrator Judgments as a Function of Victim Sexual History ........... 38 x LIST OF FIGURES Figures 1. Page Significant interaction effect of victim sexual history and clothing for the perpetrator being justified in raping the victim ......................................... 41 xi 1 CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION … much of what was actuality of our lives went unnamed. We did not speak of rape. If a woman was raped she was supposed to feel ashamed. She was shamed. The very atmosphere around her said that she wanted this rape, drawn this to her. And the atmosphere, like rape itself, seemed as if it had existed forever, were a natural phenomenon, and not made up of man-made assum0ptions and prejudices born of a particular social reality (Cuklanz, 1996, p. 14; Griffin, 1979, p. 25). This quote describes something that one in six women will experience in their lifetime (Rape, Abuse & Incest National Network [RAINN], 2009; Tjaden & Thoennes, 1998). During a hearing on the lack of reporting and investigating of rape cases, Senator Arlen Specter began by describing the statistics on rape in the United States, where over 20 million women or 18% of the population have been a victim of rape (United States, 2011). While women continue to be victimized at an alarming rate, few will report their crime to the police (Campbell et al., 1999). A major barrier to reporting is the blame and judgment victim’s experiences when reporting the rape to police, friends, and family (Campbell, 2006, 2008; Campbell et al., 1999; Cuklanz, 1996; George & Martinez, 2002; Hockett, Saucier, Hoffman, Smith, & Craig, 2009; Tjaden & Thoennes, 2006). For example, many victims are judged on how they were dressed (Denim Day USA, 2012; Hall, 1995; Moorti, 2002; Travis, 2003) and whether they were drinking alcohol prior to 2 the rape (Abbey & Harnish, 1995; Cuklanz, 1996; Finch & Munro, 2005; Krahe, Temkin, Bieneck, & Berger, 2008), among other things. Even the media and society has vilified victims of rape. For instance, when an eleven year old girl was gang-raped by eighteen men, the media reported only the ages of the alleged perpetrators, but described the victim as someone who “dressed older than her age, wearing makeup and fashions more appropriate to a woman in her 20s” and “would hang out with teenage boys” (McKinley, 2011, para. 12). These types of views take the blame of rape away from the perpetrator and place it solely on the victim. Instead of holding perpetrators accountable, the victims are expected to justify all actions taken before, during, and after their rape. Research on rape blame attributions has extensively looked at the victim’s sexual history (Brown, Hamilton, & O’Neill, 2007; Temkin & Krahe, 2008), race (Aosved & Long, 2006; George & Martinez, 2002; Varelas & Foley, 1998), clothing (Brown et al., 2007; Klein, Kennedy, & Gorzalka, 2009; Shears, 2010), and alcohol/drug use (Abbey & Harnish, 1995; Cuklanz, 1996; Krahe et al., 2008), but little research has been conducted examining victim weight (Clarke & Stermac, 2011; Ryckman, Graham, Thornton, Gold, & Lindner, 1998). Since some people believe that rape is caused by a lack of control (Martin, 2005), it seems plausible that weight would influence their beliefs in a rape scenario. For instance, some people view women wearing provocative clothing as causing their own rape since they are viewed as irresistible (Abbey et al., 1987; Brown et al., 2007; Hall, 1995; Martin, 2005; Maurer & Robinson, 2007). In the case of weight, a 3 thin rape victim may be seen as irresistible, while an obese victims may be viewed differently, which could influence blame attributions in their rape cases. Prevalence of Rape For the purposes of this study, rape is defined as follows: The penetration, no matter how slight, of the vagina or anus with any body part or object, or oral penetration by a sex organ of another person, without the consent of the victim. (Federal Bureau of Investigation [FBI], 2012, para. 1). This definition was chosen because rape laws vary from state to state and study to study. It represents the federal government’s definition of rape. In the United States, one out of every six women will be raped in their lifetime (RAINN, 2009; Tjaden & Thoennes, 1998). This equals out to be approximately 17.7 million women (Rennison, 2002). However, a more recent report by the US Senate states that the total number of rape victims in the United States is approximately 20 million, which equals 18% of women in the United States (United States, 2011). The most common form of rape is penetration of the vagina by the penis (68.2%), followed by penetration of the vagina or anus by objects or fingers (31.3%) (Tjaden & Thoennes, 2006). When examining the race/ethnicity of rape victims, studies indicate that American Indian women report significantly higher lifetime rates of rape (34%) than all other racial/ethnic groups (18%) (Tjaden & Thoennes, 1998, 2006) and that one out of three American Indian women report being raped in their lifetime (United States, 2011). This is believed to be related to the higher rates of violence that occurred in the Native American 4 population (Tjaden & Thoennes, 1998, 2006). In these same studies, Asian American women were found to be significantly less likely than other racial/ethnic groups to report being raped (Tjaden & Thoennes, 1998, 2006). However, some caution should be used when viewing these results because of the small sample size of American Indian and Asian American populations (Tjaden & Thoennes, 1998, 2006). While it is possible that the Asian American population has a significantly lower rape rate, it is also possible the reported rate is affected by cultural values that prohibit sharing information with others that could cause shame or dishonor the family (Lee, Pomeroy, Yoo, & Rheinboldt, 2005; Schneider, Mori, Lambert, & Wong, 2009; Tjaden & Thoennes, 2006). In regards to other racial/ethnic groups, African American (18.8%), White American (17.9%), and Latina (14.6%) women report a similar lifetime rate of rapes (Tjaden & Thoennes, 1998, 2006). Victim age also influences rape prevalence rates. For example, girls under the age of 18 are more likely to be raped than women over 18 years of age (Buchwald, Fletcher, & Roth, 2005; Kilpatrick, Edmunds, & Seymour, 1992; Tjaden & Thoennes, 1998, 2006). In fact, rape is most likely to occur between the ages of 10 and 29 for females (Brownmiller, 1975; Buchwald et al., 2005; Thornhill & Palmer, 2000), with 54% of rape victims experiencing their first rape before the age of 18 and 29% between the ages of 18 to 24 (Kilpatrick et al., 1992; Tjaden & Thoennes, 2006). Numerous studies have shown that women, who have been raped once, are more likely to be raped again (Brown, Testa, & Messman-Moore, 2009; Tjaden & Thoennes, 1998, 2006; Yeater, Treat, Viken, & McFall, 2010). Some research suggests this is linked to a reduced ability to recognize risk 5 or the inability to recognize clues that suggest a high risk of victimization (Yeater et al., 2010). Few studies have examined the characteristics of the rapists such as their age, ethnicity, or their attitudes on rape (Brownmiller, 1975; Bourke, 2007; Tjaden & Thoennes, 1998, 2006). However, the limited available research indicates that typically a rapist is likely to share the victim’s same social class and ethnicity (Brownmiller, 1975; United States, 2011). Rapists are also likely to be about the same age as the rape victim (Brownmiller, 1975; United States, 2011). In addition, most rape victims are familiar with their rapist, with only 20 to 40 percent of rape of victims being raped by a stranger (Buchwald et al., 2005; Hall, 1995; Martin, 2005; Rand, 2009; Tjaden & Thoennes, 2006). Usually rape victims are raped by a current or former spouse, boyfriend, friend, relative, or acquaintance (Buchwald et al., 2005; Martin, 2005; Tjaden & Thoennes, 2006; United States, 2011). Lastly, most rapes occur in a private setting (84.5%) such as a home or car (Tjaden & Thoennes, 2006). This is contradictory to most peoples’ expectations that a typical rape takes place outside, in the dark, and with the use of physical force (Anderson, 2007; Brownmiller, 1975; Chasteen, 2001; Hall, 1995). Despite this misperception, few studies have been conducted to determine why this myth persists (Anderson, 2007). In general, most women are not threatened with a weapon or beaten during the rape (Anderson, 2007; Holleran, Beichner, & Spohn, 2010; McGregor, 2005; Rand, 2009; Tjaden & Thoennes, 2006). This could be because women are usually raped by someone known to them so the perpetrator uses coercion instead of physical force. When a woman 6 is assaulted during a rape, it usually involves a slap or hit (Tjaden & Thoennes, 1998, 2006), with 32% of victims being injured during the rape (Buchwald et al., 2005; Tjaden & Thoennes, 1998; Travis, 2003). The most common injuries in rape were scratches and bruises (Tjaden & Thoennes, 1998, 2006). Brief History of Rape and Laws Prior to the 1970’s, the concept of rape was not generally viewed as a social or criminal problem in the United States’ society (Buck Doude, 2008; Chasteen, 2001) and rarely was discussed by the public, media, or researchers (Chasteen, 2001). During this time, rape was seen as a private issue which may be linked to historical beliefs that women were the property of their fathers or husbands (Catty, 1999; Cuklanz, 1996; McGregor, 2005). As such, the focus of proving a rape and the effects of rape differed greatly from present-day rape cases. In the past, in order for a father to marry off his daughter, or a husband to believe his children were biologically his own, he had to also believe his daughter or wife was a virtuous woman. A major factor in determining virtuousness was chastity. Chastity was considered highly important, helped a woman to be viewed as trustworthy, and gave her a good reputation (Cuklanz, 1996; Hall, 1995; McGregor, 2005). This virtue played a critical factor in rape trials because it influenced whether a rapist was believed to be guilty for the rape and whether a victim was believed to be trustworthy. This meant that not only did women fear being raped, but they also feared not being believed if they did not uphold society’s standard for chastity. 7 From 1671 until 1974, all juries in the United States were sent to deliberations of rape trials after the reading of the following quote by English Lord Chief Justice Matthew Hale: It is true that rape is the most detestable crime, and therefore ought severely and impartially to be punished with death; but it must be remembered, that it is an accusation easily to be made and hard to be proved, and harder to be defended by the party accused, tho never so innocent (Cuklanz, 1996, p. 19; Hall, 1995, p. 47; Martin, 2005, p. 59; McGregor, 2005, p. 31; Moorti, 2002, p. 50). While this quote was meant to be a warning to the jury, it also sent the message that women are often untrustworthy and that the jurors should be highly skeptical of claims made by purported rape victims. Besides questioning the motives of the rape victims, this type of warning also influenced how jurors would evaluate the evidence in a rape trial if the case was even brought to trial. Until 1977, rape was considered a serious offense punishable by death (Hall, 1995). However, very few men were ever convicted of rape because of how the laws were established in the United States. Because chastity was considered a high virtue, women were expected to fight to the death, although not legally required, to prevent the loss of their chastity and the shame of this loss (Hall, 1995; McGregor, 2005). Thus, rape laws required that the rape victim fight back with ‘utmost resistance’ (Cuklanz, 1996; Hall, 1995; McGregor, 2005), which meant the rape victim was required to physically fight her attacker, or else it would be considered consensual sex. Because it was assumed 8 that a woman would fight to the death to stop the rape if a woman stopped resisting it was assumed she had consented to sex (Hall, 1995; Moorti, 2002). In addition to the ‘utmost resistance’ requirement, historically a rape victim was required to prove she had fought back, yelled for help, had a good reputation, and optimally have witnesses to corroborate her story (Cuklanz, 1996; Hall, 1995). Part of the reasoning for this position was based on the popular idea by a prominent psychiatrist who stated that: Most women entertain more or less consciously at one time or another fleeting fantasies or fears that they are being or will be attacked by a man. Of course, the normal woman who has such fantasies does not confuse it with reality but it is … easy for some neurotic individuals to translate their fantasies into actual beliefs and memory falsifications (Cuklanz, 1996, p. 26; Juviler, 1960, p. 674). Thus, it was thought that men had to be protected from women’s moments of fantasy or confusion in delineating consensual sex and rape. Because of this distrust of women, a rape victim’s history of sexual encounters was allowed to be presented during rape cases (Brownmiller, 1975; Cuklanz, 1996; Hall, 1995). The handling of rape cases in the United States began to change in the 1970s because of changes to social and political structure. This change was influenced by the feminist movement that sought to empower women and end violence against women (Escobar & Hill, 2009). During this movement, rape began to be acknowledged as a major social and health problem that negatively affected women (Buck Doude, 2008; Tjaden & Thoennes, 1998, 2006). For feminists, rape is seen as a product of male 9 dominance (Buck Doude, 2008; Burt, 1980; Chasteen, 2001; Glick & Fiske, 1996), gender role stereotypes (Buck Doude, 2008; Burt, 1980; Chasteen, 2001; Glick & Fiske, 1996), power (Brownmiller, 1975; Buck Doude, 2008; Chapleau, Oswald, & Russell, 2007; Chasteen, 2001; Glick & Fiske, 1996; Hockett et al., 2009; Thornhill & Palmer, 2000), and control (Brownmiller, 1975; Buck Doude, 2008; Chapleau et al., 2007; Chasteen, 2001; Glick & Fiske, 1996; Hockett et al., 2009; Thornhill & Palmer, 2000). Others have argued that rape is a consequence of men being unable to control their sexual urges (Martin, 2005). The feminist movement brought rape to the nation’s attention and resulted in the establishment of the National Center for the Prevention and Control of Rape by the National Institute of Mental Health in 1975 (Moorti, 2002). With this national focus, federal and state laws were changed to help protect rape victims by the passing of rape shield laws, removing the ‘utmost resistance’ clause in rape laws, and the removal of the spousal rape exemption (Bourke, 2007; Cuklanz, 1996; Hall, 1995; Martin, 2005; McGregor, 2005; Moorti, 2002; Russell & Bolen, 2000). Rape shield laws have banned the inclusion of sexual history evidence from the rape victims (Cuklanz, 1996). These laws are based on Federal Rule 412, which is designed to “…protect a sexual assault victim against defense counsel’s tactics of presenting degrading and embarrassing disclosure of intimate details about the victim’s private life … for the purpose of biasing the court against her” (Hall, 1995, p. 27). However, some states currently require psychiatric evaluations and lie detectors tests of the rape victims. For instance, a judge in Ohio ordered four teenage rape victims, in separate cases, to take polygraph tests after their perpetrators were convicted of the rapes (Tarrant, 2010). While 10 women are no longer expected to fight their attacker to the death, they still must prove the rape was against their will (McGregor, 2005). In addition, despite changes to spousal rape laws, many states still view spousal rape differently from non-spousal rapes by dispensing shorter sentences in cases of spousal rape or making the rape charges a misdemeanor instead of a felony, as is the case with stranger rapes (Hall, 1995; Russell & Bolen, 2000). Factors that Influence Rape Judgments Victim Sexual History Research has found that many specific characteristics of rape victims influence rape attributions (Deitz & Byrnes, 1981; Jones & Aronson, 1973; Kanekar & Kolsawalla, 1977; Temkin & Krahe, 2008). One such factor is a victim’s moral respectability as viewed by others. In the 1970’s and 1980’s, this variable was measured by examining a woman’s marital status such as whether the rape victim was single, married (Jones & Aronson, 1973; Kanekar & Kolsawalla, 1977) or divorced (Kanekar & Kolsawalla, 1980). Using vignettes, these studies found that married women were perceived to be significantly more responsible for their rape than unmarried virgins (Jones & Aronson, 1973), but their rape perpetrators were sentenced to significantly longer sentences than the rape perpetrators of the unmarried virgins (Jones & Aronson, 1973; Kanekar & Kolsawalla, 1977). The authors of these studies argued that the longer sentences for the married women could be because participants believed the husband and children also suffered when the women were raped (Jones & Aronson, 1973; Kanekar & Kolsawalla, 1977). 11 Studies from the 1990’s to the present, have examined victim moral respectability by having participants judge the woman’s sexual history, such as the number of previous relationships that she had prior to the rape incident (Temkin & Krahe, 2008). For instance, a woman with multiple monogamous relationships back-to-back might be compared to a woman with only two monogamous relationships. Findings indicate that women who are viewed as promiscuous because of having numerous monogamous partners are less likely to be perceived as victims in rape situations (Brown et al., 2007; Temkin & Krahe, 2008). These women are usually blamed for the rape and the men are acquitted because these women are judged by their perceived flawed character rather than the facts of how the rape occurred. For example, in one noted court case, two alleged prostitutes were raped and robbed after eight men broke into the victims’ home (Hall, 1995). The victims were able to identify some of the men, and the case went to trial, but the men were all acquitted. The jurors believed that because the women were suspected to be prostitutes, they could not be trusted to tell the truth. This view is especially true of women who were raped by someone with whom they had sex with previously (Simonson & Subich, 1999; Temkin & Krahe, 2008). In those instances, some people argue that when a woman has previously given consent to sex, then she is more likely to give consent again (Krahe et al., 2008; McMahon-Howard, Clay-Warner, & Renzulli, 2009), so her credibility is questioned about her claim of rape. This type of bias can also be seen in the case of spousal rape. Previously, spouses were legally exempted from being charged with the rape of their spouses because it was believed that a married woman was legally obligated to consent to sexual relations with 12 her husband (Cuklanz, 1996; Hall, 1995). It was not until 1993 that all 50 states removed spousal rape exemption laws from their legislation (Hall, 1995). Despite this removal, numerous states still treat spousal rape and stranger rape differently. For instance, some state laws only allow prosecution of spousal rape when the victim is physically injured during the act of rape (McMahon-Howard et al., 2009). Other states fail to pursue rape charges in instances when the spouse is physically or mentally impaired, unconscious, or asleep because the victim did not have the ability to deny consent to sex. However, in these same states a rape charge can be brought against a perpetrator of stranger rape if the victim was unable to consent due to being physically or mentally impaired. The persistence of leniency in spousal rape may be related to beliefs in rape myths. For instance, some people believe that rape victims are less psychologically damaged when raped by an acquaintance than a stranger (Hall, 1995; Simonson & Subich, 1999; Travis, 2003). In one study, participants examined vignettes describing stranger, date, marital, or acquaintance rape and completed a scale describing their views about the amount of violence during the rape, responsibility for the rape, desire for intercourse, psychological damage caused by the rape, and the level of violation of the victim’s rights (Simonson & Subich, 1999). The participants rated marital rape as significantly less violent and less of a violation of a victim’s rights than stranger rape, despite the only difference in the vignettes being the relationship status between the victim and perpetrator. Victim Clothing Another factor taken into consideration when making judgments about rape, is the type of clothing worn by the victims (Brown et al., 2007; Klein, Kennedy, & Gorzalka, 13 2009; Shears, 2010). Some people who believe rape myths also believe that that rape victims who wear more provocative clothing at the time of the rape are more responsible for their rape because they had “asked for it” (Brown et al., 2007; Hall, 1995; Martin, 2005; Moorti, 2002; Osman, 2003). For example, the host of a show on CNN argued that multiple rape victims at Daytona Beach shared some blame because they were wearing bikinis (Claes, 2010). The manner of dress at the time of the rape is considered important when making decisions about rape because it is believed that women who dress in a promiscuous and seductive manner show poor judgment and must take some responsibility for the rape. Researchers who are interested in examining this phenomenon have used various photographs to study what constitutes provocative dress or clothing (Cassidy & Hurrell, 1995; Whatley, 2005; Workman & Freeburg, 1999). In a study by Workman and Freeburg (1999), the researchers took pictures of a woman with three different skirt lengths, one that was three inches below the knee, one at the knee, and one that was three inches above the knee. The rest of the picture was identical. The findings indicate that men and women believed the rape victim with the shortest skirt was more responsible for the rape than the two longer skirt victims. Other studies varied the type of skirt by adding slits, or by adjusting the victim’s blouse by the number of buttons that were unbuttoned (Abbey, Cozzarelli, McLaughlin, & Harnish, 1987; Johnson & Lee, 2000; Maurer & Robinson, 2007; Whatley, 2005). Participants believed that women in rape cases who wore more provocative clothing were more sexual and were seeking a sexual encounter with the perpetrator (Abbey et al., 1987; Maurer & Robinson, 2007). In one court case, a perpetrator was found not guilty of committing a rape because the 14 victim was wearing tight jeans at the time of the rape (Denim Day USA, 2012; Shears, 2010). The jury stated that it could not have been rape because it would have been too hard for the perpetrator to remove the rape victim’s tight jeans without her assistance and this constituted consent in the eyes of the jurors. However, the victim stated during the trial that the perpetrator ripped off her jeans in the rape attack. Victim Weight With obesity levels on the rise, bias against these victims is also likely to increase. In recent years, obesity levels have continued to increase in the United States, with 36% of its citizens being obese (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention [CDC], 2012). Despite the large number of obese citizens, and continued increase in obesity rates, discrimination and negative views toward obese individuals continue to be widely acceptable (Morrison & O’Connor, 1999; McClure, Puhl, & Heuer, 2011). For instance, obese individuals are typically viewed as unattractive (Legenbauer et al., 2009; Smith, 2012), lacking self-control (McClure et al., 2011), and lazy (Crandall, 1994; Gapinski, Schwartz, & Brownell, 2006; McClure et al., 2011). The media has also contributed to the biases against obese individuals by portraying them in negative and stereotypical images when discussing obesity in the news, and by placing obese actors and actresses in negative and stereotypical roles (Lin & Reid, 2009; McClure et al., 2011). These biases can have damaging effects on obese individuals. For instance, one study found that participants would be less likely to hire an obese applicant, would start the obese applicant at a lower wage if hired, and would be less likely to promote an obese applicant (O’Brien et al., 2008). Even mental health 15 professionals are not immune to these biases. While most mental health professionals agreed that the thin and obese clients described in a vignette would benefit from therapy, they assigned the obese client more negative psychological symptoms, like self-injurious behavior, suspiciousness, and egocentrism (Young & Powell, 1985). Despite the prejudices held against obese individuals, few studies have examined the influence of victim weight on rape judgments (Clarke & Stermac, 2011; Ryckman et al., 1998). The two studies that have examined the effect of victim weight on blame found conflicting results, with the obese victim being blamed more in one study (Ryckman et al., 1998) and the thin victim being blamed more in the other study (Clarke & Stermac, 2011). One explanation for these differences is the description of the victim and perpetrator characteristics. Ryckman et al. (1998) compared the size of the rape victim and perpetrator, while only the size of the victim was described in Clarke and Stermac’s study (2011). Another explanation is that participants judged the victims based on their perceived level of sexual activity (Clarke & Stermac, 2011), since participants might have viewed thin victims as more attractive and thus more sexually experienced. Therefore, it might have been believed that the thin victims should have been more careful or were deliberately seeking a sexual encounter. Because weight has been found to influence attractiveness (Legenbauer et al., 2009; Smith, 2012) and thus could affect blame in a rape case; studying the effects of blame attributions in rape cases involving women of varying weights could help researchers and judicial courts prepare victims for possible blame and how to counteract those beliefs. 16 Rape Myths Due to the high prevalence of rape and the long-term consequences (Campbell, 2006; Campbell et al., 1999; George & Martinez, 2002; Hall, 1995; RAINN, 2009; Tjaden & Thoennes, 1998, 2006), researchers have been studying why rape victims are continually vilified by the media and public. For instance, recently a Notre Dame student reported to campus police that she had been sexually assaulted by a Notre Dame football player (Henneberger, 2010). Despite the victim and her friend reporting the sexual assault to campus police, the campus police did not begin an investigation into the case or even question the alleged perpetrator for fifteen days. During this time, a school employee threatened the victim, via text message, about her continuing to pursue the case. Soon after, the victim committed suicide. In fact, few perpetrators are ever prosecuted or convicted of rape (Brown et al., 2007, RAINN, 2009; Tjaden & Thoennes, 2006). Research by Tjaden and Thoennes (2006), found that of all the cases of women who were raped after the age of 18, “only 7.8 percent said their rapist was criminally prosecuted, 3.3 percent said their rapist was convicted of a crime, and a mere 2.2 percent said their rapist was incarcerated” (p. 33). Other studies have found similar rates, with only 6.8% of rapists spending any time in jail (Brown et al., 2007; RAINN, 2009). While this is a shocking statistic, it should not be so surprising considering the views that people hold about rape. When the average person envisions a rape, he or she imagines a woman walking down a dimly lit street at night when a man suddenly appears and pulls the woman down an alleyway where he proceeds to rape her (Anderson, 2007; Chasteen, 2001; Ferro, 17 Cermele, & Saltzman, 2008; Varelas & Foley, 1998). However, most women are raped by a known acquaintance (Buck Doude, 2008; Tjaden & Thoennes, 2006; Yeater et al., 2010) in a private area (Tjaden & Thoennes, 2006). For example, a college student was raped in a dormitory by an acquaintance in Sacramento, California (“Accused Rapist,” 2010). While the perpetrator admitted to the police that the victim had told him “no” to his sexual advances, the case was not prosecuted by the Sacramento County District Attorney’s Office. The District Attorney refused to file charges, due to a lack of evidence, because while the perpetrator agreed his first sexual contact with the victim was rape, he argued that shortly thereafter, they had consensual sex. A major reason this perception of rape continues dominate is due the acceptance of general rape myths in society. Rape myths are beliefs or stereotypes involving rape, victims, and perpetrators (Buck Doude, 2008; Burt, 1980; Kahlor & Morrison, 2007). Specifically, these are false beliefs that influence how a person judges a rape; such as who is to blame for this act (Aosved & Long, 2006; Ferro et al., 2008). Examples or rape myths are “women who go to a man’s home after the first date want to have sex,” “only bad women are raped,” and “all women want to be raped” (Brownmiller, 1975; Burt, 1980; Hall, 1995; Moorti, 2002). Studies that have examined rape myths frequently use vignettes to describe a rape situation and then ask participants to judge the responsibility of the rape victim and perpetrator. For example, Deitz and Barnes (1981) asked participants to read a vignette describing an attractive or unattractive janitor or scientist who raped a woman. In this study, details about the woman remained constant in all vignettes. The participants were 18 then asked to determine who was at fault for the rape and to assign a jail sentence. Findings indicated that participants issued shorter sentences for the attractive scientist because they believed the victim caused the rape to occur. They also did not believe that an attractive person would need to rape someone because their attractiveness would more likely cause women to seek them out Rape Empathy Typically researchers who study rape judgments have focused primarily on understanding how blame is attributed. However, in recent years researchers have begun to examine factors that influence empathy for rape perpetrators and victims (Feldman, Ullman, & Denkel-Schetter, 1998; Jimenez & Abreu, 2003; Osman, 2011). Rape empathy is defined as the ability to understand the victim and/or perpetrator of a rape by reflecting on the victim’s and/or perpetrator’s experience, emotions, and actions at the time of the rape (Deitz, Blackwell, Daley, & Bentley, 1982; Osman, 2011; Smith & Frieze, 2003). One study found that participants who shared similar characteristics with a rape victim described in a news article, such as both being college students, had higher empathy toward the victim than those without shared characteristics (Feldman et al., 1998). Similarly participants with prior sexual victimization history were more empathetic toward the rape victim than perpetrator (Miller, Amacker, & King, 2011; Osman, 2011). Interestingly, male participants with prior sexual victimization history showed no difference in empathy levels toward male or female victims (Osman, 2011). In regards to perpetrator empathy, males tend to be significantly more empathetic toward 19 rape perpetrators than they are toward rape victim (Osman, 2011; Sinclair & Bourne, 1998). Most studies analyzing rape empathy have used the Rape Empathy Scale (Deitz et al., 1982; Deitz, Littman, & Bentley, 1984; Jimenez & Abreu, 2003; O’Donohue, Yeater, & Fanetti, 2003). Studies using this scale have found that female participants have higher empathy toward rape victims than do male participants (Jimenez & Abreu, 2003; Sinclair & Bourne, 1998). In addition, participants with low empathy for rape victims view unattractive rape victims as more responsible for the rape and are more likely to blame them than they are to blame attractive victims (Deitz, et al., 1984). While the Rape Empathy Scale has provided some useful data, critics have pointed out that a major problem with this scale is that it is unilateral and thus requires empathizing with only the victim or perpetrator (Osman, 2011; Smith & Frieze, 2003). Another problem is that some of the scale items describe rape myths, such as “I feel that it is (not) possible for a man to rape a woman against her will” (Smith & Frieze, 2003, p. 477). Because rape myths reflect biases held by participants it is possible that these scale items are not actually measuring empathy and thus could skew the results of the scale. In order to fix these problems Smith and Frieze (2003) created the Rape-Victim Empathy Scale and the Rape-Perpetrator Empathy Scale. These scales allow participants to judge empathy for the victim and perpetrator independently of one another. Thus, participants can express empathy with the victim, the perpetrator, or both. Results using this scale have had similar results to other studies on rape empathy, with men showing greater empathy to the perpetrator and women showing greater empathy to the victim (Osman, 2011; Smith 20 & Frieze, 2003). However, when Osman (2011) conducted a study having participants complete the Rape-Victim Empathy Scale and the Rape-Perpetrator Empathy Scale while imagining a woman raped by a man, or a man raped by a woman, she found that while male participants were usually empathetic with the male perpetrator; male participants were more empathic with the female victim than the male victim. In addition, female participants were more empathetic of female perpetrators than male perpetrators. Present Study With one in six women being victims of rape annually (RAINN, 2009) and 36% of the population being obese (CDC, 2012), it is surprising that little research has examined the degree of blame or empathy attributed to obese victims of rape. The present study seeks to examine the effect a victim’s weight can have on a judgment of blame and empathy in a case of acquaintance rape. Furthermore, the influence of victim’s clothing and sexual history will also be analyzed because these two factors have been found to influence participants blame attributions and empathy toward victims of rape (Brown et al., 2007; Hall, 1995; Klein et al., 2009; Shears, 2010; Temkin & Krahe, 2008). Lastly, the research will determine whether victim weight, sexual history, and clothing influences whether the victim is perceived as conforming to general rape myths (Feldman et al., 1998; Jimenez & Abreu, 2003; Osman, 2011). In this study, participants read a vignette that described an acquaintance rape. The victim’s weight (thin or obese), sexual history (chaste or promiscuous), and the type of clothing (revealing or covered) worn prior to the rape were manipulated within the vignettes. After reading the vignettes in each study, participants completed a 21 questionnaire that measured judgments about rape, empathy toward the victim and perpetrator, and rape myths in regard to the rape depicted in the vignettes. Hypothesis 1. Previous research has found that rape victims who wear revealing clothing are likely to be judged negatively (Brown et al., 2007; Hall, 1995; Osman, 2003). Therefore, it is predicted that participants will blame date rape victims more, experience less empathy toward them, and believe the rape victims conform to general rape myths if they wear provocative clothing rather than modest clothing. Hypothesis 2. Previous research has found that judgments of rape perpetrators are influenced by the clothing worn by rape victims (Brown et al., 2007; Hall, 1995; Osman, 2003). Therefore, it is predicted that participants will blame date rape perpetrators less, experience more empathy toward them, and be less likely to believe they conform to general rape myths if the rape victim was wearing provocative clothing rather than modest clothing. Hypothesis 3. Previous research has found that judgments of overweight rape victims are negatively influenced by weight (Clarke & Stermac, 2011; Ryckman et al., 1998) and that general empathy toward obese individuals has been found to be low (Gapinski et al., 2006; Lin & Reid, 2009; Teachman et al. 2003). Therefore, it is predicted that participants will blame obese rape victims more, experience less empathy toward them, and believe the rape victims conform to general rape myths if they are obese rather than thin. Hypothesis 4. Previous research has found that judgments of rape perpetrators are influenced by the weight of their victims (Clarke & Stermac, 2011; Ryckman et al., 22 1998). Therefore, it is predicted that participants will blame date rape perpetrators less, experience more empathy toward them, and be less likely to believe they conform to general rape myths if the rape victim is obese rather than thin. Hypothesis 5. Previous research has found that rape victims who are promiscuous are likely to be judged negatively (Aosved & Long, 2006; Miller et al., 2011; Osman, 2011; Temkin & Krahe, 2008). Therefore, it is predicted that participants will blame date rape victims more, experience less empathy toward them, and believe the rape victims conform to general rape myths if they are promiscuous rather than virgin. Hypothesis 6. Previous research has found that judgments of rape perpetrators are influenced by their victims’ sexual history (Clarke & Stermac, 2011; Ryckman et al., 1998). Therefore, it is predicted that participants will blame date rape perpetrators less, experience more empathy toward them, and be less likely to believe they conform to general rape myths if the rape victim is promiscuous rather than a virgin. Hypothesis 7. While no previous study has been conducted to compare the weight of the rape victim to other victim characteristics, it is predicted that the rape victim’s clothing, sexual history, and weight interact to influence rape judgments. Thus, it is expected that participants will blame date rape victims more, experience less empathy toward them, and believe the rape victims conform to general rape myths if they are obese, promiscuous and wear provocative clothing rather than thin, virgin and wear modest clothing. Hypothesis 8. It is predicted that participants will blame date rape perpetrators less, experience more empathy toward them, and be less likely to believe they conform to 23 general rape myths if the rape victim is obese, promiscuous and wear provocative clothing rather than thin, a virgin and wear modest clothing. Hypothesis 9. Previous research concerning judgments about rape and resistance, have suggested than verbal resistance would influence judgments about a rape scenario. Thus, the researcher expected the scenario to be described as depicting an acquaintance rape. 24 CHAPTER 2 METHOD Participants Participants were 214 students from a northern California university. The participants received class credit for participation in the study, which partially satisfied a requirement in their psychology undergraduate class. Due to failed manipulation checks regarding victim clothing, sexual history, and weight, 46 participants were eliminated from the study. This resulted in a sample of 168 participants (37 men, 130 women, 1 gender unspecified). The participants ranged in age from 17 to 54 years old (M = 21.98, SD = 5.74). The participants racially/ethnically identified as White (n = 65), Black (n = 11), Asian (n = 35), Hispanic (n = 27), Native American (n = 1), Arab American (n = 4), other (n = 22), and did not answer (n = 3). Lastly, the participants identified their religious preference as Protestant/Christian (n = 49), Catholic (n = 49), Hindu (n = 1), Muslim (n = 2), Atheist/Agnostic (n = 13), Buddhist (n = 2), other (n = 12), did not answer (n = 2). Design The experiment was a 2 (Victim weight: thin versus obese) x 2 (Victim clothing: revealing versus modest) x 2 (Victim sexual history: virgin versus promiscuous) between participants design. The dependent variables were victim blame, perpetrator blame, whether participants perceived the incident to be rape, empathy for the victim, empathy 25 for the perpetrator, and participants’ acceptance of rape myths in relation to the victim and perpetrator. Materials Vignettes Eight vignettes were used to depict a fictional acquaintance rape that occurred after a college party. The vignettes were adapted from a study by Harrison, Howerton, Secarea, and Nguyen (2008). The vignettes were exactly the same, except that they varied the victim’s weight, sexual history, and clothing. The victim’s weight was manipulated by describing the victim as thin or obese. The victim’s clothes were manipulated by describing her as wearing either revealing or modest clothes the night of the rape. Lastly, the victim’s sexual history was manipulated by describing the victim as either a virgin or as promiscuous. The vignettes were presented as an interview between a rape victim and an assistant district attorney. In the vignettes, the victim described attending a graduation party with a friend when she met a man she liked. Throughout the evening, the victim and the man danced and talked. When the victim was ready to leave, she discovered that her friend, who had driven them to the party, had already left. The man offered her a ride home and she accepted. After arriving home, she invited him inside the apartment and the two watched television. Eventually she allowed him to kiss her twice. However, when he continued kissing her, she repeatedly said “no” and told him to leave. Despite her protests, he raped her. (see Appendix A) 26 Blame Index To examine participants’ perceptions concerning victim blame, perpetrator blame, and whether the incident was rape, participants completed the Blame Index (see Appendix B). The Blame Index contained 21 Likert-type questions adapted from previous research (Anderson & Bissell, 2011; Langhinrichsen-Rohling & Monson, 1998). Eleven items were used to form the Victim Blame Index. Nine items were used to form the Perpetrator Blame Index. All items used a 7-point scale ranging from Not at all to Very Much. Higher scores indicated that more blame was attributed to the victim or perpetrator. In addition, a single question measured whether participants perceived the incident to be rape (Bridges, 1991). The question was measured with a 7-point Likertscale ranging from Definitely not rape to Definitely rape, with a higher score indicating acceptance of the description of the sexual assault as a rape. Internal reliability for the victim blame measure was acceptable (Cronbach’s alpha = .87). Internal reliability for the perpetrator blame was not unacceptable (Cronbach’s alpha = .64), therefore three items were separately used in the analysis. Empathy Scales To examine participants’ empathy toward the rape victim and the perpetrator (see Appendix C), participants were asked to complete the Rape-Victim Empathy Scale and the Rape-Perpetrator Empathy Scale (Smith & Frieze, 2003). Each measure contained 18 Likert-type statements that were measured with a 7-point Likert scale ranging from Strongly disagree to Strongly agree. A lower score indicated higher empathy. This scale was used because it allows participants to demonstrate their empathy toward the victim, 27 the perpetrator, and/or both. Previous scales used to measure empathy in rape cases, like the Rape Empathy Scale (Deitz et al., 1982), only allowed participants to empathize with the victim or the perpetrator. Internal reliability for the Rape-Victim Empathy Scale part of the scale was acceptable (Cronbach’s alpha = .90). The internal reliability for the Rape-Perpetrator Empathy Scale was also acceptable (Cronbach’s alpha = .91). Revised Illinois Rape Myth Acceptance Scale To examine participants’ acceptance of rape myths in relation to the rape victim and perpetrator described in the vignette, participants were asked to complete a revised version of Illinois Rape Myth Acceptance Scale (Payne, Lonsway, & Fitzgerald, 1999). See Appendix D. The original measure contains 45 Likert-type statements measured with a 7-point Likert scale ranging from Strongly Agree to Strongly Disagree. A higher score on the scale indicated an acceptance of rape myths. For this study, thirty-four items from the Illinois Rape Myth Acceptance Scale were modified to make them specifically about the rape victim and perpetrator described in the vignettes. This was similar to a previous study that modified the Illinois Rape Myth Acceptance scale to examine rape myths beliefs about the victim described in vignettes (Harrison et al., 2008). The resulting measure was measured with a 7-point Likert scale ranging from Not at all agree to Very much agree. The measure was also analyzed using the seven subscales created by Payne et al. (1999). The seven subscales are She Asked For It, It Wasn’t Really Rape, He Didn’t Mean To, She Wanted It, She Lied, Rape is a Trivial Event, and Rape is a Deviant Event. The data was recoded so that higher scores would indicate higher acceptance with rape myths. Internal reliability for the full rape 28 myth measure was acceptable (Cronbach’s alpha = .96). Internal reliability was acceptable for the following six subscales: She Asked For It (Cronbach’s alpha = .88), It Wasn’t Really Rape (Cronbach’s alpha = .82), He Didn’t Mean To (Cronbach’s alpha = .75), She Wanted It (Cronbach’s alpha = .90), She Lied (Cronbach’s alpha = .89), and Rape is a Deviant Event (Cronbach’s alpha = .70). Internal reliability was unacceptable for the Rape is a Trivial Event subscale (Cronbach’s alpha = .67), so this subscale was not used in the analysis. Demographic Questionnaire Participants completed a demographic questionnaire (see Appendix E). No personal identifying information was collected. Manipulation Checks Manipulation checks were included on the demographic questionnaire (see Appendix E). The manipulation checks were designed to check participants’ awareness of victim weight, sexual history, and clothing using forced choice responses. Data were not used in the analysis if a participant’s manipulation check did not match his or her assigned vignette. Procedure A female experimenter conducted the research in multiple sessions at a university laboratory. Each session included one to six participants. All research sessions were conducted by the same researcher. When participants arrived for the study, they were greeted by the researcher. After signing consent forms, participants were told the purpose of the study was to examine 29 perceptions of criminal behavior. Then, participants received one of the eight vignettes. The manipulations of victim weight, clothes, and sexual promiscuity were contained in the vignettes. Each participant was instructed to read the vignette and complete a questionnaire packet containing the Blame Index, the Rape-Victim Empathy Scale, the Rape-Perpetrator Empathy Scale, the Revised Illinois Rape Myth Scale, and the demographic questionnaire. Afterwards the participants were debriefed about the true purpose of the study and given course credit for their participation. 30 CHAPTER 3 RESULTS Multiple factorial ANOVAs were conducted to examine the effects of victim weight, sexual history, and clothing on judgments of rape victims and rape perpetrators. The independent variables were victim weight (thin versus obese), victim sexual history (virgin versus promiscuous) and victim clothing (revealing versus modest). The dependent variables were victim blame, perpetrator blame, whether participants perceived the incident to be rape, empathy for the victim, empathy for the perpetrator, and participants’ acceptance of rape myths in relation to the victim and perpetrator. Effects of Victim Clothing on Victim Judgment Hypothesis 1 predicted that participants would blame date rape victims more, experience less empathy toward them, and be more likely to believe rape victims conform to general rape myths if they wear provocative clothing rather than modest clothing. Contrary to predictions, victim clothing did not significantly influence judgments of victim blame, F (1, 160) = 1.75, p = .19 or participants’ empathy for the rape victim, F (1, 159) = .52, p = .47. Likewise, victim clothing did not significantly influence beliefs that the victim lied about the rape, F (1, 160) = .03, p = .86, that the victim asked for it, F (1, 159) = .29, p = .59, or that she wanted to be raped, F (1, 160) = .002, p = .97. In addition, victim clothing did not significantly influence participants’ beliefs that the rape was a deviant act, F (1, 160) = 2.62, p = .11, or that it was not really rape, F (1, 160) = .57, p = .45. See Table 1. 31 Table 1 Mean Victim Judgments as a Function of Victim Clothing ____Victim Clothing____ Measure Modest Revealing SD N Victim is to Blame for the Rape 2.34a 2.59a .95 168 Victim Empathy for the Rape 2.34a 2.47a .95 167 Victim Lied About the Rape 2.12a 2.15a 1.29 167 Victim Asked to be Raped 2.39a 2.54a 1.33 168 Victim Wanted to be Raped 1.82a 1.85a 1.18 168 Victim’s Rape Was a Deviant Act 1.58a 1.80a .76 168 Victim Was Not Really Raped 1.94a 1.87a 1.08 168 Note: Row means with different subscripts differ significantly at p < .05. Item scores ranged from 1 to 7. Higher means indicate greater attributions. Effects of Victim Clothing on Perpetrator Judgment Hypothesis 2 predicted that participants would blame date rape perpetrators less, experience more empathy toward them, and be less likely to believe they conform to general rape myths if the rape victim was wearing provocative clothing rather than modest clothing. Because internal reliability was unacceptable (Cronbach’s alpha = .64), the Perpetrator Blame measure was not used in the analysis. Therefore, three items measuring perpetrator blame, perpetrator responsibility, and the justifiability of the perpetrator’s actions were analyzed individually to determine whether victim clothing influenced perpetrator blame. However, victim clothing did not significantly influence 32 perceived perpetrator responsibility, F (1, 160) = .65, p = .42, perpetrator blame, F (1, 160) = .03, p = .87, or participants’ belief that the perpetrator was justified in his actions, F (1, 160) = .81, p = .37. In addition, victim clothing did not significantly influence perpetrator empathy, F (1, 159) = .42, p = .52, or participants’ beliefs concerning whether the perpetrator meant to rape the victim, F (1, 160) = .45, p = .51. See Table 2. Table 2 Mean Perpetrator Judgments as a Function of Victim Clothing ____Victim Clothing____ Measure Modest Revealing SD N Perpetrator Responsible for the Rape 6.51a 6.53a .89 168 Perpetrator is to Blame for the Rape 5.20a 5.45a 2.35 168 Perpetrator’s Actions Justified 6.42a 6.25a 1.39 168 Perpetrator Empathy for the Rape 4.90a 4.99a 1.23 168 Perpetrator Did Not Mean to Rape 3.46a 3.32a 1.41 168 Note: Row means with different subscripts differ significantly at p < .05. Item scores ranged from 1 to 7. Higher means indicate greater attributions. Effects of Victim Weight on Victim Judgment Hypothesis 3 predicted that participants would blame obese rape victims more, experience less empathy toward them, and believe obese rape victims conform to general rape myths more than thin rape victims. Contrary to predictions, victim weight did not significantly influence judgments of victim blame, F (1, 160) = 2.44, p = .12 or victim empathy, F (1, 159) = .03, p = .87. In addition, victim weight did not significantly 33 influence participants’ beliefs that the victim asked for it, F (1, 160) = 1.21, p = .27, that the victim wanted it, F (1, 160) = .32, p = .57, that she lied about the rape, F (1, 160) = .72, p =.40, or that rape was a deviant act, F (1, 160) = 1.89, p = .17. See Table 3. The analysis of the measure of participants’ belief that it was not really raped did approach statistical significance, F (1, 160) = 2.83, p = .09, d = .25. Participants’ were somewhat more likely to believe that the incident was not really rape when the victim was thin rather obese. Table 3 Mean Victim Judgments as a Function of Victim Weight ___Victim Weight___ Measure Thin Obese SD N Victim is to Blame for the Rape 2.56a 2.32a 1.08 168 Victim Empathy for the Rape 2.42a 2.39a .95 167 Victim Asked to be Raped 2.55a 2.35a 1.33 168 Victim Wanted to be Raped 1.87a 1.78a 1.18 168 Victim Lied About the Rape 2.20a 2.05a 1.29 167 Victim’s Rape Was a Deviant Act 1.75a 1.60a .76 168 Victim Was Not Really Raped 2.02a 1.75a 1.08 168 Note: Row means with different subscripts differ significantly at p < .05. Item scores ranged from 1 to 7. Higher means indicate greater attributions. 34 Effects of Victim Weight on Perpetrator Judgment Hypothesis 4 predicted that participants would blame date rape perpetrators less, experience more empathy toward them, and be less likely to believe they conform to general rape myths if the rape victim is obese rather than thin. Because the internal reliability of the Perpetrator Blame index was unacceptable (Cronbach’s alpha = .64), this measure was not used in the analysis. However, three items measuring perpetrator blame, perpetrator responsibility, and the justifiability of the perpetrator’s actions were analyzed individually to determine whether victim clothing influenced perpetrator blame. As expected, victim weight significantly influenced perpetrator responsibility for the rape, F (1, 160) = 6.97, p = .01, and perpetrator blame for the rape, F (1, 160) = 4.77, p = .03. Participants believed that the perpetrator was more responsible for raping the obese victim than the thin victim. Additionally, participants blamed the perpetrator more for the situation when the victim was obese rather than thin. Also, victim weight did approach significance to influence the justifiability of the perpetrator’s actions, F (1, 160) = 2.98, p = .09. Participants’ believed that the perpetrator was more justified in raping the obese victim, than the thin victim. See Table 4. Contrary to predictions, victim weight did not significantly influence perpetrator empathy, F (1, 159) = 2.07, p = .15. However, analysis indicated that participants’ belief the perpetrator did not mean to rape the victim was significant, F (1, 160) = 8.47, p = .004, d = .44. Participants’ were more likely to believe the perpetrator did not mean to rape the thin victim than the obese victim. 35 Table 4 Mean Perpetrator Judgments as a Function of Victim Weight ___Victim Weight___ Measure Thin Obese SD N Perpetrator Responsible for the Rape 6.39a 6.69b .89 168 Perpetrator is to Blame for the Rape 4.92a 5.87b 2.35 168 Perpetrator’s Actions Justified 6.19a 6.55a 1.39 168 Perpetrator Empathy for the Rape 4.83a 5.10a 1.23 167 Perpetrator Did Not Mean to Rape 3.65a 3.04b 1.41 168 Note: Row means with different subscripts differ significantly at p < .05. Item scores ranged from 1 to 7. Higher means indicate greater attributions. Effects of Victim Sexual History on Victim Judgment Hypothesis 5 predicted that participants would blame date rape victims more, experience less empathy toward them, and believe rape victims conform to general rape myths if they are promiscuous rather than virgins. Contrary to predictions, victim sexual history did not significantly influence judgments of victim blame, F (1, 160) = 2.57, p = .11 or victim empathy, F (1, 159) = .002, p = .96. Likewise, victim sexual history did not significantly influence participants’ view that it was not really rape, F (1, 160) = 2.19, p = .14. See Table 5. The analysis did indicate that participants’ belief that rape was a deviant act approached significance, F (1, 160) = 3.35, p = .07, d = .28. Participants were more likely to believe that rape is a deviant act if the victim was promiscuous rather than a virgin. 36 Also, the analysis indicated that participants’ belief that the rape victim asked for the rape approached significance, F (1, 160) = 3.35, p = .07, d = .28. Participants were more likely to believe that the promiscuous victim asked for it than the virgin victim. Likewise, the analysis indicated that participants’ belief the rape victim wanted the rape, F (1, 160) = 3.54, p = .06, d = .26, as did their belief that she lied about the, F (1, 159) = 2.86, p = .09, d = .23. Participants were more likely to believe she wanted it and that she lied about the rape if she was promiscuous than a virgin. Table 5 Mean Victim Judgments as a Function of Victim Sexual History ___Victim Sexual History___ Measure Virgin Victim is to Blame for the Rape 2.34a Victim Empathy for the Rape Promiscuous SD N 2.59a .95 168 2.40a 2.41a .95 167 Victim Was Not Really Raped 1.79a 2.03a 1.08 168 Victim’s Rape Was a Deviant Act 1.58a 1.79a .76 168 Victim Asked to be Raped 2.28a 2.65a 1.33 168 Victim Wanted to be Raped 1.68a 1.99a 1.18 168 Victim Lied About the Rape 1.99a 2.29a 1.29 167 Note: Row means with different subscripts differ significantly at p < .05. Item scores ranged from 1 to 7. Higher means indicate greater attributions. 37 Effects of Victim Sexual History on Perpetrator Judgment Hypothesis 6 predicted that participants would blame date rape perpetrators less, experience more empathy toward them, and be less likely to believe they conform to general rape myths if the rape victim was promiscuous rather than a virgin. Because internal reliability was unacceptable (Cronbach’s alpha = .64), the perpetrator blame measure was not used in the analysis. However, three items measuring perpetrator blame, perpetrator responsibility, and the justifiability of the perpetrator’s actions were analyzed individually to determine whether victim sexual history influenced perpetrator blame. Unexpectedly, victim sexual history did not significantly influence perpetrator responsibility, F (1, 160) = .43, p = .52, perpetrator blame, F (1, 160) = .16, p = .69, or participants’ believe that the perpetrator was justified in his actions, F (1, 160) = .80, p = .37. See Table 6. As expected, victim sexual history did significantly influence perpetrator empathy, F (1, 159) = 5.67, p = .02, d = .36, with participants showing higher empathy toward the perpetrator of the virgin rape victim than the promsicuous rape victim. In addition, the analysis indicated that participants’ belief that the perpetrator did not mean to rape the victim was significant, F (1, 160) = 4.82, p = .03, d = .31. Participants were more likely to believe that he did not mean to rape the promiscuous victim than the virgin victim. 38 Table 6 Mean Perpetrator Judgments as a Function of Victim Sexual History ___Victim Sexual History___ Measure Virgin Promiscuous SD N Perpetrator Responsible for the Rape 6.55a 6.48a .89 168 Perpetrator is to Blame for the Rape 5.39a 5.25a 2.35 168 Perpetrator’s Actions Justified 6.40a 6.28a 1.39 168 Perpetrator Empathy for the Rape 5.16a 4.71b 1.23 167 Perpetrator Did Not Mean to Rape 3.18a 3.61b 1.41 168 Note: Row means with different subscripts differ significantly at p < .05. Item scores ranged from 1 to 7. Higher means indicate greater attributions. Interaction Effects of Weight, Clothing, and Sexual History on Victim Judgment Hypothesis 7 predicted that participants would blame date rape victims more, experience less empathy toward them, and believe rape victims conform to general rape myths if they are obese, promiscuous and wear provocative clothing rather than thin, virgin and wear modest clothing. This hypothesis would be supported by an interaction of victim weight, clothing, and sexual history. Unexpectedly victim weight, sexual history, and clothing did not significantly interact to influence judgments of victim blame, F (1, 160) = 2.15, p = .15 or victim empathy F (1, 159) = 2.50, p = .12. Likewise, victim weight, sexual history, and clothing did not significantly interact to influence participants’ beliefs that it was not really rape, F (1, 160) = .64, p = .43, that the victim lied about the rape, F (1, 159) = 1.76, p = .19, that 39 the victim asked for the rape, F (1, 160) = .97, p = .33, or that she wanted to be raped, F (1, 160) = 1.61, p = .21. Also, victim weight, sexual history, and clothing did not significantly interact to influence participants’ beliefs that the rape was a deviant act, F (1, 160) = 1.22, p = .27. However, there were several two-way interactions that neared significance. These are discussed below. The analysis revealed a near significant interaction of victim weight and clothing on the measure of participants’ belief that the incident was not really rape F (1, 160) = 3.28, p = .07. Follow-up tests indicated participants were more likely to believe the incident was not rape when the victim wearing revealing clothing was thin (M = 2.11) rather than obese (M = 1.54), F (1, 81) = 6.02, p = .01, d = .64. However, when the victim wore modest clothing there was no significant difference on this measure for the thin victim (M = 2.11) and the obese victim (M =1.54), F (1, 83) = .02, p = .87. The interaction of victim sexual history and clothing also approached significance on the measure of whether participants believed the victim wanted the incident to occur, F (1, 160) = 2.86, p = .09. Follow-up tests indicated participants were more likely to believe the victim wanted to have sex with the perpetrator when the victim wearing revealing clothing was promiscuous (M = 2.13) rather than a virgin (M = 1.55), F (1, 81) = 4.49, p = .03, d = .47. However, when the victim wore modest clothing there was no significant difference on this measure for the promiscuous victim (M = 1.83) and the virgin victim (M =1.80), F (1, 83) = .02, p = .87. 40 Interaction Effects of Weight, Clothing, and Sexual History on Perpetrator Judgment Hypothesis 8 predicted that participants would blame date rape perpetrators less, experience more empathy toward them, and be less likely to believe they conformed to general rape myths if the rape victim was obese, promiscuous and wore provocative clothing rather than thin, virgin and wore modest clothing. Unexpectedly victim weight, sexual history, and clothing did not significantly interact to influence participant’s empathy for the perpetrator, F (1, 159) = .48, p = .49 or participants’ belief that the perpetrator did not mean to rape the victim, F (1, 160) = .000, p = .98. Likewise, victim weight, sexual history, and clothing did not significantly interact to influence participants’ belief that the perpetrator was responsible for the rape, F (1, 160) = .01, p = .91, that the perpetrator was to blame for the rape, F (1, 160) = .35, p = .56, or that the perpetrator was justified in his actions, F (1, 160) = 06, p = .81. There was a significant two-way interaction of victim sexual history and clothing on the measure of whether participants believed the perpetrator was justified in his actions, F (1, 160) = 3.76, p = .05, Follow-up tests indicated participants were more likely to believe the perpetrator was justified in his actions when the promiscuous victim wore revealing clothing rather than modest clothing, F (1, 81) = 4.13, p = .04, d = .44. However, this effect was not significant when the victim was a virgin, F (1, 83) = .76, p = .38. See Figure 1. 41 6.7 6.6 Perceived Justification 6.5 6.4 6.3 6.2 Modest 6.1 Revealing 6 5.9 5.8 5.7 5.6 Virgin Promiscuous Figure 1. Significant interaction effect of victim sexual history and clothing for the perpetrator being justified in raping the victim. Vignette Describing Rape Hypothesis 9 predicted that participants would judge the interview as depicting an acquaintance rape. As expected, participants did believe the interview described an acquaintance rape, t (25.24) = .2.51, p < .0001, d = 3.91, (M = 6.01, SD = 1.29). 42 CHAPTER 4 DISCUSSION Victims of rape are often vilified by family, friends, media, medical professionals and the judicial system (Campbell et al., 1999; Hockett et al., 2009). They are blamed for actions they took before, during, and after the rape. Everything they do is up for scrutinizing, from what they wear to what they drink. Nothing is off limits. With so much research focusing on victim blame and the causes of it, little research has looked at the effects victim weight has on victim blame or empathy. This study sought to investigate the repercussions one’s weight can have on being believed or judged after a rape. In addition, victim dress and sexual history were also examined, because these characteristics have been shown in previous studies to influence blame and empathy toward the victim, but little is known as to whether these variables interaction with victim rape to influence rape attributions (Brown et al., 2007; Feldman et al., 1998; Jimenez & Abreu, 2003; Klein, Kennedy, & Gorzalka, 2009; Osman, 2011; Shears, 2010; Temkin & Krahe, 2008). Additionally, the perpetrator was also studied because most studies only focus on judgments toward the victim, but perpetrator blame and empathy are likely to be influenced by victim characteristics as well, so analysis of the perpetrator was included. Effects of Victim Clothing on Victim Judgment Contrary to predictions, victim clothing did not significantly influence blame, empathy, or rape myth acceptance toward the victim. Prior studies on clothing have found participants to have high rape myth acceptance when a rape victim is wearing 43 provocative clothing (Brown et al., 2007; Klein et al., 2009; Shears, 2010) and to blame the rape victim wearing provocative clothing (Workman & Freeburg, 1999). Because the victim described in the research vignette was described as wearing a halter top and mini skirt, it was expected that participants would judge this victim more severely due to her provocative attire. One explanation for the non-significant findings is that participants did not judge the victim’s clothing choice, when evaluating the rape interview. This could be because participants are of a similar age to the rape victim, and have worn similar clothes to parties, so perhaps they did not perceive the described outfit as particularly provocative for a party setting. However, if this played a part in their judgments of the rape victim, would have been likely that participants would have had higher empathy for a victim in this situation, since they could see themselves in this situation, so this does not fully explain the results. Another explanation for these results is that wearing provocative clothing is becoming mainstream and acceptable attire, so participants were not influenced by it. Over the last few decades, clothing attire has become more provocative and the media now routinely portrays women in provocative or sexualized clothing (Collins, 2011). Even children’s attire could be described as provocative (Lamb & Brown, 2006; Levine & Kilbourne, 2008). For instance, similar Halloween costumes are marketed to toddlers, teenagers, and adults, with the only difference being the size and color (Buckley, 2012; Siddiquee, 2012). As exposure to provocative clothing becomes acceptable, people’s negative judgment against women who wear such clothing could be decreasing. 44 Effects of Victim Clothing on Perpetrator Judgment Contrary to predictions, victim clothing did not significantly influence blame, empathy, or rape myth acceptance toward the perpetrator. Prior studies on clothing have found participants believe that a rape victim wearing provocative clothing wanted to have sex with the perpetrator (Abbey et al., 1987; Maurer & Robinson, 2007). In addition, prior studies have found male participants to show more empathy toward the perpetrator than the victim (Osman, 2011; Sinclair & Bourne, 1998). An explanation for this study’s non-significant findings may be that as media portrayals of women have become sexualized (Lamb & Brown, 2006), people have become more accustomed to women wearing provocative clothing and thus do not judge them as negatively as they have in the past. Plus, because the media believes that “sex sells,” and has used this motto to sell items, it would also make sense for participants to not judge perpetrators of rape victims wearing provocative clothing, since men are often perceived as having limited control over their sexual urges (Martin, 2005) and to be physically aroused by visually stimulating women wearing provocative clothing (Brown et al., 2007; Hall, 1995; Martin, 2005; Moorti, 2002; Osman, 2003). Effects of Victim Weight on Victim Judgment Contrary to predictions, participants were not influenced by the victim’s weight in relation to victim blame, empathy, or rape myth acceptance. While the obese victim was expected to be blamed for the rape, generate little empathy from participants, and increase participants acceptance of rape myths the data did not support these expectations. One possible explanation is that participants did not factor the victim’s 45 weight into their analysis of the rape interview. However, because research has shown that people view obese individuals as lazy (Crandall, 1994; Gapinski, Schwartz, & Brownell, 2006; McClure et al., 2011) and lacking self-control (McClure et al., 2011), it is surprising that the victim’s weight did not seem to affect participants’ view of the victim. Another possible explanation is that the participants became distracted by other factors in the rape interview, like the victim clothing and sexual history, and thus were more focused on these victim characteristics instead of the victim’s weight. This may have occurred given that the interview with the rape victim only included two short sentences mentioning the victim’s weight, but there was a whole paragraph discussing the victim’s clothing, and multiple sentences discussing the victim’s sexual history. Consistent with Clarke and Stermac’s study (2011) victim weight influenced participants beliefs that the thin rape victim was not really raped. A possible explanation for this result is that participants believed a person would not be interested in having a sexual encounter with someone who is obese, because in this society, obese individuals are not seen as attractive (Clarke and Stermac, 2011). However, because being perceived as attractive participants may have believed that more men would be interested in having a sexual encounter with the thin victim. Effects of Victim Weight on Perpetrator Judgment Unexpectedly, participants were not influenced by victim weight in relation to perpetrator empathy. No other study has examined the effects of victim’s weight and empathy toward a rape victim or perpetrator. However, previous studies on participants’ empathy toward obese individuals has found that increasing empathy levels toward obese 46 individuals is hard to accomplish (Gapinski et al., 2006; Teachman et al., 2003), and researchers have only been able to increase empathy levels in obese participants (Teachman et al., 2003). One possible explanation for this result is that participants had low empathy toward the perpetrators in both scenarios because they could not put themselves in the place of a rapist. Contrary to predictions, participants did believe the perpetrator was responsible for raping the obese victim and blamed more for raping the obese victim than the thin victim. It is possible that because obese individuals are seen as unattractive and undeserving of sexual interest, participants would believe the victim would be willing to accept most interest and sexual offers. This hypothesis is partly supported by the participant’s belief that the perpetrator was justified in raping the obese victim. Because obese individuals are not seen as attractive as thin victims and thus not as sexual, participants may have believed the obese victim was more interested in a sexual encounter than the thin victim, so they may have viewed her rape as more justified. Finally, the participants believed the perpetrator did not mean to rape the thin victim. Because a thin victim is likely to be seen as more attractive than the obese victim, it is possible that participants believed the perpetrator could not control his attraction to the thin victim and raped her. This could explain why the perpetrator was seen as responsible and to blame for raping the obese victim. Since an obese victim would not be seen as attractive, the perpetrator must have had other motives for raping the victim and must have been seen as having control over himself. 47 Effects of Victim Sexual History on Victim Judgment As hypothesized, victim sexual history did influence participants’ rape myth acceptance. Participants who read the interview about the promiscuous rape victim reported higher rape myth acceptance in regard to the specific incidence described in the vignette. In relation to the rape myth acceptance subscales, participants perceived the promiscuous victim wanted the rape, asked for the rape, and lied about the rape more than the virgin rape victim. It is often believed that once a person consents to sex, they will always consent to sex (Krahe et al., 2008; McMahon-Howard et al., 2009), so if the promiscuous victim stated that she did not consent to sex, participants are likely to perceive her as lying.. Contrary to the hypothesis, participants belief about victim blame or empathy was not influenced by the victim’s sexual history This was surprising, because women are not expected to be interested in sex (Buchwald et al., 2005; Buck Doude, 2008), and are usually scrutinized and judged when showing interest to sex. A possible explanation for this result is that as values have evolved, people have become more accepting of women breaking out of gender norms. This may be especially relevant since most participants were between the age of 18 and 24 and thus from a younger demographic. Researchers (Brooks & Bolzendahl, 2004; Ciabattari, 2001) have found that younger individuals tend to be more accepting, tolerant, and less influenced by gender norms. Effects of Victim Sexual History on Perpetrator Judgment As hypothesized, participants believed the perpetrator did not mean to rape the promiscuous victim. Since a promiscuous victim is expected to submit to sex, any time 48 after the first sexual experience (Krahe et al., 2008; McMahon-Howard et al., 2009), most participants probably believed she was interested in sex or acted in a manner that led the perpetrator to believe she was interested in sex . Unexpectedly, participants showed the perpetrator more empathy when he raped the virgin victim, than the promiscuous rape victim. A possible explanation for this is that participants view sex with a virgin as highly valued and could empathize with the perpetrator wanting to have sex with a virgin. However, participants were not influenced by the victim’s sexual history, in relation to the perpetrator’s blame, perpetrator’s responsibility, or how justified he was in raping the victim. A possible explanation for this result is that participants were not influenced by the victim’s sexual history, because sexual history is not seen as a defining characteristic of people anymore. In addition, participants may have had similar experiences as the perpetrator, and thus did not judge the perpetrator for his actions. Effects of Weight, Clothing, and Sexual History on Victim Judgment While weight and clothing was found to interact for victims wearing provocative clothing, it did not significantly interact for the victims wearing modest clothing. In fact, the results showed that the participants were more likely to believe the obese victim wearing revealing clothing was raped, than the thin victim wearing revealing clothing. One explanation for this is that attractiveness and rape myths acceptance interacted to influence the results. Rape myths suggest that victims wearing provocative clothing deserve to be raped or caused the rape to happen, since only women wanting to have sex would dress provocatively (Brownmiller, 1975; Burt, 1980; Hall, 1995; Moorti, 2002; 49 Payne, et al., 1999). Because rape is usually viewed, as caused by a need for sex and not control (Martin, 2005), participants evaluated the rape interview using this rape myth. Thus, participants blamed the thin victim wearing revealing clothing more, because she fit the stereotype, or rape myth, more than the obese victim wearing revealing clothing. In order to process why a perpetrator would be interested in raping a victim, the participants decided that the perpetrator who raped the thin rape victim wearing revealing clothing was not in control of himself and thus it was not really rape, because women wearing revealing clothing want to have sex. However, the participants believed it was rape in the interview of the obese victim wearing revealing clothing, because even though they thought women wearing revealing clothing would be interested in having sex, they could not see how a perpetrator would be interested in having sex with an obese victim. In order to continue to believe their rape myths, they label the rape of an obese victim wearing revealing clothing as really a rape, because they believe the perpetrator must have had some other motive for raping the obese victim wearing revealing clothing, than sex. However, by labeling the thin victim wearing revealing clothing as not a rape, they can keep their views if the world as a just place, since the thin victim should have been more careful and brought the rape onto herself. This view can also be used to explain why participants believed the promiscuous victim wearing revealing clothing wanted to be raped, and the virgin victim wearing revealing clothing did not want to be raped. Since participants tend to believe the rape myth acceptance that women wearing revealing clothing are interested in sex (Brownmiller, 1975; Burt, 1980; Hall, 1995; Moorti, 2002; Payne, et al., 1999), they 50 judge these victims negatively. However, since the participants’ belief that a virgin victim dressed provocatively would break gender norms (Abrams et al., 2003; Krahe et al., 2008; Chapleau et al., 2007; Temkin & Krahe, 2008) and rape myths, they probably believed, the victim was naïve, and gave her the benefit of the doubt. This resulted in them believing the promiscuous rape victim wearing revealing clothing want to be raped, and the virgin victim wearing revealing clothing did not want to be raped. Finally participants had high empathy toward the promiscuous victim wearing revealing clothing and the virgin victim wearing modest clothing but low empathy toward the virgin victim wearing revealing clothing and the promiscuous victim wearing modest clothing. This is consistent with the results for the victim asked for it and it was not really rape. As with those results, rape myth acceptance could explain the results. Participants had high empathy toward the two victims that matched the rape myth views, but low empathy toward the two victims that did not match the participants’ rape myth views. Effects of Weight, Clothing, and Sexual History on Perpetrator Judgment Participants believed the perpetrator was justified in raping the virgin victim wearing revealing clothing, and the promiscuous victim wearing modest clothing, but not the promiscuous victim wearing revealing clothing, or the virgin victim wearing modest clothing. One possible explanation for these results is that the participants believed the virgin victim was responsible for her rape, since she was wearing revealing clothing. Thus the perpetrator was justified in raping the victim, since she broke gender norms (Abrams et al., 2003; Krahe et al., 2008; Chapleau et al., 2007; Temkin & Krahe, 2008). 51 Another possible explanation is that participants were evaluating the rape interview using rape myths they accept, but found the rape interview to contradict these rape myths, when the interview described the virgin victim wearing revealing clothing and the promiscuous victim wearing modest clothing. Since rape myths hold that virgin victims wear modest clothes and promiscuous victims wear revealing clothing (Brownmiller, 1975; Burt, 1980; Hall, 1995; Moorti, 2002; Payne, et al., 1999), participants had to search for a way to either accept that their rape myths as false, or to modify make their values and beliefs. This resulted in the participants believing the perpetrator was justified in raping these victims, because they broke their expected roles, and thus brought the rape onto themselves. Vignette Describing Rape As hypothesized, participants did believe the rape interview described a rape. Even though most participants believed the interview described a rape, it is interesting to note that participants believed the victim was raped, but that the perpetrator was justified in raping the victim, or that the victim was to blame depending on the weight and/or sexual history of the victim. Considering victims of other crimes are typically not blamed or seen as deserving of their victimization, it is interesting that rape victims continue to be seen as deserving the rape. One possible explanation, based on the “just world hypothesis,” is that the participants want to believe that the world is a just place and that only bad things happen to bad people, so they believe that the victim must have been a bad person, and thus deserved to be raped (Abrams et al., 2003; Franiuk et al., 2008; Lerner, 1980) Another explanation is that people view rape as a sexual act, and not one 52 about control (Archer & Vaughan, 2001; Bourke, 2007). Seeing that rape is seen as sexual and women are viewed as sexual objects and men are incapable of controlling themselves, then it is only natural for rape to occur, and the only way to prevent it is to control women. But, when we allow victim blaming and rape myth acceptance to continue, we give all the power to the perpetrators. We deny women control over their bodies and deny men control over their actions. We not only put down women by declaring them to be sexual objects, but men by viewing them as being incapable of controlling their urges. In the end, the only person held accountable is the victim. Limitations Because this study used a convenience sample of college students, the results of the study cannot be generalized to the general public. However, considering college students have a one in four rate of victimization (Fischer, Cullen, & Turner, 2000; Hirsch, 1990), while the general population has a one in six rate of victimization (RAINN, 2009; Tjaden & Thoennes, 1998), college students are an appropriate population to study. Another limitation of this study is the number of participants that were eliminated due to getting their manipulation check wrong. Of the 46 participants eliminated from the study, 31 were because they marked the participant as average weight when she was actually obese. One possible explanation for this is that participants did not remember the weight of the victim and thus guessed wrong. However, most participants made the same mistake, so it is unlikely that participants guessed wrong. Another more plausible explanation is that since obesity levels are on the rise and 36% of the United States population is obese (CDC, 2012), the participants viewed the obese victim as average 53 weight since a large percentage is now obese. The obesity levels and participants interaction with people who are obese could result in them viewing obesity as average weight. The last limitation of the study is the small sample size of male participants, which prevented an analysis of whether participant gender influenced the findings. Numerous studies examining rape have found male participants often blame rape victims more than female participants do (Abrams et al., 2003; Krahe et al., 2008; Chapleau et al., 2007; Temkin & Krahe, 2008). One reason for the low number of male participants is the population the participants were derived. At this university, the majority of students that are taking psychology classes are female, so the pool of male students is small. Future research should work on having equal numbers of male and female participants. Future Research While this study has made a good start at examining the effect of victim weight on blame and empathy, more research needs to continue to this exploration. One step to accomplish this would be to conduct a study using pictures of the rape victim. In this study, the researcher would use a picture of a thin victim and an obese victim in addition to including a description of the victim in the interview. If possible, the research should also adjust the clothes to depict a victim wearing modest clothing or revealing clothing. One way to accomplish this feat, without using more than one model for the victim, would be to use Photoshop or similar software to edit the weight and clothes of the victim. This will reduce the possibility of other victim characteristics influencing the participants. 54 Another study should compare the weight of the victim and the victim’s relationship to the perpetrator. For example, the study could compare a stranger rape to and acquaintance rape; because participants from previous studies have judged victims of stranger and acquaintance rape differently (Krahe et al., 2008). Researchers should examine if victim weight affects blame and empathy toward the rape victim in these two scenarios. Additionally, researchers analyzing victim weight should also ask participants, as part of their demographic questionnaire, their height and weight. Considering this can be a sensitive subject, researchers should stress the anonymity of the study. The researcher should then examine if the participant’s weight influences his or her judgment of the victim. Teachman et al. (2003) found empathy toward obese individuals only increased in obese participants, so researchers should examine if victim empathy and blame is influenced by victim’s weight. Finally, most research focuses on victim characteristics when assigning blame or empathy. While this study, showed that blame and empathy can be influenced by weight and sexual history, in relation to the victim and perpetrator, more research should focus on characteristics of the perpetrator that effect blame and empathy. Too many victims are blamed for their rape because of actions they took before and during their rape, but perpetrators are seldom blamed for their actions. However, since most studies do not analyze the perpetrator characteristics, it is not possible to know which characteristics influence participants’ views on blame and empathy toward the perpetrator. This 55 information could help researchers and sexual assault organizations devise strategies for reducing victim blame and increasing perpetrator blame. Conclusions and Implications While research on rape has continued to evolve to keep up with the society’s changing views on what constitutes respectability, and while obesity rates continue to rise, it was expected that weight would influence participants’ blame and empathy toward the victim and perpetrator. As expected, weight and sexual history did influence participants’ beliefs toward blame, empathy, and rape myth acceptance. It is interesting to note that the victim was blamed for the rape, despite participants believing the victim was raped. However, this should not be surprising given participants had applied traditional rape myths to the victim. Thus they endorsed the notion that rape is justified in certain situations, that the victim brought the rape onto herself, and the perpetrator was justified in raping her. It should not be surprising that people continue to judge victims of rape differently from victims of other crimes, when the media and legislators continue to promote rape myth acceptance and even blame victims of rape themselves. For example, when legislators’ argue that there is such a thing as “legitimate rape” or that “real” rape victims cannot become pregnant because “the female body has ways to try to shut the whole thing down” (Moore, 2012). This teaches people that not all women deserve protection, and that assaulting certain women is acceptable and legal. In addition, people learn that victims of rape should be scrutinized and judged to determine if they are trustworthy and “real” rape victims. As 56 long as beliefs like this are perpetuated; victims of rape will continue to be blamed and demonized for being raped, and few victims will come forward. The researcher hopes this study will increase awareness of the biases people hold about rape victims. Too many victims are afraid to come forward for fear of being blamed or judged. While others believe it is their fault, that they deserved the rape, or caused it to happen. By acknowledging rape myths and the factors that result in victim blame we can begin to recognize our biases and work to correct them. Each time a person reads or hears a rape case, they should evaluate their reactions to the story. What are your immediate reactions? Did you think, “why was the victim there?” Did you judge her action, clothes, etc.? Did you judge the perpetrators actions? Acknowledging these biases and working to change them can improve the way victims are treated. Finally, if a rape victim tells you they were raped, one important thing you can do is to say “I believe you.” In additions, the researcher hopes this research will be used as an aid to help therapists working with victims of rape, by helping the therapists understand their own biases and the biases of other people the victim may be interacting with. With this knowledge, therapists can help rape victims cope and prepare to deal with people who blame or judge them. Therapists can also help victims not blame themselves. Finally, it is hoped that this research can be utilized by rape victim advocates. This research can prepare them for what victims of rape may be experiencing and feeling, and for reactions rape victims may receive from medical staff and police. 57 Appendix A Participant Characteristics Measure 1. Did anyone other than the researcher tell you anything about this study before you participated in it? _________ If yes, what had you been told? 2. What is the purpose of this study? 3. What is your gender? _____________________________ 4. What is you ethnicity/race? ________________________ 5. What is your age? _______________________________ 6. What is your major? _____________________________ 7. What is your religious preference? __________________ 8. What is your family’s socioeconomic status? Lower class 9. Working class Middle class How would you describe your political beliefs? Very Liberal Liberal Moderate Conservative Conservative 10. Please describe the alleged rape victim’s weight. Don’t Know 11. Upper class Thin Obese Please describe the alleged rape victim’s sexual history. Don’t Know Virgin Multiple Partners Very 58 12. How would you describe the type of clothing the alleged rape victim was wearing the night of the alleged rape? Don’t Know Revealing Modest 59 Appendix B Vignettes Thin, Virgin, Provocatively Dressed On May 29, 2011, an Assistant District Attorney interviewed an alleged rape victim, Cindy Hall. The district attorney interviewed Cindy in order to prepare the criminal case against the alleged rapist. Below is a transcript of the interview between the Assistant District Attorney and Cindy Hall. Interview with Cindy Hall Assistant District Attorney: Ms. Hall we are currently investigating your case and are gathering evidence to determine if we will be able to prosecute the man who raped you. I need to ask you some questions and you have to be completely open and honest with me. Some of the questions will be very personal, but I have to learn as much I possibly can about you, and about what happened to you if we are going to successfully prosecute the man who hurt you. Do you understand? I need you to tell me everything, even if it makes you uncomfortable. Cindy Hall: Ok, I’ll try. Assistant District Attorney: Ok, Cindy, to begin with I want to read an excerpt from the police officer who took the report on the night of your rape. “The complainant is a twenty year old Caucasian woman. She is five feet four inches tall and weighs 115 pounds. She has long blonde hair and blue eyes. She is dressed in a skimpy blue halter top and a very short mini skirt.” Does this accurately describe you on that night? Cindy Hall: Yes, that sounds right, although, I actually weigh 110 pounds. Assistant District Attorney: The report also indicated you were wearing skimpy clothes the night of the party. Is that correct? Cindy Hall: Well, I mean, it was a party and I wanted to look good. I was wearing a mini skirt and a halter top. My skirt was short, and my shirt, well, it was short too and my breasts did show some. I mean, you could see my belly button and cleavage. Assistant District Attorney: Thank you for confirming. Ok, Cindy, now I need to ask you a few questions about your personal life. I need to ask you about your sexual history in case it comes up in court. 60 Cindy Hall: But, I thought they couldn’t ask about that in court. Assistant District Attorney: Technically they can’t. However, sometimes the defendant’s lawyer gets around that. So, we need to prepare. I need to ask you some personal questions, ok? Cindy Hall: Ok. Assistant District Attorney: Let’s start with your recent sexual history. How many men have you been with recently? Cindy Hall: Well, it’s been kind of a quiet year for me. I haven’t had a steady boyfriend for awhile so I’ve been hanging out a lot with my friend Sarah and some of my other friends. Assistant District Attorney: Ok, Cindy I want you to be honest with me. How many men have you had sexual relations within the past year? Cindy Hall: I don’t see why this is relevant. It has absolutely nothing to do with Michael raping me. He had no right! I told him NO! Assistant District Attorney: Cindy, I have to ask you questions so that I can properly prepare you case. I’m not here to judge you. I just need to make sure that I have all of my bases covered. I want to make sure that when we go to court there are no surprises. Now, tell me, how many men have you had sexual relations with during the past year. Cindy Hall: I’m a virgin, OK! I’ve never had sex. I’m waiting until I get married. Assistant District Attorney: Cindy, I appreciate your honesty. This is really helpful. Now, I need you to tell me what happened to you that night. Cindy Hall: Well, I went to this party on Saturday night about 9:00. My friend Tom had just graduated and he was celebrating. Assistant District Attorney: Are you very close friends with Tom? Cindy Hall: No, not really. We just had that one class together. We never dated or anything, but I liked him. We used to have lunch sometimes after class. Tom is nice. He’s a good guy, so when he asked me to his graduation party, I wanted to go. Assistant District Attorney: Where was the party held? 61 Cindy Hall: It was at Tom’s apartment, a few miles from school. Assistant District Attorney: Have you ever been to Tom’s apartment before? Cindy Hall: No, like I said, I had just met him last semester, and we never dated or anything. We just had lunch sometimes. Assistant District Attorney: Did you go to the party alone? Cindy Hall: No, I didn’t want to go alone so I asked my friend Sarah to go with me. She didn’t know Tom or anything, but she is really outgoing and she likes parties and meeting new people. Assistant District Attorney: When you got to the party did you know anyone there besides Tom? Cindy Hall: No, there were a bunch of people there, but I only knew Tom. Assistant District Attorney: How many people were there? Cindy Hall: I guess there were about 20 people there, but I can’t be sure, because some people were inside and some people were hanging outside near the pool. Assistant District Attorney: Were people drinking at the party? Cindy Hall: Well, Tom had a beer keg. Assistant District Attorney: Were you drinking? Cindy Hall: No, I wasn’t drinking at all. Assistant District Attorney: Why not? Cindy Hall: Well, Sarah drove, but I’ve gone to parties with Sarah before, and sometimes she drinks too much and I was afraid I would have to drive home. Assistant District Attorney: Were there drugs at the party? Cindy Hall: I didn’t see anyone using drugs and I never use them. Assistant District Attorney: So what happened during the party? 62 Cindy Hall: Well, people were hanging out, dancing, laughing. It was a good party. Assistant District Attorney: Who did you spend most of your time with? Cindy Hall: Well, I was hanging Sarah and Tom for awhile, but it became apparent that they were really into each other. So, I gave them some space. Assistant District Attorney: What happened next? Cindy Hall: Well, I was mingling and that’s when I met Michael. He asked me if I wanted to dance. We must have danced for at least a half of hour. Then we went to get a soda. I always get me own beverages at parties, you know in case someone tries to slip me something. Assistant District Attorney: Was Michael drinking? Cindy Hall: I don’t think so. I mean, I didn’t see him drinking. Assistant District Attorney: Ok, go on. Cindy Hall: Well, it was getting late, it was almost 1:00 in the morning and I was ready to go home. So, I went to look for Sarah, and she was gone! I couldn’t believe it! Tom told me they had an argument and she just left. I couldn’t believe that she would leave me like that. Assistant District Attorney: What did you do next? Cindy Hall: Well, I was stranded and I didn’t have any money. So, I started looking for a ride home. But it seemed like everyone was drunk. I was furious at Sarah. I tried to call my roommate, but she wasn’t home. Tom offered to let me stay the night at his place. A bunch of people were going to stay there because they were drunk. But, I didn’t want to stay. So, when Michael offered to give me a ride home I accepted. Assistant District Attorney: What happened when you got to the apartment? Cindy Hall: Well, Michael walked me to the door and he asked if he could come in for awhile to talk. I had spent most of the evening with him and I thought he was a good guy. He was so sweet and cute. I wanted him to like me, so I let him in. Assistant District Attorney: What happened next? 63 Cindy Hall: Well, we sat down on the couch and watched TV for a little while and then he kissed me on the mouth. It was nice, so I let him kiss me a second time. But, after the second kiss it became clear that he wanted to go further, and that he wanted to have sex. Please, I don’t want to talk about the details. Assistant District Attorney: You don’t have to right now. But, did you let him know that you didn’t want to have sex with him? Cindy Hall: Yes, I told him “no” several times. I screamed it at him, but he just wouldn’t listen. He kept telling me to relax, that I wanted it and I was a tease. But, he just didn’t care what I wanted. He raped me. Assistant District Attorney: Ok, Cindy after Michael left what happened next? Cindy Hall: Well, when he left I just sat there. ] I couldn’t believe what had happened to me. I was so angry and ashamed. I knew I needed to call the police, but I just didn’t have the energy to do anything. Then my roommate came home a little while later. She saw that I was upset and she asked me what happened. I told her everything. Then, she called the police for me, and stayed with my while they interviewed me. Then she went to hospital with me for the rape exam. She was so great. I don’t think I would have been able to get through this without her help. Assistant District Attorney: Cindy I know this is hard for you and, and I appreciate your being open with me. We’ll talk again later, ok? Cindy Hall: Ok. 64 Thin, Virgin, Modestly Dressed On May 29, 2011, an Assistant District Attorney interviewed an alleged rape victim, Cindy Hall. The district attorney interviewed Cindy in order to prepare the criminal case against the alleged rapist. Below is a transcript of the interview between the Assistant District Attorney and Cindy Hall. Interview with Cindy Hall Assistant District Attorney: Ms. Hall we are currently investigating your case and are gathering evidence to determine if we will be able to prosecute the man who raped you. I need to ask you some questions and you have to be completely open and honest with me. Some of the questions will be very personal, but I have to learn as much I possibly can about you, and about what happened to you if we are going to successfully prosecute the man who hurt you. Do you understand? I need you to tell me everything, even if it makes you uncomfortable. Cindy Hall: Ok, I’ll try. Assistant District Attorney: Ok, Cindy, before we continue, I want to read an excerpt from the police officer who took the report on the night of your rape. “The complainant is a twenty year old Caucasian woman. She is five feet four inches tall and weighs 115 pounds. She has long blonde hair and blue eyes. She is dressed in a blue long-sleeve turtleneck shirt and tan baggy pants.” Does this accurately describe you on that night? Cindy Hall: Yes, that sounds right, although, I actually weigh 110 pounds. Assistant District Attorney: The report also indicated that you were not wearing skimpy clothes the night of the party. Is that correct? Cindy Hall: Well, I mean, it was a party and I wanted to look good but my jeans weren’t tight or anything. Also, my shirt covered me pretty well, I mean, my stomach and breasts were covered you know. Assistant District Attorney: Thank you for confirming. Ok, Cindy, now I need to ask you a few questions about your personal life. I need to ask you about your sexual history in case it comes up in court. Cindy Hall: But, I thought they couldn’t ask about that in court. Assistant District Attorney: Technically they can’t. However, sometimes the defendant’s lawyer gets around that. So, we need to prepare. I need to ask you some personal questions, ok? 65 Cindy Hall: Ok. Assistant District Attorney: Let’s start with your recent sexual history. How many men have you been with recently? Cindy Hall: Well, it’s been kind of a quiet year for me. I haven’t had a steady boyfriend for awhile so I’ve been hanging out a lot with my friend Sarah and some of my other friends. Assistant District Attorney: Ok, Cindy I want you to be honest with me. How many men have you had sexual relations within the past year? Cindy Hall: I don’t see why this is relevant. It has absolutely nothing to do with Michael raping me. He had no right! I told him NO! Assistant District Attorney: Cindy, I have to ask you questions so that I can properly prepare you case. I’m not here to judge you. I just need to make sure that I have all of my bases covered. I want to make sure that when we go to court there are no surprises. Now, tell me, how many men have you had sexual relations with during the past year. Cindy Hall: I’m a virgin, OK! I’ve never had sex. I’m waiting until I get married. Assistant District Attorney: Cindy, I appreciate your honesty. This is really helpful. Now, I need you to tell me what happened to you that night. Cindy Hall: Well, I went to this party on Saturday night about 9:00. My friend Tom had just graduated and he was celebrating. Assistant District Attorney: Are you very close friends with Tom? Cindy Hall: No, not really. We just had that one class together. We never dated or anything, but I liked him. We used to have lunch sometimes after class. Tom is nice. He’s a good guy, so when he asked me to his graduation party, I wanted to go. Assistant District Attorney: Where was the party held? Cindy Hall: It was at Tom’s apartment, a few miles from school. Assistant District Attorney: Have you ever been to Tom’s apartment before? 66 Cindy Hall: No, like I said, I had just met him last semester, and we never dated or anything. We just had lunch sometimes. Assistant District Attorney: Did you go to the party alone? Cindy Hall: No, I didn’t want to go alone so I asked my friend Sarah to go with me. She didn’t know Tom or anything, but she is really outgoing and she likes parties and meeting new people. Assistant District Attorney: When you got to the party did you know anyone there besides Tom? Cindy Hall: No, there were a bunch of people there, but I only knew Tom. Assistant District Attorney: How many people were there? Cindy Hall: I guess there were about 20 people there, but I can’t be sure, because some people were inside and some people were hanging outside near the pool. Assistant District Attorney: Were people drinking at the party? Cindy Hall: Well, Tom had a beer keg. Assistant District Attorney: Were you drinking? Cindy Hall: No, I wasn’t drinking at all. Assistant District Attorney: Why not? Cindy Hall: Well, Sarah drove, but I’ve gone to parties with Sarah before, and sometimes she drinks too much and I was afraid I would have to drive home. Assistant District Attorney: Were there drugs at the party? Cindy Hall: I didn’t see anyone using drugs and I never use them. Assistant District Attorney: So what happened during the party? Cindy Hall: Well, people were hanging out, dancing, laughing. It was a good party. Assistant District Attorney: Who did you spend most of your time with? 67 Cindy Hall: Well, I was hanging Sarah and Tom for awhile, but it became apparent that they were really into each other. So, I gave them some space. Assistant District Attorney: What happened next? Cindy Hall: Well, I was mingling and that’s when I met Michael. He asked me if I wanted to dance. We must have danced for at least a half of hour. Then we went to get a soda. I always get me own beverages at parties, you know in case someone tries to slip me something. Assistant District Attorney: Was Michael drinking? Cindy Hall: I don’t think so. I mean, I didn’t see him drinking. Assistant District Attorney: Ok, go on. Cindy Hall: Well, it was getting late, it was almost 1:00 in the morning and I was ready to go home. So, I went to look for Sarah, and she was gone! I couldn’t believe it! Tom told me they had an argument and she just left. I couldn’t believe that she would leave me like that. Assistant District Attorney: What did you do next? Cindy Hall: Well, I was stranded and I didn’t have any money. So, I started looking for a ride home. But it seemed like everyone was drunk. I was furious at Sarah. I tried to call my roommate, but she wasn’t home. Tom offered to let me stay the night at his place. A bunch of people were going to stay there because they were drunk. But, I didn’t want to stay. So, when Michael offered to give me a ride home I accepted. Assistant District Attorney: What happened when you got to the apartment? Cindy Hall: Well, Michael walked me to the door and he asked if he could come in for awhile to talk. I had spent most of the evening with him and I thought he was a good guy. He was so sweet and cute. I wanted him to like me, so I let him in. Assistant District Attorney: What happened next? Cindy Hall: Well, we sat down on the couch and watched TV for a little while and then he kissed me on the mouth. It was nice, so I let him kiss me a second time. But, after the second kiss it became clear that he wanted to go further, and that he wanted to have sex. Please, I don’t want to talk about the details. 68 Assistant District Attorney: You don’t have to right now. But, did you let him know that you didn’t want to have sex with him? Cindy Hall: Yes, I told him “no” several times. I screamed it at him, but he just wouldn’t listen. He kept telling me to relax, that I wanted it and I was a tease. But, he just didn’t care what I wanted. He raped me. Assistant District Attorney: Ok, Cindy after Michael left what happened next? Cindy Hall: Well, when he left I just sat there. I couldn’t believe what had happened to me. I was so angry and ashamed. I knew I needed to call the police, but I just didn’t have the energy to do anything. Then my roommate came home a little while later. She saw that I was upset and she asked me what happened. I told her everything. Then, she called the police for me, and stayed with my while they interviewed me. Then she went to hospital with me for the rape exam. She was so great. I don’t think I would have been able to get through this without her help. Assistant District Attorney: Cindy I know this is hard for you and, and I appreciate your being open with me. We’ll talk again later, ok? Cindy Hall: Ok. 69 Thin, Promiscuous, Modestly Dressed On May 29, 2011, an Assistant District Attorney interviewed an alleged rape victim, Cindy Hall. The district attorney interviewed Cindy in order to prepare the criminal case against the alleged rapist. Below is a transcript of the interview between the Assistant District Attorney and Cindy Hall. Interview with Cindy Hall Assistant District Attorney: Ms. Hall we are currently investigating your case and are gathering evidence to determine if we will be able to prosecute the man who raped you. I need to ask you some questions and you have to be completely open and honest with me. Some of the questions will be very personal, but I have to learn as much I possibly can about you, and about what happened to you if we are going to successfully prosecute the man who hurt you. Do you understand? I need you to tell me everything, even if it makes you uncomfortable. Cindy Hall: Ok, I’ll try. Assistant District Attorney: Ok, Cindy, before we continue, I want to read an excerpt from the police officer who took the report on the night of your rape. “The complainant is a twenty year old Caucasian woman. She is five feet four inches tall and weighs 115 pounds. She has long blonde hair and blue eyes. She is dressed in a blue long-sleeve turtleneck shirt and tan baggy pants.” Does this accurately describe you on that night? Cindy Hall: Yes, that sounds right, although, I actually weigh 110 pounds. Assistant District Attorney: The report also indicated that you were not wearing skimpy clothed the night of the party. Is that correct? Cindy Hall: Well, I mean, it was a party and I wanted to look good but my jeans weren’t tight or anything. Also, my shirt covered me pretty well, I mean, my stomach and breasts were covered you know. Assistant District Attorney: Thank you for confirming. Ok, Cindy, now I need to ask you a few questions about your personal life. I need to ask you about your sexual history in case it comes up in court. Cindy Hall: But, I thought they couldn’t ask about that in court. 70 Assistant District Attorney: Technically they can’t. However, sometimes the defendant’s lawyer gets around that. So, we need to prepare. I need to ask you some personal questions, ok? Cindy Hall: Ok. Assistant District Attorney: Let’s start with your recent sexual history. How many men have you been with recently? Cindy Hall: Well, it’s been kind of a wild year for me. I haven’t had a steady boyfriend for awhile so I’ve been hanging out a lot with my friend Sarah and some of my other friends. Sometimes we would go out to parties and hook up with someone for the evening, usually guys from school. Assistant District Attorney: What do you mean by “hook up”? Cindy Hall: You know, hang out and maybe mess around. Assistant District Attorney: You mean you would have sexual intercourse with these men? Cindy Hall: Yes, we would have sex. Assistant District Attorney: Ok, Cindy I want you to be honest with me. How many men have you had sexual relations within the past year? Cindy Hall: I don’t see why this is relevant. It has absolutely nothing to do with Michael raping me. He had no right! I told him NO! Assistant District Attorney: Cindy, I have to ask you questions so that I can properly prepare you case. I’m not here to judge you. I just need to make sure that I have all of my bases covered. I want to make sure that when we go to court there are no surprises. Now, tell me, how many men have you had sexual relations with during the past year. Cindy Hall: 15, OK! I had sex with 15 men this year. Assistant District Attorney: Did you date any of these men or see them more than once? Cindy Hall: No, we never really dated. It was a casual thing. We would hook up at a party at someone’s house, spend the evening dancing and hanging out. 71 Assistant District Attorney: Did you take them back to your apartment or go to their homes? Cindy Hall: No, never! We would just hang out at the parties and find some privacy. I was afraid to go to their homes or to bring them to my place. Assistant District Attorney: Cindy, I appreciate your honesty. This is really helpful. Now, I need you to tell me what happened to you that night. Cindy Hall: Well, I went to this party on Saturday night about 9:00. My friend Tom had just graduated and he was celebrating. Assistant District Attorney: Are you very close friends with Tom? Cindy Hall: No, not really. We just had that one class together. We never dated or anything, but I liked him. We used to have lunch sometimes after class. Tom is nice. He’s a good guy, so when he asked me to his graduation party, I wanted to go. Assistant District Attorney: Where was the party held? Cindy Hall: It was at Tom’s apartment, a few miles from school. Assistant District Attorney: Have you ever been to Tom’s apartment before? Cindy Hall: No, like I said, I had just met him last semester, and we never dated or anything. We just had lunch sometimes. Assistant District Attorney: Did you go to the party alone? Cindy Hall: No, I didn’t want to go alone so I asked my friend Sarah to go with me. She didn’t know Tom or anything, but she is really outgoing and she likes parties and meeting new people. Assistant District Attorney: When you got to the party did you know anyone there besides Tom? Cindy Hall: No, there were a bunch of people there, but I only knew Tom. Assistant District Attorney: How many people were there? Cindy Hall: I guess there were about 20 people there, but I can’t be sure, because some people were inside and some people were hanging outside near the pool. 72 Assistant District Attorney: Were people drinking at the party? Cindy Hall: Well, Tom had a beer keg. Assistant District Attorney: Were you drinking? Cindy Hall: No, I wasn’t drinking at all. Assistant District Attorney: Why not? Cindy Hall: Well, Sarah drove, but I’ve gone to parties with Sarah before, and sometimes she drinks too much and I was afraid I would have to drive home. Assistant District Attorney: Were there drugs at the party? Cindy Hall: I didn’t see anyone using drugs and I never use them. Assistant District Attorney: So what happened during the party? Cindy Hall: Well, people were hanging out, dancing, laughing. It was a good party. Assistant District Attorney: Who did you spend most of your time with? Cindy Hall: Well, I was hanging Sarah and Tom for awhile, but it became apparent that they were really into each other. So, I gave them some space. Assistant District Attorney: What happened next? Cindy Hall: Well, I was mingling and that’s when I met Michael. He asked me if I wanted to dance. We must have danced for at least a half of hour. Then we went to get a soda. I always get me own beverages at parties, you know in case someone tries to slip me something. Assistant District Attorney: Was Michael drinking? Cindy Hall: I don’t think so. I mean, I didn’t see him drinking. Assistant District Attorney: Ok, go on. Cindy Hall: Well, it was getting late, it was almost 1:00 in the morning and I was ready to go home. So, I went to look for Sarah, and she was gone! I couldn’t 73 believe it! Tom told me they had an argument and she just left. I couldn’t believe that she would leave me like that. Assistant District Attorney: What did you do next? Cindy Hall: Well, I was stranded and I didn’t have any money. So, I started looking for a ride home. But it seemed like everyone was drunk. I was furious at Sarah. I tried to call my roommate, but she wasn’t home. Tom offered to let me stay the night at his place. A bunch of people were going to stay there because they were drunk. But, I didn’t want to stay. So, when Michael offered to give me a ride home I accepted. Assistant District Attorney: What happened when you got to the apartment? Cindy Hall: Well, Michael walked me to the door and he asked if he could come in for awhile to talk. I had spent most of the evening with him and I thought he was a good guy. He was so sweet and cute. I wanted him to like me, so I let him in. Assistant District Attorney: What happened next? Cindy Hall: Well, we sat down on the couch and watched TV for a little while and then he kissed me on the mouth. It was nice, so I let him kiss me a second time. But, after the second kiss it became clear that he wanted to go further, and that he wanted to have sex. Please, I don’t want to talk about the details. Assistant District Attorney: You don’t have to right now. But, did you let him know that you didn’t want to have sex with him? Cindy Hall: Yes, I told him “no” several times. I screamed it at him, but he just wouldn’t listen. He kept telling me to relax, that I wanted it and I was a tease. But, he just didn’t care what I wanted. He raped me. Assistant District Attorney: Ok, Cindy after Michael left what happened next? Cindy Hall: Well, when he left I just sat there. I couldn’t believe what had happened to me. I was so angry and ashamed. I knew I needed to call the police, but I just didn’t have the energy to do anything. Then my roommate came home a little while later. She saw that I was upset and she asked me what happened. I told her everything. Then, she called the police for me, and stayed with my while they interviewed me. Then she went to hospital with me for the rape exam. She was so great. I don’t think I would have been able to get through this without her help. 74 Assistant District Attorney: Cindy I know this is hard for you and, and I appreciate your being open with me. We’ll talk again later, ok? Cindy Hall: Ok. 75 Thin, Promiscuous, Provocatively Dressed On May 29, 2011, an Assistant District Attorney interviewed an alleged rape victim, Cindy Hall. The district attorney interviewed Cindy in order to prepare the criminal case against the alleged rapist. Below is a transcript of the interview between the Assistant District Attorney and Cindy Hall. Interview with Cindy Hall Assistant District Attorney: Ms. Hall we are currently investigating your case and are gathering evidence to determine if we will be able to prosecute the man who raped you. I need to ask you some questions and you have to be completely open and honest with me. Some of the questions will be very personal, but I have to learn as much I possibly can about you, and about what happened to you if we are going to successfully prosecute the man who hurt you. Do you understand? I need you to tell me everything, even if it makes you uncomfortable. Cindy Hall: Ok, I’ll try. Assistant District Attorney: Ok, Cindy, before we continue, I want to read an excerpt from the police officer who took the report on the night of your rape. “The complainant is a twenty year old Caucasian woman. She is five feet four inches tall and weighs 115 pounds. She has long blonde hair and blue eyes. She is dressed in a skimpy blue halter top and very short mini skirt.” Does this accurately describe you on that night? Cindy Hall: Yes, that sounds right, although, I actually weigh 110 pounds. Assistant District Attorney: The report also indicated that you were wearing skimpy clothes the night of the party. Is that correct? Cindy Hall: Well, I means, it was a party and I wanted to look good. I was wearing a mini shirt and a halter top. My skirt was short, and my shirt, well, it was short too and my breasts did show some. I mean, you could see my belly button and cleavage. Assistant District Attorney: Thank you for confirming. Ok, Cindy, now I need to ask you a few questions about your personal life. I need to ask you about your sexual history in case it comes up in court. Cindy Hall: But, I thought they couldn’t ask about that in court. 76 Assistant District Attorney: Technically they can’t. However, sometimes the defendant’s lawyer gets around that. So, we need to prepare. I need to ask you some personal questions, ok? Cindy Hall: Ok. Assistant District Attorney: Let’s start with your recent sexual history. How many men have you been with recently? Cindy Hall: Well, it’s been kind of a wild year for me. I haven’t had a steady boyfriend for awhile so I’ve been hanging out a lot with my friend Sarah and some of my other friends. Sometimes we would go out to parties and hook up with someone for the evening, usually guys from school. Assistant District Attorney: What do you mean by “hook up”? Cindy Hall: You know, hang out and maybe mess around. Assistant District Attorney: You mean you would have sexual intercourse with these men? Cindy Hall: Yes, we would have sex. Assistant District Attorney: Ok, Cindy I want you to be honest with me. How many men have you had sexual relations within the past year? Cindy Hall: I don’t see why this is relevant. It has absolutely nothing to do with Michael raping me. He had no right! I told him NO! Assistant District Attorney: Cindy, I have to ask you questions so that I can properly prepare you case. I’m not here to judge you. I just need to make sure that I have all of my bases covered. I want to make sure that when we go to court there are no surprises. Now, tell me, how many men have you had sexual relations with during the past year. Cindy Hall: 15, OK! I had sex with 15 men this year. Assistant District Attorney: Did you date any of these men or see them more than once? Cindy Hall: No, we never really dated. It was a casual thing. We would hook up at a party at someone’s house, spend the evening dancing and hanging out. 77 Assistant District Attorney: Did you take them back to your apartment or go to their homes? Cindy Hall: No, never! We would just hang out at the parties and find some privacy. I was afraid to go to their homes or to bring them to my place. Assistant District Attorney: Cindy, I appreciate your honesty. This is really helpful. Now, I need you to tell me what happened to you that night. Cindy Hall: Well, I went to this party on Saturday night about 9:00. My friend Tom had just graduated and he was celebrating. Assistant District Attorney: Are you very close friends with Tom? Cindy Hall: No, not really. We just had that one class together. We never dated or anything, but I liked him. We used to have lunch sometimes after class. Tom is nice. He’s a good guy, so when he asked me to his graduation party, I wanted to go. Assistant District Attorney: Where was the party held? Cindy Hall: It was at Tom’s apartment, a few miles from school. Assistant District Attorney: Have you ever been to Tom’s apartment before? Cindy Hall: No, like I said, I had just met him last semester, and we never dated or anything. We just had lunch sometimes. Assistant District Attorney: Did you go to the party alone? Cindy Hall: No, I didn’t want to go alone so I asked my friend Sarah to go with me. She didn’t know Tom or anything, but she is really outgoing and she likes parties and meeting new people. Assistant District Attorney: When you got to the party did you know anyone there besides Tom? Cindy Hall: No, there were a bunch of people there, but I only knew Tom. Assistant District Attorney: How many people were there? Cindy Hall: I guess there were about 20 people there, but I can’t be sure, because some people were inside and some people were hanging outside near the pool. 78 Assistant District Attorney: Were people drinking at the party? Cindy Hall: Well, Tom had a beer keg. Assistant District Attorney: Were you drinking? Cindy Hall: No, I wasn’t drinking at all. Assistant District Attorney: Why not? Cindy Hall: Well, Sarah drove, but I’ve gone to parties with Sarah before, and sometimes she drinks too much and I was afraid I would have to drive home. Assistant District Attorney: Were there drugs at the party? Cindy Hall: I didn’t see anyone using drugs and I never use them. Assistant District Attorney: So what happened during the party? Cindy Hall: Well, people were hanging out, dancing, laughing. It was a good party. Assistant District Attorney: Who did you spend most of your time with? Cindy Hall: Well, I was hanging Sarah and Tom for awhile, but it became apparent that they were really into each other. So, I gave them some space. Assistant District Attorney: What happened next? Cindy Hall: Well, I was mingling and that’s when I met Michael. He asked me if I wanted to dance. We must have danced for at least a half of hour. Then we went to get a soda. I always get me own beverages at parties, you know in case someone tries to slip me something. Assistant District Attorney: Was Michael drinking? Cindy Hall: I don’t think so. I mean, I didn’t see him drinking. Assistant District Attorney: Ok, go on. Cindy Hall: Well, it was getting late, it was almost 1:00 in the morning and I was ready to go home. So, I went to look for Sarah, and she was gone! I couldn’t 79 believe it! Tom told me they had an argument and she just left. I couldn’t believe that she would leave me like that. Assistant District Attorney: What did you do next? Cindy Hall: Well, I was stranded and I didn’t have any money. So, I started looking for a ride home. But it seemed like everyone was drunk. I was furious at Sarah. I tried to call my roommate, but she wasn’t home. Tom offered to let me stay the night at his place. A bunch of people were going to stay there because they were drunk. But, I didn’t want to stay. So, when Michael offered to give me a ride home I accepted. Assistant District Attorney: What happened when you got to the apartment? Cindy Hall: Well, Michael walked me to the door and he asked if he could come in for awhile to talk. I had spent most of the evening with him and I thought he was a good guy. He was so sweet and cute. I wanted him to like me, so I let him in. Assistant District Attorney: What happened next? Cindy Hall: Well, we sat down on the couch and watched TV for a little while and then he kissed me on the mouth. It was nice, so I let him kiss me a second time. But, after the second kiss it became clear that he wanted to go further, and that he wanted to have sex. Please, I don’t want to talk about the details. Assistant District Attorney: You don’t have to right now. But, did you let him know that you didn’t want to have sex with him? Cindy Hall: Yes, I told him “no” several times. I screamed it at him, but he just wouldn’t listen. He kept telling me to relax, that I wanted it and I was a tease. But, he just didn’t care what I wanted. He raped me. Assistant District Attorney: Ok, Cindy after Michael left what happened next? Cindy Hall: Well, when he left I just sat there. I couldn’t believe what had happened to me. I was so angry and ashamed. I knew I needed to call the police, but I just didn’t have the energy to do anything. Then my roommate came home a little while later. She saw that I was upset and she asked me what happened. I told her everything. Then, she called the police for me, and stayed with my while they interviewed me. Then she went to hospital with me for the rape exam. She was so great. I don’t think I would have been able to get through this without her help. 80 Assistant District Attorney: Cindy I know this is hard for you and, and I appreciate your being open with me. We’ll talk again later, ok? Cindy Hall: Ok. 81 Obese, Virgin, Provocatively Dressed On May 29, 2011, an Assistant District Attorney interviewed an alleged rape victim, Cindy Hall. The district attorney interviewed Cindy in order to prepare the criminal case against the alleged rapist. Below is a transcript of the interview between the Assistant District Attorney and Cindy Hall. Interview with Cindy Hall Assistant District Attorney: Ms. Hall we are currently investigating your case and are gathering evidence to determine if we will be able to prosecute the man who raped you. I need to ask you some questions and you have to be completely open and honest with me. Some of the questions will be very personal, but I have to learn as much I possibly can about you, and about what happened to you if we are going to successfully prosecute the man who hurt you. Do you understand? I need you to tell me everything, even if it makes you uncomfortable. Cindy Hall: Ok, I’ll try. Assistant District Attorney: Ok, Cindy, before we continue, I want to read an excerpt from the police officer who took the report on the night of your rape. “The complainant is a twenty year old Caucasian woman. She is five feet four inches tall and weighs 215 pounds. She has long blonde hair and blue eyes. She is dressed in a skimpy blue halter top and very short mini skirt.” Does this accurately describe you on that night? Cindy Hall: Yes, that sounds right, although, I actually weigh 210 pounds. Assistant District Attorney: The report also indicates that you were wearing skimpy clothes the night of the party. Is that correct? Cindy Hall: Well, I mean, it was a party and I wanted to look good. I was wearing a mini skirt and a halter top. My skirt was short, and my shirt, well, it was short too and my breasts did show some. I mean, you could see my belly button and cleavage. Assistant District Attorney: Thank you for confirming. Ok, Cindy, now I need to ask you a few questions about your personal life. I need to ask you about your sexual history in case it comes up in court. Cindy Hall: But, I thought they couldn’t ask about that in court. 82 Assistant District Attorney: Technically they can’t. However, sometimes the defendant’s lawyer gets around that. So, we need to prepare. I need to ask you some personal questions, ok? Cindy Hall: Ok. Assistant District Attorney: Let’s start with your recent sexual history. How many men have you been with recently? Cindy Hall: Well, it’s been kind of a quiet year for me. I haven’t had a steady boyfriend for awhile so I’ve been hanging out a lot with my friend Sarah and some of my other friends. Assistant District Attorney: Ok, Cindy I want you to be honest with me. How many men have you had sexual relations within the past year? Cindy Hall: I don’t see why this is relevant. It has absolutely nothing to do with Michael raping me. He had no right! I told him NO! Assistant District Attorney: Cindy, I have to ask you questions so that I can properly prepare you case. I’m not here to judge you. I just need to make sure that I have all of my bases covered. I want to make sure that when we go to court there are no surprises. Now, tell me, how many men have you had sexual relations with during the past year. Cindy Hall: I’m a virgin, OK! I’ve never had sex. I’m waiting until I get married. Assistant District Attorney: Cindy, I appreciate your honesty. This is really helpful. Now, I need you to tell me what happened to you that night. Cindy Hall: Well, I went to this party on Saturday night about 9:00. My friend Tom had just graduated and he was celebrating. Assistant District Attorney: Are you very close friends with Tom? Cindy Hall: No, not really. We just had that one class together. We never dated or anything, but I liked him. We used to have lunch sometimes after class. Tom is nice. He’s a good guy, so when he asked me to his graduation party, I wanted to go. Assistant District Attorney: Where was the party held? Cindy Hall: It was at Tom’s apartment, a few miles from school. 83 Assistant District Attorney: Have you ever been to Tom’s apartment before? Cindy Hall: No, like I said, I had just met him last semester, and we never dated or anything. We just had lunch sometimes. Assistant District Attorney: Did you go to the party alone? Cindy Hall: No, I didn’t want to go alone so I asked my friend Sarah to go with me. She didn’t know Tom or anything, but she is really outgoing and she likes parties and meeting new people. Assistant District Attorney: When you got to the party did you know anyone there besides Tom? Cindy Hall: No, there were a bunch of people there, but I only knew Tom. Assistant District Attorney: How many people were there? Cindy Hall: I guess there were about 20 people there, but I can’t be sure, because some people were inside and some people were hanging outside near the pool. Assistant District Attorney: Were people drinking at the party? Cindy Hall: Well, Tom had a beer keg. Assistant District Attorney: Were you drinking? Cindy Hall: No, I wasn’t drinking at all. Assistant District Attorney: Why not? Cindy Hall: Well, Sarah drove, but I’ve gone to parties with Sarah before, and sometimes she drinks too much and I was afraid I would have to drive home. Assistant District Attorney: Were there drugs at the party? Cindy Hall: I didn’t see anyone using drugs and I never use them. Assistant District Attorney: So what happened during the party? Cindy Hall: Well, people were hanging out, dancing, laughing. It was a good party. 84 Assistant District Attorney: Who did you spend most of your time with? Cindy Hall: Well, I was hanging Sarah and Tom for awhile, but it became apparent that they were really into each other. So, I gave them some space. Assistant District Attorney: What happened next? Cindy Hall: Well, I was mingling and that’s when I met Michael. He asked me if I wanted to dance. We must have danced for at least a half of hour. Then we went to get a soda. I always get me own beverages at parties, you know in case someone tries to slip me something. Assistant District Attorney: Was Michael drinking? Cindy Hall: I don’t think so. I mean, I didn’t see him drinking. Assistant District Attorney: Ok, go on. Cindy Hall: Well, it was getting late, it was almost 1:00 in the morning and I was ready to go home. So, I went to look for Sarah, and she was gone! I couldn’t believe it! Tom told me they had an argument and she just left. I couldn’t believe that she would leave me like that. Assistant District Attorney: What did you do next? Cindy Hall: Well, I was stranded and I didn’t have any money. So, I started looking for a ride home. But it seemed like everyone was drunk. I was furious at Sarah. I tried to call my roommate, but she wasn’t home. Tom offered to let me stay the night at his place. A bunch of people were going to stay there because they were drunk. But, I didn’t want to stay. So, when Michael offered to give me a ride home I accepted. Assistant District Attorney: What happened when you got to the apartment? Cindy Hall: Well, Michael walked me to the door and he asked if he could come in for awhile to talk. I had spent most of the evening with him and I thought he was a good guy. He was so sweet and cute. I wanted him to like me, so I let him in. Assistant District Attorney: What happened next? Cindy Hall: Well, we sat down on the couch and watched TV for a little while and then he kissed me on the mouth. It was nice, so I let him kiss me a second 85 time. But, after the second kiss it became clear that he wanted to go further, and that he wanted to have sex. Please, I don’t want to talk about the details. Assistant District Attorney: You don’t have to right now. But, did you let him know that you didn’t want to have sex with him? Cindy Hall: Yes, I told him “no” several times. I screamed it at him, but he just wouldn’t listen. He kept telling me to relax, that I wanted it and I was a tease. But, he just didn’t care what I wanted. He raped me. Assistant District Attorney: Ok, Cindy after Michael left what happened next? Cindy Hall: Well, when he left I just sat there. I couldn’t believe what had happened to me. I was so angry and ashamed. I knew I needed to call the police, but I just didn’t have the energy to do anything. Then my roommate came home a little while later. She saw that I was upset and she asked me what happened. I told her everything. Then, she called the police for me, and stayed with my while they interviewed me. Then she went to hospital with me for the rape exam. She was so great. I don’t think I would have been able to get through this without her help. Assistant District Attorney: Cindy I know this is hard for you and, and I appreciate your being open with me. We’ll talk again later, ok? Cindy Hall: Ok. 86 Obese, Virgin, Modestly Dressed On May 29, 2011, an Assistant District Attorney interviewed an alleged rape victim, Cindy Hall. The district attorney interviewed Cindy in order to prepare the criminal case against the alleged rapist. Below is a transcript of the interview between the Assistant District Attorney and Cindy Hall. Interview with Cindy Hall Assistant District Attorney: Ms. Hall we are currently investigating your case and are gathering evidence to determine if we will be able to prosecute the man who raped you. I need to ask you some questions and you have to be completely open and honest with me. Some of the questions will be very personal, but I have to learn as much I possibly can about you, and about what happened to you if we are going to successfully prosecute the man who hurt you. Do you understand? I need you to tell me everything, even if it makes you uncomfortable. Cindy Hall: Ok, I’ll try. Assistant District Attorney: Ok, Cindy, before we continue, I want to read an excerpt from the police officer who took the report on the night of your rape. “The complainant is a twenty year old Caucasian woman. She is five feet four inches tall and weighs 215 pounds. She has long blonde hair and blue eyes. She is dressed in a blue long-sleeve turtleneck shirt and tan baggy pants.” Does this accurately describe you on that night? Cindy Hall: Yes, that sounds right, although, I actually weigh 210 pounds. Assistant District Attorney: The report also indicated that you were not wearing skimpy clothes the night of the party. Is that correct? Cindy Hall: Well, I mean, it was a party and I wanted to look good but my jeans weren’t tight or anything. Also, my shirt covered me pretty well, I mean, my stomach and breasts were covered you know. Assistant District Attorney: Thank you for confirming. Ok, Cindy, now I need to ask you a few questions about your personal life. I need to ask you about your sexual history in case it comes up in court. Cindy Hall: But, I thought they couldn’t ask about that in court. 87 Assistant District Attorney: Technically they can’t. However, sometimes the defendant’s lawyer gets around that. So, we need to prepare. I need to ask you some personal questions, ok? Cindy Hall: Ok. Assistant District Attorney: Let’s start with your recent sexual history. How many men have you been with recently? Cindy Hall: Well, it’s been kind of a quiet year for me. I haven’t had a steady boyfriend for awhile so I’ve been hanging out a lot with my friend Sarah and some of my other friends. Assistant District Attorney: Ok, Cindy I want you to be honest with me. How many men have you had sexual relations within the past year? Cindy Hall: I don’t see why this is relevant. It has absolutely nothing to do with Michael raping me. He had no right! I told him NO! Assistant District Attorney: Cindy, I have to ask you questions so that I can properly prepare you case. I’m not here to judge you. I just need to make sure that I have all of my bases covered. I want to make sure that when we go to court there are no surprises. Now, tell me, how many men have you had sexual relations with during the past year. Cindy Hall: I’m a virgin, OK! I’ve never had sex. I’m waiting until I get married. Assistant District Attorney: Cindy, I appreciate your honesty. This is really helpful. Now, I need you to tell me what happened to you that night. Cindy Hall: Well, I went to this party on Saturday night about 9:00. My friend Tom had just graduated and he was celebrating. Assistant District Attorney: Are you very close friends with Tom? Cindy Hall: No, not really. We just had that one class together. We never dated or anything, but I liked him. We used to have lunch sometimes after class. Tom is nice. He’s a good guy, so when he asked me to his graduation party, I wanted to go. Assistant District Attorney: Where was the party held? Cindy Hall: It was at Tom’s apartment, a few miles from school. 88 Assistant District Attorney: Have you ever been to Tom’s apartment before? Cindy Hall: No, like I said, I had just met him last semester, and we never dated or anything. We just had lunch sometimes. Assistant District Attorney: Did you go to the party alone? Cindy Hall: No, I didn’t want to go alone so I asked my friend Sarah to go with me. She didn’t know Tom or anything, but she is really outgoing and she likes parties and meeting new people. Assistant District Attorney: When you got to the party did you know anyone there besides Tom? Cindy Hall: No, there were a bunch of people there, but I only knew Tom. Assistant District Attorney: How many people were there? Cindy Hall: I guess there were about 20 people there, but I can’t be sure, because some people were inside and some people were hanging outside near the pool. Assistant District Attorney: Were people drinking at the party? Cindy Hall: Well, Tom had a beer keg. Assistant District Attorney: Were you drinking? Cindy Hall: No, I wasn’t drinking at all. Assistant District Attorney: Why not? Cindy Hall: Well, Sarah drove, but I’ve gone to parties with Sarah before, and sometimes she drinks too much and I was afraid I would have to drive home. Assistant District Attorney: Were there drugs at the party? Cindy Hall: I didn’t see anyone using drugs and I never use them. Assistant District Attorney: So what happened during the party? Cindy Hall: Well, people were hanging out, dancing, laughing. It was a good party. 89 Assistant District Attorney: Who did you spend most of your time with? Cindy Hall: Well, I was hanging Sarah and Tom for awhile, but it became apparent that they were really into each other. So, I gave them some space. Assistant District Attorney: What happened next? Cindy Hall: Well, I was mingling and that’s when I met Michael. He asked me if I wanted to dance. We must have danced for at least a half of hour. Then we went to get a soda. I always get me own beverages at parties, you know in case someone tries to slip me something. Assistant District Attorney: Was Michael drinking? Cindy Hall: I don’t think so. I mean, I didn’t see him drinking. Assistant District Attorney: Ok, go on. Cindy Hall: Well, it was getting late, it was almost 1:00 in the morning and I was ready to go home. So, I went to look for Sarah, and she was gone! I couldn’t believe it! Tom told me they had an argument and she just left. I couldn’t believe that she would leave me like that. Assistant District Attorney: What did you do next? Cindy Hall: Well, I was stranded and I didn’t have any money. So, I started looking for a ride home. But it seemed like everyone was drunk. I was furious at Sarah. I tried to call my roommate, but she wasn’t home. Tom offered to let me stay the night at his place. A bunch of people were going to stay there because they were drunk. But, I didn’t want to stay. So, when Michael offered to give me a ride home I accepted. Assistant District Attorney: What happened when you got to the apartment? Cindy Hall: Well, Michael walked me to the door and he asked if he could come in for awhile to talk. I had spent most of the evening with him and I thought he was a good guy. He was so sweet and cute. I wanted him to like me, so I let him in. Assistant District Attorney: What happened next? Cindy Hall: Well, we sat down on the couch and watched TV for a little while and then he kissed me on the mouth. It was nice, so I let him kiss me a second 90 time. But, after the second kiss it became clear that he wanted to go further, and that he wanted to have sex. Please, I don’t want to talk about the details. Assistant District Attorney: You don’t have to right now. But, did you let him know that you didn’t want to have sex with him? Cindy Hall: Yes, I told him “no” several times. I screamed it at him, but he just wouldn’t listen. He kept telling me to relax, that I wanted it and I was a tease. But, he just didn’t care what I wanted. He raped me. Assistant District Attorney: Ok, Cindy after Michael left what happened next? Cindy Hall: Well, when he left I just sat there. I couldn’t believe what had happened to me. I was so angry and ashamed. I knew I needed to call the police, but I just didn’t have the energy to do anything. Then my roommate came home a little while later. She saw that I was upset and she asked me what happened. I told her everything. Then, she called the police for me, and stayed with my while they interviewed me. Then she went to hospital with me for the rape exam. She was so great. I don’t think I would have been able to get through this without her help. Assistant District Attorney: Cindy I know this is hard for you and, and I appreciate your being open with me. We’ll talk again later, ok? Cindy Hall: Ok. 91 Obese, Promiscuous, Modestly Dressed On May 29, 2011, an Assistant District Attorney interviewed an alleged rape victim, Cindy Hall. The district attorney interviewed Cindy in order to prepare the criminal case against the alleged rapist. Below is a transcript of the interview between the Assistant District Attorney and Cindy Hall. Interview with Cindy Hall Assistant District Attorney: Ms. Hall we are currently investigating your case and are gathering evidence to determine if we will be able to prosecute the man who raped you. I need to ask you some questions and you have to be completely open and honest with me. Some of the questions will be very personal, but I have to learn as much I possibly can about you, and about what happened to you if we are going to successfully prosecute the man who hurt you. Do you understand? I need you to tell me everything, even if it makes you uncomfortable. Cindy Hall: Ok, I’ll try. Assistant District Attorney: Ok, Cindy, before we continue, I want to read an excerpt from the police officer who took the report on the night of your rape. “The complainant is a twenty year old Caucasian woman. She is five feet four inches tall and weighs 215 pounds. She has long blonde hair and blue eyes. She is dressed in a blue long-sleeve turtleneck shirt and tan baggy pants.” Does this accurately describe you on that night? Cindy Hall: Yes, that sounds right, although, I actually weigh 210 pounds. Assistant District Attorney: The report also indicated that you were not wearing skimpy clothes the night of the party. Is that correct? Cindy Hall: Well, I mean, it was a party and I wanted to look good but my jeans weren’t tight or anything. Also, my shirt covered me pretty well, I mean, my stomach and breasts were covered you know. Assistant District Attorney: Thank you for confirming. Ok, Cindy, now I need to ask you a few questions about your personal life. I need to ask you about your sexual history in case it comes up in court. Cindy Hall: But, I thought they couldn’t ask about that in court. 92 Assistant District Attorney: Technically they can’t. However, sometimes the defendant’s lawyer gets around that. So, we need to prepare. I need to ask you some personal questions, ok? Cindy Hall: Ok. Assistant District Attorney: Let’s start with your recent sexual history. How many men have you been with recently? Cindy Hall: Well, it’s been kind of a wild year for me. I haven’t had a steady boyfriend for awhile so I’ve been hanging out a lot with my friend Sarah and some of my other friends. Sometimes we would go out to parties and hook up with someone for the evening, usually guys from school. Assistant District Attorney: What do you mean by “hook up”? Cindy Hall: You know, hang out and maybe mess around. Assistant District Attorney: You mean you would have sexual intercourse with these men? Cindy Hall: Yes, we would have sex. Assistant District Attorney: Ok, Cindy I want you to be honest with me. How many men have you had sexual relations within the past year? Cindy Hall: I don’t see why this is relevant. It has absolutely nothing to do with Michael raping me. He had no right! I told him NO! Assistant District Attorney: Cindy, I have to ask you questions so that I can properly prepare you case. I’m not here to judge you. I just need to make sure that I have all of my bases covered. I want to make sure that when we go to court there are no surprises. Now, tell me, how many men have you had sexual relations with during the past year. Cindy Hall: 15, OK! I had sex with 15 men this year. Assistant District Attorney: Did you date any of these men or see them more than once? Cindy Hall: No, we never really dated. It was a casual thing. We would hook up at a party at someone’s house, spend the evening dancing and hanging out. 93 Assistant District Attorney: Did you take them back to your apartment or go to their homes? Cindy Hall: No, never! We would just hang out at the parties and find some privacy. I was afraid to go to their homes or to bring them to my place. Assistant District Attorney: Cindy, I appreciate your honesty. This is really helpful. Now, I need you to tell me what happened to you that night. Cindy Hall: Well, I went to this party on Saturday night about 9:00. My friend Tom had just graduated and he was celebrating. Assistant District Attorney: Are you very close friends with Tom? Cindy Hall: No, not really. We just had that one class together. We never dated or anything, but I liked him. We used to have lunch sometimes after class. Tom is nice. He’s a good guy, so when he asked me to his graduation party, I wanted to go. Assistant District Attorney: Where was the party held? Cindy Hall: It was at Tom’s apartment, a few miles from school. Assistant District Attorney: Have you ever been to Tom’s apartment before? Cindy Hall: No, like I said, I had just met him last semester, and we never dated or anything. We just had lunch sometimes. Assistant District Attorney: Did you go to the party alone? Cindy Hall: No, I didn’t want to go alone so I asked my friend Sarah to go with me. She didn’t know Tom or anything, but she is really outgoing and she likes parties and meeting new people. Assistant District Attorney: When you got to the party did you know anyone there besides Tom? Cindy Hall: No, there were a bunch of people there, but I only knew Tom. Assistant District Attorney: How many people were there? Cindy Hall: I guess there were about 20 people there, but I can’t be sure, because some people were inside and some people were hanging outside near the pool. 94 Assistant District Attorney: Were people drinking at the party? Cindy Hall: Well, Tom had a beer keg. Assistant District Attorney: Were you drinking? Cindy Hall: No, I wasn’t drinking at all. Assistant District Attorney: Why not? Cindy Hall: Well, Sarah drove, but I’ve gone to parties with Sarah before, and sometimes she drinks too much and I was afraid I would have to drive home. Assistant District Attorney: Were there drugs at the party? Cindy Hall: I didn’t see anyone using drugs and I never use them. Assistant District Attorney: So what happened during the party? Cindy Hall: Well, people were hanging out, dancing, laughing. It was a good party. Assistant District Attorney: Who did you spend most of your time with? Cindy Hall: Well, I was hanging Sarah and Tom for awhile, but it became apparent that they were really into each other. So, I gave them some space. Assistant District Attorney: What happened next? Cindy Hall: Well, I was mingling and that’s when I met Michael. He asked me if I wanted to dance. We must have danced for at least a half of hour. Then we went to get a soda. I always get me own beverages at parties, you know in case someone tries to slip me something. Assistant District Attorney: Was Michael drinking? Cindy Hall: I don’t think so. I mean, I didn’t see him drinking. Assistant District Attorney: Ok, go on. Cindy Hall: Well, it was getting late, it was almost 1:00 in the morning and I was ready to go home. So, I went to look for Sarah, and she was gone! I couldn’t 95 believe it! Tom told me they had an argument and she just left. I couldn’t believe that she would leave me like that. Assistant District Attorney: What did you do next? Cindy Hall: Well, I was stranded and I didn’t have any money. So, I started looking for a ride home. But it seemed like everyone was drunk. I was furious at Sarah. I tried to call my roommate, but she wasn’t home. Tom offered to let me stay the night at his place. A bunch of people were going to stay there because they were drunk. But, I didn’t want to stay. So, when Michael offered to give me a ride home I accepted. Assistant District Attorney: What happened when you got to the apartment? Cindy Hall: Well, Michael walked me to the door and he asked if he could come in for awhile to talk. I had spent most of the evening with him and I thought he was a good guy. He was so sweet and cute. I wanted him to like me, so I let him in. Assistant District Attorney: What happened next? Cindy Hall: Well, we sat down on the couch and watched TV for a little while and then he kissed me on the mouth. It was nice, so I let him kiss me a second time. But, after the second kiss it became clear that he wanted to go further, and that he wanted to have sex. Please, I don’t want to talk about the details. Assistant District Attorney: You don’t have to right now. But, did you let him know that you didn’t want to have sex with him? Cindy Hall: Yes, I told him “no” several times. I screamed it at him, but he just wouldn’t listen. He kept telling me to relax, that I wanted it and I was a tease. But, he just didn’t care what I wanted. He raped me. Assistant District Attorney: Ok, Cindy after Michael left what happened next? Cindy Hall: Well, when he left I just sat there. I couldn’t believe what had happened to me. I was so angry and ashamed. I knew I needed to call the police, but I just didn’t have the energy to do anything. Then my roommate came home a little while later. She saw that I was upset and she asked me what happened. I told her everything. Then, she called the police for me, and stayed with my while they interviewed me. Then she went to hospital with me for the rape exam. She was so great. I don’t think I would have been able to get through this without her help. 96 Assistant District Attorney: Cindy I know this is hard for you and, and I appreciate your being open with me. We’ll talk again later, ok? Cindy Hall: Ok. 97 Obese, Promiscuous, Provocatively Dressed On May 29, 2011, an Assistant District Attorney interviewed an alleged rape victim, Cindy Hall. The district attorney interviewed Cindy in order to prepare the criminal case against the alleged rapist. Below is a transcript of the interview between the Assistant District Attorney and Cindy Hall. Interview with Cindy Hall Assistant District Attorney: Ms. Hall we are currently investigating your case and are gathering evidence to determine if we will be able to prosecute the man who raped you. I need to ask you some questions and you have to be completely open and honest with me. Some of the questions will be very personal, but I have to learn as much I possibly can about you, and about what happened to you if we are going to successfully prosecute the man who hurt you. Do you understand? I need you to tell me everything, even if it makes you uncomfortable. Cindy Hall: Ok, I’ll try. Assistant District Attorney: Ok, Cindy, before we continue, I want to read an excerpt from the police officer who took the report on the night of your rape. “The complainant is a twenty year old Caucasian woman. She is five feet four inches tall and weighs 215 pounds. She has long blonde hair and blue eyes. She is dressed in a skimpy blue halter top and very short mini skirt.” Does this accurately describe you on that night? Cindy Hall: Yes, that sounds right, although, I actually weigh 210 pounds. Assistant District Attorney: The report also indicated that you were wearing skimpy clothes the night of the party. Is that correct? Cindy Hall: Well, I mean, it was a party and I wanted to look good. I was wearing a mini skirt and a halter top. My skirt was short, and my shirt, well, it was short too and my breasts did show some. I mean, you could see my belly button and cleavage. Assistant District Attorney: Thank you for confirming. Ok, Cindy, now I need to ask you a few questions about your personal life. I need to ask you about your sexual history in case it comes up in court. Cindy Hall: But, I thought they couldn’t ask about that in court. 98 Assistant District Attorney: Technically they can’t. However, sometimes the defendant’s lawyer gets around that. So, we need to prepare. I need to ask you some personal questions, ok? Cindy Hall: Ok. Assistant District Attorney: Let’s start with your recent sexual history. How many men have you been with recently? Cindy Hall: Well, it’s been kind of a wild year for me. I haven’t had a steady boyfriend for awhile so I’ve been hanging out a lot with my friend Sarah and some of my other friends. Sometimes we would go out to parties and hook up with someone for the evening, usually guys from school. Assistant District Attorney: What do you mean by “hook up”? Cindy Hall: You know, hang out and maybe mess around. Assistant District Attorney: You mean you would have sexual intercourse with these men? Cindy Hall: Yes, we would have sex. Assistant District Attorney: Ok, Cindy I want you to be honest with me. How many men have you had sexual relations within the past year? Cindy Hall: I don’t see why this is relevant. It has absolutely nothing to do with Michael raping me. He had no right! I told him NO! Assistant District Attorney: Cindy, I have to ask you questions so that I can properly prepare you case. I’m not here to judge you. I just need to make sure that I have all of my bases covered. I want to make sure that when we go to court there are no surprises. Now, tell me, how many men have you had sexual relations with during the past year. Cindy Hall: 15, OK! I had sex with 15 men this year. Assistant District Attorney: Did you date any of these men or see them more than once? Cindy Hall: No, we never really dated. It was a casual thing. We would hook up at a party at someone’s house, spend the evening dancing and hanging out. 99 Assistant District Attorney: Did you take them back to your apartment or go to their homes? Cindy Hall: No, never! We would just hang out at the parties and find some privacy. I was afraid to go to their homes or to bring them to my place. Assistant District Attorney: Cindy, I appreciate your honesty. This is really helpful. Now, I need you to tell me what happened to you that night. Cindy Hall: Well, I went to this party on Saturday night about 9:00. My friend Tom had just graduated and he was celebrating. Assistant District Attorney: Are you very close friends with Tom? Cindy Hall: No, not really. We just had that one class together. We never dated or anything, but I liked him. We used to have lunch sometimes after class. Tom is nice. He’s a good guy, so when he asked me to his graduation party, I wanted to go. Assistant District Attorney: Where was the party held? Cindy Hall: It was at Tom’s apartment, a few miles from school. Assistant District Attorney: Have you ever been to Tom’s apartment before? Cindy Hall: No, like I said, I had just met him last semester, and we never dated or anything. We just had lunch sometimes. Assistant District Attorney: Did you go to the party alone? Cindy Hall: No, I didn’t want to go alone so I asked my friend Sarah to go with me. She didn’t know Tom or anything, but she is really outgoing and she likes parties and meeting new people. Assistant District Attorney: When you got to the party did you know anyone there besides Tom? Cindy Hall: No, there were a bunch of people there, but I only knew Tom. Assistant District Attorney: How many people were there? Cindy Hall: I guess there were about 20 people there, but I can’t be sure, because some people were inside and some people were hanging outside near the pool. 100 Assistant District Attorney: Were people drinking at the party? Cindy Hall: Well, Tom had a beer keg. Assistant District Attorney: Were you drinking? Cindy Hall: No, I wasn’t drinking at all. Assistant District Attorney: Why not? Cindy Hall: Well, Sarah drove, but I’ve gone to parties with Sarah before, and sometimes she drinks too much and I was afraid I would have to drive home. Assistant District Attorney: Were there drugs at the party? Cindy Hall: I didn’t see anyone using drugs and I never use them. Assistant District Attorney: So what happened during the party? Cindy Hall: Well, people were hanging out, dancing, laughing. It was a good party. Assistant District Attorney: Who did you spend most of your time with? Cindy Hall: Well, I was hanging Sarah and Tom for awhile, but it became apparent that they were really into each other. So, I gave them some space. Assistant District Attorney: What happened next? Cindy Hall: Well, I was mingling and that’s when I met Michael. He asked me if I wanted to dance. We must have danced for at least a half of hour. Then we went to get a soda. I always get me own beverages at parties, you know in case someone tries to slip me something. Assistant District Attorney: Was Michael drinking? Cindy Hall: I don’t think so. I mean, I didn’t see him drinking. Assistant District Attorney: Ok, go on. Cindy Hall: Well, it was getting late, it was almost 1:00 in the morning and I was ready to go home. So, I went to look for Sarah, and she was gone! I couldn’t 101 believe it! Tom told me they had an argument and she just left. I couldn’t believe that she would leave me like that. Assistant District Attorney: What did you do next? Cindy Hall: Well, I was stranded and I didn’t have any money. So, I started looking for a ride home. But it seemed like everyone was drunk. I was furious at Sarah. I tried to call my roommate, but she wasn’t home. Tom offered to let me stay the night at his place. A bunch of people were going to stay there because they were drunk. But, I didn’t want to stay. So, when Michael offered to give me a ride home I accepted. Assistant District Attorney: What happened when you got to the apartment? Cindy Hall: Well, Michael walked me to the door and he asked if he could come in for awhile to talk. I had spent most of the evening with him and I thought he was a good guy. He was so sweet and cute. I wanted him to like me, so I let him in. Assistant District Attorney: What happened next? Cindy Hall: Well, we sat down on the couch and watched TV for a little while and then he kissed me on the mouth. It was nice, so I let him kiss me a second time. But, after the second kiss it became clear that he wanted to go further, and that he wanted to have sex. Please, I don’t want to talk about the details. Assistant District Attorney: You don’t have to right now. But, did you let him know that you didn’t want to have sex with him? Cindy Hall: Yes, I told him “no” several times. I screamed it at him, but he just wouldn’t listen. He kept telling me to relax, that I wanted it and I was a tease. But, he just didn’t care what I wanted. He raped me. Assistant District Attorney: Ok, Cindy after Michael left what happened next? Cindy Hall: Well, when he left I just sat there. I couldn’t believe what had happened to me. I was so angry and ashamed. I knew I needed to call the police, but I just didn’t have the energy to do anything. Then my roommate came home a little while later. She saw that I was upset and she asked me what happened. I told her everything. Then, she called the police for me, and stayed with my while they interviewed me. Then she went to hospital with me for the rape exam. She was so great. I don’t think I would have been able to get through this without her help. 102 Assistant District Attorney: Cindy I know this is hard for you and, and I appreciate your being open with me. We’ll talk again later, ok? Cindy Hall: Ok. Note. Adapted from “Effects of Ingroup Bias and Gender Role Violations on Acquaintance Rape Attributions” by L. A. Harrison, D. M. Howerton, A. M. Secarea and C. Q. Nguyen, 2008, Sex Roles, 59, p. 717. Copyright 2008 by Lisa A. Harrison, Dawn M. Howerton, Ashley M. Secarea and Chau Q. Nguyen. 103 Appendix C Blame Index Measure Victim Blame 1. How responsible was Cindy for this situation? 2. How much was Cindy to blame for this situation? 3. How much should Cindy blame herself in this situation? 4. How much control did Cindy have in this situation? 5. How much should Cindy feel guilty for this situation? 6. How much should Cindy feel ashamed for this situation? 7. How psychologically damaged do you feel Cindy will be from this experience? 8. How much did Cindy enjoy this situation? 9. How obligated was Cindy to engage in sexual relations in this case? 10. Do you believe that Cindy acted in a way that showed she was interested in having sexual intercourse with Michael? 11. How believable do you think Cindy is in her description of what actually occurred during the evening? Perpetrator Blame 1. How responsible was Michael for this situation? 2. How much was Michael to blame for this situation? 3. How much should Michael blame himself for this situation? 104 4. How much control did Michael have in this situation? 5. How much should Michael feel guilty for this situation? 6. How much should Michael feel ashamed for this situation? 7. To what degree were Michael’s actions a violation of Cindy’s rights? 8. Do you believe that Michael thought sexual advances of any kind would be welcomed by Cindy? 9. Do you believe that Michael’s behavior toward Cindy was justified? Rape Index 1. How certain are you that this incident would be considered rape? Note. Adapted from “Blame and Fault Attributions in Sexual Violence: Are These Distinct?” by I. Anderson and H. Bissell, 2011, Journal of Aggression, Conflict, and Peace Research, 3(4), p. 225, “Marital Rape: Is the Crime Taken Seriously Without Cooccurring Physical Abuse?” by J. Langhinrichsen-Rohling and C. M. Monson, 1998, Journal of Family Violence, 13(4), p. 439, and “Perceptions of Date and Stranger Rape: Difference in Sex Role Expectations and Rape-supportive Beliefs” by J. S. Bridges, 1991, Sex Roles, 24, p. 297. 105 Appendix D Empathy Scale Measure Rape-Victim Empathy 1. I find it easy to take the perspective of a rape victim. 2. I can imagine how a victim feels during an actual rape. 3. I get really involved with the feelings of a rape victim in a movie. 4. I can understand how helpless a rape victim might feel. 5. I can feel a person’s humiliation at being forced to have sex against their will. 6. Hearing about someone who has been raped makes me feel that person’s upset. 7. It’s not hard to understand the feelings of someone who is forced to have sex. 8. I can empathize with the shame and humiliation a rape victim feels during a trial to prove rape. 9. I know if I talked to someone who was raped I’d become upset. 10. I imagine the emotional trauma a rape victim might feel if the rape trial were publicized in the press. 11. I imagine the courage it takes to accuse a person in a court of rape. 12. I can understand why a rape victim feels bad for a long time. 13. I imagine the anger a person would feel after being raped. 106 14. I find it difficult to know what goes on in the mind of a rape victim. 15. I don’t understand how a person who is raped would be upset. 16. I can’t understand how someone who has been raped can blame their partner and not take some of the responsibility. 17. I can see how someone who had been raped would get upset at their rape trial. 18. I can feel the emotional torment a rape victim suffers when dealing with the police. Rape-Perpetrator Empathy 1. I find it easy to take the perspective of a person who rapes. 2. I can imagine how a person who rapes might feel during an actual rape. 3. I get really involved with the feelings of a rapist in a movie. 4. I can understand how powerful a rapist might feel. 5. Hearing about a rape, I can imagine the feelings the rapist felt. 6. It’s not hard to understand the feelings that would drive someone to force sex on another person. 7. I know if I talked to someone accused of rape I’d become upset at their upset. 8. I can feel a person’s humiliation at being accused of forcing someone to have sex. 9. I can empathize with the shame and humiliation an accused rapist feels during a trial to prove rape. 107 10. I imagine the anger a person would feel at being accused. 11. I can feel the emotional trauma that a person accused of rape might feel if the rape trial were publicized in the press. 12. I imagine the courage it takes to defend oneself in a court against the charge of rape. 13. I can understand a rapist’s feelings after a rape. 14. I find it difficult to know what goes on in the mind of a rapist. 15. I don’t see how a person accused of rape could be upset. 16. I can’t understand how someone accused of rape can blame their victim. 17. I can see how someone accused of rape would become upset at their rape trial. 18. I can feel the emotional torment a person accused of rape suffers when dealing with the police. Note: From “Examining Rape Empathy from the Perspective of the Victim and the Assailant” by C. A. Smith and I. H. Frieze, 2003, Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 33, pps. 497-498. 108 Appendix E Illinois Rape Myth Acceptance Scale Measure 1. If this woman is raped while she is drunk, she is at least somewhat responsible for letting things get out of control. 2. Although this woman wouldn’t admit it, she generally finds being physically forced into sex a real ‘‘turn-on.’’ 3. When this man raped, it was because of his strong desire for sex. 4. If that woman is willing to ‘‘make out’’ with a guy, then it’s no big deal if he goes a little further and has sex. 5. This woman was caught having an illicit affair and claimed that it was rape. 6. Newspapers should not release the name of a rape victim to the public. 7. This so-called rape victim is actually a woman who had sex and ‘‘changed her mind’’ afterwards. 8. This woman secretly desired to be raped. 9. Rape mainly occurs on the ‘‘bad’’ side of town. 10. This woman does things like hang out in bars and sleeps around, so she was raped. 11. Most rapists are not caught by the police. 12. If this woman didn’t physically fight back, you can’t really say that it was rape. 13. This man from a nice middle-class home never rapes. 14. Rape isn’t as big a problem as some feminists would like people to think. 109 15. When this woman goes around wearing low-cut tops or short skirts, she’s just asking for trouble. 16. A rape accusation was used as a way of getting back at this man. 17. A rape probably didn’t happen if this woman has no bruises or marks. 18. This woman finds being forced to have sex very arousing. 19. If this woman goes home with a man she doesn’t know, it is her own fault if she is raped. 20. Rapists are usually sexually frustrated individuals. 21. All women should have access to self-defense classes. 22. This woman was raped because she dressed suggestively. 23. This woman prefers to have sex forced on her so she doesn’t have to feel guilty about it. 24. If this man didn’t have a weapon, you really can’t call it a rape. 25. When this woman is a sexual tease, eventually she is going to get into trouble. 26. Being raped isn’t as bad as being mugged and beaten. 27. Rape is unlikely to happen in this woman’s own familiar neighborhood. 28. In reality, women are almost never raped by their boyfriends. 29. This woman tends to exaggerate how much rape affects her. 30. When this man is very sexually aroused, he may not even realize that the woman is resisting. 31. This woman led this man on and then she cried rape. 110 32. It is preferable that a female police officer conduct the questioning when a woman reports a rape. 33. This woman claimed she was raped because she has emotional problems. 34. If this woman didn’t physically resist sex-even when protesting verbally-it really can’t be considered rape. 35. Rape almost never happens in the woman’s own home. 36. This woman ‘‘teased’’ the man and deserves anything that might happen. 37. When this woman was raped, it’s because the way she said ‘‘no’’ was ambiguous. 38. This man doesn’t usually intend to force sex on a woman, but sometimes he gets too sexually carried away. 39. This society should devote more effort to preventing rape. 40. When this woman dresses in skimpy clothes she should not be surprised if a man tries to force her to have sex. 41. Rape happened because this man’s sex drive got out of control. 42. This woman who invited a man to her apartment is implying that she wants to have sex. 43. This woman actually enjoys sex after a guy uses a little force. 44. If this woman claims to have been raped but has no bruises or scrapes, she probably shouldn’t be taken too seriously. Note. Adapted from “Rape Myth Acceptance: Exploration of Its Structure and Its Measurement Using the Illinois Rape Myth Acceptance Scale” by D. L. Payne, K. A. Lonsway, and L. F. Fitzgerald, 1999, Journal of Research in Personality, 33, pps. 49-50. 111 Appendix F CONSENT FORM I hereby agree to participate in research that will be conducted by Mary Bell, a graduate student in psychology. In this research, I will receive a packet of materials containing some demographic questions, a transcript of an interview between an Assistant District Attorney and crime victim and four questionnaires assessing my views on the crime. The research will take place in one of the research rooms on the third floor of Amador Hall and will require 30 minutes of my time. I understand that I will receive one half-hour of credit toward satisfying the Psychology Department’s research participation requirement by participating in this study. I understand that I may not personally benefit from participating in this research, but it is hoped that the research may lead to a better understanding of how people develop beliefs about crimes and crime victims. I understand that this research may cause me to feel uncomfortable and that I may discontinue my participation at any time without any penalty other than loss of research credit and that the researcher may discontinue my participation at any time. This information was explained to me by Mary Bell. I understand that she will answer any questions I may have now or later about this research. Mary Bell can be reached at sac38369@csus.edu. Signature: _____________________________ Date: _______________________ 112 Appendix G Debriefing Research Purpose and Procedure The purpose of the study was to investigate the effect victim’s weight, clothing, and sexual history can have on the evaluation of a rape case. Specifically, the study was looking at how these three manipulations affected the degree of blame and empathy toward the victim and perpetrator. The study was also analyzing how these three manipulations influence rape myth beliefs. Therefore, participants began the study by reading a transcript that varied the rape victim’s weight, clothing, and sexual history. Following this, participants completed a measure evaluating the rape victim’s and perpetrator’s degree of blame for the rape. After this task, participants completed a measure examining their beliefs on rape myths. Finally, participants completed a measure evaluating their degree of empathy toward the rape victim and perpetrator. Theoretical Implications While blame against rape victims has been studied extensively, little research has been conducted examining rape victim’s weight (Clarke & Stermac, 2011; Ryckman, Graham, Thornton, Gold, & Lindner, 1998). Prior research on rape has found that obese rape victims (Ryckman et al., 1998), victims wearing provocative clothing (Brown, Hamilton, & O’Neill, 2007; Klein, Kennedy, & Gorzalka, 2009), and sexually promiscuous victims (Brown et al., 2007; Temkin & Krahe, 2008) are held more responsible and blamed more for their rape. Thus it is hypothesized that blame against rape victims will be higher when she is obese, promiscuous, and wearing revealing clothing. While few studies have been conducted examining empathy in instances of rape depicted in a story, those that did found woman in general, but especially victims of rape, to be more empathic towards rape victims than men (Iong-Szeto, 1999; Osman, 2011) and that blame of the victim affected empathy levels with increased empathy toward the perpetrator when the victim was blamed for the rape (Smith & Frieze, 2003). Studies on empathy toward the obese have not had positive results, with most participants not being empathic toward obese individuals, even after conducting programs to increase empathy (Gapinski, Schwartz, & Brownell, 2006; Teachman, Gapinski, Brownell, Rawlins, & Jeyaram, 2003). Thus it is hypothesized that empathy toward rape victims will be lower when she is obese, promiscuous, and wearing revealing clothing. Lastly, numerous studies has shown that participants who hold rape myth beliefs to blame rape victims significantly more than those without rape myth beliefs (Cohn, Dupois, & Brown, 2009; Ferro, Cemele, & Saltzman, 2008). Thus it is hypothesized that rape myth beliefs will be higher when the rape victim is obese, promiscuous, and wearing revealing clothing. PLEASE, DON’T DISCUSS THIS RESEARCH WITH OTHERS! 113 It is very important that you do not discuss this research with your fellow students. If you discuss the research with others that might be in the research at a future date, it may influence the way they respond to the research. Or, if you make them aware of the hypothesis, they may try to give the answers they think the experimenter wants. PLEASE DON’T DISCUSS THIS RESEARCH WITH OTHERS. Further Information The results of this study will be available by May 17, 2013. If you have any questions or concerns regarding this study, please contact Mary Bell at sac38369@csus.edu at your convenience. If you found any of this material to be personally troubling and you would like to speak to a counselor, you are encouraged to contact CSUS Psychological Services (278-6416). 114 References Abbey, A. & Harnish, R. J. (1995). Perception of sexual intent: The role of gender, alcohol consumption, and rape supportive attitudes. Sex Roles, 32, 297-313. Abbey, A., Cozzarelli, C., McLaughlin, K., & Harnish, R. J. (1987). The effects of clothing and dyad sex composition on perceptions of sexual intent: Do women and men evaluate these cues differently. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 17(2), 108-126. Abrams, D., Viki, G. T., Masser, B., & Bohner, G. (2003). Perceptions of stranger and acquaintance rape: The role of benevolent and hostile sexism in victim blame and rape proclivity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(1), 111-125. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.1.111 Accused rapist admits to being told ‘no.’ (2010, November 30). KCRA.COM. Retrieved from http://www.kcra.com/news/25954947/detail.html. Anderson, I. (2007). What is a typical rape? Effects of victim and participant gender in female and male rape perception. British Journal of Social Psychology, 46, 225245. doi: 10.1348/014466606X101780 Anderson, I., & Bissell, H. (2011). Blame and fault attributions in sexual violence: Are these distinct? Journal of Aggression, Conflict, and Peace Research, 3(4), 222229. Aosved, A. C., & Long, P. J. (2006). Co-occurrence of rape myth acceptance, sexism, racism, homophobia, ageism, classism, and religious intolerance. Sex Roles, 55, 481-492. doi: 10.1007/s11199-006-9101-4 115 Archer, J., & Vaughan, A. E. (2001). Evolutionary theories of rape. Psychology, Evolution & Gender, 3(1), 95-101. doi: 10.1080/1461666011004960 9 Bourke, J. (2007). Rape: Sex, violence, history. Great Britain: Virago Press. Bridges, J. S. (1991). Perceptions of date and stranger rape: A difference in sex role expectations and rape-supportive beliefs. Sex Roles, 24, 291-307. Brooks, C. & Bolzendahl, C. (2004). The transformation of us gender role attitudes: Cohort replacement, social-structural change, and ideological learning. Social Science Research, 33(1), 106-133. doi: 10.1016/S0049-089X(03)00041-3 Brown, A. L., Testa, M., & Messman-Moore, T. L. (2009). Psychological consequences of sexual victimization resulting from force, incapacitation, or verbal coercion. Violence Against Women, 15, 898-919. doi: 10.1177/1077801209335491 Brown, J. M., Hamilton, C., & O’Neill, D. (2007). Characteristics associated with rape attrition and the role played by skepticism or legal rationality by investigators and prosecutors. Psychology, Crime & Law, 13(4), 355-370. doi: 10.1080/10683160601060507 Brownmiller, S. (1975). Against our will. New York: Random House Publishing. Buchwald, E., Fletcher, P. R., & Roth, M. (Eds.). (2005). Transforming a rape culture. Minneapolis, MN: Milkweed Editions. Buck Doude, S. E. (2008). Defining acquaintance rape: College students’ perceptions of sexual consent and coercion. (Doctoral Dissertation) Retrieved from Dissertations Abstracts International Section A: Humanities and Social Sciences database. (UMI No. 2009-99030-349) 116 Buckley, K. (2012, October 29). Buckley: Halloween costumes should not be subjected to gender stereotypes. The Daily Nebraskan. Retrieved from http://www.dailynebraskan.com/opinion/article_c6b90186-217a-11e2-a07a001a4bcf6878.html Burt, M. R. (1980). Cultural myths and supports for rape. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38(2), 217-230. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.38.2.217 Campbell, R. (2006). Rape survivors’ experiences with the legal and medical systems. Violence Against Women, 12(1), 30-45. doi: 10.1177/1077801205277539 Campbell, R. (2008). The psychological impact of rape victims. American Psychologist, 63(8), 702-717. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.63.8.702 Campbell, R., Sefl, T., Barnes, H. E., Ahrens, C. E., Wasco, S.M., & Zaragoza-Diesfeld, Y. (1999). Community services for rape survivors: Enhancing psychological wellbeing or increasing trauma? Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 67(6), 847-858. Cassidy, L. & Hurrell, R. M. (1995). The influence of victim’s attire on adolescent’s judgments of date rape. Adolescence, 30(118), 319-323. Catty, J. (1999). Writing rape, writing women in early modern england. New York: St. Martin’s Press. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC). (2012). Defining overweight and obesity. Retrieved from http://www.cdc.gov/obesity/adult/defining.html 117 Chapleau, K. M., Oswald, D. L., & Russell, B. L. (2007). How ambivalent sexism toward women and men support rape myth acceptance. Sex Roles, 57, 131-136. doi: 10.1007/s11199-007-9196-2 Chasteen, A. L. (2001). Constricting rape: feminism, change, and women’s everyday understandings of sexual assault. Sociological Spectrum, 21(2), 101-139. doi: 10.1080/02732170121403 Ciabattari, T. (2001). Changes in men’s conservative gender ideologies: Cohort and period influences. Gender and Society, 15(4), 574-591. Claes, C. (2010, March 22). HLN: ‘Wearing a bikini on spring break is asking for rape’ [Web log post]. Retrieved from http://open.salon.com//blog//colleenclaes/2010/03/22/hln_bikinis_on_spring_brea k_are_to_blame_for_rape Clarke, A. K., & Stermac, L. (2011). The influence of stereotypical beliefs, participant gender, and survivor weight on sexual assault response. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 26, 2285-2302. Cohn, E. S., Dupois, E. C., & Brown, T. M. (2009). In the eye of the beholder: Do behavior and character affect victim and perpetrator responsibility for acquaintance rape? Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 39(7), 1513-1535. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2009.00493.x Collins, R. L. (2011). Content analysis of gender roles in media: Where are we now and where should we go? Sex Roles, 64(3-4), 290-298. 118 Cuklanz, L. M. (1996). Rape on trial: How the mass media construct legal reform and social change. Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania Press. Deitz, S. R., & Byrnes, L. E. (1981). Attribution of responsibility for sexual assault: The influence of observer empathy and defendant occupation and attractiveness. Journal of Psychology, 108, 17-29. Deitz, S. R., Blackwell, K. T., Daley, P. C., & Bentley, B. J. (1982). Measurement of empathy toward rape victims and rapists. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43(2), 372-384. Deitz, S. R., Littman, M., & Bentley, B. J. (1984). Attribution of responsibility for rape: The influence of observer empathy, victim resistance, and victim attractiveness. Sex Roles, 10(3/4), 261-280. Denim Day USA (2012). Italy 1990s. Retrieved from http://denimdayusa.org/about/history Escobar, An., & Hill, J. (Eds.). Take back the night foundation event guidebook. Retrieved from www.takebackthenight.org Federal Bureau of Investigation (FBI). (2012, January 6). Attorney general eric holder announces revisions to the uniform crime report’s definition of rape: data reported on rape will better reflect state criminal codes, victim experiences. Federal Bureau of Investigation. Retrieved from fbi.gov Feldman, P. J., Ullman, J. B., & Dunkel-Schetter, C. (1998). Women’s reactions to rape victims: motivational processes associated with blame and social support. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 28, 469-503. 119 Ferro, C., Cermele, J., & Saltzman, A. (2008). Current perceptions of marital rape: Some good and not-so-good news. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 23, 764-779. doi: 10.1177/0886260507313947 Finch, E. & Munro, V. E. (2005). Juror stereotypes and blame attribution in rape cases involving intoxicants. British Journal of Criminology, 45, 25-38. doi: 10.1093/bjc/azh055 Fischer, B. S., Cullen, F. T., & Turner, M. G. (2000). The sexual victimization of college women. National Institute of Justice. Franiuk, R., Seefelt, J. L., Cepress, S. L., & Vandello, J. A. (2008). Prevalence and effects of rape myths in print journalism: The kobe bryant case. Violence Against Women, 14, 287-309. DOI: 10.1177/1077801207313971 Gapinski, K. D., Schwartz, M. B., & Brownell, K. D. (2006). Can television change antifat attitudes and behavior? Journal of Applied Biobehavioral Research, 11, 1-28. George, W. H., & Martinez, L. J. (2002). Victim blaming in rape: Effects of victim and perpetrator race, type of rape, and participant racism. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 26, 110-119. doi: 10.1111/1471-6402.00049 Glick, P., & Fiske, S. T. (1996). The ambivalent sexism inventory: Differentiating hostile and benevolent sexism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70(3), 491512. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.70.3.491 Griffin, S. (Ed.). (1979). Rape, the power of consciousness. San Francisco: Harper & Row. Hall, R. (1995). Rape in america. Santa Barbara, CA: ABC-CLIO. 120 Harrison, L. A., Howerton, D. M., Secarea, A. M., & Nguyen, C. Q. (2008). Effects of ingroup bias and gender role violations on acquaintance rape attribution s. Sex Roles, 59, 713-725. doi: 10.1007/s11199-008-9472-9 Henneberger, M. (2010, December 27). Notre dame fr. jenkins pins delays in lizzy seeberg case on ‘discrepancies.’ Woman Up @ Politics Daily. Retrieved from http://www.politicsdaily.com/2010/12/27/notre-dames-fr-jenkins-pins-delays-inlizzy-seeberg-case-on-d/ Hirsch, K. (1990). Fraternities of fear: Gang rape, male bolding, and the silencing of women. Ms., 1(2), 52-56. Hockett, J. M., Saucier, D. A., Hoffman, B. H., Smith, S. J., & Craig, A. W. (2009). Oppression through acceptance?: Predicting rape myth acceptance and attitudes toward rape victims. Violence Against Women, 15, 877-897. doi: 10.1177/1077801209335489 Holleran, D., Beichner, D., & Spohn, C. (2010). Examining charging agreement between police and prosecutors in rape cases. Crime & Delinquency, 56(3), 385-413. doi: 10.1177/0011128707308977 Iong-Szeto, A. (1999). Effects of victimization, just world beliefs, empathy, and stereotypes on the attribution of blame. Doctoral Dissertation) Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses: The Humanities and Social Sciences database. (UMI No. 9934171) 121 Jimenez, J. A., & Abreu, J. M. (2003). Race and sex effects on attitudinal perceptions of acquaintance rape. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 50(2), 252-256. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.50.2.252 Johnson, K. K. P. & Lee, M. (2000). Effects of clothing and behavior on perceptions concerning an alleged date rape. Family and Consumer Sciences Research Journal, 28(3), 332-356. Jones, C. & Aronson, E. (1973). Attribution of fault to a rape victim as a function of respectability of the victim. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 26(3), 415-419. Juviler, M. (1960). Psychiatric opinions as to credibility of witnesses: A suggested approach. California Law Review, 48, 648-683. Kahlor, L., & Morrison, D. (2007). Television viewing and rape myth acceptance among college women. Sex Roles, 56, 729-739. doi: 10:1007/s11199-007-9232-2 Kanekar, S. & Kolsawalla, M. B. (1977). Responsibility in relation to respectability. The Journal of Social Psychology, 102(2), 183-188. Kanekar, S. & Kolsawalla, M. B. (1980). Responsibility of a rape victim in relation to her respectability, attractiveness, and provocativeness. The Journal of Social Psychology, 112, 153-154. Kilpatrick, D. G., Edmunds, C. N., & Seymour, A. E. (1992). Rape in america: A report to the nation. Arlington, VA: National Crime Victims Center. 122 Klein, C., Kennedy, M. A., & Gorzalka, B. B. (2009). Rape myth acceptance in men who completed the prostitution offender program of british columbia. International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology, 53, 305-315. doi: 10.1177/0306624X08316969 Krahe, B., Temkin, J., Bieneck, S., & Berger, A. (2008). Prospective lawyers’ rape stereotypes and schematic decision making about rape cases. Psychology, Crime & Law, 14(5), 461-479. doi: 10.1080/10683160801932380 Lamb, S. & Brown, L. M. (2006). Packaging our girlhood: Rescuing our daughters from marketers’ schemes. New York: St. Martin’s Press. Langhinrichsen-Rohling, J., & Monson, C. M. (1998). Marital rape: Is the crime taken seriously without co-occurring physical abuse? Journal of Family Violence, 13(4), 433-443. Lee, J., Pomeroy, E. C., Yoo, S., & Rheinboldt, K. T. (2005). Attitudes toward rape: A comparison between asian and Caucasian college students. Violence Against Women, 11(2), 177-196. doi: 10.1177/1077801204271663 Legenbauer, T., Vocks, S., Schafer, C., Schutt-Stromel, S., Hiller, W., Wagner, C., Vogele, C. (2009). Preference for attractiveness and thinness in a partner: Influence of internalization of the thin ideal and shape/weight dissatisfaction in heterosexual women, heterosexual men, lesbians, and gay men. Body Image, 6, 228-234. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2009.04.002 Lerner, M. J. (1980). The belief in a just world: A fundamental delusion. New York: Pienam. 123 Levine, D. E. & Kilbourne, J. (2008). So sexy so soon: The new sexualized childhood and what parents can do to protect their kids. New York: Ballentine Books. Lin, L., & Reid, K. (2009). The relationship between media exposure and antifat attitudes: The role of dysfunctional appearance beliefs. Body Image, 6, 52-55. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2008.09.001 Martin, P. Y. (2005). Rape work: Victims, gender, and emotions in organization and community context. New York: Routledge. Maurer, T. W., & Robinson, D. W. (2007). Effects of attire, alcohol, and gender on perceptions of date rape. Sex Roles, 58, 423-434. doi: 10.1007/s11199-007-9343-9 McClure, K. J., Puhl, R. M., & Heuer, C. A. (2011). Obesity in the news: Do photographic images of obese persons influence antifat attitudes? Journal of Health Communication, 16, 359-371. doi: 10.1080/10810730.2010.535108 McGregor, J. (2005). Is it rape? Burlington, VT: Ashgate Publishing Company. McKinley, J. C. (2011, March 8). Vicious assault shakes texas town. The New York Times. Retrieved from http://www.nytimes.com/2011/03/09/us/09assault.html?_r=1&emc=eta1 McMahon-Howard, J., Clay-Warner, J., & Renzulli, L. (2009). Criminalizing spousal rape: The diffusion of legal reforms. Sociological Perspectives, 52(4), 505-531. doi: 10.1525/sop.2009.52.4.505 Messman-Moore, T. L. & Brown, A. L. (2006). Risk perception, rape, and sexual revictimization: A prospective study of college women. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 30, 159-172. 124 Miller, A. K., Amacker, A. M., & King, A. R. (2011). Sexual victimization and perceived similarity to a sexual assault victim: a path model of perceiver variables predicting victim culpability attributions. Sex Roles, 64, 372-381. Moore, L. (2012). Rep. todd akin: The statement and the reaction. New York Times. Retrieved from http://www.nytimes.com/2012/08/21/us/politics/rep-todd-akinlegitimate-rape-statement-and-reaction.html Moorti, S. (2002). Color of rape: Gender and race in television’s public spheres. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press. Morrison, T. G., & O’Connor, W. E. (1999). Psychometric properties of a scale measuring negative attitudes toward overweight individuals. The Journal of Social Psychology, 139(4), 436-445. O’Brien, K. S., Latner, J. D., Halberstadt, J., Hunter, J. A., Anderson, J., & Caputi, P. (2008). Do antifat attitudes predict antifat behaviors? Obesity, 16, 87-92. O’Donohue, W., Yeater, E. A., & Fanetti, M. (2003). Rape prevention with college males: The roles of rape myth acceptance, victim empathy, and outcome expectancies. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 18, 513–531. Osman, S. L. (2003). Predicting men’s rape perceptions based on the belief that “no” really means “yes.” Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 33(4), 683-692. Osman, S. L. (2011). Predicting rape empathy based on victim, perpetrator, and participant gender, and history of sexual aggression. Sex Roles, 64, 506-515. doi: 10.1007/s11199-010-9919-7 125 Payne, D. L., Lonsway, K. A., & Fitzgerald, L. F. (1999). Rape myth acceptance: Exploration of its structure and its measurement using the illinois rape myth acceptance scale. Journal of Research in Personality, 33, 27-68. doi: 10.1006/jrpe.1998.2238 Rand, M. R. (2009). Criminal victimization. Findings from the national crime victimization survey. National Institute of Justice. Retrieved from http://bjs.ojp.usdoj.gov/index.cfm?ty=pbdetail&iid=1975 Rape, Abuse & Incest National Network (RAINN). (2009). Statistics. Retrieved from http://www.rainn.org Rennison, C. M. (2002). Rape and sexual assault: Reporting to police and medical attention. Bureau of Justice Statistics. Retrieved from http://bjs.ojp.usdoj.gov/index.cfm?ty=tp&tid=317 Russell, D. E. H., & Bolen, R. M. (2000). The epidemic of rape and child sexual abuse in the united states. Thousand Oaks, California: Sage Publications. Ryckman, R. M., Graham, S. S., Thornton, B., Gold, J. A., & Lindner, M. A. (1998). Physical size stereotyping as a mediator of attributions of responsibility in an alleged date-rape situation. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 28, 1876-1888. Schneider, L. J., Mori, L. T., Lambert, P. L., & Wong, A. O. (2009). The role of gender and ethnicity in perceptions of rape and its aftereffects. Sex Roles, 60, 410-421. doi: 10.1007/s11199-008-9545-9 126 Shears, R. (2010, May 1). You’re not guilty of rape: Those skinny jeans were too tight to remove by yourself, jury rules. Mail Online. Retrieved from http://www.dailymail.co.uk/news/article-1270113/Youre-guilty-rape-Thoseskinny-jeans-tight-remove-jury-rules.html Siddique, I. (2012, October 19). #notbuyingit: Spirit halloween’s costumes for girls. Retrieved from http://www.missrepresentation.org/media/nbi/notbuyingit-spirithalloweens-costumes-for-teen-girls/ Simonson, K., & Subich, L. M. (1999). Rape perceptions as a function of gender-role traditionality and victim-perpetrator association. Sex Roles, 40(7/8), 617-634. doi: 10.1023/A:1018844231555 Sinclair, H. C., & Bourne, L. E. (1998). Cycle of blame or just world: Effects of legal verdicts on gender patterns in rape-myth acceptance and victim empathy. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 22, 575-588. Smith, C. A. (2012). The confounding of fat, control, and physical attractiveness for women. Sex Roles, 66, 628-631. doi: 10.1007/s11199-011-0111-5 Smith, C. A., & Frieze, I. H. (2003). Examining rape empathy from the perspective of the victim and the assailant. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 33(3), 476-498. Tarrant, S. (2010, March 19). Judge orders rape survivors to take lie-detector test. Ms. Magazine. Retrieved from msmagazine.com 127 Teachman, B. A., Gapinski, K. D., Brownell, K. D., Rawlins, M., & Jeyaram, S. (2003). Demonstrations of implicit anti-fat bias: The impact of providing causal information and evoking empathy. Health Psychology, 22(1), 68-78. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.22.1.68 Temkin, J., & Krahe, B. (2008). Sexual assault and the justice gap: A question of attitude. Portland, OR: Hart Publishing. Thornhill, R. & Palmer, C. T. (2000). A natural history of rape. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Massachusetts Institute of Technology Press. Tjaden, P., & Thoennes, N. (1998). Prevalence, incidence, and consequences of violence against women: Findings from the national violence against women survey. National Institute of Justice. Retrieved from http://www.ojp.usdoj.gov/nij/pubssum/172837.htm Tjaden, P., & Thoennes, N. (2006). Extent, nature, and consequences of rape victimization: Findings from the national violence against women survey. National Institute of Justice. Retrieved from http://www.ojp.usdoj.gov/nij/pubssum/210346.htm Travis, C. B. (Ed.) (2003). Evolution, gender, and rape. Cambridge, MA: Massachusetts Institute of Technology Press. United States. (2011). Rape in the United States: The chronic failure to report and investigate rape cases : hearing before the Subcommittee on Crime and Drugs of the Committee on the Judiciary, United States Senate, One Hundred Eleventh Congress, second session, September 14, 2010. Washington: U.S. G.P.O. 128 Varelas, N., & Foley, L. A. (1998) Blacks’ and whites’ perceptions of interracial and intraracial date rape. Journal of Social Psychology, 138(3), 392-400. Whatley, M. A. (2005). The effect of participant sex, victim dress, and traditional attitudes on casual judgments for marital rape victims. Journal of Family Violence, 20(3), 191-200. doi: 10.1007/s10896-005-3655-8 Workman, J. E. & Freeburg, E. W. (1999). An examination of date rape, victim dress, and perceiver variables within the context of attribution theory. Sex Roles, 41, 261-277. Yeater, E. A., Treat, T. A., Viken, R. J., & McFall, R. M. (2010). Cognitive processes underlying women’s risk judgments: Associations with sexual victimization history and rape myth acceptance. Journal of Consulting Clinical Psychology, 78(3), 375-386. doi: 10.1037/a0019297 Young, L. M., & Powell, B. (1985). The effects of obesity on the clinical judgments of mental health professionals. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 26(3), 233246.