1 The Multi-layered Reading Goals of Expert Readers: Bridging Between Knowledge, Interest, and Strategy Use Emily Fox Liliana Maggioni Daniel L. Dinsmore Patricia A. Alexander University of Maryland Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research Association, March, 2008, New York 2 Abstract Expert readers’ pursuit of multi-layered reading goals was examined within the framework of Alexander’s Model of Domain Learning (MDL). Seven experts in the field of reading completed knowledge pre-tests, think-aloud protocols using both reading and history passages, and openended outcome questions. The think-aloud protocols were coded for strategic and evaluative/monitoring behaviors in addition to evidence of reading goals. Evidence supported the experts’ pursuit of multi-layered reading goals, as they evaluated the text and their understanding of it on multiple levels. Higher-level goals differed for the reading and history passages, and were also associated with specific knowledge for the topic of the reading passage. 3 The Multi-layered Reading Goals of Expert Readers: Bridging Between Knowledge, Interest, and Strategy Use Alexander’s Model of Domain Learning (MDL) provides a characterization of the trajectory followed by learners on the path to expertise in academic domains in terms of distinctive profiles for knowledge, interest, and strategy use at different stages of development (Alexander, 1997, 1998, 2002). In particular, experts are expected to have breadth and depth of knowledge in their domain of expertise, to be driven primarily by individual interest in domainrelated activities and materials, and to use deep-level processing strategies as they learn. Such a multidimensional developmental characterization implies that knowledge, interest, and strategy use are bound up together in shaping our understanding of both the learner’s progress over time in a domain and also the nature of the learner’s activity in a given learning situation at a specific point in their developmental trajectory. How might knowledge, interest, and strategy use interact for the learner in a given learning situation? In this study, we will investigate the interaction of knowledge, interest, and strategy use for expert readers in terms of their pursuit of multi-layered reading goals. Theoretical Framework Readers actively construct their understanding of the text as they read (RAND Reading Study Group, 2002). As they read, they monitor their progress toward a desired level of understanding, typically by reference to some standard of coherence (Pressley & Afflerbach, 1995; Tapiero, 2007). Because reading is a complex and multi-layered task, readers must monitor their understanding on multiple levels. In order to build a connected understanding, the reader has to understand at the level of words, phrases, propositions, and paragraphs, as well as at the level of an entire argument or an entire passage. These local and global levels of 4 understanding make up a hierarchy of tasks and related goals. Beyond an understanding of the text, readers may also evaluate the text in a number of ways, including its qualities as a piece of writing, its credibility, fairness or balance, and for its accuracy in relation to what they may already know concerning the topic or in relation to other texts (Pressley & Afflerbach, 1995; Scaradamalia & Bereiter, 1991). Yet another set of possible tasks and goals for the reader would be the development of such evaluations (Fox, Maggioni, & Riconscente, 2005). Pursuit of these reading goals typically requires activation of knowledge, metacognitive awareness, and strategic processing. However, goals at different levels are likely to require different levels of effort from the reader, as well as variable reliance on domain-specific or domain-general knowledge and strategies. Thus, we would expect that higher-level goals would be more likely to be pursued by readers at a higher level of experience or expertise. This understanding of goals as guiding self-regulatory activities on multiple levels during learning and as reflecting the learners’ level of expertise in terms of their task definition is in line with the relevant literatures on both self-regulated learning (e.g., Pintrich, 2000) and expertise (e.g., Scardamalia & Bereiter, 1991). The specific framework used here, the MDL, offers an account of how learners’ varying levels of knowledge, interest, and strategy use are related to their level of development in an academic domain (Alexander, 1997, 1998, 2002). Looking specifically at the reading goals pursued by readers provides a way to illuminate the interaction of knowledge, interest, and strategy use in a particular learning situation for individuals at a given point in expertise development. In particular, we would expect that the learner’s levels of knowledge and interest related to the text would drive their selection of higher-level reading goals and their level of effort in interacting with the text, and would require them to use the appropriate type of strategic processing necessary to achieve those goals. Reading goals are thus an indicator of the 5 reader’s level of engagement and act as a bridge between readers’ knowledge and interest and their strategic processing. Knowledge in the MDL has two aspects, with both breadth of domain knowledge and depth of topic knowledge increasing across the stages of development. The broadness of domain knowledge refers to coverage of topics relevant to a given domain, while depth of topic knowledge concerns the extent of learners’ familiarity with specific domain-related topics and the principled nature of that knowledge. Interest in this model can take the form of either situational or more temporal interest and individual or long-term interest in a particular domain. An individual interest in a subject-matter, by definition, implies the desire to learn more about it. Readers are therefore expected to be more highly engaged in reading about a content area in which they have an individual interest. In early stages of learning, situational interest plays more of a role, while the influence of individual interest is low. These roles are reversed for an expert learner in the domain. Strategy use also displays dual aspects for learning in the MDL. In the early stages of learning, surface-level strategies (e.g., rereading the text or ascertaining word meanings) play the predominant role and the learning outcome is comprehension and limited recall. For expert learners, deep-processing strategies (e.g., relating current text to other readings) become the focus, while surface-processing strategies may be less often required. Such a distinction between deep and surface levels of processing has also been drawn by researchers investigating approaches to learning from text (Marton & Säljö, 1976, 2005), where a surface learning approach focuses on retention and replication of the text itself, while a deep learning approach looks beyond the text and attends more to the author’s intention and what the text is trying to say, its meaning. The processing of readers with a surface learning approach tends to be 6 atomistic and local, while that of readers with a deep learning approach is more holistic and global (Marton & Säljö, 2005). In terms of Kintsch’s Construction-Integration model of comprehension (1998), surface-level processing assists the reader in constructing a coherent textbase (propositional representation of the text’s content), while the more expert deepprocessing strategies pull the reader into deeper interaction with the text in developing a situation model (integrated representation of the reader’s understanding of the meaning of the text). Expert reading involves moving beyond just constructing a coherent textbase to developing an integrated situation model, for difficult domain-related texts (Scardamalia & Bereiter, 1991). Skilled readers can develop coherent textbases easily and automatically–it may not demand much engagement with the text, depending on its level of difficulty and their level of knowledge, and can result in effective recall. To go beyond that and develop an integrated situation model means asking what the text means, forming an interpretation and integrating it with prior knowledge and experience. A further level of reading expertise might be to create a coherent textbase, an integrated situation model, and also position yourself to question whether you agree with what the author is saying and how it is being said (Fox et al., 2005; Scardamalia & Bereiter, 1991). Expert readers are therefore likely to be pursuing multiple levels of reading goals when they are reading in the area of their expertise. Experts’ learning outcomes can thus include substantial recall, as well as critical evaluations, cross-text comparisons, and welldeveloped interpretations Specifically, in this study we are interested in whether we see experts engaging in processing behaviors as they read that indicate that they are pursuing goals related to the most basic level of processing, or remembering the text. Beyond that, we would expect that they might be trying to understand the text content, to understand the argument presented in the text, and to 7 understand how the argument is situated in the context of its associated domain discourse. And beyond that, we would expect that they might be working on evaluating the text content, the content of the argument, and the argument as it is positioned in the relevant domain discourse. Finally, we would also expect that they might be evaluating the quality of the text as well as the quality of the argument it presents. Although we would hypothesize that readers are pursuing these multiple goals simultaneously, rather than taking them up in turn, typically only one would be expected to be at forefront of consciousness and thus revealed through think-aloud verbalizations. In addition, we recognize that the text-related goals we have identified do not exhaust the possible range of goals readers could be engaged in pursuing in the complex interaction of reader, text, and task that is reading (RAND, 2002). In this study, we investigated the reading of seven expert readers within the domain of reading. We looked at the types of on-line reading goals they pursued as they processed a reading text pertinent to their area of expertise, and also for a history text outside their expertise. We expected to see them pursuing multiple levels of goals in the course of their reading, and to see a clear difference in the types of goals they established for themselves for these two texts. We also expected to see some degree of variation among the experts in their level of interest and knowledge of the specific topic of the reading text, and to see a corresponding divergence in the nature of their engagement in terms of goal-directed efforts for that text. Methods Participants Our participants were seven experts in the field of reading, all associated with the same university, a large research institution in the Mid-Atlantic region. Participants were recruited via e-mail request by the first author. One invited participant (a full professor) declined. Of the seven 8 who agreed to participate, five were faculty members (three full professors, one associate professor, and one assistant professor), one was a recent PhD recipient, and one was a doctoral student with many years of experience as a reading specialist. Five were females, and two males. All identified themselves as of European-American background. Their ages ranged from 30 to 63, and the years they had received their PhD ranged from 2005 to 1968 (with one who had not yet received her PhD.) Table 1 presents our participants’ demographic data. Data were collected in the summer and early fall of 2005. Measures Demographics. Participants completed a demographics questionnaire giving background information on gender, ethnicity, age, current educational level, year they received their PhD, and area of specialization. Interest. A 20-item interest rating questionnaire assessed interest in the academic domains of science, reading, history, and art. Responses were on a 1 to 5 scale, where 1 was “Strongly Agree” and 5 was “Strongly Disagree.” A sample item for history was, “History is personally important to me.” There were five items for each academic domain, and items were parallel in construction. Two items for each domain were negatively phrased and reverse coded. Reliability was relatively poor, with α = .552; this may have been partially due both to the small sample and to the lack of variability in responses to the reading items, where our participants were uniformly high. Interest was further assessed by a 30-item activities questionnaire asking about participants’ frequency of participation in history-related general and professional activities (15 items) and in a parallel set of reading-related general and professional activities (15 items). Responses were on a 1 to 5 scale, where 1 was “Rarely/Never” and 5 was “About Daily.” A sample item for general history-related activities read, “[How often do you:] Visit history 9 museums or exhibits.” A sample item for professional reading-related activities read, ““[How often do you:] Read scholarly journals related to reading.” Reliabilities for both activity-based interest subscales were strong, with α = .830 for reading and α = .916 for history. Mean scores for both knowledge and interest appear in Table 2. . Knowledge. Participants completed four knowledge measures assessing the breadth of their domain knowledge and depth of their topic knowledge in reading and in history. The knowledge measures were validated by a history and a reading expert for appropriateness of domain and topic representation and accuracy of answer choices. These knowledge measures were originally designed for a broader cross-sectional study including domain experts and younger participants as well (as were the interest measures described above), and were intended to span a wide range of possible levels of knowledge. They should thus be interpreted with caution as far as how well they discriminate between participants like those in this study, who are similar in level of expertise. The domain knowledge measures were 20-item multiple choice assessments in which participants were asked to choose the response that best completed the given analogy. A graduated response scoring scheme was used, which will be described below. Here we were specifically interested in our participants’ topic knowledge for reading, that is, knowledge related to the topic of the passage these experts would be reading. The topic knowledge measures for both reading and history consisted of 15 multiple choice items in which participants were asked to choose the response best completing the sentence stem. The general topic covered by the history topic knowledge measure was slavery, and the topic of the reading topic knowledge measure was psycholinguistic theory. In each item, the response choices represented four possible tiers of knowledge (Alexander, Murphy, & Kulikowich, 1998): the correct response that best completed the sentence stem (4 points), a response that was relevant 10 but was not the best answer (2 points), a response that related to psycholinguistics but was not related to the topic of the sentence (1 point), and an unrelated response (0 points). A sample item for reading was: “phonology : word pronunciation :: morphology : _______ ,” where response choices were: word meaning (4 points); word identification (2 points); word attack (1 point); word processing (0 points). Reliability for this topic knowledge measure was adequate (again not unexpected, given the small sample), at α = .661. Learning outcomes. Learning outcomes in this study were assessed by four open-ended questions completed from memory after reading each passage: (1) Write down as many important points as you can remember from this passage; (2) Write down anything else you can remember from the passage; (3) Write down the argument the author is making in this passage; (4) Write down what you think about the argument. Participants were given a full blank sheet of paper for each question. One participant (P6) dictated his responses to these outcome questions, and the audiotape of his dictated responses was transcribed into a text document by the first author. Texts and task Passages. The reading task used in this study involved texts from the domains of reading and history that were selected and adapted to be as parallel as possible. The texts chosen were expected to address relatively unfamiliar topics. Each of the passages chosen indirectly presents an argument. In the reading passage, the argument is that skilled reading does not occur primarily through the application of grapho-phonemic information or decoding, presented in the context of an explanation of the psycholinguistic model of reading. In the history passage, the author argues that the slavery system based on race evolved partially as an attempt to address the 11 class struggle that was mounting in the colonies; although this interpretation is directly stated, the passage is not constructed explicitly as an argument for this thesis. The selected passages were intended to present similar levels of difficulty for readers in terms of length, structure, and reading level. The reading passage was taken from an article on practical applications of a psycholinguistic model of reading (Pearson, 1978) that appeared in a book targeted for reading researchers, graduate students, or teachers. In its adapted form, it is 1173 words long with two footnotes, with a Flesch-Kincaid reading level of 12.0, and a Flesch reading ease score of 40.5 (from Microsoft Word). Although not from a textbook, the history passage was taken from a text on U.S. history (Takaki, 1993) usually used at a college level, professionally written and appealing also to an adult audience interested in history. In its adapted form, it is 1393 words long with six footnotes, and had a Flesch-Kincaid reading level of 12.0 and a Flesch reading ease score of 38.5. The adaptations made were all excisions, in order to create a self-contained passage of a reasonable length. Task. Participants were asked to think aloud while reading each of these passages, and their think-aloud was recorded on an audiotape. They were told to verbalize what they were thinking and doing as they read each passage, and practiced thinking aloud on a short passage before moving on to the reading and history passages. They were told that when they finished reading the passage, they would be asked to respond to open-ended questions regarding what they remembered and what they thought about the passage. These think-alouds were intended to provide data on strategy use, interest, and overall engagement with the reading activity. Coding. All 14 think-alouds (two for each participant) were jointly coded for strategic and evaluative/monitoring behaviors by the first three authors. We initially coded them separately and then met and created a consensus coding reflecting our joint understanding. We 12 started out using the set of 24 codes developed in our pilot study in which undergraduates read these same two passages (Fox et al., 2007). In order to account for what we saw in the processing of these expert readers, we found it necessary to add seven new codes relating to: evaluating the argument, evaluating the author’s intent, using text features, connecting to context (domain discourse), arguing with text, rehearsing, and establishing goal. We also split one code into two separate codes: elaborating and interpreting/hypothesizing. We dropped or merged three codes, those relating to: evaluating task difficulty, visualizing, and expressing interest/surprise. This modified set of 29 codes proved to be adequate to capture the vast majority of relevant behaviors demonstrated; a list of these codes with examples from the transcripts is given in Appendix A. After this initial fine-grained coding, the transcripts were gone over again, looking for evidence of particular goal-related behaviors. This was accomplished in two steps. In the first step, a catalogue of all codes was created across participants and passages. This served as a secondary reliability and validity check, in that the consistency with which the codes had been assigned could be more directly assessed. These code categories were then associated with possible goals at which the behaviors falling under each category could reasonably be aimed. A theoretically-derived taxonomy of possible goals while reading was developed (Fox et al., 2005); this appears in Appendix B. These hierarchically ordered goals are: remembering the text, understanding the text, evaluating the text’s message, and evaluating the text as a medium, with each being broken out into possible sub-goals related to the local or global level of the outcome. Each code category was either determined to be not assignable to a specific goal, or was assigned to one or more possible goals with which the behaviors identified under that category might be associated. In some cases this was unambiguous and immediately obvious, while in other cases 13 additional contextualization would be necessary. A table giving examples of codes associated with specific goals is given in Appendix C. We then returned to the transcripts and the first and second author independently recoded the transcripts for evidence of goals. Although it is possible and likely that our participants were aiming at multiple goals at the same time, we aimed at identifying the primary and directly evident text-related goal associated with these instances of their verbalizations and behaviors. We summarized those goal codings in terms of the strength of the evidence they provided that our participants were working toward these levels of goals – we either saw no evidence, saw a single instance of a behavior suggesting a particular level of goal, saw multiple instances of the same type of behavior suggesting a particular level, or saw multiple instances of a variety of behaviors suggesting a particular level of goal. We collapsed the local/global distinction here and concentrated on nine basic levels of goals. We had substantial agreement on the nature of the evidence we detected for these goals, with 86% exact agreement across all transcripts. Table 3 gives the combined codings for these identified goal levels. Procedures Participants were told that their participation would involve two components. In the first part, participants were given, in order, the demographics questionnaire, interest questionnaire, activities questionnaires, domain knowledge and topic knowledge measure for either reading or history, and then the domain and topic knowledge measures for the other domain. Administration of the knowledge measures was counterbalanced such that. There were no time limits for completion. Participants were encouraged to respond to every item on each measure, and for the knowledge measures, were told that guessing was okay. The average time for completing the 14 first part was roughly 45 minutes. They could then take a brief break before beginning the second part. In the second part, participants practiced thinking-aloud with a short passage on mosquitoes, from a popularly written science article by Marston Bates (1975) originally published in Natural History magazine. This passage was intended to be mildly interesting but not too challenging, so that participants might naturally find themselves responding to the topic and descriptions. After practice, participants then read and thought aloud for either the reading or history passage. This thinking-aloud was audiotaped. Participants were instructed that they could either read the text itself aloud or not, as they chose, while they were reading and thinking aloud. Participants were also told that if they remained silent for an extended period, they would be prompted to say what they were thinking. When participants indicated that they were finished reading the passage, the passage was taken away, and they were given the packet of outcome questions. They were told that they could complete the questions in any order and look back and forth between their responses on the questions if they wished. Upon completing the outcome questions, the participants were again reminded of the think-aloud directions and given the other passage, following the same procedure as outlined above. Administration of passages was counterbalanced such that the four participants who saw the reading knowledge measures first also saw the reading passage first, and similarly for the three participants who saw the history measures first. There was no time limit for these tasks. Participants tended to take very nearly the same time for each passage, and the average time for the second session was again roughly 45 minutes. The think-alouds were transcribed into text documents by the first author. Results 15 Knowledge and interest Overall, participants’ scores on the domain and topic knowledge measures for reading and history were high, and their scores for history knowledge considerably lower, as would be expected. Their average interest also reflected the expected difference between domains, with reading-related interest scores much higher than history-related interest scores. Table 2 provides overall mean scores for knowledge and interest for both domains, and Table 3 gives the breakouts by participants. We did see some differences between participants in terms of level of knowledge of the topic of psycholinguistics. Two participants got perfect scores on the topic knowledge measure (56 points) and one was near perfect (53), while others came in with considerably lower performance.. Goals Our three expectations regarding goals tended to be supported by the data from our coding for goals, which are given in Table 4. Our first expectation was that we would see expert readers pursuing multiple levels of goals. We did see that overall, these expert readers appearing to be pursuing multiple levels of goals while reading these texts. Further, we saw relatively strong evidence that each of these specific theoretically-derived levels of goals was being pursued, in that both coders identified multiple instances of different types of behaviors aimed at each of these levels. The one exception was evaluation of text quality, and the reason for that was that only one type of behavior was coded as indicating evaluation of text quality. But both coders did see multiple instances of particular readers engaging in evaluation of text quality. Our second expectation was that these readers who were experts in the domain of reading would, by virtue of their greater levels of knowledge and interest for reading, engage more frequently in higher-level processing associated with higher-level goals such as evaluation and 16 situation of the text and its argument in context for the reading passage. This expectation was also borne out by the evidence as presented in Table 4. Each reader was seen to engage in a greater variety of higher-level goals for the reading passage than for the history passage. In particular, five of the seven participants tried to understand the argument in context for the reading passage, but only two for the history passage. Our third expectation was that participants who started out with stronger background knowledge of psycholinguistics would tend to engage more deeply with the reading text than those who did not have such knowledge. Table 5 presents participants’ levels of goals for the reading passage organized by their topic knowledge scores. It appears that readers with lower levels of knowledge tended to be not quite as active in pursuing higher-level goals as those with higher levels of knowledge, but this expectation is not unambiguously supported by our findings. Outcomes One interesting piece of information emerged from the outcome performances of these experts with regard to their evaluation of the author’s argument (question 4). All seven participants offered criticisms of the author’s argument for the reading passage, while the majority of them did not feel qualified to criticize the argument for the history passage, making comments such as: “Compelling + believable, although I’ve not too much knowledge too challenge major assertions,” (P1); “Cannot judge as I read the article as a statement of historical fact rather than a position statement,” (P3); and “Seems reasonable – I don’t have any prior knowledge to dispute the author’s claims” (P7). And of the three who did offer criticisms of the author’s argument in the history passage (P4, P5, P6), only one (P6) had attempted to understand the argument in context while reading. None had attempted to evaluate the argument in context, and two (P4, P6) had shown no evidence of evaluating the content of the argument itself. 17 Overall reading activity and performance To present a more complete picture of what we saw from these expert readers, descriptive qualitative profiles of their approach to this reading situation follow. P1 – Reading as Meaningful Dialogue. P1 was a male full professor, 48 years old, who had received his PhD in 1985. P1’s think-alouds were extremely rich and long; the transcript of his reading think-aloud was 12 pages, while that for his history think-aloud was 8 pages. He read nearly all of each passage aloud. Although P1 began the reading of the first passage acknowledging that the study created an atypical reading situation, he seemed very quick to set personal goals for reading the texts. He approached both passages in a similar way, looking for context and argument from the very beginning, although he was ready to focus on issues of understanding when necessary. One of the most striking features of P1’s approach emerging from the transcript is the number of evaluations and elaborations of the texts. For P1, almost every sentence became an occasion to comment on the specific content, on the quality of the argument and of the text, and on the broader implications of the ideas encountered both for the domains and for the larger socio-political environment. Far from being a mere exercise in rhetoric, P1 engaged the authors of the texts at a very personal level, as a meaningful and personally relevant dialogue. However, these elaborations on the texts did not prevent P1’s ability to build a contextualized and critical understanding of the content and of the arguments. Similarly, the openness of his views on the topics did not prevent him from evaluating the grounding of his judgments. Thus, although he fully identified and contextualized the arguments in both passages, he acknowledged that he needed more information about the specific historical period in order to decide about the merit of the historical argument. On the contrary, P1 had no trouble in evaluating the merit and the 18 implications of the reading passage, a text that allowed him to revisit a part of domain history that is also his own. P2 – Reading with a Local Focus. P2 was a 49 year old female doctoral student and reading specialist. P2 read considerable portions of each passage aloud during her think-alouds, the transcripts of which were each 5 pages in length. In reading the texts, P2 focused almost exclusively on understanding and evaluating individual ideas. This overall stance was consistent across the reading of both passages and is well documented by the analyses of the think-alouds. For example, P2 often restated the content of sentences or short paragraphs, linking the information from the texts with her prior knowledge and asking questions when she was not satisfied by her level of comprehension. However, she very rarely focused on global understanding or evaluation of the content of the texts. Beside these similarities, a few interesting differences emerged across the two domains. In approaching the reading passage, P2 immediately noticed the date of the article and situated it in the domain discourse of the time; in approaching the history passage, she looked at the title, searching for some clues about the content of the text, but she did not pay attention to the author, or to the date of publication. However, in restating and evaluating the content of both texts, P2 never acknowledged the authors, but attributed these claims to the “it” or “that” of an authorless text. A second difference regarded the amount of evaluation of claims across the two domains. In the reading passage, restatement and evaluation of content followed each other almost constantly, while evaluation of content happened more rarely during the reading of the history text. However, in both domains, these evaluations regarded single aspects of the texts and did not 19 address the arguments advanced by the authors, an occurrence that is consistent with her lack of acknowledgment of the writers. P3 – Variations Across Domains. P3 was a female assistant professor. She was 30 years old, and had received her PhD in 2002. Overall, the way in which P3 read the two texts differed markedly, although she spent approximately the same amount of time reading each. The transcript of her think-aloud for each passage was two pages long; she read small portions of each text aloud, and read under her breath as well. In reading the history passage, she focused almost exclusively on the local understanding of text content. The think-aloud did not reveal any attempt at building a global understanding of the passage; evaluation of the content or of the argument were also absent. In contrast, P3 repeatedly reported that she was trying “to figure out” what the author was saying in the reading passage, although she concluded the think-aloud questioning her understanding of his argument. P3’s comparatively low prior knowledge of the topic did not facilitate her comprehension of the reading text, making her dismiss those claims that probably contrasted with her prior understandings. Finally, although P3 never named the authors of either text, she repeatedly addressed the author of the reading passage as “he”, while she linked the claims in the history passage to a more generic “they” only rarely. P4 – Reading for Content. P4 was a female post-doctoral student. She was 38 years old and had received her PhD in 2005. She spent longer and verbalized more while reading the reading text than the history text, with a four page transcript for reading and a 2 page transcript for history. She read aloud while skimming and paraphrasing the text content for both passages. The main feature of P4’s reading was a focus on understanding individual ideas expressed in the text. P4 was the only participant who underlined and circled parts of the texts while reading and her markings seemed to serve the purpose of identifying the main ideas expressed in a paragraph. 20 Her attention was mainly on identifying and understanding the content of the texts; none of her comments suggested that she was engaging with the arguments made by the authors, nor that she was recognizing them as such, although, in the case of the reading passage, she demonstrated her agreement or disagreement with individual statements. In addition, she treated the content of the text as authorless, although she checked the date of publication of the reading article. Although this process was consistent across the two passages, P4 reached this goal in a different way across the two domains. In reading the history text, she alternated a focus on understanding individual sentences with a focus on the main idea expressed in the paragraph, while in the reading passage she immediately aimed at a more global level of understanding. P5 – Reading Globally and Contextually. P5 was a female assistant professor, 62 years old, who had received her PhD in 1990. P5 spent similar amounts of time and verbalized a similar amount for each passage, with a 2 page transcript for the reading passage, and a 1 ½ page transcript for the history passage. She did no reading aloud of the texts. P5 approached the reading task similarly across the domains, focusing immediately on contextualizing the texts by identifying their authors. For both passages, she also tried to build a global understanding of the text content and of the authors’ arguments. However, the way in which she reached these goals was different across the two domains. With the history text, she focused at first on the local understanding of individual ideas and began only later in the reading to weave these units together, reaching a global comprehension and evaluation of the argument only when she arrived at the end of the passage. On the other hand, her familiarity with the topic of the reading text allowed her to engage the content and the argument of the text from the very beginning, so that the author’s claims were continuously contextualized and evaluated. Interestingly, P5 occasionally focused her attention on particular words employed by the authors and, in the case 21 of the history passage, she also wondered whether anybody knew how the distinction between indentured servants and slaves actually developed, both occurrences that signal her awareness of the author in the text. P6 – Reading for Global, Contextualized Argument. P6 was a male full professor, 63 years old, who had received his PhD in 1968. He spent similar amounts of time and verbalized similarly across the two passages, with 5-page transcripts of his think-alouds for each passage. He read aloud small portions, but did more skimming and paraphrasing for both passages. P6 was the only participant in our study who clearly expressed awareness of a difference between his usual reading goals and the goals imposed by the study’s task. He was also the only participant who explicitly revisited and rehearsed the main ideas of each text once he completed the readings. Across the two passages, P6 actively tried to understand the author’s argument, situating it in the domain context. However, the way in which he reached these goals differed across the two texts. Being very familiar with the content of the reading passage, P6 focused mainly on the rhetorical purpose of the author and on the significance of his argument for the reading domain; almost no effort was spent on understanding the content. Although P6 began reading the history passage in a similar way, trying to understand its “theme,” a word that he later used to indicate the rhetorical purpose of the text, he then alternated understanding of content at the local and global level with understanding of argument, initially also at the local level. However, his use of the specific features of the history text (footnotes) allowed him to identify the author’s argument of the author, and to begin placing it in the domain context In addition, P6’s awareness of the text’s author was very evident; however, while in the reading passage the author was immediately identified as “a friend and colleague” and always referred to as “he,” in the history text this personal identification appeared only later on in the 22 reading, once the historical argument became more evident. It was only at this point that P6 began to refer to the author of specific claims as “he,” in contrast to the more general “they” previously used. P7 - Local Understandings and Global Evaluations. P7 was a female full professor. She was 53 years old and had received her PhD in 1984. P7 read the two texts in a very similar way, and the transcripts of her think-alouds were equally brief, one page for each passage. She did no reading aloud. P7 began by trying to predict the overall content of the text and then focused on understanding it locally; in both cases, she concluded by evaluating the global argument of the text, although she did not seem to pay much consideration to its nuances. In addition, she evidenced awareness of the author only in the reading passage, but seemed to conceive the history text as authorless; thus, it is difficult to determine whether she read the author’s historical interpretation as an argument or whether she treated it as factual information. Conclusions/Implications The reading experts demonstrated the expected high levels of individual interest and domain knowledge in the domain of reading, and lower levels of interest and domain and topic knowledge for history. They varied considerably in their response to the reading text and in their pre-assessed knowledge of psycholinguistics. There was considerable variation in their individual approaches to the reading task, in terms of their level of verbalization during the think-alouds and their observed level of interaction with the text. Readers tended to be consistent in their approach and amount of verbalization across the two passages. The reading experts also tended to evaluate the argument more critically for the reading passage than for the history passage. 23 There was evidence of pursuit of multi-layered reading goals, as readers evaluated their own understanding of words, sentences, paragraphs, of the passage as a whole, of the author’s argument, and of the position of the argument within the realm of domain discourse. However, these reading experts appeared to be much more strongly positioned for pursuit of higher level goals in regard to the reading passage.. The road to domain expertise in academic domains is traveled with the aid of reading. Proficient reading and its role in knowledge building can facilitate a reader’s development of academic expertise. Learning from text in an academic domain does not happen automatically or identically; the roles of knowledge and interest in shaping strategic processing and supporting learning have long been recognized (James, 1992). One way to illuminate the nature of that shaping and support is to look at reading goals. In order to understand what readers get out of reading it seems essential to consider what they are trying to get out of reading. Development of an articulated understanding of the role of self-determined reading goals for readers at varying levels of knowledge and interest will provide a vocabulary and structure around which instruction can be framed. This is a messy and complex task, particularly when considering how readers approach real-life reading situations, but it seems one well-worth beginning to address. 24 References Alexander, P. A. (1997). Mapping the multidimensional nature of domain learning: The interplay of cognitive, motivational, and strategic forces. In M. L. Maehr & P. R. Pintrich (Eds.), Advances in motivation and achievement (Vol. 10, pp. 213-250). Greenwich, CT: JAI press. Alexander, P. A. (1998). The nature of disciplinary and domain learning: The knowledge, interest, and strategic dimensions of learning from subject-matter text. In C. Hynd (Ed.), Learning from text across conceptual domains (pp. 263-287). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Alexander, P. A. (2002, December). Profiling the developing reader: The interplay of knowledge, interest, and strategic processing. Oscar Causey address presented at the annual meeting of the National Reading Conference, Miami, FL. Alexander, P. A., Murphy, P. K. & Kulikowich, J. M. (1998). What responses to domain-specific analogy problems reveal about emerging competence: A new perspective on an old acquaintance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 397-406. Fox, E., Maggioni, L., & Riconscente, M. (2005, August). Exploring expertise in reading and with reading: Characteristics and methodological issues. In P. Alexander (Chair). The road to domain expertise: Texts, situations, technology, and methodology. Symposium presented at the annual meeting of the American Psychological Association, Washington, DC. Fox, E., Dinsmore, D. L., & Alexander, P. A. (2007, April). Situational success at reading challenging texts: Exposing the fragile understanding of college students. In P. Alexander 25 (Chair). Fragile understanding: When good ideas go bad. Symposium presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, Chicago, IL. James, W. (1992). Writings 1878-1899. NY: The Library of America. Kintsch, W. (1998). Comprehension. New York: Cambridge University Press. Marton, F., & Säljö, R. (1976). On qualitative differences in learning: I-Outcome and process. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 46, 4-11. Marton, F., & Säljö, R. (2005) Approaches to learning. In Marton, F., Hounsell, D. and Entwistle, N., (Eds.) The experience of learning: Implications for teaching and studying in higher education (pp. 39-58). 3rd (Internet) edition. Edinburgh: University of Edinburgh, Centre for Teaching, Learning and Assessment. Pearson, P. D. (1978). Some practical applications of a psycholinguistic model of reading. In S. J. Samuels (Ed.), What research has to say about reading instruction (pp. 84-97). Newark, DE: International Reading Association. Pintrich, P. R. (2000). The role of goal orientation in self-regulated learning. In M. Bokaerts, P. Pintrich, & M. Zeidner (Eds.) Handbook of self-regulation (pp. 451-502). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Pressley, M., & Afflerbach, P. (1995). Verbal protocols of reading: The nature of constructively responsive reading. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. RAND Reading Study Group (2002). Reading for understanding: Toward an R&D program in reading comprehension. Santa Monica, CA: RAND. Scardamalia, M., & Bereiter, C. (1991). Literate expertise. In K. A. Ericsson & J. Smith (Eds.) Toward a general theory of expertise (pp. 172-194). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. 26 Takaki, R. (1993). A different mirror: A history of multicultural America. Boston: Little, Brown, & Co. Tapiero, I. (2007). Situation models and levels of coherence: Toward a definition of comprehension. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 27 Appendix A Codes for Think-Alouds Strategic behaviors Reading aloud Re-reading Reading on (following statement of non-comprehension) [“I’ll have to see what he has to say.”] Skimming (reading aloud while skipping portions) Guessing the meaning of a word in context [“Liminality. I’m assuming that means that they were – I’m not sure what that means. They were within some type of limit, I guess.”] Predicting [“He’s about, he’s about to explain it, I think.”] Questioning [“So this distinction exists, what is the distinction?”] Underlining [“I’m underlining grapho-phonemic and symbol-sound. … … … I’m underlining syntactic and semantic. And circling distinction.”] Using text feature [“This is the new sub-heading”] Restating (paraphrase) or repeating text information - local (word, phrase, sentence level) [“So the order of the words, he’s saying, is less … valuable than what the words mean.”] - global (paragraph, passage level) [“Oh, so they wanted to decrease the number of white indentured servants because they saw them as much more of a threat, in, to their economic advantage. And to, to increase the black population.”] Making connections 28 - to background knowledge [“And that, that explanation of indentured servitude pretty much fits my mental model.”] - to personal experience [“I see that it’s written by Dave Pearson who’s a close friend of mine and we actually are working on quite a number of projects right now.”] - to prior text [“So as the passage said earlier, the issues were more rooted in class than in race.”] - to topic knowledge test [“Well, that item was in the test we just took.”] - to context (domain discourse) [“I know that Takaki’s work is very, um, debated. Some people love it, some people hate it.”] Interpreting/hypothesizing (a statement building upon what directly said in the text) [“But the issue is whether or not, how consciously, um, the landowners – pushed the whiteblack class issue.”] Elaborating (a statement moving away from what is directly said in the text) [“I mean, those are exact- exactly the kind of strategies that I think a lot of students are being taught today, with the, with the great big push on standardized assessment.”] Arguing with the text [“But you’d have to have some grapho-phonic clues to figure that out, because it could be the astronomer looked through the window, the astronomer looked through something else. So you still need the grapho-phonic clues to confirm it. Which he does say there, but.”] Establishing a goal for reading [“So what I’m trying to get now is, what is this about, you know, the theme.”] 29 Rehearsing material from the text [“So if I was gonna try to remember this for future purposes of my own recollection, I would say, I would kind of go back across major points.”] Monitoring/Evaluative behaviors Evaluating comprehension (positive or negative) [“I am not following this at all.”] Evaluating agreement with text (positive or negative) [”I don’t know that I agree with that.”] Evaluating text quality [“And this is a nice sentence and set of following questions that I think can help people focus on the importance of syntax and how much we do use it sometimes to help us construct meaning.”] Evaluation of interest (positive or negative) [“This is new and interesting to me.”] Evaluation of importance of text [“And I – this is a really important paragraph, too.”] Evaluation of argument [“Well that, thank you, Ron Takaki, that’s part of what I was asking, but I’m still not sure where you’re getting your stuff from.”] Evaluation of author intent [“Um, who’s he writing for here, search and destroy. Hm, maybe teachers, maybe graduate students.”] Expression of empathy (sympathy or feelings felt or imputed to others) [“And that’s really sad, the, the men and women being in the same line as the cows, and the oxen.”] 30 Appendix B Coding for Text-related Goals A. Remembering 1. Remembering text details 2. Remembering text important points B. Understanding 1. Understanding the text content a. local understanding of text (word or sentence level) b. global understanding of text (paragraph or larger level) 2. Understanding the author’s meaning – the argument a. local understanding of argument (sentence level) b. global understanding of argument (paragraph or larger level) 3. Understanding the argument in context – as situated in domain discourse C. Evaluating the Message 1. Evaluating agreement with the content of the text (accuracy of text) a. local b. global 2. Evaluating agreement with the argument a. local b. global 3. Evaluating the argument in context – as situated in domain discourse D. Evaluating the Medium 1. Evaluating text quality a. local b. global 2. Evaluating argument quality a. local b. global 31 Appendix C Rehearse –Evidence of goal A1, A2, remember important points and details. “So if I was gonna try to remember this for future purposes of my own recollection, I would say, I would kind of go back across major points. First one, indentured servants outnumbered everyone else, seventy-five percent in 1664. Um, They were vagabonds and cheats. Um, then whites and blacks were a little bit, initially, leery of each other, but then started to escape together, after a while, there was uneven treatment. Um, blacks were treated much worse, and, uh, whites were given lesser sentences. And by nine, by sixteen sixty one, or, no, eight years later, sixteen sixty nine, seems to me like a cool date. Sixteen sixty nine, Virginia legislature defined them as property. Um, blacks, slaves, didn’t increase too much, white slaves increased more, or – and started to become free.” Read on–Evidence of goal B1a – local understanding of text “But we’ll see where we go with this.” Guess meaning of word in context – Evidence of goal B1a – local understanding of text “Liminality. I’m assuming that means that they were – I’m not sure what that means. They were within some type of limit, I guess.” Question – Possible evidence of goal B1a – local understanding of text or goal B3 – understanding the argument in context “Ok, I’m thinking, um, where, which Africans, when, what part of the colonies” B1a “what’s the rhetorical, you know, purpose of this text. Um, planters who bought slaves instead of servants. Takaki, what’s his interest in this whole thing.” B3 Interpret/hypothesize – Evidence of goal B1 – understand text content [P6 R p2-3, 22-1] “I’m thinking of these words, these vocabulary words as being those labels that represent these concepts.” Restate (local) – Evidence of goal B1 – understanding text content “Ok, a landed elite enacting legislation to advance and protect their class interests.” Re-read – Possible evidence of goal B1 – understanding text content “Okay. I’m gonna re-read this, ‘cause I want to make sure I understand the distinction here.” Skim – Possible evidence of goal B1 – understanding text content 32 “… the active role of the store of semantic and syntactic – already existing in the reader’s knowledge. Okay. … assign primary activity to decoding processes, semantic syntactic to the role of verifying…” Connect to prior text – Possible evidence of goal B1b – global understanding of text “but it says up here that they were, um, that there were basically in the same class at that point.” Mark on text – Possible evidence of goal B1b – global understanding of text “I like to write when I read, and underline important words, and think about – what they mean. … And I’m underlining certain sets of words appearing in the same context fit together.” Restate (global) – Possible evidence of goal of B1b – global understanding of text or B2b – global understanding of argument “This article has been talking about psycholinguistics primarily” B1b “What I’m learning here though is it seems that in the beginning the Virginia colony did not have a class of slave; it had indentured servants and that there was an evolution for the African American servants into slavery.” B2b Predict – Possible evidence of goal B1b – global understanding of text or B2b global understanding of argument “Okay, so it’s going to be about multiculturalism in America.” B1b “I’m looking forward to a couple paragraphs at least talking about how the class of a particular people allows them to eventually adopt a system of slavery.” B2b Interpret/hypothesize – Evidence of goal B2 – understanding the argument, goal B3 – understand argument in context or goal C3 – evaluate argument in context “I think he’s saying that syntax and semantics are more important, and he doesn’t really like phonics.” B2 “but it seems to me to be carrying some foundational knowledge and claims about the importance of language knowledge to reading, to learning how to read, to successfully learning how to read.” C3 “it shows one historian playing with another’s ideas and in this case saying, ‘here’s a guy who wrote a whole book about this whole issue and I’d like to push it a little bit farther.” B3 Identify/use text feature – Possible evidence of goal B1 – understanding the text content, goal B2 - understanding the argument, or goal B3 – understanding the argument in context 33 “I’m reading the title, because I’m hoping it’s gonna give me an idea of what’s – It gives me an idea of what I’m reading about here” B1 “I’m going back to see when this was written.” B3 “Okay, I’m reading the footnote for, seeing how that is, what this is from.” B2 Evaluate importance – Evidence of goal B2b – Global understanding of argument. “And I – this is a really important paragraph, too” Evaluate author intent – Evidence of goal B3 – understanding the argument in context “In this last paragraph, he, he sees himself as extending, um, a more, let’s see, views of reading where efficient readers assign primary activity to decoding. So he’s, he’s extended it to syntax and to semantics.” Connect to context (domain discourse) – Evidence of goal B3 – understanding the argument in context or goal C3 – evaluating the argument in context “Okay. Now I’m understanding a little better why Pearson is taking this point of view. Uh, I didn’t notice that this was published in 1978. So that really is before so much of the work on total text comprehension had even been started.” B3 “I like, uh, I like histories of America that are I’d say non-traditional, traditional being you know, white men, powerful white men and what they did to make our country great.” C3 Argue/disagree with text– Evidence of goal C1a – evaluate agreement with content of the text or goal C2b – evaluating agreement with argument - global “Well, you know, I’d quibble with that, um, the notion of demonstrating that you’ve understood what you’ve read is ah - that’s a really short um – it’s a really small slice of how you might demonstrate. Um - It’s the most typical and it’s the most valued assessment-wise, answering these questions, but we know there’s lots of ways to demonstrate what you’ve understood and actually you don’t get much past I would say surface demonstration of understanding when you do these tricks to answer questions, because the questions themselves are surface level.” C1a “one difference that I can see that they don’t mention here is that it looks to me as if the white indentured servants from England chose to come, and, uh, … slaves from Africa didn’t.” C2b Evaluate agreement – Evidence of goal C1a – evaluating agreement with the content of the text (accuracy of the text) or goal C2b – Evaluating agreement with the argument. “Um, I’m not sure that the last 2 decades of what’s it called – eye movement data support that assertion that there’s sort of a minimal or as much as needed sampling of print that we need to try to affirm our predictions. I think it’s actually much closer to word-by-word reading.” C1a 34 “But I do agree that, with good readers, decoding is less important.” C2b Connect to prior knowledge – knew – Possible evidence of goal C1 – evaluating agreement with the content of the text “I thought slaves automatically served for the rest of their life. You know, once they were slaves, they were slaves forever.” Evaluate text quality – Evidence of goal D1 – Evaluating text quality, either local or global. “That wasn’t written very well, I don’t think. [laughs]. That was a little too confusing.” Evaluate argument (source, structure, quality/agreement) – Evidence of goals D2 – evaluating agreement with the argument or evaluating argument quality. “I haven’t read up to a point where I even start engaging with footnotes so this is all stuff that the author’s telling me. I pretty much have to take it as legitimate claims” D2 Establish/state goal – Evidence of stated goal. “So what I’m trying to get now is, what is this about, you know, the theme,” B1b Connect to personal experience - Not necessarily goal-related. Connect to prior knowledge – Not necessarily goal-related. Connect to topic knowledge test/earlier task – Not goal-related Elaborate – Not necessarily goal-related Express interest/surprise – Not necessarily goal-related Express empathy/moral judgment – Not goal-related Read aloud – Not goal-related 35 Table 1 Demographic Data ID Gender/Age Ethnic Background Year Received PhD Academic Position (when data collected) Full professor 1 M - 48 European-American 1985 2 F - 49 European-American -- 3 F - 30 European-American 2002 Doctoral student, Reading specialist Assistant professor 4 F - 38 European-American 2005 Post-doctoral student 5 F - 62 European-American 1990 Associate professor 6 M - 63 European-American 1968 Full professor 7 F - 53 European-American 1984 Full professor Table 2 Overall Reading and History Knowledge and Interest Means Measure Domain knowledge Topic knowledge Subject-related Interest Activity-related Interest Reading mean (sd) (n = 7) History mean (sd) (n = 7) 63.00 (8.23) 35.57 (6.80) 49.00 (6.63) 24.57 (0.53) 35.86 (6.09) 18.43 (5.97) 52.57 (6.97) 27.29 (8.88) Note: Maximum possible scores: domain knowledge = 80 topic knowledge = 56 subject-related interest = 25 activity-related interest = 75 36 Table 3 Individual Reading and History Knowledge and Interest Scores Participant 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Reading Domain Knowledge 66 64 52 62 78 63 64 Note: Maximum possible scores: domain knowledge = 80 topic knowledge = 56 subject-related interest = 25 activity-related interest = 75 Reading Topic Knowledge 53 56 44 38 47 56 56 Reading Subject Interest 24 25 24 25 24 25 25 Reading Activity Interest 53 46 55 63 44 59 48 History History Domain Topic Knowledge Knowledge 47 37 40 43 31 29 26 26 38 38 34 38 40 43 History Subject Interest 25 22 15 23 22 10 12 History Activity Interest 39 24 20 41 25 21 21 37 Table 4 Goal Level by Participant, Passage, and Type of Evidence GOAL LEVEL 1 R 1 H 2 R 2 H 3 R 3 H 4 R 4 H 5 R 5 H 6 R 6 H 7R 7 H A - Remember text 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 X X 0 0 B1 - Understand text X X X X X X X X X X X X X X B2 - Understand argument x X 0 0 0 0 0 x X X 0 0 B3 - Understand argument in context X X x 0 0 0 x 0 X X X 0 0 C1 - Evaluate text content X X X X X 0 X x 0 x X 0 x x 0 0 0 0 0 X 0 0 0 X 0 0 0 x X 0 0 C2 – Evaluate argument content C3 - Evaluate argument in context X X 0 0 0 0 D1 - Evaluate text quality X X X 0 x 0 D2 - Evaluate argument quality X X 0 x 0 0 0 0 0 x = one piece of evidence, as determined by both coders X = multiple instances of single type of behavior, as determined by both coders X = multiple instances of multiple types of behaviors, as determined by both coders 0 = no evidence, as determined by both coders R = Reading passage H – History passage 0 0 38 Table 5 Goal Level for Reading Passage by Score on Topic Knowledge Test Participant ID 6 2 1 7 5 3 4 Score on Psycholinguistic Knowledge 56 56 53 49 47 44 38 A - Remember text X 0 0 0 0 0 0 B1 - Understand text X X X X X X X B2 - Understand argument X 0 x 0 0 0 B3 - Understand argument in context X x X 0 X 0 x C1 - Evaluate text content x X X X X C2 – Evaluate argument content 0 0 C3 - Evaluate argument in context X 0 D1 - Evaluate text quality D2 - Evaluate argument quality GOAL LEVEL X X x 0 X 0 0 0 X X X x 0 X 0 0 0