PCB 5936 Autumn 2004 EXERCISE 4: RANDOM EFFECTS AND MIXED MODELS AND MORE THOUGHT A scientist was interested in whether birth rates responded to density variation in different ways in different populations. More specifically, the scientist thought that in populations with a history of high densities, birth rates would be lower at low densities and higher at high densities when compared to the birth rates at low and high density in populations with a history of low densities. The scientist designed an experiment to investigate whether this was the case. The scientist constructed experimental “populations” in mesocosms (artificial environments); from this point onward, each experimental unit will be called a “replicate.” The mesocosms are large enough to capture typical natural rates of individual movement and interactions among individuals and they have their own renewable food resources for the animals under study. The scientist had extensive background data on density distributions in many natural populations of his/her focal organism, so the experiment was not designed in a vacuum. The experiment used stocks of animals drawn from two types of natural populations, “low density history” populations and “high density history” populations. The scientist used a nested design in which stocks from eight different natural populations were “nested” within each level of “history” (i.e. four “low density” populations and four “high density” populations). The scientist examined the birth rates of these stocks at three experimental densities, 16, 32, and 64 females per replicate. The scientist had five replicates at each combination of treatment density and original population. Hence there were 120 replicates, 40 per experimental density; 20 of the replicates were drawn from “low density history” populations, 5 replicates from each of 4 individual natural populations nested within that history, and 20 drawn from “high density history” populations with the same type of nesting. The scientist measured the birth rates of females in each replicate (number of offspring produced per 60-day measurement period) and averaged those rates among all females in a replicate so as to have one number per replicate. But once the data are collected, the scientist gets conflicting advice about the analysis. a) One consultant tells the scientist to consider each of the three effects (history, population within history, treatment) to be qualitative random effects. Write the expected mean square for each effect in the analysis of variance (treatment, history, population within history, the interaction of population within history and treatment, the interaction of history with treatment). Remember the basic principles of finding EMS and work from there; don’t forget to multiply through by the actual numbers (five replicates per combination, 4 populations nested within each history level, etc.). b) Another consultant tells the scientist that the treatment effect is a qualitative fixed effect. Without passing judgment on the advice, write the expected mean square for each effect if treatment is fixed but the others are random, as they were in part a). c) A third consultant tells the scientist that “history” and “treatment” should be fixed effects and that only the effect of population nested within history is truly random. Again, without passing judgment on the advice, write the expected mean square for each effect under this scenario. d) Now pass judgment on the advice: tell the scientist what you think the proper classification should be and why.