Running Head: PARENTING BEHAVIORS AND CHILDREN COPING WITH CANCER Children Coping with Cancer: Cross-Sectional and Prospective Relations between Parenting Behaviors and Children’s Coping Psychology Honors Research Thesis Jennifer Ragan Vanderbilt University PARENTING BEHAVIORS AND CHILDREN COPING WITH CANCER 2 Abstract Objective. Children diagnosed with cancer face numerous sources of stress and are at risk for emotional problems such as anxiety and depression. Parenting behavior and children’s coping are two important factors that may impact children’s adjustment to cancer. The purpose of the current study is to examine cross-sectional and prospective relations between mothers’ parenting behaviors and children’s coping in a sample of childhood cancer patients. Methods. Children ages 5-17 who had recently been diagnosed with new or relapsed cancer (n=108) and their parents were recruited from two hospitals in the Southern and Midwestern United States. Child and parent reports of parenting behaviors and child’s coping were obtained at two time points: near diagnosis (T1) and 12 months after diagnosis (T2). Results. Significant cross-sectional and longitudinal associations of parental warmth and parental psychological control with children’s coping were found near the time of child’s cancer diagnosis and one year later. These findings have the potential to guide future intervention studies to enhance adjustment outcomes for children with cancer by using parenting as an avenue to improve children’s coping. PARENTING BEHAVIORS AND CHILDREN COPING WITH CANCER 3 Introduction Recent estimates reported by the American Cancer Society (2013) suggest that about 12,000 children in the United States are diagnosed with cancer every year. Although this number is alarming, the 5-year survival rate for childhood cancer patients has been increasing significantly over the past 40 years due to advancements in treatment methods. Estimates predict the current 5-year survival rate for children with cancer to be 83% (American Cancer Society, 2013). While the increase in survival rates brings good news to childhood cancer patients and their parents, living with an illness such as cancer introduces many stressors into the lives of these children. Sources of stress for pediatric cancer patients include uncertainty about cancer and its outcomes, pain from medical procedures, side effects of treatment, separation from family, and changes in daily roles and functioning (e.g., Rodriguez, Dunn, Zuckerman, Vannatta, Gerhardt, & Compas, 2012; Rourke, Stuber, Hobbie, & Kazak, 1999). Many years after diagnosis, patients may continue to experience side effects from treatment such as impaired organ functioning, secondary cancers, and cognitive deficits (American Cancer Society, 2013). Making the transition into young adulthood may bring about new stress for childhood cancer survivors as they face the late effects of treatment and attempt to incorporate their traumatic past into their developing identity (Rourke et al., 1999). Because childhood cancer and progression into survivorship involve numerous sources of stress, these children are a vulnerable population for emotional problems. Children with cancer tend to report elevated levels of anxiety, depression, and symptoms of post-traumatic stress (e.g., Kazak, Alderfer, Rourke, Simms, Streisand, & Grossman, 2004; Sawyer, Antoniou, Toogood, & Rice, 1997; Schultz, Ness, Whitton, Recklitis, Zebrack, Robison, Zeltzer, & Mertens, 2007). PARENTING BEHAVIORS AND CHILDREN COPING WITH CANCER 4 Coping is one factor that has the potential to facilitate or impair children’s emotional adjustment to cancer (Compas et al., in press). Furthermore, research indicates that certain parenting behaviors are closely associated with children’s coping (Gaylord-Harden, Campbell, & Kesselring, 2010; Watson et al., in press). Therefore, parenting behaviors and children’s coping are two important factors to analyze in the context of children’s emotional adjustment to cancer. The purpose of the present study is to examine the relationship between mothers’ parenting behaviors and children’s coping in a sample of pediatric cancer patients. It is important to understand the specific associations between parenting and children’s coping, as findings may help clarify how to develop interventions that enhance emotional adjustment in children with cancer. Coping and Adjustment to Cancer The way a child copes with the stress of having cancer may affect emotional adjustment. Compas et al. (2001) define coping as conscious, volitional efforts to regulate oneself or the environment in response to stressful events or circumstances. Past research on the relationship between coping strategies and emotional adjustment for children with cancer has been summarized in a meta-analysis conducted by Aldridge and Roesch (2007). The researchers summarized the relationship between coping and emotional problems for pediatric cancer patients based on a synthesis of 26 past studies. Among these studies, a variety of coping measures and taxonomies were used. In their analysis, Aldridge and Roesch classified coping strategies from the 26 past studies based on two major dichotomous coping taxonomies: (a) approach (e.g., seeking information, positive reappraisal) vs. avoidance (e.g., denial, self-blame); and (b) problem-focused (e.g., problem solving, seeking support) vs. emotion-focused (e.g., acceptance, optimism). After assigning the coping strategies described in each study to the PARENTING BEHAVIORS AND CHILDREN COPING WITH CANCER 5 appropriate taxonomy, Aldridge and Roesch examined the associations between these 4 coping strategies and 6 emotional problems (overall adjustment, physical problems, depression, overall distress, overall anxiety, overall pain). For example, a study by Frank, Blount, and Brown (1997) was included in Aldridge and Roesch’s (2007) meta-analysis. In their study, Frank et al. used a brief 10-item self-report measure to assess the coping strategies of 86 children being treated for cancer. Coping strategies were classified as positive/approach or negative/avoidance. The researchers used the Child Behavior Checklist to measure participants’ internalizing and externalizing problems. Frank and colleagues found that children’s use of avoidance coping accounted for part of the variance in both depression and anxiety scores. They concluded that avoidance coping is a negative coping style that can result in emotional problems for children with cancer. Another study included in Aldridge and Roesch’s meta-analysis was by Tyc, Mulhern, Jayawardene, and Fairclough (1995). Tyc and colleagues assessed coping strategies and distress levels among 57 pediatric cancer patients undergoing chemotherapy. The researchers found that the children who reported low levels of distress about chemotherapy treatment were more likely to problem solve and seek social support as coping strategies (Tyc et al., 1995). These strategies fit into both the approach and problem-focused categories of coping. Tyc and colleagues also found that children who reported high levels of distress about chemotherapy treatment did not employ problem solving as a coping strategy. Together, these findings suggest that approach and problem-focused coping styles are associated with more positive emotional adjustment in children with cancer. Despite the findings of individual studies included in the meta-analysis, Aldridge and Roesch (2007) were unable to reach a significant consensus about coping and emotional PARENTING BEHAVIORS AND CHILDREN COPING WITH CANCER 6 adjustment. In their synthesis of the 26 studies, Aldridge and Roesch calculated effect sizes relating the four coping classifications to the six emotional problems. Of the 24 mean effect sizes, only one revealed a significant association between coping and emotional problems, indicating a small, positive association between emotion-focused coping and depression. Overall, the results of Aldridge and Roesch’s synthesis reveal that past studies have failed to provide evidence of a significant or meaningful association between coping strategies and emotional adjustment. Since Aldridge and Roesch (2007), little research has been added to the literature regarding children coping with cancer and emotional adjustment. Several researchers have reported on types of coping strategies used by childhood cancer patients (Engvall et al., 2011; Hildebrand et al., 2011; Li et al., 2010; Rollins, 2009; Smorti, 2012). However, the association between coping and emotional adjustment has not been a focus of analyses in these studies. One exception is Maurice-Stam, Oort, Last, Brons, Caron, and Grootenhuis (2009), who reported on coping strategies and quality of life of childhood cancer patients after the end of treatment. Maurice-Stam et al. (2009) found a small, negative correlation between the use of interpretative control coping (seeking more information about the disease) and quality of life after treatment ended. Results also revealed a small, positive correlation between the use of vicarious control coping (relying on expertise of physician and attributing power to cancer treatment) and quality of life. Despite the individual findings of Maurice-Stam and colleagues, however, there is still wide variation among researchers regarding the conceptualization and categorization of coping. Overall, recent literature still indicates a lack of consensus about the association between coping and emotional adjustment for children with cancer. It is possible that this discrepancy is due to the lack of consistency in the way researchers have measured and categorized coping. The PARENTING BEHAVIORS AND CHILDREN COPING WITH CANCER 7 present study is a part of a 5- to 6- year longitudinal study designed to overcome these limitations. The first goal is to use a valid measure of coping. Many measures of coping rely on brief methods of assessment. For example, the KIDCOPE is a widely used checklist developed by Spirito (1988) that uses 10 items to measure coping styles. Although a brief checklist is an efficient way conduct research, more detailed information about coping mechanisms can be captured with the use of longer questionnaires. In the current study, we use the Responses to Stress Questionnaire (RSQ), a valid and adaptable 57-item questionnaire created by ConnorSmith, Compas, Wadsworth, Thomsen, and Saltzman (2000) to evaluate a wide range of coping responses. Another limitation we aim to overcome is the wide variety in the categorization of coping styles. In the past, researchers have used dichotomous classifications such as approach versus avoidance responses or problem-focused versus emotion-focused coping. Based on the idea that there are actually multiple dimensions of coping to take into consideration, the RSQ was designed to evaluate coping according to three main dimensions of coping: primary control coping, secondary control coping, and disengagement coping (Connor-Smith et al., 2000). Primary control coping is an engagement strategy that involves controlling and changing the objective condition, such as altering the source of stress or one’s reaction to the stress (e.g., problem solving, emotional modulation). Secondary control coping is an engagement strategy that involves changing oneself to adapt to the stressor (e.g., acceptance, cognitive reappraisal). Disengagement responses involve active orientation away from a stressor (e.g., avoidance, denial). The RSQ and these three dimensions of coping are becoming more widely recognized in research and establish a strong basis for measuring coping (Hamill 2005; Silk et al., 2003; Wadsworth et al., 2004, 2005; Yao 2010). By using a valid measure and categorization of PARENTING BEHAVIORS AND CHILDREN COPING WITH CANCER 8 coping, the current study aims to resolve the discrepancy in past research and uncover an accurate relationship between coping styles and emotional adjustment for children with cancer. In baseline analyses with the sample that is the focus of the current study, Compas et al. (in press) found significant correlations of secondary control coping and disengagement coping with anxiety/depression within and across informants. Primary control coping was associated with anxiety/depression within informants. Further, linear multiple regression analyses indicated that secondary control coping accounted for unique variance in symptoms of anxiety/depression both within and across reports from children and their mothers. Parenting and Children’s Coping Findings from past research reveal that parenting behaviors, including parental warmth, psychological control, and behavioral control are associated with children’s coping (GaylordHarden et al., 2010; Kliewer, Fearnow, & Miller, 1996; McIntyre & Dusek, 1994; Watson et al., in press). Warm parenting is characterized by variations of responsiveness, acceptance, support, and sympathy (Morris, Silk, Steinberg, Myers, & Robinson, 2007). Parents who convey a sense of warmth and available support to their child provide a safe and secure environment for the child to develop. According to attachment theory, children in this environment tend to seek social support more often and engage problems more actively (Kliewer et al., 1996; McIntyre & Dusek, 1994). There have been several studies designed to examine the association between warm parenting and coping. For example, children’s perceptions of maternal acceptance are related to higher levels of active coping and support seeking in children (Gaylord-Harden et al., 2010; Kliewer et al., 1996). Observed maternal responsiveness is associated with an increased use of problem-focused coping in preadolescents with spina bifida (McKernon et al., 2001). Family social support has been associated with less wishful thinking, and more problem-focused PARENTING BEHAVIORS AND CHILDREN COPING WITH CANCER 9 coping and resiliency among children in rural, low-income populations (Markstrom, Marshall, & Tryon, 2000). Watson et al. (in press) examined the effects of a preventive intervention to improve parenting skills among a sample of mothers with a history of depression and to increase their children’s use of secondary control coping. From baseline to the 6-month follow-up, parents exhibited a significant increase in observed parental warmth and responsiveness, and from baseline to the 18-month follow-up, children showed a significant increase in use of secondary control coping strategies. Furthermore, increases in parental warmth and responsiveness significantly accounted for (mediated) the effects of the intervention on increases in children’s use of secondary control coping strategies over time. The findings of this intervention highlight the importance of the effects of warm and responsive parenting on children’s coping. Parental control is another parenting behavior associated with children’s coping. Control is commonly categorized into psychological control and behavioral control (Bean, Barber, & Crane, 2006; Schaefer, 1965). Psychological control refers to parental intrusion and manipulation of the child’s psychological world (Bean et al., 2006). Common characteristics of psychological control, including poor communication and parental intrusiveness, have been linked to more avoidant and disengagement coping strategies in children (Jackson, Bijstra, Oostra, & Bosma, 1998; Langrock, Compas, Keller, Merchant, & Copeland, 2002). Past research has also shown that psychological control leads to more avoidant coping strategies specifically among children in high stress environments (Gaylord-Harden et al., 2010). Therefore children who face the stressors of cancer diagnosis may be more vulnerable to the negative impact that psychological control has on coping. PARENTING BEHAVIORS AND CHILDREN COPING WITH CANCER 10 Behavioral control, sometimes referred to as firm control, is the extent of parental regulation and structure imposed upon the child’s behavioral world (Bean et al., 2006). The association between behavioral control and children’s coping is less straightforward. McIntyre and Dusek (1994) found that parental behavioral control was associated with increased problemfocused coping and reduced emotion-focused coping. Power and Manire (1992) found that behavioral control has a curvilinear relationship with children’s coping. That is, mothers using moderate levels of behavioral control provide sufficient guidelines for their children without being over- or under-controlling, providing a more positive context for learning (Hardy, Power, & Jaedicke, 1993). Present Study The purpose of the present study was to examine the relationship between mothers’ parenting behaviors (e.g., warmth, psychological control, and behavioral control) and children’s coping (e.g., primary control coping, secondary control coping, and disengagement coping) in a sample of pediatric cancer patients. Children diagnosed with cancer are exposed to numerous sources of stress, making them an important population of focus for adjustment interventions. No studies to date have examined the specific relationship between parenting behaviors and coping among pediatric cancer patients. There are several methodological strengths of the present study. Past researchers have often relied on single informants to gather data. In the current study, we used multiple sources to gather data, including the child and his or her mother. This allowed us to identify correlations using cross-informant reports and gain a more accurate depiction of possible associations. Furthermore, the design of the current study is longitudinal, whereas the majority of past studies have been cross-sectional. By using a longitudinal design, we were able to examine if changes occur over time in the association between parenting and children’s PARENTING BEHAVIORS AND CHILDREN COPING WITH CANCER 11 coping. By learning about the association across time, we will be better prepared to develop effective interventions that are relative to a child’s specific cancer timeline. Finally, the current study has the largest sample size to date for a longitudinal study following children from diagnosis to survivorship. Together, the strengths of the current study provided an opportunity to examine the relationship between parenting behaviors and coping styles in children with cancer. At this time, all of the participants enrolled in the current longitudinal study have been assessed at baseline (near the time of diagnosis) and 12 months after diagnosis. As noted above, baseline data on children’s coping and emotional problems (anxiety, depression) have been analyzed (Compas et al., in press). Significant baseline results suggest that at diagnosis, children who use primary control coping and secondary control coping tend to report less anxiety and depression. Disengagement coping, on the other hand, is associated with higher levels of anxiety and depression. These findings near diagnosis provide the rationale for the present study examining parenting behaviors and children’s coping. Now that we know children’s coping styles are related to symptoms of anxiety and depression, it is important to know what factors might impact children’s coping. Thus the goal of the present study was to examine the impact of parenting behaviors on children’s coping at baseline and at the 12-month follow-up. Hypotheses In this study we examined cross-sectional and prospective relations between mothers’ parenting behaviors and children’s coping among a sample of childhood cancer patients. Data was collected from mothers and children near the time of the child’s diagnosis (T1), and 12 months after diagnosis (T2). We tested the following hypotheses: Hypothesis 1: In bivariate correlation analyses, maternal warmth will be correlated with more primary control coping and secondary control coping and less disengagement coping. PARENTING BEHAVIORS AND CHILDREN COPING WITH CANCER 12 Maternal psychological control and behavioral control will be correlated with less primary control coping and secondary control coping and more disengagement coping. These results will apply cross-sectionally at T1 and T2, as well as prospectively from T1 to T2. Hypothesis 2: In linear multiple regression analyses, maternal warmth, psychological control, and behavioral control will have unique effects as predictors of children’s coping crosssectionally at both T1 and at T2. Hypothesis 3: In linear multiple regression analyses, maternal warmth, psychological control, and behavioral control will have unique effects as predictors of children’s coping prospectively from T1 to T2. Exploratory analyses were also conducted to examine the effect of age as a covariate with our parenting and coping variables, as well as a predictor of children’s coping. Method Participants Families who were eligible for enrollment in the study had a child that was: (1) age 5-17 years old at study entry, (b) had been recently diagnosed with a new or relapsed cancer, (c) was receiving treatment through the oncology division, and (d) had no pre-existing developmental disability. In the initial sample, participants included 336 children with cancer and their parents. Only mothers and children who completed reports on parenting behaviors and children’s coping were included in the analysis. Mother reports on parenting behaviors and their child’s coping were obtained near diagnosis (T1; n = 240) and 12 months after diagnosis (T2; n = 163). Child reports of parenting behaviors and children’s coping were also obtained from children who were old enough to complete the self-report measures at T1 (n = 114) and T2 (n = 81). Therefore, the PARENTING BEHAVIORS AND CHILDREN COPING WITH CANCER 13 final sample for this study included 240 mothers and 114 children at T1. Due to participant attrition in the sample, 163 mothers and 81 children remained in the study at T2. The children included in the present sample at T1 (near diagnosis) were on average 10.38 years old (SD = 3.89). In this group, 52.1% of the children were male and 47.9% were female. Race of the children included 83.3% White, 10.8% Black/African American, and 5.9% other. Children had diagnoses of leukemia (38.8%), lymphoma (22.1%), brain tumor (6.7%), or other solid tumor (32.5%). Annual family income levels varied: 30.4% earned $25,000 or less, 24.6% earned $25,001-$50,000, 13.8% earned $50,001-$75,000, 10.4% earned $75,001-$100,000, and 17.9% earned $100,001 or more. The children who remained in the sample at T2 (12 months after diagnosis) were on average 10.22 years old (SD = 3.94). In this group, 54% of the children were male and 46% were female. Race of the children included 85.9% White, 9.2% Black/African American, and 4.9% other. Children had diagnoses of leukemia (38.7%), lymphoma (22.7%), brain tumor (6.7%), or other solid tumor (31.9%). Annual family income levels varied from: 28.2% earned $25,000 or less, 26.4% earned $25,001-$50,000, 16.0% earned $50,001-$75,000, 11.7% earned $75,001-$100,000, and 15.3 % earned $100,001 or more. Measures Demographics and Medical Data. Parents provided demographic information including age, education level, race, family income, and marital status. Participants gave permission for the research staff to access medical data, where data regarding the child’s diagnosis/relapse status was extracted. Children’s Coping. The Responses to Stress Questionnaire-Pediatric Cancer version (RSQ-PC; Compas et al., in press; Connor-Smith et al., 2000; Miller et al., 2009; Rodriguez et PARENTING BEHAVIORS AND CHILDREN COPING WITH CANCER 14 al., 2012) was used to obtain adolescents’ self-reports and mothers’ reports of their children’s coping with cancer. The RSQ-PC version includes a list of 12 cancer-related stressors (e.g., missing school, frequent hospital or clinic visits, changes in personal appearance), and 57 items reflecting voluntary (coping) and involuntary (automatic) stress responses of children/adolescents in response to cancer-related stressors. For the purpose of this study, only the three voluntary coping scales are reported. The coping scales include: primary control coping (i.e., problem solving, emotional modulation, emotional expression), secondary control coping (i.e., acceptance, cognitive restructuring, positive thinking, distraction), and disengagement coping (i.e., avoidance, denial, wishful thinking). The factor structure of the RSQ has been supported in confirmatory factor analytic studies with children and adolescents from a wide range of ethnic and cultural backgrounds coping with a variety of stressors (Benson et al., 2011; Compas et al., 2006; Connor-Smith et al., 2000; Connor-Smith & Calvete, 2004; Wadsworth et al., 2004; Yao et al., 2011). Parenting Behaviors. The Children’s Report of Parent Behavior Inventory-30 (CRPBI30; Schludermann & Schludermann, 1988) was used to obtain children’s perceptions of their mothers’ parenting behavior. Mothers provided self-reports of their own parenting behavior using the Parent Behavior Inventory-30 (PBI-30), a first-person, but otherwise identical, version of the CRPBI-30. Children and mothers responded to 30 items that evaluate parenting behaviors using a 3-point scale (1 = “not like,” 2 = “somewhat like,” 3 = “a lot like”). The 30 items yielded three categories of parenting behaviors: Warmth, Psychological Control, and Behavioral Control. The CRPBI-30 and PBI-30 are shortened versions of an original instrument developed by Schaefer (1965) that included over 260 items but yielded the same three factors of Warmth, Psychological Control, and Behavioral Control. The factor structure has been supported through PARENTING BEHAVIORS AND CHILDREN COPING WITH CANCER 15 various modifications of the original instrument and in culturally diverse studies (Renson, Schaefer, & Levy, 1968; Schludermann & Schludermann, 1971, 1983). Procedure The Institutional Review Boards at two hospitals in the Southern and Midwestern United States approved the study protocol. Children were identified from cancer registries at two pediatric oncology centers, and a member of the research team approached parents in the clinic or hospital to introduce the study. Parents who were willing to participate completed an informed consent form, and children (ages 10-17 years) completed an assent form. In the case that only one parent was present, consent forms were sent home for the other parent. For both the T1 (near diagnosis) and T2 (approximately 12 months after diagnosis) assessments, questionnaire packets were either given to participants at the hospital or mailed to their home. For T1, families in this sample were enrolled between several weeks and months after the child’s first diagnosis or relapse (M = 39.94 days, SD = 23.58). Parents and children returned the T1 questionnaires between several weeks and months after the child’s first diagnosis or relapse (M = 64.75 days, SD = 35.35). For T2, follow-up questionnaire packets were given to families once they were 12-months past first diagnosis or relapse. Parents and children returned the T2 questionnaires approximately one year after first diagnosis or relapse (M = 407.96 days, SD = 60.71). Families received compensation at each assessment when at least one parent or child completed the measures. Results Data Analytic Strategy Using SPSS (21st ed.), we ran three main sets of analyses. First, in our preliminary analyses section, we ran descriptive analyses to examine the mean levels of reported parenting PARENTING BEHAVIORS AND CHILDREN COPING WITH CANCER 16 behaviors and children’s coping among children and mother reports. We also ran correlations to examine child age as a possible covariate among parenting behaviors and children’s coping. Next, to test Hypothesis 1, we ran bivariate Pearson correlation analyses to examine crosssectional and prospective associations between parenting behaviors and children’s coping. Finally, to test Hypotheses 2 and 3, we used linear multiple regression analyses to examine the unique cross-sectional and prospective associations of parenting behaviors as predictors of children’s coping. Child age was also entered into each regression equation to control for its possible unique effects as a predictor of children’s coping. Preliminary Analyses Descriptive Statistics. Means and standard deviations of parenting behaviors and children’s coping are shown in Table 1 for T1 and T2 using mother and child reports. Parenting behaviors are reported as means of a 3-point rating scale. At both time points, mothers and children consistently rated parental warmth the highest, suggesting that mothers in the current sample use high levels of parental warmth (M = 2.62 to 2.85). Behavioral control was consistently rated the second highest (M = 1.49 to 1.70), followed by psychological control as the lowest (M = 1.35 to 1.55). Paired samples t-tests showed that parental warmth was significantly higher than both behavioral control and psychological control. Furthermore, psychological control was significantly higher than behavioral control. These differences hold true for both mother self-reports and children’s reports about their mothers. Children’s coping scores are presented as ratio scores. At both time points, mothers and children consistently reported that children use secondary control coping the most often (M = .28 to .29). Primary control coping was the next coping style most often used by children (M = .18 to .19), while disengagement was consistently reported as the least used coping style (M = .14 to .15). PARENTING BEHAVIORS AND CHILDREN COPING WITH CANCER 17 Possible Covariate: Child age. Bivariate correlations were used to assess possible associations between the mother and child reports of all our parenting behavior variables and children’s coping variables with child age. Some significant differences emerged. Child age was negatively correlated with maternal warmth consistently across the T1 mother reports (r = -.14; p < .05), T2 mother reports (r = -.24; p < .01), and T2 child reports (r = -.29; p < .01). Child age was also correlated negatively with primary control coping according to the T2 mother reports (r = -.21; p < .01), positively with secondary control coping according to the T1 mother reports (r = .17; p < .01), and positively with disengagement coping according to the T2 mother reports (r = .18; p < .01). Correlation Analyses Hypothesis 1: Maternal warmth would be correlated with more primary control coping and secondary control coping and less disengagement coping. Maternal psychological control and behavioral control would be correlated with less primary control coping and secondary control coping and more disengagement coping. These results would apply cross-sectionally at T1 and T2, as well as prospectively from T1 to T2. Cross-sectional correlations between child and mother reports of parenting behaviors and children’s coping at T1 are presented in Table 2. Three of the nine correlations within the children’s reports of parenting and coping were significant (p < .05). Five of nine correlations within mothers’ reports of parenting and coping were significant (p < .05). Four of the eighteen cross-informant correlations were significant (p < .05). Correlations ranged from small to medium in magnitude (r = .15 to .30). Significant correlations followed the expected pattern. Mothers’ warmth was associated with children’s increased use of primary control coping and secondary control coping, and decreased use of disengagement coping. Maternal psychological control was associated with children’s decreased PARENTING BEHAVIORS AND CHILDREN COPING WITH CANCER 18 use of primary control coping and secondary control coping, and increased use of disengagement coping. Maternal behavioral control was associated with children’s decreased use of secondary control coping and increased use of disengagement coping. At T1, there were no significant correlations between behavioral control and children’s primary control coping. Cross-sectional correlations between child and mother reports of parenting behaviors and children’s coping at T2 are presented in Table 3. At T2, seven of the nine correlations within the children’s reports of parenting and coping were significant, four of nine correlations within mothers’ reports of parenting and coping were significant, and eight of the eighteen crossinformant correlations were significant (all p < .05). Correlations ranged from small to medium in magnitude (r = .22 to .47). As with T1, the significant correlations at T2 followed the expected patterns. Maternal warmth was positively associated with children’s primary control coping and secondary control coping, and was negatively associated with disengagement coping. Maternal psychological control was negatively associated with children’s primary control coping and secondary control coping, and was positively associated with disengagement coping. Maternal behavioral control was negatively associated with children’s primary control coping. At T2, behavioral control was not significantly associated with secondary control coping or disengagement coping. Prospective correlations between child and mother reports of parenting behaviors at T1 and children’s coping at T2 are presented in Table 4. Significant correlations followed the expected pattern and ranged from small to medium in magnitude (r = .18 to .32). Within the children’s reports, only one of nine correlations was significant over time (p < .05). Maternal warmth at T1 was associated with decreased use of children’s disengagement coping at T2. Within the mother’s report, three of nine correlations were significant (p < .05). Maternal PARENTING BEHAVIORS AND CHILDREN COPING WITH CANCER 19 warmth at T1 was associated with increased use of children’s primary control coping at T2 and decreased use of disengagement coping at T2. Maternal psychological control at T1 was associated with decreased use of children’s secondary control coping at T2. Finally, only two of eighteen cross-informant correlations were significant over time (p < .05). Maternal warmth at T1 was positively associated with children’s primary control coping at T2, and maternal psychological control at T1 was negatively associated with primary control coping at T2. Maternal behavioral control at T1 was not significant with children’s coping at T2 among withinor cross-informant reports. In summary, Hypothesis 1 was partially supported. Several significant correlations emerged in the expected pattern between maternal warmth and psychological control with children’s coping, cross-sectionally and prospectively. Behavioral control, however, had very few significant associations with children’s coping cross-sectionally, and no significant associations over time. Multiple Linear Regression Analyses Hypothesis 2: Maternal warmth, psychological control, and behavioral control would have unique effects as predictors of children’s coping cross-sectionally at both T1 and at T2. Linear multiple regression analyses were conducted to predict children’s coping from parenting behaviors using mothers’ reports for both measures. Table 5 shows three regression equations predicting the three types of children’s coping at T1 using maternal warmth, psychological control, and behavioral control at T1 as predictors. Maternal warmth at T1 was a significant predictor of children’s primary control coping ( = .14, p < .05), secondary control coping ( = .18, p < .01), and disengagement coping ( = -.14, p < .05) at T1. Maternal psychological control at T1 was a significant predictor of children’s secondary control coping ( = -.20, p < PARENTING BEHAVIORS AND CHILDREN COPING WITH CANCER 20 .01) and disengagement coping ( = .21, p < .01) at T1. Maternal behavioral control did not have any unique effects as a predictor of children’s coping at T1. In each regression equation, R2 values ranged from .037 to .085. Table 6 shows three regression equations predicting the three types of children’s coping at T2 using maternal warmth, psychological control, and behavioral control at T2 as predictors. Maternal warmth at T2 was a significant predictor of children’s primary control coping ( = .18, p < .05) and disengagement coping ( = -.20, p < .05) at T2. Psychological control at T2 was a significant predictor of children’s primary control coping ( = -.31, p < .01) and secondary control coping ( = -.19, p < .05) at T2. Behavioral control had no unique effects as a predictor of children’s coping at T2. In each regression equation, R2 values ranged from .063 to .155. In summary, Hypothesis 2 was partially supported. At T1 and T2, maternal warmth and psychological control had unique effects as predictors of children’s coping. Behavioral control had no unique effects as a predictor of children’s coping at either T1 or T2. Hypothesis 3: Maternal warmth, psychological control, and behavioral control would have unique effects as predictors of children’s coping prospectively from T1 to T2. Linear multiple regression analyses were conducted to predict children’s coping at T2 from parenting behaviors at T1, accounting for children’s coping at T1. Mothers’ reports were used for both measures. Table 7 shows three regression equations predicting the three types of children’s coping at T2 using maternal warmth, psychological control, behavioral control, and children’s coping at T1 as predictors. As expected, children’s coping from T1 was a strong predictor of children’s coping at T2 for primary control coping ( = .48, p < .01), secondary control coping ( = .56, p < .01), and disengagement coping ( = .48, p < .01). Maternal warmth at T1 was a PARENTING BEHAVIORS AND CHILDREN COPING WITH CANCER 21 significant predictor of primary control coping ( = .19, p < .01) and disengagement coping ( = -.14, p < .05) at T2. In each regression equation, R2 values ranged from .314 to .373. In summary, Hypothesis 3 was partially supported. After accounting for children’s coping at T1, warmth was the only parenting behavior at T1 that had a unique effect on children’s coping at T2. Maternal psychological control and behavioral control did not have unique effects as predictors of children’s coping over time. Exploratory Analysis: Child age. Child age was entered into each regression equation to control for its possible unique associations with children’s coping. Child age was a significant predictor of children’s coping in five of the nine regression equations included in the analyses (see Tables 5-7). Cross-sectionally near diagnosis (T1), child age predicted more secondary control coping and less disengagement coping (Table 5). Cross-sectionally 12 months after diagnosis (T2), child age predicted less primary control coping (Table 6). Additionally, in our prospective regression equation predicting children’s coping at T2, child age predicted less primary control coping and more disengagement coping (Table 7). Discussion Childhood cancer patients face numerous sources of stress that may lead to increased levels of emotional problems including anxiety and depression (e.g., Kazak et al., 2004; Sawyer et al., 1997; Schultz et al., 2007). The way a child copes with the stress of having cancer has a significant impact on such emotional problems. When children use more primary control coping and secondary control coping, they tend to have lower levels of anxiety and depression. When they use more disengagement coping, they tend to have higher levels of anxiety and depression (Compas et al., in press). Therefore, healthy and adaptive coping is an important concern in the context of childhood cancer. Past research suggests that parenting behaviors influence children’s PARENTING BEHAVIORS AND CHILDREN COPING WITH CANCER 22 coping, but no studies to date have examined the relationship with childhood cancer patients coping (Gaylord-et al., 2010; McIntyre and Dusek, 1994; Watson et al., in press). The present study addresses this gap in existing literature by analyzing cross-sectional and prospective relations between parenting and children’s coping in a sample of childhood cancer patients. We found partial support for our first hypothesis when we tested cross-sectional and prospective correlations between parenting behaviors and children’s coping with cancer. Specifically, warmth and psychological control emerged as parenting behaviors that are significantly associated with children’s coping. As predicted, our findings suggest that higher levels of maternal warmth are associated with more positive styles of children’s coping. In crosssectional analyses at both T1 and T2, maternal warmth was significantly associated with more primary control coping, more secondary control coping, and less disengagement coping among children. Prospectively, maternal warmth near diagnosis was correlated with more primary control coping and less disengagement coping among children 12 months later. Our correlation analyses also suggest that higher levels of maternal psychological control, as predicted, are associated with more negative styles of children’s coping. Cross-sectionally at T1 and T2, maternal psychological control was significantly associated with less primary control coping, less secondary control coping, and more disengagement coping among children. Prospectively, psychological control near diagnosis was associated with less primary control coping and less secondary control coping among children 12 months later. Overall, correlation analyses indicated fairly consistent associations between maternal warmth with positive children’s coping styles, and maternal psychological control with negative children’s coping styles. Correlations between maternal behavioral control and children’s coping PARENTING BEHAVIORS AND CHILDREN COPING WITH CANCER 23 were less consistent cross-sectionally, and were nonexistent prospectively, indicating a weak association that was not expected. We address this issue further below. Regression analyses were used to examine more comprehensive associations between parenting behavior and children’s coping, by assessing the unique effects of parenting behaviors as predictors of children’s coping. Maternal warmth emerged as a very consistent predictor of children’s positive coping styles. Maternal warmth consistently predicted more primary control coping and less disengagement coping in cross-sectional regressions and in prospective regressions after controlling for initial levels of each type of coping. Warmth also predicted more secondary control in cross-sectional analyses near diagnosis. Maternal psychological control was a fairly consistent predictor of children’s negative coping styles, but only from a cross-sectional standpoint. Psychological control predicted less secondary control coping at T1 and at T2. Psychological control also predicted more disengagement coping at T1 and less primary control coping at T2. Finally, maternal behavioral control had no significant effects as a predictor of children’s coping cross-sectionally or over time. Overall, the pattern of findings from this study is consistent with past research on parental warmth. Maternal warmth was a consistent predictor of children’s coping, particularly of increases in children’s use of primary control coping and decreases in use of disengagement coping. It was also the only parenting behavior that predicted children’s coping over time from diagnosis to 12-months. It is possible that parents who exhibit high levels of warmth towards their children provide a sense of safety and support for the child (Kliewer et al., 1996; McIntyre & Dusek, 1994; Watson et al., in press). Researchers have found that children in this environment tend to use more engagement coping strategies, such as seeking social support and problem solving. They tend to use disengagement strategies, such as wishful thinking, less often PARENTING BEHAVIORS AND CHILDREN COPING WITH CANCER 24 (Gaylord-Harden et al., 2010; Markstrom, et al., 2000). The associations between maternal warmth and children’s primary control coping and disengagement coping that emerged in the present study match these findings from past research. It is important to note that maternal warmth only had a significant effect as a predictor of children’s secondary control coping cross-sectionally at T1. We expected a longitudinal association to emerge as well. Watson et al. (in press) found that parental warmth and responsiveness predicted increases in children’s use of secondary control coping in the context of a longitudinal family intervention for mothers with a history of depression. Possible mechanisms by which parental warmth may increase secondary control coping in children are parental modeling of adaptive coping, direct coping coaching, or engaging children in positive distraction activities (Watson et al., in press). It is possible that this relationship only emerged in the present study at T1 because the time of diagnosis is the most emotionally intense point in time. Furthermore, the stressors facing children near the time of their cancer diagnosis are mostly beyond their control and are especially well suited to the use of secondary control coping. Maternal warmth may serve to facilitate this type of coping at this crucial time. At 12 months after the diagnosis, parents of children with cancer may engage less often in behaviors that facilitate secondary control coping in their children, and instead use behaviors that tend to facilitate primary control coping when children have more opportunity to exert control over the stressors in their lives as they move into long-term survivorship. Overall, the pattern of findings on parental psychological control from this study is also fairly consistent with past research. Parental psychological control emerged as a predictor of children’s coping cross-sectionally, but not prospectively. Past research suggests that parental psychological control is associated with parenting characteristics such as intrusiveness and poor PARENTING BEHAVIORS AND CHILDREN COPING WITH CANCER 25 communication, which often lead to more avoidant coping strategies among children (Jackson et al., 1998; Langrock et al., 2002). Our results on psychological control are consistent with these past findings. Maternal psychological control was associated with higher levels of disengagement coping and lower levels of primary and secondary control coping. In fact, parental psychological control had the most consistent impact on secondary control coping. It is possible that psychological control, defined as intrusion and manipulation of the child’s psychological world, leads to a greater difficulty with secondary control coping strategies (e.g., acceptance, cognitive restructuring, positive thinking, distraction). These types of coping strategies require cognitive and psychological command over one’s thoughts and emotions. High levels of parental psychological control may thwart these types of abilities. Because maternal psychological control did not predict children’s coping prospectively from T1 to T2, our results suggest that the impact of psychological control on children’s coping is not as permanent as maternal warmth was found to be. Finally, the overall pattern of findings for maternal behavioral control did not emerge as we had predicted. Regression analyses showed no significant effects of behavioral control as a predictor of children’s coping cross-sectionally or prospectively. This lack of support for our hypothesis, however, does reflect the mixed findings past research has shown. Behavioral control has a complex relationship with children’s coping and adjustment, and findings in the past have been less consistent (McIntyre and Dusek, 1994; Power and Manire, 1992). It is possible that our lack of significant associations is due to an issue with the measure used to evaluate behavioral control. It is also possible that in the specific context of children with cancer, maternal behavioral control (i.e., regulation and structure of child’s behavior) is an expected parental behavior. A highly protective environment and a very structured and scheduled routine may be PARENTING BEHAVIORS AND CHILDREN COPING WITH CANCER 26 normal for a child newly diagnosed with cancer. Thus, children may accept behavioral control as a normal part of life, leading it to have little impact on their ability to cope. Overall, the significant findings of the present study supported and extended relevant findings from past research. This study was the first to evaluate the association between parenting behaviors and children’s coping in the context of childhood cancer patients, which may explain why some of our findings diverge from past research. The most important result that emerged was the relationship between maternal warmth and children’s coping. The consistent and long-term association between these two factors suggests that parental warmth is a strong and positive parenting behavior that has important implications for children coping with cancer. There were strengths as well as limitations to the present study. One strength was the longitudinal design of the study, which allowed for prospective analyses between parenting behaviors and children’s coping. The novel context of childhood cancer patients and their mothers is another strength that contributes to the expanding body of knowledge on children’s adjustment to cancer. Additional strengths include the relatively large sample size for a longitudinal study on children with cancer and a wide age range. The primary limitation on this study is attrition. Between the initial assessment near diagnosis and the 12-month follow up assessment, we lost about 30% of the mothers and children included in this sample. Some of the attrition in our sample is due to families choosing to drop out of the study, and unfortunately, part of the attrition is due to the death of a patient. In the future, emphasis should be placed on reducing the number of families who leave the study based on choice. Another limitation is the ethnic and racial homogeneity. Future studies will benefit from a more diverse sample to generalize findings to a wider population of children. PARENTING BEHAVIORS AND CHILDREN COPING WITH CANCER 27 The findings from the present study have important implications. First, they replicated past findings on the association between parenting behaviors and children’s coping. Second, they extended those findings to a new context, children with cancer and their mothers. This is key because this population of children is an important target group for interventions that reduce emotional distress. The findings from the present study can be used as an impetus to conduct parenting interventions to enhance children’s coping. We found that maternal warmth consistently predicts more primary control coping and less disengagement coping, and those effects extend through one year after diagnosis. Maternal psychological control also has crosssectional effects on children’s coping. These results suggest that improving parenting can be used as an avenue to enhance children’s coping. An important next step for future research is to examine the relationship between parenting behaviors and children’s coping with children’s symptoms of anxiety and depression. This extended analysis will give a greater picture of how a potential intervention will work for children with cancer and their families. One noteworthy finding from the present study that has important implications for future interventions is the set of associations between child age, parenting, and children’s coping. In the preliminary analyses of this study, we found that child age was negatively correlated with maternal warmth. This association was significant in mother reports at both T1 and T2 as well as child reports at T1. As children get older, mothers tend to exhibit less parental warmth. This is concerning in light of the positive impact that parental warmth has on children’s coping. Furthermore, when child age was included in the regression analyses, we found that 12 months after diagnosis, child age predicted less primary control coping and more disengagement coping (Tables 6 and 7). As children get older, they are using more negative styles of coping. Age is therefore an important factor to consider in the consideration of future interventions. Emphasis PARENTING BEHAVIORS AND CHILDREN COPING WITH CANCER 28 should be placed on teaching mothers’ to continue using warmth even as their child ages. Such an intervention has the potential to offset the reduction in children’s primary control coping and the increase in disengagement coping, and ultimately enhance coping. In conclusion, the findings from this study have the potential to guide future intervention studies to enhance adjustment outcomes for children with cancer by using parenting as an avenue to improve children’s coping. Past research has shown that parenting has significant associations with children’s coping. Further research is needed to confirm this association in the context of childhood cancer patients and to explore the function of age further, so that preventative interventions can be developed to reduce the emotional distress these children face during their diagnosis and recovery. PARENTING BEHAVIORS AND CHILDREN COPING WITH CANCER 29 References Achenbach, T.M., & Rescorla, L.A. (2001). Manual for the ASEBA school-age forms and profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, and Families. Aldridge, A. A., & Roesch, S. C. (2007). Coping and adjustment in children with cancer: A meta-analytic study. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 30(2), 115-129. American Cancer Society. Cancer Facts & Figures 2013. Atlanta: American Cancer Society; 2013. Bean, R. A., Barber, B. K., & Crane, D. R. (2006). Parental support, behavioral control, and psychological control among African American youth. Journal of Family Issues, 27(10), 1335-1355. Benson, M.A., Compas, B.E., Layne, C.M., Vandergrift, N., Pasalic, H., Katalinski, R., & Pynoos, R.S. (2011). Measurement of post-war coping and stress responses: A study of Bosnian adolescents. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 32, 323-335. Compas, B. E., Connor-Smith, J. K., Saltzman, H., Thomsen, A. H., & Wadsworth, M. E. (2001). Coping with stress during childhood and adolescence: Problems, progress, and potential in theory and research. Psychological Bulletin, 127(1), 87-127. Compas, B.E., Boyer, M.C., Stanger, C., Colletti, R.B., Thomsen, A.H., Dufton, L.M., & Cole, D.A. (2006). Latent variable analysis of coping, anxiety/depression, and somatic symptoms in adolescents with chronic pain. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 1132-1142. Compas, B. E., Desjardins, L., Vannatta, K., Young-Saleme, T., Rodriguez, E. M., Dunn, M., Bemis, H., Snyder, S., & Gerhardt, C. A. (in press). Children and adolescents coping with PARENTING BEHAVIORS AND CHILDREN COPING WITH CANCER 30 cancer: Self- and parent-reports of coping and anxiety/depression. Health Psychology. Connor-Smith, J. K., Compas, B. E., Wadsworth, M. E., Thomsen, A. H., & Saltzman, H. (2000). Responses to stress in adolescence: Measurement of coping and involuntary stress responses. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68(6), 976-992. Connor-Smith, J.K., & Calvete, E. (2004). Cross-cultural equivalence of coping and involuntary responses to stress in Spain and the United States. Anxiety, Stress & Coping, 17, 163-185. Engvall, G., Mattson, E., von Essen, L., & Hedström, M. (2011). Findings on how adolescents cope with cancer—a matter of methodology? Psycho-Oncology, 20, 1053-1060. Frank, N. C., Blount, R. L., & Brown, R. T. (1997). Attributions, coping, and adjustment in children with cancer. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 22(4), 563-576. Gaylord-Harden, N. K., Campbell, C. L., & Kesselring, C. M. (2010). Maternal parenting behaviors and coping in African American children: The influence of gender and stress. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 19, 579-587. Hamill, S. K. (2003). Resilience and self-efficacy: The importance of efficacy beliefs and coping mechanisms in resilient adolescents. Colgate University Journal of the Sciences, 115146. Hardy, D. F., Power, T. G., & Jaedicke, S. (1993). Examining the relation of parenting to children’s coping with everyday stress. Child Development, 64(6), 1829-1841. Hildenbrand, A. K., Clawson, K. J., Alderfer, M. A., & Marsac, M. L. (2011). Coping with pediatric cancer: Strategies employed by children and their parents to manage cancerrelated stressors during treatment. Journal of Pediatric Oncology Nursing, 28(6), 344354. Jackson, S., Bijstra, J., Oostra, L., & Bosma, H. (1998). Adolescents’ perceptions of PARENTING BEHAVIORS AND CHILDREN COPING WITH CANCER 31 communication with parents relative to specific aspects of relationships with parents and personal development. Journal of Adolescence, 21, 305-322. Kazak, A. E., Alderfer, M., Rourke, M. T., Simms, S., Streisand, R., & Grossman, J. R. (2004). Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and posttraumatic stress symptoms (PTSS) in families of adolescent childhood cancer survivors. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 29(3), 211-219. Kliewer, W., Fearnow, M. D., & Miller, P. A. (1996). Coping socialization in middle childhood: Tests of maternal and paternal influences. Child Development, 67(5), 2339-2357. Langrock, A. M., Compas, B. E., Keller, G., Merchant, M. J., & Copeland, M. E. (2002). Coping with the stress of parental depression: Parents’ reports of children’s coping, emotional, and behavioral problems. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 31(3), 312324. Li, H. C. W., Chung, O. K. H., Ho, K. Y. E., Chiu, S. Y., & Lopez, V. (2011). Coping strategies used by children hospitalized with cancer: An exploratory study. Psycho-Oncology, 20, 969-976. Luyckx, K., Goossens, E., Missotten, L., & Moons P. (2011). Adolescents with congenital heart disease: The importance of perceived parenting for psychosocial and health outcomes. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 32, 651-659. Markstrom, C. A., Marshall, S. K., & Tryon, R. J. (2000). Resiliency, social support, and coping in rural low-income Appalachian adolescents from two racial groups. Journal of Adolescence, 23, 693-703. Maurice-Stam, H., Oort F. J., Last B. F., Brons, P. P. T., Caron, H. N., & Grootenhuis, M. A. (2009). School-aged children after the end of successful treatment of non-central nervous PARENTING BEHAVIORS AND CHILDREN COPING WITH CANCER 32 system cancer: Longitudinal assessment of health-related quality of life, anxiety, and coping. European Journal of Cancer Care, 18, 401-410. McIntyre, J. G., & Dusek, J. B. (1994). Perceived parental rearing practices and styles of coping. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 24(4), 499-509. McKernon, W. L., Holmbeck, G. N., Colder, C. R., Hommeyer, J. S., Shapera, W., & Westhoven, V. (2001). Longitudinal study of observed and perceived family influences on problem-focused coping behaviors of preadolescents with spina bifida. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 26(1), 41-54. Miller, K.S., Vannatta, K., Compas, B.E., Vasey, M., McGoron, K.D., Salley, C.G., Gerhardt, C.A. (2009). The role of coping and temperament in the adjustment of children with cancer. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 34, 1135-1145. Morris, A. S., Silk, J. S., Steinberg, L., Myers, S. S., & Robinson, L. R. (2007). The role of the family context in the development of emotion regulation. Social Development, 16(2), 361-388. Power, T. G., & Manire, S. H. (1992). Child rearing and internalization: A developmental perspective. In J. Janssens & J. geris (Eds.). Child rearing: Influence on prosocial and moral development (pp.101-123). Amsterdam: Swets & Zeitlinger B. V. Renson, G. J., Schaefer, E. S., & Levy, B. I. (1968). Cross-national validity of a spherical conceptual model for parent behavior. Child Development, 39, 1229-1235. Rodriguez, E. M., Dunn, M. J., Zuckerman, T., Vannatta, K., Gerhardt, C. A., & Compas, B. E. (2012). Cancer-related sources of stress for children with cancer and their parents. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 37(2), 185-197. Rollins, J. A. (2009). The influence of two hospitals’ designs and policies on social interaction PARENTING BEHAVIORS AND CHILDREN COPING WITH CANCER 33 and privacy as coping factors for children with cancer and their families. Journal of Pediatric Oncology Nursing, 26(6), 340-353. Rourke, M.T., Stuber, M. L., Hobbie, W. L., & Kazak, A. E. (1999). Posttraumatic stress disorder: Understanding the psychosocial impact of surviving childhood cancer into young adulthood. Journal of Pediatric Oncology Nursing, 16(3), 126-135. Silk, J. S., Steinberg, L., & Morris, A. S. (2003). Adolescents’ emotion regulation in daily life: Links to depressive symptoms and problem behavior. Child Development, 74(6), 18691880. Sawyer, M., Antoniou, G., Toogood, I., & Rice, M. (1997). Childhood cancer: A two-year prospective study of the psychological adjustment of children and parents. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36(12), 1736-1743. Schaefer, E. S. (1965). Children’s reports of parental behavior: An inventory. Child Development, 36(2), 413-424. Schludermann, S., & Schludermann, E. (1971). Adolescent perception of parent behavior (CRPBI) in Hutterite communal society. Journal of Psychology, 79, 29-39. Schludermann, S. M., & Schludermann, E. H. (1983). Sociocultural change and adolescents’ perceptions of parent behavior. Developmental Psychology, 19, 674-685. Schludermann, S., & Schludermann, E. (1988). Children’s report of parent behavior inventory. Winnipeg, Man. Canada: University of Manitoba. Schultz, K. A., Ness, K. K., Whitton, J., Recklitis, C., Zebrack, B., Robison, L. L., Zeltzer, L., & Mertens, A. C. (2007). Behavioral and social outcomes in adolescent survivors of childhood cancer: A report from the childhood cancer survivor study. Journal of Clinical Oncology, 25(4), 3649-3656. PARENTING BEHAVIORS AND CHILDREN COPING WITH CANCER 34 Smorti, M. (2012). Adolescents’ struggle against bone cancer: An explorative study on optimistic expectations of the future, resiliency, and coping strategies. European Journal of Cancer Care, 21, 251-258. Spirito, A., Stark, L. J., & Williams, C. (1988). Development of a brief coping checklist for use with pediatric populations. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 13(4), 555-574. Steinberg, L. (1990). Autonomy, conflict, and harmony in the family relationship. In Feldman, S.S. and Elliot, G.R. (Eds.). At the threshold: The developing adolescent (pp.255-276). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Tyc, V. L., Mulhern, R. K., Jayawardene, D., & Fairclough, D. (1995). Chemotherapy-induced nausea and emesis in pediatric cancer patients: An analysis of coping strategies. Journal of Pain and Symptom Management, 10(5), 338-347. Wadsworth, M. E., Rieckman, T., Benson, M. A., & Compas, B. E. (2004). Coping and responses to stress in Navajo adolescents: Psychometric properties of the responses to stress questionnaire. Journal of Community Psychology, 32(4), 391-411. Wadsworth, M. E., Raviv, T., Compas, B. E., & Connor-Smith, J. K. (2005). Parent and adolescent responses to poverty-related stress: Tests of mediated and moderated coping models. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 14(2), 283-298. Watson, K. H., Dunbar, J. P., Reising, M. M., Hudson, K., McKee, L., Forehand, R., & Compas, B. E. (in press). Observed parental responsiveness/warmth and children’s coping: Crosssectional and prospective relations in a family depression preventive intervention. Journal of Family Psychology. Yao, S., Xiao, J., Zhu, K., Zhang, C., Auerbach, R. P., McWhinnie, C. M., Abela, J. R., & Wang, C. (2010). Coping and involuntary responses to stress in Chinese university students: PARENTING BEHAVIORS AND CHILDREN COPING WITH CANCER 35 Psychometric properties of the responses to stress questionnaire. Journal of Personality Assessment, 92(4), 356-361. PARENTING BEHAVIORS AND CHILDREN COPING WITH CANCER 36 Table 1. Means and standard deviations for mothers’ parenting behaviors and children’s coping. Child Reports (T1; n=114) Parenting Behaviors (T1) Warmth Psychological Control Behavioral Control Children’s Coping (T1) Primary Control Coping Secondary Control Coping Disengagement Coping Mother Reports (T1; n=240) M SD M SD 2.70 1.55 1.70 .34 .41 .26 2.85 1.36 1.54 .22 .31 .26 .18 .29 .15 .04 .06 .03 .19 .28 .14 .03 .06 .03 Child Reports (T2; n=81) Mother Reports (T2; n=163) M SD M SD Parenting Behaviors (T2) Warmth 2.62 .39 2.83 .27 Psychological Control 1.47 .37 1.35 .28 Behavioral Control 1.68 .28 1.49 .23 Children’s Coping (T2) Primary Control Coping .18 .04 .19 .03 Secondary Control Coping .29 .06 .28 .06 Disengagement Coping .15 .03 .15 .03 Note. T1: near diagnosis; T2: 12 months after diagnosis. Scores for parenting behavior are presented as means of a 3-point scale on the CRPBI and PBI. Scores for children’s coping are presented as proportion scores on the RSQ. PARENTING BEHAVIORS AND CHILDREN COPING WITH CANCER 37 Table 2. Correlations between child and mother reports of parenting behaviors at T1 with children’s coping at T1. Child Self Report Child Report Mother SelfReport Warmth T1 Psychological Control T1 Behavioral Control T1 Warmth T1 Mother Report Primary Control Coping T1 Secondary Control Coping T1 Disengage -ment Coping T1 Primary Control Coping T1 Secondary Control Coping T1 Disengage -ment Coping T1 .30** .14 -.03 .07 .06 -.07 -.24** -.22* .13 -.03 -.26** .14 -.09 -.15 .06 -.15 -.26** .20* .23* .09 .10 .15* .18** -.15* -.15* .17** .00 .02 Psychological -.17 -.11 .01 -.09 Control T1 Behavioral -.13 -.09 .03 .04 Control T1 Note. Sample sizes: n = 108 children; n = 240 mothers. T1: near diagnosis. Note. * p<.05; ** p<.01. PARENTING BEHAVIORS AND CHILDREN COPING WITH CANCER 38 Table 3. Correlations between child and mother reports of parenting behaviors at T2 with children’s coping at T2. Child Self Report Child Report Mother SelfReport Warmth T2 Psychological Control T2 Behavioral Control T2 Warmth T2 Mother Report Primary Control Coping T2 Secondary Control Coping T2 Disengage -ment Coping T2 Primary Control Coping T2 Secondary Control Coping T2 Disengage -ment Coping T2 .36** .23* -.25* .09 .24* .03 -.43** -.47** .24* -.45** -.25* .34** -.24* -.15 .10 -.31** -.04 .19 .16 .14 -.24* .25** .14 -.23** Psychological -.46** -.11 .30** -.26** -.22** Control T2 Behavioral -.17 -.01 .08 -.01 -.12 Control T2 Note. Sample sizes: n = 74 children; n = 163 mothers. T2: 12 months after diagnosis. Note. * p<.05; ** p<.01. .13 .05 PARENTING BEHAVIORS AND CHILDREN COPING WITH CANCER 39 Table 4. Correlations between child and mother reports of parenting behaviors at T1 with children’s coping at T2. Child Self Report Child Report Mother SelfReport Warmth T1 Psychological Control T1 Behavioral Control T1 Warmth T1 Mother Report Primary Control Coping T2 Secondary Control Coping T2 Disengage -ment Coping T2 Primary Control Coping T2 Secondary Control Coping T2 Disengage -ment Coping T2 .20 .15 -.26* .13 .11 .00 .00 -.19 .04 .00 -.17 .12 -.18 -.10 .15 -.05 -.09 .02 .23* -.01 -.17 .32** .11 -.26** Psychological -.25* -.07 .22 -.15 -.18* .11 Control T1 Behavioral -.10 -.02 .10 .03 .01 -.05 Control T1 Note. Sample sizes: n = 78 children; n = 163 mothers. T1: near diagnosis; T2: 12 months after diagnosis. Note. * p<.05; ** p<.01. PARENTING BEHAVIORS AND CHILDREN COPING WITH CANCER 40 Table 5. Linear multiple regression analyses predicting children’s coping at T1 from parenting behaviors at T1 using mother reports. Primary Control Coping (T1) Variable β t R2 F Child Age .02 .23 .037 2.28 Warmth T1 .14 2.11* Psychological Control T1 -.14 -1.84 Behavioral Control T1 .12 1.51 Secondary Control Coping (T1) Variable β t R2 F Child Age .16 2.60* .085 5.50 Warmth T1 .18 2.86** Psychological Control T1 -.20 -2.68** Behavioral Control T1 .13 1.72 Disengagement Coping (T1) Variable β t R2 F Child Age -.14 -2.22* .075 4.80 Warmth T1 -.16 -2.44* Psychological Control T1 .21 2.83** Behavioral Control T1 -.11 -1.49 Note. * p<.05; ** p<.01. PARENTING BEHAVIORS AND CHILDREN COPING WITH CANCER 41 Table 6. Linear multiple regression analyses predicting children’s coping at T2 from parenting behaviors at T2 using mother reports. Primary Control Coping (T2) Variable β t R2 F Child Age -.16 -2.07* .155 7.28 Warmth T2 .18 2.33* Psychological Control T2 -.31 -3.61** Behavioral Control T2 .12 1.39 Secondary Control Coping (T2) Variable β t R2 F Child Age .02 .22 .063 2.70 Warmth T2 .14 1.69 Psychological Control T2 -.19 -2.06* Behavioral Control T2 -.04 -.39 Disengagement Coping (T2) Variable β t R2 F Child Age .13 1.66 .083 3.64 Warmth T2 -.20 -2.52* Psychological Control T2 .10 1.13 Behavioral Control T2 .04 .35 Note. * p<.05; ** p<.01. PARENTING BEHAVIORS AND CHILDREN COPING WITH CANCER 42 Table 7. Linear multiple regression analyses predicting children’s coping at T2 from parenting behaviors at T1 using mother reports, accounting for children’s coping at T1. Primary Control Coping (T2) Variable β t R2 F Child Age -.15 -2.33* .373 18.83 PC-Coping T1 .48 7.24** Warmth T1 .19 2.93** Psychological Control T1 -.10 -1.29 Behavioral Control T1 .03 .37 Secondary Control Coping (T2) Variable β t R2 F Child Age -.08 -1.23 .332 15.73 SC-Coping T1 .56 8.10** Warmth T1 -.04 -.52 Psychological Control T1 -.12 -1.47 Behavioral Control T1 .07 .83 Disengagement Coping (T2) Variable β t R2 F Child Age .20 2.95** .314 14.53 D-Coping T1 .48 6.93** Warmth T1 -.14 -2.04* Psychological Control T1 .05 .57 Behavioral Control T1 -.05 -.61 Note. * p<.05; ** p<.01.