The Auk 112(4):860-869, 1995 THE ROLE OF PARENTS REDUCTION IN SIBILICIDAL OF TWO BOOBY BROOD SPECIES DAVID J. ANDERSON Departmentof Biology,LeidyLaboratory, Universityof Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania 19104,USA;and Departmentof Biology,WakeForestUniversity,Winston-Salem, North Carolina27109USA• ABSTRACr.--Parental regulation of the probability and timing of avian brood reduction is expectedto existand evolve becausenatural selectionshould favor parentsthat matchbrood size to food availabilitymostefficiently.Sincefood availabilityvariesamongspecies,interspecificvariation in this regulation is expected.Comparativestudy of specieswith different brood reduction systemsis one way to test the hypothesisthat these mechanismsevolve as adaptations.Previouswork on obligately siblicidal Masked Boobies(Sula dactylatra)and facultativelysiblicidalBlue-footedBoobies (S.nebouxii) hasshownthattheirdifferenthatching asynchroniescannotfully explain the qualitativedifferencein their broodreductionsystems. In this study,I reportinterspecificdifferencesin nestshapethat appearto contributeto early siblicide in Masked Boobies,but that suppressearly siblicide in Blue-footedBoobies.BluefootedBoobynestshapeis closelyregulatedby parents.Differencesin eggsizesof nestmates do not appear to contributeto the differencein socialsystems.Received19 January1995, accepted 2 July 1995. FATAL SIBLING AGGRESSION,known as "siblicide," often reduces the brood size of a number of bird species(Stinson1979,Mock et al. 1990). Lack's(1954)brood-reductionhypothesisis the basisfor the common evolutionary interpreta- 1987). Thus, parentsmay benefit from regulating siblicide. Specifically, they are expectedto have experiencednatural selectionfor regula- tory mechanismsthat rank offspring for sur- strategyof hatching as many nestlingsas can vival within a competitive hierarchy, but suppress siblicide that is not in the parents' best interests.Whether the first or secondobjective is emphasizedduring selectionwill depend on the extent of brood reduction that is optimal for parents,and is expectedto vary acrossspe- be raised in the best conditions, cies. tion of siblicide, and of brood reduction (Rick- lefs 1965) in general. When food supply for nestlings cannot be predicted reliably at the time of laying, selectionis expectedto favor a and then of Past interest has focused on two mechanisms eliminating nestlingsafter hatching, if necessary,to bring demandinto line with supply(see to regulatesibling competition.Variation in the also Temme and Charnov 1987, Forbes and Ydenberg 1992). Siblicide provides parents an optimizing tool with a notable advantage: individual nestlings are best able to gauge their own physiological condition and can use that interval between successivehatchings of nest mates (hatching asynchrony) and in egg size explain much of the variation in competitive ability within a brood (Ricklefs 1965, Parsons 1970, 1975, Hahn 1981, Braun and Hunt 1983, information,in conjunctionwith a competitive Slagsvoidet al. 1984,Hebert and Barclay1986, hierarchy, to maximize efficiencyof brood re- Plogerand Mock 1986,Skagen1987,Amundsen duction (Mock et al. 1987a). Reliance on dominant offspring to conduct siblicide also carries a notable disadvantage. Offspring may commit siblicide under lessstringentconditionsthan is optimal for parents, especially if parent-offspring conflict exists (Trivets 1974, O'Connor 1978, Dickins and Clark and Stockland 1988, Hebert and Barclay 1988, Anderson1989a,Magrath 1992,Joveret al. 1993), with hatching asynchrony being the more important (Stokland and Amundsen 1988, Magrath 1992). In specieswith sibling aggression, available evidence shows that food distribution parallels the competitive hierarchy within the brood (Poole 1979, Braun and Hunt 1983, Fu- Present jioka 1985, Inoue 1985, Mock 1985, Cash and Evans1986,Drummond et al. 1986,Hagen 1986, Ploger and Mock 1986). Parents create acom- address. 860 October 1995] Siblicide inBoobies petitive hierarchyand, by not distributingfood counterto the hierarchy, reinforceit through positive feedback. Only rarely have comparativestudiestested the hypothesisthat differencesbetweenspecies in siblicidal phenomena are explained by differencesin regulatorymechanisms. Ideally,such studies should compare speciesthat differ in outcomeof siblingcompetition,they shouldfocuson interspecificvariation in traitsknown to influence competitive hierarchies, and they should eliminate to the extent possible confoundingeffectsof phylogeneticand ecological dissimilarity. Two studieshave compared the hatching asynchroniesof obligately siblicidal eagles(Edwardsand Collopy 1983)and boobies (Anderson1989a)with thoseof faculativelysiblicidal congeners.Longer hatching asynchronies (presumablyunder at leastpartial control of parents)give greater competitiveadvantageto elder chicks of many siblicidal species (see above);both studiesfound that hatching asynchronies of obligately siblicidal specieswere longerthan thoseof facultativelysiblicidalcongeners.Mock and associates found that the degreeto which food deliveredby parentscanbe monopolized explains variation between two ardeid speciesin the probability of siblicide (Mock 1984, 1985, Mock et al. 1987a, Mock et al. 1987b);however, whether parentsactively manipulatefood monopolizabilityas a regulatory mechanismis unknown. In this study,I presentcomparativedatafrom two siblicidal booby speciesof potential influ- encesof sibling aggression.My goal is to identify mechanismsthat parentsuseto regulatethe outcomeof sibling aggression,and to test the hypothesisthat differencesin regulatorymechanisms contribute to differences in the social systemsof thesespecies. THE STUDY SYSTEM Masked Boobies(Sula dactylatra)and Blue-footed Boobies(S. nebouxii)are colonial, ground-nesting, piscivorousseabirdsthat breed sympatricallythroughout most of the Blue-footedBooby'srange (Nelson 1978). Blue-footedBoobieslay one to four eggsper clutch (usually two) and are faculatively siblicidal; first-hatchedchicks("A-chicks")kill youngersiblings in times of food stress(Drummond et al. 1986, Drummond and Garcia Chevelas 1989). Nonetheless, more than one offspring often (Nelson 1978) or usually fledge(66%in caseof Drummond et al. 1986).Masked Boobieslay one- or two-egg clutches,and are obli- 861 gatelysiblicidal.If both eggshatch,the A-chick ejects the second-hatched chick ("B-chick") from the nest scrape,and the victim dies from exposureor predation. The timing of siblicidealso differs;the average age at death of victim Blue-footedBoobiesis 18 days (Drummond et al. 1986),while that of Masked Booby victims is 1.8 days (Anderson 1989a).Masked Booby parentsare preventedfrom fledging two offspringby unrelenting sibling aggression(Nelson 1978,Anderson 1989a); the insurance value of the second egg againstthe first egg'sfailure to hatch apparently explains why they lay two eggs (Dorward 1962, Anderson1990a).Somedatasuggestthat parent Masked Boobiescouldprovide enoughfood for two surviving nestlings (Anderson 1990b, Anderson and Ricklefs 1992),but A-chicks do not permit parentsto try. Even if the parents could suppresssiblicide behaviorally, both parentsare absentfrom the nest site for up to 8 h daily in experimentally-managedtwo-chickbroods (Anderson 1990b). During these periods a subordinate has no protectionfrom its sibling'sattacks.Parents avoid investing in a doomed offspring and Achicksexpendlesseffortwhen siblicideoccursshortly after hatching of the B-chick. A previousexperimental study showed that the degree of hatching asynchronyinfluenced the probability and timing of siblicidein MaskedBoobies(Anderson 1989a).Broodshatching at intervals of at least 4 daysvirtually alwayslostthe B-chickwithin 20 days (total nestling period ca. 100 days);the averagetime to brood reduction was 1.8 days. Early brood reduction becameincreasinglyunlikely as hatching asynchrony was decreasedfrom three to one days in experimental broods.As expectedunder the "doomedoffspring"scenarioabove,the meanhatchinginterval in Masked Booby broods was 5.4 days (range 3-10 days),abovethe "early reductionthreshold"(Anderson 1989a)of 3 days.Anderson (1989a)also showed that hatching intervals of Blue-footedBoobies(:• = 3.5 days) were significantly shorter than those of Masked Boobies, but that more than 50% of Blue- footed Boobybroodshatched at intervals exceeding the three day early reductionthreshold.A Masked BoobyB-chickhatchesfour daysafter its sibling and can expectto be killed within three daysof hatching. However, a Blue-footedBooby B-chick in a similar situationin the Galfipagos will probablyfledge,if it escapespredators(Anderson 1989a, 1991, Anderson and Hodurn 1993)and bad weather (this paper). The degreeof hatching asynchronyis the primary regulatoryvariableinvestigatedin studiesof aviansibling competition,but it cannot explain the qualitative differencebetween the socialsystemsof these two siblicidal species. A MaskedBoobyA-chick ejectsits sibling by grasping in its beak the sibling'sneck, appendage,or skin and extending its neck to thrust the B-chick across the nest scrape.Then, an A-chick often repeatsthe thrust after moving its own body 2 to 10 cm toward 862 DAVIDJ. ANDERSON the displacedB-chick.A seriesof thrustsoften moves the B-chick from the shade castby the parent. Behavioral data, both observational (Nelson 1978:565, this study) and experimental(Lougheed1995),show that hatchlingBlue-footedBoobyA-chicksdisplaythe same behavior, but at lower frequency and without lethaloutcome.Experimentalcross-fostering of chicks of one speciesinto nests of the other speciesdemonstratedthat both parentsand chicksplay regulatory roles vis-a-visexpressionand outcomeof sibling aggression(Lougheed1995).In the presentstudy,I focuson parentalinfluencesthat facilitatethe B-chick's ejectionin MaskedBoobybroodsand suppressit in Blue-footedBoobybroods. MATERIALS AND METHODS Data presented here were collected from 1984 through 1986 as part of a long-term study of the breeding ecologyand behavior of Masked and Bluefooted boobiesat Punta Cevallos,Isla Espafiolain the GallpagosArchipelago(for detailsof studysite,see Anderson and Ricklefs 1987). This period fell between the E1 Nifio-Southern Oscillation events of 1982-1983 and 1986-1987 (Anderson 1989b). My assistants and I recorded nest histories and measured chick growth of approximately250 Masked Booby and 100Blue-footedBoobybreedingattemptsin each season.We checked nests daily between 1200 and 1430,markednewly laid eggs,andweighedand measuredchicksdaily until the ageof 10 days.We measuredthe maximumlength and breadthof eachegg, andapproximatedthe egg'svolumewith the equation [Auk, Vol. 112 gressionor of their own disorientation.Thus, hatching asynchronyand time required for brood reduction were measuredin increments of one day. Both specieslay their eggsdirectly on the ground in circular nest scrapesthat have been cleared of debris. I measured the diameters of 75 Blue-footed Boo- by and 45 Masked Boobynest scrapesby placing a meter stick acrossthe scrapeat its maximum diameter and recordingthe distancebetweenpointsof contact with the ground. A secondmeasurementwas made horizontally perpendicular to the first, and the av- erageof the two wastakenasthe nest'sdiameter.The maximum depth was measured at the center of the scrapewith another ruler held perpendicularto the horizontal meter stick. The nest scrape'smaximum steepness (r, the angle in degreesmadeby horizontal plane and tangent to surfaceof scrapewhere surface nearsground level) is approximatedby I • = 180ø - 2[tan-•(R/D)], (2) where R is the radius (cm) and D is the maximum depth (cm; seeAppendix for a proof). Statisticalanalyseswere done with SYSTAT (SYSTAT, Inc. 1984), CSS:STATISTICA (StatSoft, Inc. 1991), and STATISTICA (StatSoft,Inc. 1994) software. Stepwise multiple regressionsof dependentvariableson hatchinginterval and egg-volumeratio attemptedto include hatching interval in the model first, at a-toenter = 0.15, where a is determined by an independentvariable'st-value(e.g.Neter andWasserman1974: 386). RESULTS V = •rLB2/6, (1) where V is volume (cc), L is length (cm), and B is breadth (cm; Preston 1974). We measuredchick masses with Pesolaspring scales;wing length was measured by holding the metacarpalsat a right angle to the radius and ulna, and straightening the manus or longestprimary. The A- and B-chickswere individually identified using ink spots we placed on the head,plumagedevelopment,and, after about30 days of age,their U.S. Fish and Wildlife Servicealuminum leg bands. I excludedfrom analysespresentedhere three three-eggBlue-footedBoobyclutches,and two Masked Boobybroodsin which brood reduction did not occurby 20 days after the B-chickhatched(see Anderson 1989a). A chickwas recordedashatchedat a given midday nestcheckwhen the chickwascompletelyoutsideits egg shell, or the chick was still attachedto the egg shell but had split the eggshell into two halves.Brood reduction was recorded when a chick was absent from its nest scrapeat a particular day's nest checkand, subsequently, did not return;frequently,chicksof bothspeciesreturnedto their nestscrapeafterbeing recordedoutsidethe scrapeas a result of sibling ag- Egg-volume differences.--Variation in egg size explainssignificantvariation in the posthatch- ing growth rate of somebird species,and differencesin egg size within a clutch could influence the outcome of siblicidal behavior throughan effecton body-sizedifference(Anderson 1989a).To do so, egg volume must influencebody size.In order to explain interspecific differences in siblicidal behavior, the two speciesmust have different egg-volumeratios. Eggvolumeexplains46.4and50.9%of variation in hatching massof Masked and Blue-footed boobies,respectively,and the effectdecayswith increasingage but remainssignificantat least througheight daysof age(Table1).Egg-volume ratios(i.e. volumeA-egg/volumeB-egg)of the two speciesdiffer in associationwith the broodreduction system: Masked Booby A-eggs usu- ally were largerthan B-eggs(Anderson1990a), while 24.4%of Blue-footedBoobyA-eggswere the smallerof the two (Fig. 1, Table 2). A twoway ANOVA, with speciesand year as main October 1995] Siblicidein Boobies Blue-lootedBoobies MaskedBoobies 70 863 the hatching-massratio on hatching interval and egg-volume ratio showed,that both independent variables accounted for significant variation in the hatching-massratio in Masked Boobies(hatching asynchrony, t = 5.54, P < 60 0.001; volume ratio, t = 2.91, P = 0.005), but not in Blue-footed Boobies (volume ratio did not 4o enter model at P = 0.15). Thus, the egg-volume ratio appearsto favor A-chicks in Masked Boobies, but favors neither chick consistentlyin • 2o Blue-footedBoobies.However, the egg-volume lO ratio had no significant effect on an indicator of competitiveadvantage(i.e. number of days o Masked BoobyA-chicks required to kill their siblings). In a multiple regressionof days to brood reduction on hatching asynchrony and egg-volumeratio, the effectof egg-volumeratio Egg-volume Ratio was not significant (t = 0.18, P = 0.86). Fig. 1. Frequencyhistogram of egg-volume ratios Rather than indicating adaptive differences (volume of A-egg/volume of B-egg) for Masked (n = in the brood-reduction system,the differences 171) and Blue-footed(n = 74) booby clutches. between the species in the egg-volume ratio may be proximate phenotypic effectsof differencesin food supply. Blue-footedBoobieslay effects,revealeda significantspecieseffect(F•,239 larger clutches,raise larger broods,and make = 24.91,P < 0.001).MaskedBoobieshad a larger shorter foraging trips than do Masked Boobies mean egg-volume ratio than did Blue-footed (Nelson 1978, Anderson and Ricklefs 1987), all 3o Boobies (œ= 1.11 + SD of 0.11, and œ= 1.03 ___ consistentwith greaterfood availability for fe- of 0.07,respectively; seeTable 2). male Hatching interval explains significant variation in the ratio of the A-chick's mass to the B-chick'smasson the day of the B-chick'shatching (hatching-massratio; r 2 = 0.41 for Masked Boobies, r 2 = 0.39 for Blue-footed Boobies;An- Blue-footed Boobies than for female Masked Boobieswhen allocatingphysiological resourcesfor secondeggs. If so, Blue-footed Booby B-eggs should vary less in size across yearsthan thoseof MaskedBoobies;data from 1984through 1986supportthis prediction.The derson1989a).Stepwisemultiple regressionof volume of A-eggs in two-egg Masked Booby TABLE1. Proportionsof variance (r2-values)in body size(asindicatedby massor wing length) explained by variation in egg size. Only A-chicks from twoegg clutchesused so as to minimize variation due to parent quality. clutchesdid not vary significantly acrossthe three years (F2,•68 = 1.66, P > 0.05), while that of B-eggsdid (F•,•s= 8.36, P < 0.001);neither A-/nor B-eggvolumesof Blue-footedBoobies were heterogeneousacrossyears (A-eggs, F•,• = 0.46, P > 0.05; B-eggs,F•,• = 0.33, P > 0.05). As a result, the egg-volume ratio was hetero- Masked Booby Age (days) 0a 2 4 6 8 10 Mass Wing length 0.464* 0.163' 0.110' 0.139' 0.154' 0.068* 0.050* 0.083* 0.062* 0.032 0.050* 0.042 Blue-footedBooby Mass 0.509* 0.722* 0.387* 0.444* 0.382* 0.021 geneousacrossyearsfor Masked Boobies(F2,]• = 6.40, P < 0.01), but not for Blue-footed Boo- Wing length bies (F•;• = 0.16, P > 0.05). 0.179' 0.449* 0.090* 0.120 0.400* 0.266* Nest-shape differences.--Both speciesnest on the ground, but while MaskedBoobiessimply clear debris from a nest site and deposit eggs there, Blue-footedBoobyeggsand small chicks are containedwithin a bowl-shapeddepression * P < 0.05. ßRegression equationsof mass(M, in g) on eggsize(E,in cc)for age 0 days:MaskedBooby,M = 0.820E 5.081(significance of slope,Ft,• = 76.2,P < 0.001;of intercept,t = 0.78,P > 0.08);Blue-footedBooby, M = 0.583E- 9.867(significance of slope,Fx,•t= 32.2, P < 0.001;of intercept,t = 1.60,P > 0.08). (Nelson 1978:plate 12). On two occasionsdur- ing nestwatches,I observedBlue-footedBooby A-chickspushingtheir hatchlingsiblingsfrom under the brooding parent in the manner of Masked Boobychicks(unpubl. data). However, 864 D^VID J. ANDERSON [Auk, Vol. 112 TABLE 2. Mean egg volumesand egg-volumeratios(+SD) for two-eggboobyclutches.Mean egg-volume ratiosdiffer fromratioof meanA-volumeto meanB-volumebecause A- andB-eggvolumescovary(Anderson 1989c; see Welsh et al. 1988). Volume (cc) Year n A -egg 1984 1985 1986 66 46 59 70.68 + 5.44 68.82 + 4.53 69.80 + 5.78 1984 1985 1986 45 18 11 59.24 + 5.30 60.15 + 5.70 60.84 + 5.83 Egg-volume ratio B-egg Masked Booby 65.64 + 5.43 62.16 + 6.12 61.40 + 6.92 1.079 + 0.058 1.113 + 0.089 1.148 + 0.156 Blue-footed Booby in both casesthe Blue-footed Booby B-chicks rolled down the steep sidesof the nest into the shade of the parent when releasedby the Achick, suggestingthat parents could use nest shapeto influence the outcomeof the A-chick's efforts.Specifically,obligatelysiblicidalMasked Boobieswill facilitate early siblicideby using a flat nest,and facultativelysiblicidalBlue-footed Boobieswill suppressearlysiblicidewith a steepsided, bowl-shaped nest. A two-wayANOVA of I', the maximumsteepnessof the nest scrape,with speciesand nestage class(10- to 13-dayintervalssincelaying of first egg;seeFig. 2) asmain effects,showedthat Blue-footed Booby nests were significantly steeperthan were Masked Boobynests(species effect,F•,•32= 64.1, P < 0.001). This difference 33 30 ß Blue-lootedBooby ß Masked Booby 27 57.42 + 5.03 58.48 + 5.55 58.29 + 5.25 1.034 + 0.072 1.030 + 0.030 1.045 + 0.068 was most marked preceding and during the 44to 53-day age class,when Masked Boobysiblicide occurs. Blue-footed Booby chicks in this age classwere in wider (radius of 175 vs. 127 mm, t = 4.36, df = 14, P < 0.001) and deeper (44.7 vs. 21.6 mm, t = 4.71, df = 14, P < 0.001) nest scrapesthan were Masked Booby chicks. This is a meaningful differenceto a highly altricial (i.e. feeble) nestling with a body length of 15 mm. A chickattemptingto evict its sibling from a Blue-footed Booby nest would face a slope 64% steeper and 38% longer than would a chick in a Masked Boobynest. In comparisonto ground-nestingseabirdspecies that are not obligately siblicidal, Masked Boobieshad the flattest nests measured (Table 3), further suggestinga link between nest steepnessand the brood-reductionsystem.Masked Boobyparentsused existing slight depressions as nest-scrapelocations,but Blue-footedBooby parents actively excavatedand regulated bowl depth. This point was demonstrated experimentally with eight Blue-footedBoobynestsat the eggstageby filling them with dirt and pack- ing the fill. The nests'dimensionswere measuredthree timesin the subsequent48.5 h, and the measurementscomparedwith the original 24 21 e 18 nest dimensions. (/'3 15 tently affectedby the manipulation.Nest depth was initially significantly different from the 12 Nest width was not consis- original depth, but returned gradually to the original dimensionswithin 48.5 h (Table 4). Nest Age (days) When I' valueswere classedby time sincethe (I•) of MaskedandBlue- eggswere laid (Fig. 2), variation acrossageclass Fig.2. Maximumsteepness = footedboobynestsin relation to time sincelaying of was significant in Blue-footed Boobies(F6,9o nest'sfirstegg.Eggshatchedat approximately44 days. 3.19,P < 0.01), but not in MaskedBoobies(Fs,• Period of Masked Boobysiblicideindicatedby shad- = 1.41, P > 0.05). Blue-footed Booby parents ing. Data presentedas œ+ SE. were remarkablyattentive to the shapeof their 0-12 13-22 23-33 34-43 44-53 54-63 64-73 October 1995] 865 Siblicidein Boobies TABLE 3. F-values(• + SD with n in parentheses)for ground-nestingseabirds. Nesting-cycle stage Species Single-egg,single-chickspecies Red-tailedTropicbird(Phaethon rubricauda) Egg Hatchling P (degrees) Source a 32.4 + 6.0 (18) 29.2 + 3.9 (8) 1 1 54.6 + 10.6 (16) 64.9 + 3.6 (7) 63.6 (21) 65.6 (21) 24.8 + 4.6 (51) 29.5 + 3.7 (48) 2 2 3 3 4 4 17.8 + 3.4 (27) 18.0 + 4.5 (17) 4 4 Multiple-chick species Cape Cormorant (Phalacrocorax capensis) Crowned Cormorant (P. coronatus) Egg Egg Egg Hatchling Egg BankCormorant(P. neglectus) Blue-footedBooby Hatchling Obligately siblicidal species Egg Hatchling MaskedBooby ßSource:(1) D. J. Anderson,unpublished datafrom Johnston Atoll, CentralPacificOcean;(2) R. E. Ricklefs,unpublisheddatafrom Malgas Island,South Africa;(3) Cooper 1986;(4) this study. nests. Parents restored their experimentally filled neststo a steepnessthat closelymatched the original steepness(Spearmanrank correlation, r = 0.93, n = 6, P < 0.01). In nonexperimental nests,F increasedimmediatelyprior to hatching, with peak values and lowest variability during the critical period in which siblicide occursin MaskedBoobynests(i.e. during the first 10daysafter hatching),but not in Bluefooted Boobynests(Fig. 2). I did not attempt to lingswere partially submergedin a pool of water, and Masked Booby hatchlings were completely dry. In 1986,rain fell on 25 of the total daysthat I was presenton the island (Fig. 3). The totalaccumulationduring 17 of thosedays was lessthan 10 mm; little mortality occurred of Blue-footedBoobynestlingsof 20 days or less(threedeaths).On the remainingeightdays, 10 to 43 mm were recordeddaily, filling Bluefooted Booby nests with water and causing observe the behavior used to form nest bowls. hatchlingbody temperaturesto drop (pets.obs.). However,the soil in Blue-footedBoobynesting On theseeight days,17 Blue-footedBoobynestareasis dry and fine-grained,and easilycanbe lings of 20 daysor lessdied (Fig. 3). Causesof excavatedwith a tool resemblinga booby'sfoot mortality other than hypothermia(e.g. predation and starvation) could be ruled out in most (K. Huyvaert unpubl. data). Blue-footedBoobynest steepnessis greatest of these17 cases.Nestlingsolder than 20 days at preciselythe time that steepnessis required were not affectedby heavy rainfall: only 2 of for suppressionof hatchling siblicide,and is 24 (0.08) deathsin this age group occurredon lesssteepand deep at other times (Fig. 2). I dayswith at least10mm of rain, comparedwith identified a possiblecost to maintenanceof a 17 of 32 (0.53) deaths in the 0- to 20-day age deep nest: hypothermia of hatchlingsduring group. heavy rains, when Blue-footed Booby hatchI comparedthe mortality rates of nestlings TAnrE4. Nestshapes(widthand length,• + SD;ram)followingexperimental filling of Blue-footed Booby nest scrapes.An egg hatchedin two nestsbefore the secondmeasurement;thesenestsnot measuredafter hatching occurred. Hours after Paired filling of nest Paired comparison n Width (ram) t comparison P Depth (ram) t P Original 8 298.8 + 41.9 -- -- 29.0 + 5.42 -- -- 4.5 24.5 48.5 8 6 6 284.5 + 36.2 270.0 + 27.2 293.3 + 28.8 1.35 2.91 1.38 >0.20 <0.05 >0.10 15.6 + 6.55 24.2 + 6.68 31.8 + 5.91 6.30 2.36 0.06 <0.001 0.07 >0.90 866 D^VIDJ. ANDERSON 50 [Auk,Vol. 112 prior to hatching,Blue-footedBoobynestswere still steeperthan Masked Boobynestsduring the first four nest age classes(Fig. 2; two-way ANOVA, nest age classand speciesas main effects;specieseffect F•,70= 32.52, P < 0.001). •,• 30 r• 10 DISCUSSION • •o Jan 1 Feb 1 Mar 1 Apr 1 May Fig. 3. Rainfall (above) and Blue-footed booby nestling mortality for birds in 0- to 20-day age class in 1986. from the 0- to 20-dayagegroupthat were present on a given day as a function of rainfall on that day. I useddata from the period of 6 January through 7 March only, becauseafter 7 March the number of nestlingsin this age group never exceededthree, reducing the precisionof the mortality-rate estimate. During the 6 January to 7 March period, the number present averaged11.4+ 5.8 nestlingsper day. A higher proportion of these nestlings present died on dayswith at least I0 mm of rain than died on dayswith lessthan I0 mm (mean arc-sintransformed daily proportions were 0.39 and 0.03, respectively;t = 7.44, df = 68, P < 0.001).This test is conservativewith respectto the conclusionthat rainfall and mortality are linked, given that three of the four deaths that occurred after 7 March happenedon dayswith at least I0 mm of rain. Masked Boobies in their flatter nests re- mained dry during rainfalls, and none of the 17 deathsof A-chicksin the 0- to 20-day posthatching age group occurredon days with at least I0 mm of rainfall. Blue-footedBoobyparents that had nests characteristic My goal was to identify factorsthat contribute to the differences Date of Masked Boo- bies might have similarly low hatchling mortality, but the limited variation in Blue-footed Boobynest shapearound the time of hatching (Fig. 2) did not allow that comparison.However, nest shapesof Blue-footedBoobiesprior to hatching were consistentwith regulation toward flatness:a one-wayANOVA of F showed significantheterogeneityacrossnestageclasses (F6,90 = 3.19, P < 0.01), and nestswere consistently flatterprior to hatchingthan during and shortly after hatching (Fig. 2). Masked Booby F-valuesdid not showsimilarheterogeneity(Fs,• = 1.41,P = 0.24).In spite of the relative flatness between Masked and Blue- footed boobies in their brood-reduction systems. I detected no contribution of within-clutch egg-sizedifferences,althoughinterspecificdifferencesin this factorare consistentwith a regulatoryrole.However,a novel factor,nestshape, differs between the two species:Blue-footed Boobynests,like thoseof other ground-nesting seabirdssampled,have steepsidesthat impede ejectionby hatchling chicks,but Masked Boobies have atypicallyflat nests,facilitatingejection. Moreover,nestshapeis closelyregulated by Blue-footedBoobiesin a manner that should suppresssiblicidal ejection of hatchling offspring. Comparativestudiesallow one to testthe hypothesisthat regulatorymechanismsare adapted to maximizereproductivesuccess. Two studies have approachedthis issue by comparing hatching asynchroniesof obligately and facultatively siblicidal eagles (Edwards and Collopy 1983)and boobies(Anderson 1989a).Both found that parentsof obligately siblicidal species hatched eggsat longer intervals (thus establishing more biasedcompetitive hierarchies) than did parentsof facultatively siblicidal species.Becauseobligatelysiblicidal offspringprevent parentsfrom raising more than one chick, even with short hatching intervals (Anderson 1989a),thesedataare consistentwith a hypothesisof adaptive adjustmentof hatching asynchrony that cutslossesearly. However, they are alsoconsistentwith a proximate-levelhypothesis that the food limitation that restricts brood size also restrictsthe rate of egg formation. The contrastingnestshapesof parentMasked and Blue-footed boobies offer clearer evidence of an adapted regulatory mechanismthat has respondedevolutionarily to both costsand benefits.The differenceis not explainedby the most likely alternative hypotheses. For example, thermalconsiderationssuggestthat nestlingsin the hotter environment should have a flatter nest to increaseexposureto air flow. The study site is equatorial,and both speciesnest in ther- October 1995] Siblicidein Boobies mally stressfulsituations,but the pattern is the oppositeof that predicted:Masked Boobieshave significantlycooler,windiet nest sitesthan do Blue-footed Boobies(Anderson et al. unpubl. manuscript).Another alternativehypothesisin- 867 G. Hunt, Jr., D. Mock, R. Ricklefs, D. Rubenstein, J. Smith, and D. Westneat for comments on an earlier version of the manuscript;and P. Petraitisfor statistical advice. volves the nature of the substrate:possiblya LITERATURE CITED bowl-shapednest is optimal for both species, but perhapsthe soil in Masked Boobycolonies AMUNDSEN,T., ANDJ. N. STOKLAND.1988. Adaptive does not permit formation of a bowl. Masked and Blue-footedboobiesdo show nesting segregation into different areas (Duffy 1984), but the easeof digging in the two areasis not substantially different. A similar degreeof digging effort yields a hole approximately20%shallower in Masked Boobyareasthan in Blue-footed Booby areas (Anderson et al. unpubl. manuscript), but the difference in nest depths that I found in this studyapproached100%at the time of hatching. In conclusion,hatching asynchronyclearly influences the speed of obligate siblicide in Masked Boobies,but cannot provide a proximate-levelexplanationfor the differencein social systemsbetween Masked and Blue-footed boobyhatchlings(Anderson1989a).Hatchlings of both speciesare capableof siblicidal behavior, but Blue-footedBoobyA-chicks face a sig- nificantobstacleto ejectingsiblingsin the wider, steepernestbowl than MaskedBoobychicks face. Experimentalmodificationsof nest shape in thesetwo speciesthat do not disturb parents and especially during periods of heavy rain would complementthis comparativeapproach to costsand benefitsof nest steepness.Manipulation of the "arena" in which sibling aggression occurshasnot been suggestedpreviously as a mechanismfor regulating the outcomeof the interactions;this appearsto have a proximate effect on brood size in these two species. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS significanceof asynchronoushatching in the Shag:A test of the brood reductionhypothesis. J. Anim. Ecol. 57:329-344. ANDERSON, D. J. 1989a. The role of hatching asynchrony in siblicidalbrood reductionof two booby species.Behav.Ecol.Sociobiol.25:363-368. ANDERSON, D. J. 1989b. Differential responsesof boobiesand other seabirdsin the Gal•pagosto the 1986-87 E1 Nifio-Southern Gal•pagos,and in particularto S. Harcourt,for lo- event. hypothesis.Am. Nat. 135:334-350. ANDERSON, D.J. 1990b. Evolution of obligate sibli- cide in boobies.2: Food limitation and parentoffspringconflict.Evolution 44:2069-2082. ANDERSON, D. J. 1991. Apparent predator-limited distributionof Gal•pagosRed-footedBoobiesSula sula. Ibis 133:26-29. ANDERSON,D. J., m',lD P. J. HODUM. 1993. Predator behaviorfavorsclumped nestingin an oceanic seabird. Ecology 74:2462-2464. ANDERSON,D. J., m,ao R. E. RICKLEFS. 1987. Radio- tracking Masked and Blue-footed boobies (Sula spp.)in the Gal•pagosIslands.Natl. Geogr.Res. 3:152-163. ANDERSON,D. J., m,ao R. E. RICKLEFS. 1992. Brood size and food provisioningin Maskedand Bluefootedboobies(Sulaspp.).Ecology73:1363-1374. BRAUN,B. M., AND G. L. HUNT, JR. 1983. Brood re- ductionin Black-leggedKittiwakes.Auk 100:469476. CASH, K. J., AND R. M. Evm',ls. 1986. Brood reduction in the American White Pelican (Pelecanus erythrorhynchos). Behav.Ecol. Sociobiol. 18:413-418. COOPER, J. 1986. Biologyof the BankCormorant,part 4: Nest construction I am grateful to the staff of the Charles Darwin ResearchStationand the ServicioParque Nacional Oscillation Mar. Ecol. Progr. Ser. 52:209-216. ANDERSON, D. J. 1990a. Evolution of obligate siblicide in boobies.1. A test of the insurance egg and characteristics. Ostrich 57:170-179. DICKtNS, D. W., ANDR. A. CLARK.1987. Gamestheory andsiblicidein the kittiwakegull, Rissatridactyla. gisticalsupport during fieldwork and to R. Ricklefs J. Theor. Biol. 125:301-305. for sharingunpublisheddata. Fieldwork was supported by the National GeographicSociety,Sigma Xi--the ScientificResearch Society,a Universityof MichiganBlockGrant,the FrankM. ChapmanFund, and the GeorgeD. Harris Fund. I wassupportedby DRUMMOND, H.,AND C. GARCIA-CHq. 1989. Food shortageinfluencessiblingaggression in the Bluefooted Booby.Anim. Behav.37:806-819. DRUMMOND,H., E. GONZALEZ,ANDJ. L. OSORNO. 1986. an NSF Pre-doctoralFellowship during parts of this Parent-offspringcooperationin the Blue-footed Booby(Sulanebouxii): Socialroles in infanticidal work. I thank: R. Brubaker, L. Hamilton, P. Hodurn, brood reduction. R. Ricklefs,and especiallyS. Fortner for their field assistance; D. Cheney, D. Duffy, A. Dunham, F. Gill, 372. DUFFY,D.C. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 19:365- 1984. Nest site selection by Masked 868 DAVID J. ANDERSON and Blue-footedboobieson Isla Espafiola,Gal•pagos.Condor 86:302-304. EDWARDS T. C., AND M. W. COLLOP¾.1983. Obligate and facultativebrood reduction in eagles:An examination of factors that influence fratricide. Auk 100:630-635. FORnES,L. S., AND R. C. YDENn•RG. 1992. Sibling rivalry in a variable environment. Theor. Popul. [Auk, Vol. 112 cific test of the prey-sizehypothesis.Anim. Behav. 35:1386-1393. NEI_SON, J.B. 1978. The Sulidae. Oxford Univ. Press, Oxford. NETER,J.,AND W. W,•.SSERMAN. 1974. Applied linear statistical models. Richard D. Irwin, Inc., Homewood, Illinois. O'CONNOR,R.J. 1978. Brood reduction in birds: Selection for fratricide, Biol. 41:135-160. FUJIOKA, M. 1985. Sibling competitionand siblicide in asynchronously-hatching broodsof the Cattle Egret Bubulcus ibis.Anim. Behav.33:1228-1242. HAGEN,J. M. 1986. Temporal patterns in pre-fledging survival and brood reduction in an Osprey colony.Condor88:200-205. HAHN, D.C. 1981. Asynchronoushatching in the LaughingGull: Cutting lossesand reducing rivalry. Anim. Behav. 29:421-427. HEnERT, P. N., AND R. M. R. BARCt. A¾. 1986. Asynchronousand synchronoushatching:Effect on Anim. infanticide, and suicide? Behav. 26:79-96. PARSONS, J. 1970. Relationshipbetweeneggsizeand post-hatchingchickmortality in the Herring Gull (Larusargentatus). Nature 228:1221-1222. PARSONS, J. 1975. Asynchronous hatchingand chick mortality in the Herring Gull, Larusargentatus. Ibis 117:517-520. PLOGER, B. J.,ANDD. W. MOCK. 1986. Role of sibling aggression in fooddistributionto nestlingCattle Egrets(Bubulcus ibis).Auk 103:768-776. POOLE, A. 1979. Sibling aggressionamong nestling Ospreysin Florida Bay.Auk 96:415-416. early growthand survivorshipof Herring Gull, F.W. 1974. The volume of an egg.Auk 91: Larusargentatus, chicks.Can.J.Zool. 64:2357-2362. PRESTON, HESERT,P. N., AND R. M. R. BARCt.•¾. 1988. Parental investmentin Herring Gulls: Clutch apportionment and chick survival. Condor 132-138. RICKLEFS,R.E. billed 90:332-338. 1965. Brood reduction in the Curve- Thrasher. Condor 67:505-510. INOUE,Y. 1985. The processof asynchronous hatching and sibling competition in the Little Egret Egrettagarzetta.Colon. Waterbirds8:1-12. SKAGEN, S.K. 1987. Hatching asynchronyin American Goldfinches:An experimental study. Ecology 68:1747-1759. JOV•R,L., X. RU•Z, AND M. GONZaLEz-MARTiN. 1993. SLAGSvOLD, T., J. SANDVIK,G. ROFSTAD,O. LORENTSEN, Significanceof intraclutch egg size variation in the Purple Heron. Urnis Scand.24:127-134. LACK,D. 1954. The natural regulation of animal numbers. Clarendon Press, Oxford. LOUGHEED, L. W. 1995. Proximate level investigations of behavioral reproductive tacticsof two Galfipagos boobyspecies.M.S. thesis,WakeForest Univ., Winston-Salem, North Carolina. MAGRATH,R. D. 1992. Roles of egg massand incubation pattern in establishmentof hatching hierarchies in the Blackbird (Turdus merula). Auk 109:474-487. MOCK, D. W. 1984. Siblicidal aggression and resourcemonopolizationin birds.Science225:731- 685-697. STATSOFt, INC. 1991. CSS:STATISTICA, version 3.1. StatSoft, Inc., Tulsa, Oklahoma. STATSOFt, INC. 1994. STATISTICA for Windows, version 4.5. StatSoft, Inc., Tulsa, Oklahoma. STINSON, C. H. 1979. On the selectiveadvantageof fratricide in raptors. Evolution 33:1219-1225. STOKLAND,J. N., AND T. AMUNDSEN. 1988. Initial size hierarchyin broodsof the Shag:Relativesignificanceof eggsizeand hatchingasynchrony.Auk 105:308-315. SYSTAT,INC. 1984. SYSTAT,the systemfor staffstics, versions 1.1 and 1.3. SYSTAT, Inc., Evanston, 733. MOCK, D. W. 1985. Siblicidal brood reduction: The MOCK, D. W., H. DRUMMOND, AND C. H. STINSON. Avian siblicide. Am. Sci. 78:438-449. MOCK, D. W., T. C. LAMEY,AND B. J. PLOGER. 1987a. Proximate and ultimate roles of food amount in regulating egret sibling aggression.Ecology68: adjustmentin birds: Economicaltracking in a temporallyvarying environment.J. Theor. BioL 126:137-147. TmVERS, R. L. 1974. Parent-offspringconflict. Am. Zool. 14:249-264. WELSH, A. H., A. T. PETERSON,AND S. A. ALTMANN. 1760-1772. MOCK, D. W., T. C. LAM•, Illinois. TEMME, D. H., AND E. L. CHARNOv. 1987. Brood size prey-sizehypothesis. Am. Nat. 125:327-343. 1990. ANDM. HUSBY.1984. On the adaptive value of intraclutch egg-sizevariation in birds. Auk 101: C. F. WILLImaS, AND A. PELLL•rlER. 1987b. Flexibility in the development of heron sibling aggression:An intraspe- 1988. The fallacyof averages.Am. Nat. 132:277288. October 1995] Siblicide inBoobies APPENDIX C This proof,in conjunctionwith Figure4, showsthat the angle FAD (which approximatesthe steepnessof a nest) can be calculated when the lengths of lines AD and DE are known from field measurements. 869 The stepsin the proof are as follows: (1) Angle AED = tan-• (AD/DE). (2) Angle ACE = 180ø - 2(AED) becausetriangle ACE is isocelestriangle. (3) Angle ACB is complementof angle ACD, and angle CAD is complement of angle FAD. (4) Angle ACB equalsangle CAD becauselines BCand AD parallel, soangle ACD = angle FAD = angle ACE. (5) Thus, from 1 and 2, angle FAD = 180ø - 2[tan-• (AD/DE)]. D ••.•//G ro u n• u rfa ce FJõ.•. Idealized•e•tJcalc•oss-secQo• o• a boobx •est. O•IX shaded•e•Jo• existsat actual •est site. A•ies a•d shapesdescribed cula•shape•ep•ese•tsassumptJo• that •est-bowlsu••acesconform to that o• a sectJo•o• a sphere.