Evolution of Feeding Preferences in Phytophagous Insects Author(s): V. G. Dethier Reviewed work(s): Source: Evolution, Vol. 8, No. 1 (Mar., 1954), pp. 33-54 Published by: Society for the Study of Evolution Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/2405664 . Accessed: 17/01/2012 11:10 Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at . http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org. Society for the Study of Evolution is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Evolution. http://www.jstor.org EVOLUTION OF FEEDING PREFERENCES IN PHYTOPHAGOUS INSECTS V. G. DETEIER Baltimore Department of Biology,The JohnsHopkins University, ReceivedMay 28. 1952 The extent to which biological races representa transientstage in the evolution of species, especiallyamong insects, has been the subject of extendeddiscussion ever sincetheexistenceof thesecategories was firstdemonstratedby Walsh in 1864. It is consideredby many that isolation,at least to some extent,by host preferences is a sine qua non forbiological races (Thorpe, 1930; Mayr, 1942; Huxley, 1942). With such emphasis placed on host preferenceit is indeed surprisingthatonlycomparatively meagre attentionhas been directedtoward the physiologyand evolutionof host habits. A thoroughanalysis of the problemsof host selectionwould appear to be a logical prerequisiteto an intelligentunderstanding of the role of biologicalraces in evolution. This reviewhas been undertaken in .the hope that it will reveal in some measurethe lacunae as well as the matricesin our knowledgeof one well known aspect of host selection,namely,the host preferencesof plant feedinginsects. I Conceive,forthe moment,of an organism as a complicatedbiochemicalmachine surroundedby a hostile environment. It may isolate itself completely and remainquiescentfor long periods of timeas do seeds, animal spores,and certaindiapausinginsects. Under theseconditionsenergiesare directedtowardmaintainingthe status quo. If the organism is to accomplishanythingbeyond static existence,and is to reproduce,theremust be energy transfersacross the surfaces separatingthe internalfromthe external environment.And this too requiresenergy. If an organismis small and contentto sit in one place takingin energv EVOLUTION 8: 33-54. March,1954. 33 in the formin which it is to be found there and dyingwhen the supply is exhausted, then the principal, and practicallyonly,requirementfor existenceis a selectivelypermeable surface through which the environmentcannot act adversely. From passivityto aggressiveness is a step which involvesfood procurementand locomotion. Execution of these accomplishmentsin turn requires a signalsystem,coordinating mechanisms, and furthercomplexities. With the acquisitionof mobility,animals also acquired the potentialitiesfor expanded opportunitiesfor self selection of diet, a talentwhich is largelydenied to sessile organismsand constitutesno problemfor dormantones: Sessile marine-organisms,for example, can select only from those food substanceswhich the environmenttransports to them. Among sessile species some, such as sea anemones, do indeed exercise a degree of selection (Pantin and Pantin, 1943). Other species, notably cockles, oysters, and mussels,are generallybelievedto ingest indiscriminately all particleswhose size permitshandling(Verwey, 1952). Throughouttheanimalkingdomthe selectionof diethas becomemosthighlydevelopedamongmobilespecies. It has attained finestdiscrimination among parasitic forms and, of free-livingspecies, among those insects which feed upon plants. Plant feeders account for approximatelyfifty per cent of livinginsect species. In no other groups of animals are feedingpreferencesso sharplydelineated. The questionsas to the originsand evolution of these precise relationships existing between certain phytophagous insects and their host plants are indeed thornyones. Until recentlythe mecha- 34 V. G. DETHIER nism underlyingfood preferenceand the are remarkablysimilar in their requiredirectingforces operatingin the execu- ments. All require an adequate supply tion of the relationshipwere poorly un- of amino acids, a sterol,eight to ten of derstood and a matter of considerable the known B vitamins,carbohydratein speculation. Now 'witha clearer under- readilyavailable form,and a numberof standingof these factorsit is possible to minerals(cf. Trager, 1947 and thepapers scrutinizemore intelligently the mass of of Fraenkel and Leclercq). Insects difrecords of insect/plant relationships fer in their quantitativeand qualitative and do not forcarbohydrate which have been bequeathed to us by requirements scores of field entomologistsand to de- as a rule require fat (Fraenkel, 1953). rive fromthismiddenof notes some ink- The most successfulof the few attempts formson a synthetic ling of the directionwhich the develop- to feed leaf-feeding mentof intimateinsect/plant biocoenoses dietwas thatmade withcornborerlarvae (Pyrausta nubilalis). These grow well may have taken. Considered solely from the point of on glucose, casein, cholesterol,corn oil, view of energy source, organisms are minerals,yeast, choline,and lyophilized limitedonly by their biochemicalinade- corn leaves. Growthis reduced in the quacies, and among heterotrophs the ca- absence of leaf preparation and with pabilitiesof utilizingenergysources un- yeastreplacedby pure B vitamins(Beck, der a multiplicity of formsare such that Lilly, and Stauffer,1949). the scantyknowledge Notwithstanding the majorityof heterotrophsare potentialy omnivorous. In seeking to under- at presentavailable to us it appears unstand the fact of diet selectionthe ques- reasonableto imagine that the,basic rephytophagous tions then arise: (1) do the various quirementsof the different species of phytophagousinsectshave dif- species are not essentialythe same, or ferentnutritionalrequirements? (2) are. that the enzymesystemsof polyphagous thepreferred plantssuperiorfromthenu- forms differ radically from those of tritivepointof view? (3) is the under- strictlymonophagousforms (cf. Brues, lying basis of plant preferencea nutri- 1920). Perhaps there exist quantitative degrees whichreflectdifferent tional one? These are extraordinarily differences in the utilizationof availdifficultquestions to answer unequivo- of efficiency cally because our knowledgeof the nu- able materials. It is a fact that phytotrientrequirementsof phytophagousin- phagous insectsas a group do not pracsects and of the food value of different tice economyof feeding. From one half plantsand of the same plantundervary- to two thirdsof the food is eliminatedas ing conditionsis patheticallyscanty. It feces. Most oftheprotein,fat,and simple must be emphasizedthat thereis a vast carbohydratemay be utilized; but starch betweenthe food ingestedand and cellulose are excreted mostly undifference the nutrientsactually required and uti- changed (Fraenkel, 1953). Chlorophyll lized. - It is an all too commonmistake is degradedbutapparentlynotused. The to equate gross food intakeand utilizable waste is strikinglyillustratedby the recent work of Gray (1952), who has constituents(cf. Uvarov, 1928). Even a reasonably complete under- shown that the pineapple mealybug, standingof the nutritionalrequirements Pseudococcus brevipes, secretes in its of phytophagousinsects is not now en- honeydewlarge amountsof as many as joyed (Uvarov, 1928; Trager, 1947; nine differentamino acids and amides. Fraenkel,1953). Of those insectswhich The numberof amino acids secretedinhave been studied more or less inten- creases as feedingis increased. Excess sively, Tribolium confusum, Tenebrio carbohydrateis not, therefore,ingested molitor, Ephestia kuehniellac,Blatella merelyas incidentalto an attemptto ingermanica,and Drosophila melanogaster sure an adequate proteinsupply. FEEDING PREFERENCES IN INSECTS 35 On the otherhand,underthreatof im- tended, that insects may pupate before pendingstarvationmany insectstend to- maximumgrowthhas been attained,with ward widerpolyphagyand can surviveon the resultantproductionof runts, that increasesto varyingextents,and the altereddiet (Brues, 1939). Further- mortality more,as Dethier (1947) and othershave thatfecundity may declineand population pointedout, even insectswith restricted densitydecrease (Painter, 1951; Fennah, diets can, by various experimentalarti- 1953; Kennedy,1953). In certainspecific fices,be induced to feed successfullyon cases the failure of insects to maintain foreignfood plants. themselveshas exhibiteda distinctcorAs far as can be judged from avail- relationwith the presenceor absence of able data, leaves possess ample supplies specificcompoundsin the plant. For exof linoleicacid oftheessentialnutrients(Fraenkel,1953), ample,a low concentration from in somefoodplantsofthesugar beetweband thereare not great differences one species to the next. It is not known worm (Loxostege sticticalis) has been offemaleswhich to what extent optimal concentrations associatedwithinfertility vary with season, growthstage, soil, etc. fed thereonas larvae (Pepper and HastThese changes have been studied inten- ings, 1943). Deposition of lignin and sively only in a few agriculturalcrops pentosesin alfalfaunderalteredconditions (and mulberry). In the otherfew spe- of light and moisturehas paralleled recies investigatedthe changes have been ducedfeedingon thepartofthepea aphis, studied exclusivelywith respectto spe- Macrosiphumpisi (Emery, 1946). maniHow directlythe aforementioned cializedcompoundssuch as lignin,carotenoids, and essential oils (Guenther, festationsreflectthe nutritionaladequacy 1948). Fraenkel (1953) considersthat of the plant is a matterof considerable evidenceis lackingto the effectthat sea- doubt. They do not prove (nor can the sonal changes in plants are of great im- possibilitybe excluded) that optimum varycriticallyfromone portancein the nutritionof insects. Fen- levelsof nutrients nah (1953), on the other hand, main- species of plant to the next or withina tains, at least insofaras Homoptera in speciesfromtimeto time. In attempting observationssuchas theseone the humid areas of the Neotropical re- to interpret gion are concerned,that the qualitative must take into considerationthe possible and toxins repellents, compositionof food solutes is not con- actionofattractants, stantlyoptimal and that relative abun- in influencingthe quantityof food inand min- gested. The studyby Wagner (1944) of dance of nitrogen,carbohydrate, eral fractions(i.e., the overall metabolic the feedinghabits of Drosophila mulleri rate of the host plant) is of greaterim- and D. aldrichiis a case in point. Several portancethan such gross factorsas total species of drosophilidsfeed in the cactus or even the C/N Opuntia lindheimeribut actually subsist nitrogenor carbohydrate espeupon the various microorganisms, ratio. The criteria customarilyaccepted in ciallyyeasts,foundin the cactus. Of the assessing the nutritionalvalue of food yeast species present,nine supportedthe are growth,development,longevity,fe- growthof D. mulleribut only six were to D. aldrichi. This resultby cundity,and populationdensity. On these satisfactory bases there can be no doubt whatsoever itselfis not prima-facieevidenceof a difthatcertainspecies,varieties,and organs ferencein the basic nutritionalrequireof plantsserve the growthand reproduc- mentsof the two drosophilids. tive requirementsof particular insects Dethier (1947) and Fraenkel (1953) more effectivelythan do others. The believe that nutritionally tounimportant factsare amplydocumentedthat the du- ken stimuli (attractantsand repellents) ration of immaturestages of insectsfed are predominantly responsiblefor reguon unfavorableplantsmay be greatlyex- lating the feeding preferencesof phy- 36 V. G. DETHIER It is suggested (loc. cit.) that one set tophagousinsects. Kennedyand his coworkers(1950-1953) and Fennah (1953), of compoundsmay be involvedin differon the otherhand,are of the firmopinion entiatingplantspeciesand anotherin leaf that the relationbetweenpreferenceand age differences.This conceptwould cernutritionalrequirements,especiallyas it tainly agree with evidence from other is of a more quarters(Dethier, 1953). It would harapplies to tissue preference, causal nature and of considerablymore monizewiththe idea thatone set of comacknowledged. pounds subserveselaborateecologicaldifimportance thanheretofore and theothera moreprobably Kennedyhas investigatedin great detail, ferentiation suit- uniformnutritionalrequirement.A tenchieflywithrespectto thedifferential abilityof leaves of a -singleplant,thefeed- tativealternativeexplanationmay be ofing preferencesof two species of aphids, fered,namely,that these aphids are esthe black bean aphis (Aphis fabae) and sentiallypolyphagous,attackinganyavailthe peach potatoaphis (Mysus persicae). able plantspeciesnot containingrepellent on or toxic compounds,and that leaf age Bothoftheseinsectsfeedpreferentially are governedby one or more young growingleaves and old senescing preferences leaves, on both customaryhosts and un- compoundsacting on a contact (gustausual hosts. There is an activebehavioral tory) chemicalsense. These may or may preferencefor these leaves quite in addi- not be directlyrelatedto nutrition. Such would not disagreewith tionto a speciesdiscrimination.Physical an interpretation stimuliare notinvolved,olfactory maybe, the observationthat A. fabae appears to more sharplybetweenleaves and gustatorymostcertainlyare (cf. Fife discriminate and Frampton,1936,and Day and McKin- of the same plant than betweenleaves of non, 1951). While fecundityis admit- two plant species (Kennedy and Booth, tedlynot an accurateindex of the actual 1951). Thus, while the recognitionof food by nutrientvalue of the food ingested(since tokenstimuliis unimportant thereis no controlover the quantityin- nutritionally differences certainlywell established(Fraenkel and gested), thefactthatfecundity correlatedmoredirectlywithleafage with- Gunn, 1940; Dethier, 1947, 1953; Tinin a plant than with plant species differ- bergen, 1951; Thorsteinson,1953), the good and ence has promptedKennedy (1953) to relationbetween nutritionally argue that the factors mediatingintra- sensoriallyattractivematerialsremainsan dif- open question. It is clear that the four are fundamentally specificpreferences ferentfromthoseoperatingin the case of relevantpropertiesof a host plant-odor, interspecificpreferences. Both involve taste,toxins,nutrients-occurin varying as Chin (1950) has shown but the former combinations, behavioraldiscrimination, has been termeda "nutrient"discrimina- for Solanaceae (cf. also Fennah, 1953). tionin that,it is postulated,A. fabaereacts Kennedyis of the firmopinionthat it is behaviorallyto certainnutrientsor com- improbablethat these vary randomlyor poundsassociatedwiththem. The actual that insectsalways react to tokenswhich involvementof nutrientsrequires proof. mayonlyhappento be associatedwithnuIt has beennotedthatthetwoaphisspecies trients. Yet it has been amply demonin that the physiologi- stratedthatall nutrientsare not stimulatbehave differently cal ages of the preferedleaves differ ing and that some,compoundswhichare slightly. Also, there appear to be dif- eminentlyacceptable to insects are nuferencesin the behaviorof alatae and ap- tritionallyinadequate (Vogel, 1931; von terae (Kennedyet al., 1950). If thepref- Frisch,1934; Trager,1947; Hassett et al., a questionofnutrition, 1950). erenceis so entirely thenthese different species and different It is whenthe selectionof plantsby the nutrient ovipositingfemaleof holometabolousinformsmusthave slightlydifferent sects is consideredthat a predominantly requirements. FEEDING PREFERENCES IN INSECTS 37 nutritionalapproach is most difficult to made of birds are particularlypertinent. invoke. The femaleselectsa plantor part Lack (1949) has observedthatcloselyrethereofwhichis to feed the larvae and is latedbirdslivingin thesame habitatdiffer not at all the properfoodforherself. In in diet,thattwomonophagousspeciescanthe case of gall insectsand certainminers not persistif theycompetefor the same whose presencein the planttissue stimu- diet,thatpolyphagousspecieslivingin the lates that tissue to growthwhich is said same habitatfrequentlyshow an overlap to be high in nutrients,it can hardlybe in diet. Amadon (1950), in speakingof argued that the plant or tissue is chosen the Hawaiian honeycreepers, has pointed initiallyon a nutritionalbasis since the out thatwherespeciesa alone occupiesan choice is made before the hypertrophy island it has generalizedfeedinghabits commences. An outstandinginstanceis but whereit sharesan island withspecies thatof certainsingle-tissueleaf minersin b each has a restricted diet. Indirect evidence of the value of rewhichovipositionon the top surfaceof a leaf leads the larva to mine in the nutri- strictedfeedingis seen in the numerous tionallypreferredpalisade layer,and an examplesof widespread,althoughnot inegg on the under surface presents the variable, specialized sharing of the enemerginglarva with the disadvantageof vironment(Huxley, 1942). Rather exmining in the nutritionallyinadequate tensivedata relatingto butterflies of the spongyparenchyma(Hering, 1926). All genus Colias have been gatheredby Hoof these lines of evidence indicate con- vanitz (1949, 1950). In NorthAmerica, is nottheprimedi- C. philodice,feedingon Trifolium,overvincingly thatnutrition rectingprincipleunderlyingpreferential laps geographicallyand ecologicallywith feeding. The failureto surviveand re- C. eurytheme,feedingon Medicago; C. produceon various plantscannotbe con- christina, feedingon Astragalusand other strued ipso facto as indicativeof nutri- legumes,overlapswith C. gigantea,feedtionaldeficiency.As willbe shownbelow, ing on Salix; C. hecla,feedingon Astrabehavioral factors and toxic principles galus, with C. nastes on Astragalusand play a considerablepart. Trifolium;C. interioron Vaccinium,with We must,then,seek elsewhereforpur- C. philodice on Trifolium. C. palaeno, poses served by food plant preference. C. interior,C. pelidne,and C. behri,all Obviously,it is ecologicallyexpedientthat feedingon Vaccinium,show some overlap some specializationof feedinghabits ex- but also considerablegeographicreplaceist. From the point of view of species ment. In the butterfly genus Basilarchia competation thereare advantagesin filling thespeciesB. archippusoverlapstherange all ecologicalnichesso thatas manyplants ofeach ofthefourremainingspecies (Hoas possible serve as host plants. This vanitz,1950). The foodplantsare identiwould be accomplishedif all insectswere cal for four of the five species. Among unrestrictedly polyphagous,but viewed in copperbutterflies ofthegenusLycaena,L. terms of interspecificcompetitionpoly- hypophlaeas,feedingon Rumnex, overlaps inferiorto monophagy in theeasternUnited StateswithL. epixphagyis distinctly or oligophagy. To just how greatan ex- anthe,feedingon Vacciniumand withL. tentcompetition varieswiththenumberof thoe,feedingon Rumex. speciesrestricted to a give foodis a probPolyphagyis a luxurywhichcan be enlem concerningwhichfew pertinentdata joyed under certain conditions only. are available. Numerous studies have Amongits obviousadvantagesare thelack been made of protozoa (Gause, 1934, of completedependenceupon the fluctu1937) and of predatorybirds (e.g. Cush- atingfortunesofa singleplantspeciesand, ing, 1944) but few of insects (cf. Park, too, at least superficially, betterpotenti1941; Wagner,1944; Crombie,1945; Sol- alities for world-widedistribution.Moomon, 1949). Some of the observations nophagyis a realisticsolutionto theprob- 38 V. G. DETHIER lem of speciespressure. It too permitsof world-widedistribution if it is associated with a cosmopolitanplant. The milkweed butterfly (Danaus plexippus),which in North America feeds exclusivelyon Asclepias,is an outstandingexampleof a restricted feederof wide geographicaldistribution. The very apt statementof Kennedy (1953) that "the favouredhost plant is not merelysomethingfed on, it is something lived on" suggests other services renderedby preferredfood plants. The its plantis acceptedwithits microclimate, shelter, its predators, its diseases. A is selected. Comparable wholecommunity instances of habitat selection by vertebrates are well known (Miller, 1942). Finally the plant is not a passive partner to the association. It also plays a part in preference,respondingas an organism and developing protective mechanisms whichinterfere with the utilizationof its tissuesas foodby insects. This aspect of preferencehas been expounded at great lengthby Painter (1951) in his book on plant resistance. In short,the course of indevelopmentof foodplantpreferences volves simultaneousevolutionof resistant mechanismsbyplantsand oftoleranceand preference by insectsso thatthepictureat any time is one of dynamicequilibrium moving toward a perfectsymbioticrelationship. When we considertheforegoing facts,it is apparentthat we should not expect a prioria close correlation betweennutrition and plantpreferences.The end servedby foodplantpreferenceis neithersolelynor primarilya nutritiveone, and in orderto appreciatetheevolutionary trendsin feeding preferencesit is desirableto inquire firstinto the mechanismof the phenomenon. II The wholeproblemof foodplant selectionmaybe statedin theformof two primaryquestions: (1) how is thepreference implemented? (2) what is the genetic basis and evolutionaryhistoryof specific plantpreferences? As a fixedpoint of departureone can beginwiththe observationthatinsectsas a group exhibitall degrees of preference fromthe polyphagousspecies which will feed on a vast numberof plants to the monophagousspecies whichrestricttheir feedingto a singleplantspeciesor variety, evento a specifictissuetherein. As Brues (1946) has emphasized,however,there may exist among the most polyphagous formsmarkedpreferences in diet (cf. also Koyama, 1951). The innatepotentialto choosedietsthusappearsto be commonto mostinsectsand differswithintheclass in degree only. Before considering the mechanismof plant selection,however,it is advisable to examine brieflyand to eliminatesome of thosefactorswhichinfluence superficiallythe expression of preference buthave littleor no directbearing on the geneticsof the performance or theimmediatephysiologyofhostselection. To indicatethata plant is preferredis to implyin the broadestsense thatit will supporta populationof insectsand permit their propagation. From the point of view of an individualinsect,however, a preferred plantmaybe one thatis more inacceptable even though nutritionally adequateor highlytoxic. In thelatterinstancea preferredplant is not to be confused with a susceptibleone. Since the insect'sdeathis hastened,theplantis protected,and such a plant is as much resistantas thatwhichis unacceptablein the firstplace. The diversequalitiescomprising resistancehave been studied extensively by Painter,and the subject is excellentlytreatedby him (1951) (cf. also Snelling,1941). Preferencepatternsare moldedby geographical expediency, ecological variability,and seasonal cycles. It goes without saying that many plants might be eminently acceptableto a giveninsectwere the geographicalranges of the two coextensive. This has been borne out by laboratorytests but even more spectacularlyby the behaviorof introducedpests, FEEDING PREFERENCES whichin various parts of the world have tastedand foundto theirlikingplantsnot formerly available. It is noteworthy that very little thoughtfulinvestigationhas been directedtoward the restraintsimposedupontheecologicaland geographical distributionof an insectby the range of its food plants (but see Huxley, 1942, p. 185; Wulff,1943; Brues, 1946, p. 112; and Pfadt, 1949). Similarly neglected are considerationsof the role whichplant ecology plays in delineatingfood plant preferences. Of considerableimportare the inequalitiesof microclimateswithin the area occupied by an insect or plant species (see, for example,Wolfe, Wareham,and Scofield,1949). Thus, thereare manyplantswhichgrow undersuch conditionsoftemperature, soil charhumidity, acteristics,and lightas to place thembeyond the pale for certain insects. And finally,unless the growing season of a plantcoincideswiththe appearanceof the feedingstages of an insectthe plant obviouslycannot serve as food. From the point of view of the plant the foregoing conditionshave been termedevasion or pseudoresistance(Painter,1936). In certain cases thepreferences of an individual insectchange with the season in accordance withtheseasonalsuccessionofplants. One very fineexample is offeredby the swallowtailbutterfly Papilio machaonL., which, in desert areas, feeds on a succession of different host plants (Buxton, 1923). Althoughthe phenomenonof evasion playsa limitedrolein theobjectiveexpression of preferences, it sheds littlelighton the dynamicsof selection. Given a number ofavailableplants,preference operates by selection on the part of the insect. But in manyformspreference,as far as the individualis concerned,is apparent ratherthan real. It fitsonly the broad definitionstated previously. An insect populationmayinitiateattackon a variety of plants indiscriminately and be unable to maintainitselfbecause either (a) the plantchosen (randomly) was deficientin required nutrients,(b) contained toxic IN INSECTS 39 materials,(c) was protectedby physical whichimpededfeeding,or characteristics (d) containedrepellentcompoundswhich mediated against feeding (cf. e.g. Ishii, 1951). Painter (1936) has classifieda and b as antibiosis; c and d, as passive resistance. In all of thesecases thepopulation retiresto a different plant lacking or failsto migrate adverse characteristics and is decimated. In the absence of migration,survivorsmightformthe cadre whichcould developby selectionthrough successivegenerationsa straincapable of survivingon the resistantplant. This end has been achieved several times experimentally(see Brues, 1924). In nature it is comparativelyinfrequentbecause insects usually desert an unfavorable plant or are completelywiped out. In anyevent,thefinalselectionofa susceptibleplantis by defaultand maybe termed passiveselection. Whereasthismayhave been the evolutionary forerunner of active selection (see p. 42), it is uneconomical to thespeciesin theimmediatesense since such passive selection is regulated by survivaland involveshigh mortality. Active selection,on the otherhand, as with any self selection of diet, presupposes a means of recognition. While adult insectsare at libertyto select their own diets, many larval formsare never granted this opportunitysince they are restricted by spatialfactorsto thefoodselected by the ovipositingfemale. Such larvae, however,in common with those which employa greateror lesser degree of foraging are none-the-lessendowed with the ability to identifya preferred plant. Whereas thisabilitymay serve no apparentusefulpurpose in the more sedentaryforms,it is as essential to free foraginglarvae as to adults. This is especially true in cases where the female mayovipositon thewrongplant,and many instancesare recordedin whichthe range of choice of the ovipositingadult fails to correspondto that of the larvae (e. g. Salt, 1935). Then too, many species simplyovipositin the vicinityof the preferredfood (e.g. Argynnis); and others, 40 V. G. DETHIER of the subfamily tions. For monophagousand oligophasuch as some butterflies Hesperiinae,may eject eggs into space to gous species these are chemicallyspecific fallby the wayside. It is imperativethat stimuli which rigidly control feeding. larvaeofthesespeciesbe able to searchfor Polyphagousspecies, which usually feed on any materialthat lacks repellents,are and recognizesuitablefoodplants. While vision,phototaxis,geotaxis,and not held undertightreinby specificcomattractiveness play a part in di- pounds,but the differential hygrotaxisundoubtedly rectinginsectsto the properenvironment of various acceptablenonrepellentplants for ovipositionand feeding,the ultimate undoubtedlyowes its being to different, forcesworkingat close range are largely ratherlabile taste preferences(cf. Brues, chemical(cf. Painter,1936, and Dethier, 1947,p. 120). The remaininglink in the chain of 1947 for reviewand discussionof attracby feedinglies in the tantsand physicalfactorsgoverningovi- eventsconsummated position). In order to understandthe realm of plant physiologyand biochemmechanismof action of these, it is con- istry. The plant,whichis the source of venientto break down into three basic the.chemical stimuli,is by no means a ofspecificcompounds. componentparts the behavior pattern standardrepository which culminates in normal feeding, It is an extremelycomplicatedorganism in its own right. Among its constituents namely: (a) orientationto the food, (b) biting response, (c) continuedfeeding. whichare of importancefromthe insects' The entirecomplexis underthepredomi- point of view are chiefly:carbohydrates, nant controlof the chemicalsenses, but fats,proteins,minerals,alkaloids,and eseach phase may be directedby discrete sentialoils. The last two mentionedcomthemaprisethosegroupsthatcontribute chemicals. and different If theinsectis notalreadyin theproper jorityof odors and tastes. The essential as a resultof having oils include benzene derivatives,straight feedingenvironment emergedfromeggs laid there,it is guided chaincompounds,and terpenes. The first thereby vision,light,gravity,and mois- twogroupsare believedto be derivedfrom ture. Final orientationto the plant or fat and carbohydratemetabolismrespecproperpart thereofby monophagousand tively. As forthe terpenes,availableevioligophagousspecies is effectedby odors dence suggeststhattheyare derivedfrom emanatingfromtheplanttissues. In many a commonprecursorin a stepwisefashion polyphagous species orientationto the (Guenther,1948, 1949). It has been shownthattheseplantconplantor propertissueis, up to thispoint, random. At thistime,in theusual course stituentsvary with the time of day, the of events,a strongbitingresponseis initi- seasons,thegrowthstage of theplant,the ated by odor,contactchemicalstimuli,or tissue,climaticconditions,and soil condiotherless specificstimuli. The chemicals tions. Consequently,the plant species or thesetwophasesofbehaviormay variety,althougha botanicentity,is not a initiating or may not be identical. Many poly- chemical entity. It is not homogenous. phagous species are regulated in their Rather is it a heterogeneousmicrochemchieflyby the presenceor ab- ical environmentchanging in time and preferences sence of taste substanceswhich are pre- space withchangingconditionsof growth, sumablynon-specific.The physiologyof climate,and soil. A realizationof this thesefactorshas been discussedin detail situationrendersunderstandablemanyof the reactionsof insectstowardtheirhost byDethier(1947, 1953). mechanismof plants. It is commonknowledgethatinIn short,thephysiological active selectiondepends largelyupon ol- sects ovipositonly on certain organs or factoryand contactchemical (gustatory) tissues,thattrophicformswill eat certain stimulationby compoundswhich are ef- partsto the exclusionof others,thatnew low concentra- growthis frequentlypreferredto older fectiveat extraordinarily FEEDING PREFERENCES tissue, that the plant may be relatively immuinefrom attack at certain seasons, that the same plantvarietygrownin differentgeographicalareas may be attacked to different degrees,thatindividualplants in a standmayappear moreresistantthan others,and that susceptibility may vary withsoil conditions. Of especialimportance is theknowledge that the syntheticactivityof plants and the compositionof theiroils is greatlyalteredbyhybridization and polyploidy, and thatmutationscan interruptthe stepwise productionofterpenesat anypoint(Guenther,1948). Changes of this nature,occurring in the evolution of plants, act directlyupon the insectsand are of profound,significancein molding feeding preferences. The consequence of these interactionswill be realized below. III In attemptingto solve the riddles of the evolutionaryhistoryof active preferenceswe are inquiringintothe,emergence of definitivebehaviorpatternswhich develop in insects in' conjunctionwith an evolvingstimulusfieldprovidedby plants. Many stimuliare preceptedby the sense organs,but onlycertainones have meaningand set offor releasean aspectoffeeding response. The action of releasingor of token stimuliis believed to be innate (Tinbergen,1951), thatis, the natureof the response is instinctiveand its basis genetic. Its survivalvalue depends,upon the closenesswith which it fitsthe need to be filled. How have the instinctive responses to diverse token stimuli been evolved? Since the pattern of insects' feeding habitsis by no means static,the progress of evolutioncan be studiedprofitably by reconstructing the past fromthe current phylogeneticpicture and by examining carefullycontemporary changesand fluxes in feedingpreferences.Phytophagousinsects were undoubtedlyoriginallypolyphagous (Brues, 1920, 1946; Hering, 1926; Takahashi, 1938). Certainlythe polyphagous state is more widespread IN INSECTS 41 amongthe oldergroupsalthoughnot confinedto them. Furthermore,under duress the majorityof species are drivento greaterpolyphagy. Environmentalsituations which released the feedingbehaviour ofprimitive polyphagousinsectswere probablyvery general and non-specific. This conceptionis certainlyin accordwith the behavior of contemporarycounterparts. Insectsare obligedto learn which among a greatvarietyof objects are edible. Two processes,accordingto Thorpe (1950), may expedite the accomplishment: (1) associativelearningon a trial and errorprincipleand (2) habituation. The latter has been definedas "an activityofthecentralnervoussystemwhereby innate responsesto certain relatively simple stimuli,especiallythose of potential value . . . wane as the stimulicontinuefora longperiodwithoutunfavorable results." Habituationcould not operate in monophagousand oligophagousinsects but may have been influential in the developmentof them. If, as Thorpe (loc. cit.) states,"learninginvolvedin perception must . . . be phylogenetically prior to anythingwhichcould be classed as instinctand must indeed be an essential component,on the perceptoryside at least,ofeventhemostrigidinstinct," then it is not difficult to conceivehow the instinctivebehavior characteristicof monophagy and oligophagy might have arisen froma more generalizedbehavior associated with polyphagy. We can imagine the change as havingbeen brought about throughthe agency of passive selectionaided by associativelearning,hanaturalselection,and isolation,a bituation, collective process culminatingeither in well developedinnatepreferencesor perpetuated phenotypicpreferences. Five lines of developmentfrom a primitive polyphagousstatemay be postulated(fig. 1). (1) Even in themostliberallypolyphagous formsas, forexample,grasshoppers (Brunsenand Painter,1938; Pfadt,1949) and the gipsy moth (Mosher, 1915) the phenomenonof passive selectionis wide- V. G. DETHIER 42 / / NO SELECTION NEW MONOPHAGY r PASSVEP\REFERENCE SECIE SELECTIO0 PLANT MUTATES | / PLANT MUTATES OLFACTORY Q ~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~CONDITIONI AND SELECTS NEW PLANT MUTATIONS MUTATION, SELECTION \ ISOLATIONREMOVED HYBRIDIZATION PHENOTYPICPERPETUATION (HOPKINSHOSTSELECTION) l l MONOPHAGOUS ISOLATON NO ISOLATION ETC. (V\ / \ NEW MONOPHAGY / MUTATIONS NON-GENETIC RACES (LABILE FEEDING PREFERENCES) FREE HYBRIDIZATION ISOLATION CONTINUED GENETIC RACES (RIGID FEEDING HABITS) FIG. and oligophagy of monophagy 1. Postulatedlines of development state. polyphagous froma primitive spread. For mnany species of insects it survivors were then isolated by such may be of minorimportanceonly, serv- eventsas prolongedimmaturestages,ening merelyto channelthe activitiesof the forced hibernation,reduced activity,or species intorestrictedecologicalnichesor comparablealterationsin growthand degeographicalareas or forcingpopulation velopment which would prevent them movementsof greateror less magnitude. spatiallyor temporallyfrommatingwith Caution should be exercised,however,in othersof the species,the stage would be relatingperiodic migrationscausally to set forthe developmentof a so-calledrefeeding requirements(Williams, 1930). sistantrace. It is reasonableto imagine Where the impactof passive selectionis thegenotypeof manypolyphagousspecies slightor transient,the action leads to no as being constitutedof large stabilized significant changein feedingbehavior;and gene complexesfromwhichspecial-strains frequencyof thepolyphagoushabitsofa speciesare re- characterizedby a different tained. Plant-feedingorthopteroidin- genes may be selected. Spatial isolation sects representexamplesof groups which would then prevent the mixing of the apparentlyhave retainedpolyphagyun- genes of such strainswiththose of other changedfora long periodof time (Brues, segments of the population. It would 1920) and are still affectedby a type of fosterinbreeding. Thus, from a single polyphagouspopulation it ought to be passive selection. (2) Types of passive selectionexempli- possible to separategroups characterized fied by antibiosisand passive resistance by dissimilar feeding habits. Indeed, (see p. 39) can, under favorablecondi- such groups have appeared in natureas tions,formthebasis forthe establishment resistantvarietiesof plantshave been inof new feedinghabits by introducinga troducedor developed. Resistantstrains selective factor. Of a population of exist in polyphagousinsectsand in those polyphagous insects which settled by of more restrictedfeedinghabits as well chance upon an unfavorableplant, only and merely await selection. Painter a small proportionwould survive. If the (1930, 1941) and Painter and Jones FEEDING PREFERENCES IN INSECTS 43 (1948) have shown,forexample,thatthe sects were reared on privet,the normal hessianfly(Phytophagadestructor)pop- food,foreverygeneration,but samplesof ulationof Kansas has consistedof a num- each generation were tested for their ber of strainswhichhave been selectedin abilityto accept ivy by presentationand the manner indicatedabove. Similarly, preferencetests. Only a poor abilityto by selectionon a laboratoryscale it is a eat ivy was developed. Ivy-bredinsects, strains on theotherhand,developedan increased simplematterto produceartificially of a polyphagousspecies whichwill feed preferencefor ivy with each successive successfully on a plantwhichwas, in large generation. There was no mortalityto measure, unacceptable to the original be considered,and it wouldappearthatseruledout. The most population. Representativeexperiments lectionwas definitely include: transference of the chrysomnelidsatisfactoryexplanationof these results condibeetlePhratoravitellinaefromSalix fra- invokesthemechanismof olfactory gilis to S. viminalis (Schroder, 1903); tioning,which will be discussed below. thescale Lecaniumrobinarium frompeach For furtherdiscussion of Sladden's exto locust (Marchal, 1908) ; the caterpillar perimentsthe works of Thorpe (1940) Lasiocampa quercus from oak to pine and Huxley (1942) should be consulted. All insectscannotbe manipulatedwith (Pictet,1911); thesawflyPontaniasalicis fromSalix andersonianac to S. rubra (Har- equal facility,nor can any and everytype rison, 1927). The shiftswere attended of food shiftbe undertaken. Such diwtih high mortalityrates, and survivors versityindicatesthat the degree of plastended at firstto be puny. Eventually, ticityvaries greatlyfromone species to however,a normallyvigorouspopulation the next. Generallyspeaking,the more was builtup over a periodof severalgen- restricteda species is in its feeding,the in alteringits feeding erations. Most of the timepioneerfeed- greaterthedifficulty ers experiencedas great difficulty in re- habits. turningto theirancestralplantas theydid (3) Olfactoryconditioningaffordsanin adapting to the new one. This fact, otheropportunity for the developmentof plus the initial high mortality,indicates altered feeding preferences. From the veryclearlythata selectiveprocesswas in timeofWalsh's (1864, 1865) pioneerdisprogress,and thatit screenedout froma coveries of biologicalraces, the idea has heterozygouspopulationthoseformswith persistedthat some sort of memoryof changedfeedinghabits. While selection larvalfeedinghabitspredisposesadults of seems both logical and adequate, it must a polyphagousspecies to ovipositon the be admittedthatno geneticworkhas been same species of plantas thatupon which to confirm undertaken this. Furthermore, theythemselveshad fed. This idea was accurateinformation regardingindividual laterenunciatedas the Hopkins Host Sevariationin host preferencesamong in- lectionPrinciple(Hopkins, 1917). Since sects and the variabilitywithinpolypha- Hopkins' time numerous investigations gous populations is completelylacking. aimed at testingthe hypothesishave been Carefulstudiesof feedinghabitvariability undertaken. At firstglance the collective among populationsof monophagous,oli- results appear contradictory. In sumgophagous,and polyphagousspeciesof in- mary,it may be statedthat the principle is valid forsome speciesofinsectsand not sects would be invaluable. There are on recordseveralexperiments for others (consult Thorpe, 1930 for a involvingfood transferencewhere selec- list of those whichdo show a preference tion does not sufficeto explain the re- for the plant upon whichtheymatured). sults. Most noteworthyare those con- No evidence favoringthe principlewas ductedwith the parthenogenetic stickin- obtained for: the pea weevil, Bruchus sect Dixippus morosus (Sladden, 1934, quadrimaculatus(Larson, 1927) ; thecodwhich 1935; Sladden and Hewer, 1938). In- ling moth,Carpocapsa pom4onella, 44 V. G. DETHIER feeds usually on apples but not infre- on fromgenerationto generationa feeding quentlyon walnuts (Quayle, 1926); the "tradition." In thisway non-genetic feedhymenopterousparasite Trichogramma, ing races are perpetuated. groups of whichwere'raised on different The crucialproblemis whethera phehostsforas manyas 43 to 63 generations nomenonlike olfactoryconditioningcan (Salt, 1935); the European corn borer, bringabout sympatricsplittingof a single Pyrausta nubilalis (Thompson and populationinto two non-breedingpopuParker, 1928). Artemisiawas believed lations. Mayr (1947) has discussedthis to have been the originalhost plant for problemcriticallyin considerabledetail. the corn borer; yet there is no striking It is clear thatin the absence of effective ovipositionalpreferencefor it. Maize is isolatingmechanismstherewould be free thepreferred plant,bothfortheArtemisia hybridizationbetween individuals with feedingpreferencesso that the strainand themaize strain;butArtemisia different exertsa weak attractionforfemalesof its preferencesfosteredby olfactorycondistrainwhilefailingto attractthose of the tioning-could never become sorted out maize strain. To thisextentthe Hopkins genetically. Instead theywould at most Host Principleapplies. There is no rea- be perpetuated(because of the female's races." Thus, son a priorito expectidenticalconditions preference)as "phenotypic of olfactory to exist in different species. The ermine in evaluatingthe significance factorforthe as an effective moth,Hyponomeutacognatella,although conditioning closelyrelatedto H. padella,whichshows establishmentof populations with new a questionof fundaa pronouncedtendencyto split into bio- feedingpreferences, logicalraces,is itselfquitestable (Thorpe, mental importancearises, i.e., whether olfactoryconditioningby itselfcan lead 1929, 1931). ofolfactory and to any kindof isolationand bringabouta Untilthedemonstration pre-imaginalconditioning by Thorpe and non-geneticmatingpreferenceor restricJones (1937) (see also Thorpe, 1938), tion (cf. Thorpe, 1943, 1944), or whether there was 'no sound physiologicalbasis the isolationmustbe interjectedfromanwhich would satisfactorilyexplain the otherquarter. If, associated with olfactherewere also specific principle. Thorpe, and later Cushing toryconditioning, formemberssimilarly (1941), were able to show that certain matingpreferences insects,whilein thelarvalor pupal stages, conditioned,that behavior would supply became conditionedto the specificodors thenecessaryisolatingfactor. This, howof theirhost or host plant. The parasite ever, is a point upon which few experiHabrobraconcan be conditionedto such mentaldata are available. Whereselective an extentin thelarvalstageto theodor of matinghas been put to test, the results an abnormalhostthatit will show an ovi- have been positivethoughnotalwaysconforthathost; Droso- vincinglyso (Thorpe, 1930). Of trepositionalpreference phila can evenbe conditionedin the larval mendous import is the knowledge of stage to the odor of peppermint(Thorpe, whetherselectivemating,when present, 1938). While these preferencesarising occursin races thatare geneticor phenofrom conditioningare in no wise so typic. Again, the geneticsof the cases stronglyingrainedor permanentlyfixed studiedare in doubt. The apple-feeding strains of Hypoas any germinalpreferences thatmay ex- and hawthorn-feeding ist,theymaypossiblybe adequateto serve nomeutapadella do exhibitselectivematin establishingnon-geneticraces charac- ing (Thorpe, 1929). Yet manygood spefeedinghabitsdo not terizedby the possessionof specificfeed- cies withdistinctive ing preferences. Cushing (1944) has show selectivematingand do hybridize presentedevidence that the learning of freely,as, for example,Colias eurythemne specificfeedinghabits by the young of and C. philodice (Hovanitz, 1948, 1949a, certainraptorialbirds resultsin passing 1949b). Two strains of Pyrausta nu- FEEDING PREFERENCES IN INSECTS 45 bilalis,the European corn borer,do ex- typicraces would never,in theabsence of hibit-matingpreferencesfor membersof spatial isolation,lead to geneticallydetheirown strain,but hereit is knownthat terminedpreferences. With spatial isothe strainsare fixedon a germinalbasis lationit can be the firststep in evolution(Arbuthnot,1944). If it should develop ary divergencethroughthe operationof thatthe preferencesexist only in genetic the Baldwin-LloydMorgan Principle of races, then other isolating mechanisms organic selection. This states,in effect, that modificationsrepeated for a numwould have to be invoked. Olfactoryconditioningcould lead in- ber of generationsmay hold a strain directlyto isolationthroughfactorsother withinan environmentwhere mutations thanpreferential mating. For example,it tending in the same directionwill be is conceivablethat the rates of develop- selected. (4) Passive selectionmightoperatedimenton different plantsmightvary sufficientlyto isolate adults temporally,that rectlyas an isolatingfactorso as to aid of specializedfeeding changesin distribution oftheplantsmight in the establishment effectit, thattheremightbe selectivefac- habits. A polyphagousspecies, by virtors, quite apart fromthe plants them- tue of its catholicityof feeding,is freselves, in the respective environments. quentlyable to occupy widely divergent Or, in thecase ofan insectwhosesphereof ecologicalnichesand geographicalranges. activityis restrictedto the host or its Different segments of the population immediatevicinity,therewould be some could become restrictedto special groups degree of spatial isolation. Any of these of plantsmerelyby extendingtheirrange factors,arisingas an indirectconsequence intoareas whereone speciesof planthapofrestricted feeding,wouldtendto prevent pens to grow abundantlyto the virtual hybridization.Isolationarisingfromthis exclusion of others. Or, discontinuities or any other origin would enhance the in plantrangesarrangedso thatone segchancesof the new feedinghabitsbecom- mentof the insectpopulationfounditself ing germinallyfixed,but it must be em- in a different plant communityfromthe phasizedthatthe provenexistenceof iso- other,and isolated, would assist in the latingmechanismsotherthan spatial,es- differentiation of distinctgene frequencies peciallythe preferential matingof pheno- in the two segmentsof the population. typic races, is at the moment lacking. Plant diseases, fire,increasingxeric conFurthermore, thereare activeforceswork- ditions,inundation,normalplant succesing againstthe establishment of sympatric sion, glaciation,and cultivationin recent groups isolated by feeding preference times,all may be operativeat one timeor alone. One of these is the tendencyof another. animalsto pass througha dispersalstage Very few examples of direct isolation in which they would come into contact are well documented,but two, based on with other segmentsof the population. indirectevidence,maybe givenas illustraAnotheris the reversibility of all condi- tive. Larvae of thegeometridmothOpotioningthat is not reenforced. The re- rabia autumnatafeedin Europe on Betula, view of Mayr (1947) should be con- Alnus,Larix, and Pinus. The subspecies sultedforfurtherdetails. filigrammaria feedsin the larval stage on The feedinghabitsof phenotypicraces Calluna, Erica, and Vacciniun. Harriwould be expected to be rather labile. son (1920) believed that filigrammaria Furthermore,it would be expected that evolved fromautumnatawith the assistunder stress of adverse circumstancesor ance of climax glaciationwhich isolated changed geographic extension of either the parent species into two groups, one the plant or the insecttherewould be a in the southeastof Europe and the other facile reversionto other feedinghabits. to the west of Great Britain. It was preIn any event,the establishment of pheno- sumedthatconditionsin the west favored 46 V. G. DETHIER the growthof Calluna and relatedspecies to an extentwhichcaused a reductionin the ancestralplants,Betula, etc. Consequently,one groupofmothswas restricted to feedingon Ericaceae while the other remainedon the originalfood,plants. At larvae prethepresenttimefiligrammaria ferCallunaand otherericaceousplantsbut retain the abilityto eat and develop on birchas evidencedby successfulbreeding for fivegenerations. Conversely,autumnata feedssuccessfully on Calluna. Within historicaltimesa larchfeedingrace of the Betula-Alnus-feeding species has developed as a resultof isolationfollowing the replacementof alders by plantingsof testswere not conlarch. Unfortunately, ductedto determinewhetherthe races so developed are now genotypicor phenotypic. Nevertheless,the example illustratesone way in whicha populationmay and vagaries be splitby thediscontinuities of plantdistribution. A rather similar account is given of race evolutionwith concomitantfeeding preferencedevelopmentin the silkworm mothgenusPlatysamia(Sweadner, 1937). At the beginningof the ice age in North America Platysamiastock was presumed isolated in two groups,one in the southeast and theotherin thesouthwest. From the formerthepresentspeciesP. cecropia arose; from the latter, the species P. euryalisand P. gloveri. The factorwhich isolatedthe two groupswas consideredto have been the presenceof treelessplains since the larvae are restrictedin their feedingto deciduoustreesand shrubs. P. cecropia is liberallypolyphagousin its habits; P. gloverifeeds on Salix, Ribes, and Alnus. As the ice retreatedand the trees followed,the mothsextendedtheir respectiveranges north. Hybridization of P. gloveriand P. euryalisin the region of Nevada gave rise to the formP. kasloensis. When forms migratingup the Rocky Mountain chain reached Canada, there appeared a bridge to the eastern forestsin the form of the wolf willow Elaeagnus. Part of the genus became adapted to this food plant and to Salix and gave rise to the species P. nokomis. Once establishedin the east P. nokomis is supposedto have splitinto two groups one of whichbecameadaptedto larchand gave rise to P. columbia. Today theform P. nokomisfeedsprimarilyupon Elaeagnus and Salix; thespeciesP. columbia,on Larix americana. feeders (5) The evolutionof restricted frompolyphagousformshas been postulated as arising directlyby mutations becamereproductively whichsubsequently isolatedfromtheparentstock. It is difficult to imagine how this end could be achievedin practice. In general,the importanceof genetic changes as primary isolating mechanismsis seriously questioned (cf. Dobzhansky, 1941; Huxley, 1942; Mayr, 1942). Specifically,the problem of isolating mutants with refroma parentpolyphstrictedpreferences agous stock does not appear possible especiallysincethepolyphagousstockcould by equallywell feedon theplantpreferred the mutant. There remain for discussion the histories of the aphids, leaf hoppers, leaf miners, and gall wasps, groups about which an enormousvolume of unrelated factsrelatingto feedinghave accumulated, but in each case the lack of key experiinterpretamentaldata makesany detailedtionof theevolutionof theirfeedingpreferencesin termsof modernconceptshazardous. It also seems wise to hold in abeyancenew discussionsoftheintriguing problemof host alternation(see Mordwilko,1928; Kennedyand Booth, 1951). The gall wasp (Cynips) story is only slightlymoreamenableto analysis. This genus, which is restrictedto white oaks, is presumed to have originatedin the southwesternNorth America duringthe Oligoceneor Miocene. Oaks are believed to have originatedin theidenticalarea and era. It is assumed that Cynips was always associated with oak. The Pacific coast populationBesbicus is restrictedto the group of oaks centering around Quercuslobata,Q. dumosa,and Q. garryana. It is thoughtto have reached the FEEDING PREFERENCES IN INSECTS 47 Sierras before the Great Basin became Pergesa elpenor& which feeds on Epiarid in the Miocene. Ancestralstocksof lobium and Celerio hippophaes ? which Cynipsechinusand C. guadaloupensisare feeds on Hippophae rhamnoides. The said to have perpetuateda preferencefor gravid females naturallylaid their eggs Q. lobata-dumosaand Q. chrysolepsisre- on H. rhamnoides,but the larvae would spectively. C. centricolais restrictedto feedonlyon Epilobium. Harrison( 1926), Q. stellata. The C. melleagroupsh6wsa workingwith geometridmoths,crossed preferencefor Q. stellata; the C. pezomna- males of Lycia hirtaria,which feed on choidesgroup forthe Q. alba group; the many species of deciduous trees a-nd C. gemmucla groupfortheQ. prinusgroup. shrubs,and femalesof Nyssia graecaria Kinsey (1936) believedthatspeciationin and N. zonaria, which feed on Achillea and other low-growingherCynipsarose mostcommonlyby mutation millefolium, and hybridization followedby isolationin baceous plants. Hybrid larvae fed only which stricthost preferencesplayed an upon the food of the male parent. They important role (but cf. Dobzhansky, were sterile,so furthercrosses could not 1941). The lack of physiologicaland be made. Since hybridsof Poecilopsis ecological experimentsdesigned specifi- pomonaria,whichfeedson manydeciduous cally to elucidatethe mechanismof host trees and shrubs, including Crataegus, plant selectioncoupled with the absence and P. isabellae, which feeds solely on of pertinentbiochemicalstudies on the larch, are cross-fertile,more extensive oaks, which are a heterogenicgroup in- studies could be undertakenwith these cluding a great number of biotypes species. F1 larvaefromreciprocalcrosses (Chaney, 1949), are obstaclesto further fedsolelyon larch. All othercrossesand analysis. back crosses up to the F4 producedonly If geneticraces characterizedby differ- larvae which would feed exclusivelyon entfeedingpreferences could have become hawthorn. This reportedresult is diffiestablished,thereafterthe usual mecha- cult to explain on any genetic basis. nisms of isolation and selection would Sweadner's (1937) experimentson hywork towardspeciation. Usually, spatial bridizationin thegenusPlatysamiayielded isolation operates first. This goal, the no information of pertinencehere because origin of new species, is related to the all hybrids fed successfullyon Prunus presentdiscussiononly insofaras the es- serotina. Two subspecies of the moth tablishmentof a species mightsignal the Celerio,C. euphorbiaeand C. mauretanica hchievementof a new fixed feedingbe- normallyfeed on Euphorbia and refuse havior. The questionof the actual exist- Salix. Yet thehybridfromC. euphorbiae ence of geneticallyestablished feeding ? x C. mauretanicae and fromthe recipraces has not been sufficiently well in- rocalcrossfedreadilyon Salix (Goeschen, vestigatedto indicate the frequencyof 1913; cf. also Schulze, 1913). Hybridithese groups in nature. In very many zation experimentsin the butterfly genus cases crucial experimentswhich would Basilarchiaby Edwards (1877) and Field identifygenetic and phenotypic races (1910, 1914) yieldedlittleinformation of have never been undertaken. Some per- value, again because the feedinghabitsof tinentdata are available foreightgroups, theseveralspeciesare ratherlabile. Two but no clear pictureemergesfromthese. species are concerned,B. astyanax and There is littledoubt that preferencesin B. arthem'is.The hybridof these two is strictmonophagousand oligophagousspe- the formB. proserpina. Larvae froma cies are germinallyfixed. The extentto crossbetweenB. astyanax? and B. arthewhich genetic and acquired preferences mis e fed only on cherry,the food plant intergradein polyphagous species and preferred by B. astyanax. A capturedfevarious races remains untested. Denso male B. proserpina,oviposited only on (1909) crossed the two hawk moths, Prunus serotina the preferredplant of 48 V. G. DETHIER B. astyanax and refused birch, poplar, Once races have become firmlyestaband willow, the preferredplants of B. lished theywould tend to maintaintheir arthemis. Larvae fromthe eggs laid by identityby one or more of the several this female fed on Prunus and meta- mechanismsof isolation. Should these morphosedintoadultsof whichninewere be discontinued, thecourseis openforfree B. proserpina and seven were white hybridizationwith other races to occur. bandedB. arthemis. Larvae froma cross By such a process secondarypolyphagy betweenB. archippus ? and B. arthemis can arise. This has undoubtedly occurred ,3fedon willow,a foodplantacceptableto quite frequentlyin evolution. On the bothparents. These larvaedevelopedinto otherhand, effectiveisolation,now reintypical B. proserpina. Inability to ex- forcedby associationwith special plants tendtheexperiments to succeedinggenera- and ecological niches would lead to the tionsprecludeda thoroughanalysisof the developmentof species withnarrowlyregeneticsof the feedingpreference. Ho- strictedfeedinghabits. These in turnby vanitz (1944), workingwith Colias but- directmutationmightgive rise to forms terflies,has shown that C. eurythemeis withaberrantfeedinghabits,and, if spasterile if the larvae are reared on red tially or reproductivelyisolated, would clover; C. philodice,sterileif the larvae representthegenesisofraceswithina speare reared on alfalfa. In neithercase, cies separated solely by feedingpreferhowever,do the adultsexhibitoviposition ences. In the last case the problemis nor the larvae,feedingpref- again thatof separatingthe mutantsfrom preferences, erences. The different diet requirements the parentstock so that geneflowis preof the two species,and phenotypicaltera- vented. Mayr (1947) has pointedoutthat tions,as well as thepresenceor absenceof new mutantsin diploid bisexual animals diapause, have a genetic basis which is always occur firstas heterozygotes which multifactorial. will forma bridgewithindividualsin the Two different strainsof the European parent stock. A possible mechanism cornborer,Pyraustanubilalis,were intro- wherebya shiftof feedinghabits might duced into the United States. The one occurwas suggestedby Stern (see Mayr, in New England is a multivoltine strain; 1947) : i.e., it producesmorethanone generation "Let theanimalAA, whichis host speper year. The mid-westernstrain was cificon plant1, have themutationa which originallyunivoltine. It has been shown in homozygousconditionproduces host by Arbuthnot(1944) that the eastern forplant2. Let us thenmake specificity strain is homozygous multivoltine;the the followingassumptions: Toledo strain, a mixture of univoltine and multivoltinegenerations. A strain "Assumption1: Let AA live only on of homozygousunivoltinemoths could plant 1. be isolatedfromthe latter,but no homo"Assumption2: Let aa live only on zygous multivoltine mothscould be proplant2. duced. Evidence indicatedthat the uni"Assumption3: Let the heterozygotes voltinestateis recessive. There is a clear Aa be exactlylike AA. indication-of a difference in hostplantre4: Let therebe littleor no "Assumption lationshipsin the two strains (Painter, the reproductivephase in dispfersal 1951). The New England multivoltine A so animals do not meet aa that populationfeedsupon200 different plants; animals. the midwesternstrainis more rigidlyre"'Assumption5: Let A be ill adaptedto strictedto corn and a few weedy plants plant2. associated with corn. Moths of the two strains exhibit distinct mating prefer"Assumption6: Let aa be ill adaptedto ences formembersof theirown strain. plant 1. FEEDING PREFERENCES IN INSECTS 49 sequently,feedingpreferences maychange to accommodateplant changes,and monophagousinsectsas well as polyphagous ones enlarge their menu to include additionalplant species as convergentplant "Then there will be littlemixing be- evolutionprogresses (8). The scheme tweenthe populationson 1 and 2 and the of evolutionof feedingpreferencesin the opportunity is providedfora gradual pil- butterfly genusPapilio proposedby Dething up of additional genetic differences ier (1941) is a case in point. It illustrates betweenthesepopulations." a sequence by which an insect may be Whether or not such a scheme can broughtinto contactwith new odors to work remainsto be seen. All evidence which it can become conditioned,and favoringtheoriginof new restricted feed- thus,by incorporating additionalbotanical ing habits is at best indirectand poorly unitsinto its life,becomeexposed to new documented. Food plant changes in the streamsof geneticchange. pierid butterfliesconstitute one case. The fourhundredor so species in the Pierid larvae are essentiallycruciferae- genus Papilio may be grouped into four feeders,buta fewgeneraattackthe Legu- categories: (1) species whose feeding minosae. -Membersof the genera Catop- habits are varied; (2) Aristolochia-feedsilia and Callidryasare restrictedto the ers; (3) Umbelliferae-feeders; and (4) singleplant genus Cassia in widelysepa- Rutaceae-feeders. Investigationinto the rated parts of the world. It has been -feedinghabitsof the lattertwo groupsreproposed (Brues, 1924) that the ac- veals some suggestivetrendsin the evoluquisitionof a leguminousfoodplantarose tion of feedinghabits in that section of as theresultofa mutationor someequally thegenus. basic change. Only eleven species of Papilio are now Within historicaltime a change which known to eat Umbelliferae. These are: can best be explainedon the basis of mu- P. ajax and its races, P. bairdi and its tationshas occurredin the feedinghabits races, P. zelicaon,P. indra, P. machaon of the apple-maggotfly,Rhagoletis po- and its races,P. hospiton,P. alexamor,P. monella. The apple-maggotflyappearsto demoleus,P. ophidecephalus, P. constanibe nativeto NorthAmerica,whereit ap- nus, and P. paeon. It is curiousthatin a parently bred originally in Crataegus. large cosmopolitangroup with more or At the presenttimethereare two strains, less standardfeedinghabitsa few speciec one of whichbreedsin blueberriesand the should have acquired the Umbelliferaeotherin apples. In thestateof Maine the feedinghabit. The factthatUmbelliferaestrainhas beenpresentfromear- feedersas exemplifiedby P. ajax will eat blueberry liesthistoricaltimes;theapple formorigi- species of Ruta suggeststhat theyprobnatedwithinveryrecenttimes. Attempts ably formerlybelongedto the Rutaceaeto transferthe apple strainsto blueberry feeders. To understandthe transition and -viceversa have been entirelyunsuc- from Rutaceae-feedersto Umbelliferaecessful (Woods, 1915). feeders,it is necessaryfirstto consider Up to this point all postulatesimply thechangesthathave takenplace in Rutachangesand mutationslargelyon thepart ceae-feeders. Rutaceous plants may be of the insectagainsta ratherstableor in- dividedon the basis of theiressentialoils effectiveplant background. This idea, into four groups: those with lemon or of course, hardly representsan accurate orange-likeodor containingcitral (e.g. descriptionof events. The pla-ntunder- Citrus), thosewitha rue odor containing goes evolutionand exerts a selectiveef- methylnonylketone (e.g. Ruta), thoseconfecton bothpolyphagous(fig. 1, stage 2) tainingboth oils in varyingproportions and monophagousinsects (7-10). Con- (e.g. Zanthoxylum),and those of a mint "Assumption 7: Let segregated aa formedon plant1 have difficulties in findingplant 2, even though the originalaa found2. 50 V. G. DETHIER or camphortype. Most of the evidence exposed to insect attack for the longest points toward species of Citrus as being periodsof timewould by naturalselection the originalhost plantsof Rutaceae-feed- develop resistantfactorsto a greaterdeers. The transitionof Citrus-feeders to gree than others (Painter, loc. cit.). In Ruta-feedersmay have been gradual,that some species preadaptationoccurs. A is, Citrusto Zanthoxylumto Ruta. Then wellknowninstanceofthedevelopment of the transitionfrom Rutaceae-feedersto resistantstrainsin the absence of insect Umbelliferae-feeders may have been af- attack is that characteristicof sorghum fectedby means of the rutaceous plant (Snelling et al., 1937; Painter, 1941). Dictamnus fraxinella, which contains Varieties broughtto the United States methylchavicoland anethole(compounds from Africa and Asia, where neither foundin Umbelliferae),orPelea madagas- chinch bugs nor similar sorghumpests carica,whichcontainsanethole. It seems exist,alreadyexhibtedmarkedresistance probable, however, that the change to to chinchbugs. such plantsas D. frcaxinella and P. madaAs an alternativeto the evolvingplant gascaricaproceededby way of intermedi- actingas a selectivescreen for those inates, Zanthoxylumn and Ruta, and not di- sects which alone can accommodateto a rectlyfromCitrus. The proposedline of particularplant mutant,thereis the posevolution would then be Citrus (and sibility of those insects being selected plants containingcitral) to Zanthoxylum whichwill accept all of the mutants(9). (or other plants containingcitral and Greaterplasticityof this sort would.lead methylnonylketone in varying propor- to the developmentof a polyphagous tions) to Ruta (or plants containing group. Or, if the various plant mutants methylnonylketone) to D. fraxinella(or were to exerciseno selectivevalue as far similarplantscontainingmethylchavicol, as the insect is concerned, polyphagy anethole,etc.). Or Ruta may have been wouldagain develop. A similarset ofciromittedfromthe series. P. cresphontes, cumstances,that is, no selective action, for example,feeds,on a wide varietyof could and does workin anotherdirection Rutaceae of differentchemical constitu- forthoseinsectspecieswhichundergooltion (Dethier, 1941). Some species such factoryconditioning(10). From a moas P. andraemon,P. cresphontes, P. ma- nophagousspecies phenotypicraces theochaon,and P. ajax representvarioussteps reticallywould arise. in the changefromone feedinghabitto a SUMMARY new and different one. The acceptanceof In viewing the evolution of feeding food plants from other families (e.g. Piper) may be explainedin part by their habits withinhistoricaltime we observe some insectsremainingconstantfor long chemicalcomposition. In just whichdirectionP. hospitonand periods, some shiftinggraduallyto new P. alexanor are progressingis difficult plants or enlargingtheirmenu,some alquite suddenly, to determinein the face of such a paucity teringfeedingpreferences of facts. Whether they eat any plants some polyphagousspecies formingbioother than species of Ferula and Seseli, logical ratces,others not, introducedinwhichare characterizedby thepresenceof sects attacking plants not previously known to be acceptable,and native insulfides,is not known. As indicatedearlier,those characteris- sects shiftingtheir attack to introduced tics of plantswhichare operativein host plants. These kaleidoscopicpatternsof selection,be it active or passive, have a behaviormerelyreflectthe diverse ways insectspecies may regenetic basis. It has been shown that in whichdifferent some are due to single genes and others spond to changes withinthe plant. The to-multiplefactors(Painter, 1951). It is drama of changing feeding preferences even possiblethatplantswhichhave been observedcurrentlyand in the past thus FEEDING PREFERENCES resolvesitselfinto a dynamicequilibrium betweentwo changingsystems,the plant and the insect. The outcomeat any particulartimedependsin large measure on the geneticpotentialities and relativerates of changeand stabilityin the two organisms. LITERATURE CITED D. 1950. The Hawaiian honeycreepers (Aves, Drepaniidae). Bul. Am. Mus. Nat. Hist., 95: 155-262. ARBUTHNOT, K. D. 1944. Strains of the European corn borer in the United States. U. S. Dept. Agri. Tech. Bul., 869: 1-20. BECK, S. D., AND J. H. LILLY. 1949. Report on European corn borer resistanceinvestigation. Iowa State CollegeJ.Sci., 23: 249259. BECK, S. D., J. H. LILLY, AND J. F. STAUFFER. 1949. Nutrtionof the Europeancornborer, nubilalis(Hbn.). I. Development Pyrau,6sta of a satisfactorypurifieddiet for larval growth. Ann.Ent. Soc. Amer.,42: 483-496. BRUES, C. T. 1920. The selectionof foodplants by insects,with special referenceto lepidopterous larvae. Am. Nat.,54: 313-332. . 1924. The specificity of food-plantsin theevolutionofphytophagous insects. Amer. Nat., 58: 127-144. . 1939. Food, drinkand evolution. Science,90: 145-149. 1946. Insect dietary. Harvard Univ. Press,Cambridge. BRUNSON, A. M., AND R. H. PAINTER. 1938. Differential feedingof grasshoppers on corn and sorghums. Amer. Soc. Agron. J., 30: 334-346. BUXTON, P. A. 1923. Animallife in deserts. Edward Arnoldand Co., London. CHANEY, R. W. 1949. Evolutionarytrends AMADON, in the angiosperms. Genetics, Paleontology, and Evolution, ed. by Jepsen, Mayr, and Simpson, pp. 190-201. Princeton Univ. Press, Princeton,N. J. CHIN, CHUN-TEH. 1950. Studieson thephysiological relations between the larvae of Leptiniotarsa decemlineata Say and some solanaceous plants. H. Veenman and Zoneni, Wageningen,Holland. F. C. 1921. Hopkins'shost-selectionprincipleas relatedto certaincerambycid beetles. J. Ag. Res., 22: 189-220. CROMBIE, A. C. 1945. On competition between CRAIGHEAD, different species of graminivorous insects. Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond.,B 132: 362-395. J. E. 1941. An experiment on olfactoryconditioning in Drosophilaguttifera. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci., 27: 496-499. CUSHING, TN INSECTS 51 1944. The relationof non-heritable food habitsto evolution.The Condor,46: 265-271. DAY, M. F., AND A. MCKINNON, A. 1951. A studyof some aspectsof the feedingof the jassid Orosiu,s. Austrl.J. Sci. Res., B 4: 125-135. DENSO, P. 1909. Contribution a l'etude des sphingides hybridespalearctiques.Bul. Soc. Lep. Geneve,1: 295-319. DETHIER, V. G. 1941. Chemicalfactorsdeterminingthe choiceof foodplantsby Papilio larvae. Am. Nat., 75: 61-73. -. 1947. Chemical insect attractants and repellents.BlakistonCo., Phila. -. 1953. Host plantperception in phytophagousinsects. Trans.IXth Int.CongressEnt., Amsterdam, 1951: 81-89. DOBZHANSKY, T. G. 1941. Geneticsand the originof species. 2d ed. ColumbiaUniv. Press,N. Y. EDWARDS, W. H. 1877. Notes on Limenitis proserpinaand arthemis. Canad. Ent., 9: 114. ELLER, K. 1936. Die Rassen von Papilio machaon I. Abh. Bayer. Akad. Wiss. MathnaturwissAbt.,36: 1-96. in EMERY, W. T. 1946. Temporary immunity alfalfa ordinarilysusceptibleto attack by thepea aphid. J. Ag. Res., 73: 33-43. FENNAH, R. G. 1953. Some aspects of the of Homopterain the humid food-problem areas of theneotropical region. Trans. IXth Int. CongressEnt., Amsterdam, 1951: 260265. of a FIELD, W. L. W. 1910. The offspring captured female Basilarchia proserpina. Psyche,17: 87-89. of the genus -. 1914. Hybrid butterflies Basilarchia. Psyche,21: 115-117. FIFE, J. M., AND V. L. FRAMPTON. 1936. The fromthe phloeminto pH gradientextending the parenchymaof the sugar beet and its relationto the feedingbehaviorof Eutettix tenellus. J. Ag. Res., 53: 581-593. on the FLANDERS, S. E. 1935. Host influence and size of Trichogramma.Pan. prolificacy Pac. Ent.,11: 175-177. . 1939. Environmental controlof sex in insects. Ann. Ent. Soc. hymenopterous Amer.,32: 11-26. . 1940. Environmental resistanceto the establishmentof parasitic Hymenoptera. Ann. Ent. Soc. Amer.,33: 245-253. 1942. Abortivedevelopment in parasitic Hymenopterainducedby the food-plantof the insecthost. J. Econ. Ent., 35: 834-835. FRAENKEL, G. 1953. The nutritional value of green plantsfor insects. Trans. IXth Int. CongressEnt., Amsterdam, 1951: 90-100. FRAENKEL, G., AND M. BLEWETT. 1943. The naturalfoods and the food requirements of -. 52 v (X lThT-TITTZP * 1945b. The distribution several species of stored productsinsects. of Colias in the Trans. Roy. Ent. Soc. Lon., 93: 457-490. equatorialAndes. Caldasia, Bol. Inst. Cien. Nat. Univ. Nac. Colombia,Bogota, 3: 283FRAENKEL, G., and D. L. GUNN, 1940. The 300. orientation of animals. OxfordUniv. Press, . 1948. Ecological segregationof interLondon. fertilespeciesof Colias. Ecology,29: 461VON FRISCH, K. 1934. Uber den Geschmacks469. sinn der Biene. Zeit. vergl. Physiol.,21: 1949a. Change of host preferences in 1-156. Colias philodice. J. Econ. Ent.,41: 980. GAUSE,G. F. 1934. The strugglefor exist1949b. Increasedvariabilityin populaence. Williamsand WilkinsCo., Baltimore. tions followingnatural hybridization.Ge. 1937. Experimentalpopulationsof minetics,Paleontology,and Evolutioned. by croscopicorganisms. Ecology,18: 173-179. Jepsen,Mayr, and Simpson. pp. 339-355. F. 1913. Salix babylonicaL., als GOESCHEN, Futterfur Hybridender Schwarmergattung PrincetonUniv. Press, Princeton,N. J. 1949c. Interspecificmatings between Celerio. Zeit. wiss. Insektenbiol., 9: 72-73. Colias eurythemeand Colias philodice in GOLDSCHMIDT, R. B. 1940. The materialbasis wild populations.Evolution,3: 170-173. ofevolution.Yale Univ. Press,New Haven. 1950a. The biologyof the Colias butterGRAY, R. A. 1952. Composition of honeydew -. flies. I. The distributionof the North excretedby pineapplemealybugs. Science, Americanspecies. WasmannJ. Biol.,8: 49115: 129-133. 75. E. 1948-1949. The essential oils. GUENTHER, -. 1950b. The biologyof the Colias butterVols. I-III. D. Van NostrandCo., N. Y. flies. II. Parallel geographicalvariationof HARRISON, J. W. H. 1920. Geneticalstudies dimorphiccolor phases in North American on themothsofthegeometrid genusOporabia species. WasmannJ. Biol., 8: 197-219. of (Oporinia) with a special consideration in theLepidoptera.J.Gen.,9: 195- HUXLEY,J. 1942. Evolution:themodernsynmelanism thesis. Geo. Allen and Unwin,London. 280. . 1926. On the inheritance of food habits IBBOTSON, A., AND J. S. KENNEDY 1950. The distribution in relationto of aphidinfestation in the hybridsbetweenthe geometridmoths leaf age. II. The progressof Aphis fabae PoecilopsispomonariaHb. and P. isabellae on sugar beet in pots. Scop. infestations Harrison. Proc. Univ. Durham Phil. Soc., Ann.Applied,Biol.,37: 680-696. 7: 194-201. 1927. Experiments on the egg-layingin- ISHII, S. 1951. Studieson the host plantsof the cowpea weevil (Callosobruchuschinenstinctsof thesaw fly,Pontaniasalicis Christ, sis L.). IX. On the relationbetweenthe of acand,theirbearingon the inheritance chemicalconstituents of sterolsand the dequiredcharacters;withsome remarkson a of larva. Botyu-Kagaku,16: 83velopment inevolution.Proc. Soc., B 101: newprinciple 90. 115-126. and hostKENNEDY, J. S. 1950. Host-finding HASSETT, C. C., V. G. DETHIER, AND J. GANS. alterationin aphides. EighthInt. Congress 1950. A comparison of nutritivevalues and Ent. Stockholm. taste thresholds of carbohydrates for the 1953. Host plantselectionin Aphididae. blowfly.Biol. Bul., 99: 446-453. Trans. IXth Int. CongressEnt.,Amsterdam, HERING, M. 1926. Die Okologieder blattmini1951: 106-113. erendenInsektenlarven. Borntraeger, BerKENNEDY, J. S., AND C. 0: BOOTH. 1951. Host lin. in Aphisfabae Scop. I. Feeding alternation HOPKINS, A. D. 1917. A discussionof C. G. preferences and fecundity in relationto the J. Hewitt's paper on "Insect Behaviour." age and kindof leaves. Ann. AppliedBiol., Econ. Ent.,10: 92-93. 38: 25-64. 1942. Genetic and ecologic KENNEDY, J.S., A. IBBOTSON, AND C. 0. BOOTH. HOVANITZ, W. analysesof wild populationsin Lepidoptera. 1950. The distribution of aphid infestation I. Pupal size and weightvariationin some in relationto leaf age. I. Myzus persicae Californiapopulationsof Melitaea chalce(Sulz.) and Aphis fabae Scop. on spindle dona. Ecology,23: 175-188. trees and sugar-beetplants. Ann. Applied 1944. Geneticdata on the two races of Biol.,37: 651-679. Colias chrysotheme in North America and KINSEY, A. C. 1930. The gall wasp genus on a whiteformoccurring in each. Genetics, Cynips. A studyin the origin of species. 29: 1-30. Indiana Univ. Studies,16: 1-577. 1945a. Physiologicalbehaviorand geog1936. The originof highercategoriesin raphy in control of the alfalfa butterfly. Cynips. IndianaUniv. Pub. Sci. Ser., 4: 1J. Econ. Ent.,37: 740-745. 334. FEREDTNG PREFE1RENCES TN TNSECTS 53 Univ. WyomingAg. Exp. Sta. Bul., 290: N. 1951. Studieson the foodeating 1-51. habits of the large 28-spottedlady beetle, Epilachna vigintioctomaculata Motschulsky. PICTET, A. 1911. Un nouvel exemple de l'hereditedes caracteresacquis. Arch. Soc. II. The quantityof food-plants eatenby the Phys.Nat. Geneve,31: 561-563. adult. Oyo-Kontyu, 6: 192-197. LACK, D. 1949. The significance of ecological QUAYLE, H. J. 1926. The codlingmoth in walnuts. Univ. Cal. Coll. Agric. Exp. Sta. isolation. Genetics,Paleontology, and EvoBul., 402: 33. lution ed. by Jepsen,Mayr, and Simpson. studiesin insect pp. 299-308,PrincetonUniv. Press, Prince- SALT,G. 1935. Experimental parasitism.III. Host selection. Proc. Roy. ton,N. J. Soc. Lon.,B 117: 413435. LARSON, A. 0. 1927. The host-selection prinerciple as appliedto Bruchusquadrimaculatus SCHRODER, C. 1903. Ueber experimentell Verh. deutsch. zielte Instinktsvariationen. Fab. Ann. Ent. Soc. Amer.,20: 37-80. Zool. Ges., p. 158. LECLERCQ, J. 1949. La biotine, facteurde croiszu Artikel sance pour "Tenebrio molitorL." Arch. SCHULZE, P. 1913. Bemerkungen Goeschens. Zeit. wiss. Insektenbiol., 9: 73Int. Physiol.,57: 67-70. 74. MARCHAL, P. 1908. Le Lecaniumdu Robinia. SLADDEN, D. E. 1934. Transferenceof inC. R. Soc. Biol., Paris, 65: 2-5. duced food-habitfromparentto offspring. MAYR, E. 1942. Systematics and the originof Part I. Proc. Roy. Soc. Lon., B 114: 441species. Columbia Univ. Press, N. Y. 449. 1947. Ecological factorsin speciation. Evolution,1: 263-288. 1935. Transferenceof induced foodMEYRICK, R. 1927. The hereditary choice of habit fromparentto offspring.II. Proc. food plantsin the Lepidopteraand its evoRoy. Soc. Lon.,B 119: 31-46. lutionary significance.Nature,119: 388. SLADDEN, D. E., AND H. R. HEWER. 1938. MILLER, A. H. 1942. Habitatselectionamong Transferenceof induced food-habitfrom highervertebrates and its relationto intraparentto offspring.III. Proc. Roy. Soc. specificvariation. Am. Nat., 76: 25-35. Lon.,B 126: 30-44. MORDWILKO, A. 1928. The evolutionof cycles SNELLING, R. 0. 1941. Resistanceof plants and the originof heteroecy(migrations)in to insectattack. Bot. Rev.,7: 543-586. plantlice. Ann.Mag. Nat. Hist.,2: 570-582. SNELLING, R. O., R. H. PAINTER,J. H. PARKER, MOSHER, F. H. 1915. Food plantsof thegipsy AND W. M. OSBORN. 1937. Resistanceof mothin America. U. S. Dept. Agri. Bul., sorghum to thechinchbug. U. S. Dept.Agr. 250: 33-39. Tech. Bul., 585: 1-56. PAINTER, R. H. 1936. The foodof insectsand SOLOMON, M. E. 1949. Thlenaturalcontrolof its relation to resistance of plants to insect animalpopulations.J. AnimalEcol., 18: 1attack. Am. Nat., 70: 547-566. 35. . 1941. The economicvalue and biologic SWEADNER,W. R. 1937. Hybridizationand the KOYAMA, significanceof insect resistance in plants. Econ. Ent., 34: 358-367. J. . 1951. InsectResistancein Crop Plants. MacmillanCo., N. Y. PAINTER, R. H., AND E. T. JONES. 1948. The hessian fly resistance of the recently dis- tributedPawneewheatvarietyunderKansas farmconditions.J. Econ. Ent.,41: 882-886. PANTIN, C. F. A., AND A. M. P. PANTIN. 1943. The stimulusto feedingin Anemoniasulcata. J. Exp. Biol., 20: 6-13. PARK, T. 1941. The laboratory population as a test of a comprehensive ecological system. Quart. Rev. Biol., 16: 274-293,440-461. PEPPER, J. H., AND E. HASTINGS. 1943. Biochemicalstudiesof the sugarbeet webworm (Loxostege sticticalisL.) with special referenceto the fattyacids and theirrelation to dispauseand sterility.Mont.Agr. Expt. Sta. Tech. Bul., 413: 1-36. PFADT, R. E. 1949. Food plants as factors in the ecologyof the lessermigratory grasshopper, Melanoplus itiexicanus (Sauss.). phylogeny of the genus Platysamia. Ann. Carnegie Mus., 25: 163-242. TAKAHASHI,R. 1938. Monophagy and polyphagy among phytophagousinsects: An evolutionary consideration. Kontyu, 12: 130-139. THOMPSON, W. R., AND H. L. PARKER. 1927- 1928. Host selectionin Pyrausta nubilalis Hiibn. Bul. Ent. Res., 18: 359-364. THORPE, W. H. 1929. Biological races in Hy- ponomeuta padellaL. J. Linn.Soc. Lon.,36: 621-634. 1930. Biological races in insects and allied groups. Biol. Rev., 5: 175-212. -. 1931. Further observations on biological races in Hyponomeuta padella (L.). J. Linn. Soc. Lon., 37: 489-492. -. 1938. Further experiments on olfactory conditioning in a parasitic insect. The nature of the conditioningprocess. Proc. Roy. Soc. Lon., B 126: 370-397. -. 1940. Ecology and the future of systematics. The New Systematics ed. J. Huxley. Oxford Univ. Press. 54 V. G. DETHIER 1943-1944. Types of learningin insects and otherarthropods.Brit.J. Psychol.Gen. Sec., 33: 220-234; 34: 20-31,66-76. . 1950. The conceptsof learningand their relationto thoseof instinct.SymposiaSoc. Exp. Biol., 4: 387-408. Academic Press, N. Y. THORPE, W. H., AND F. G. W. JONES. 1937. in a parasiticinsect Olfactoryconditioning and its relationto the problemof host selection. Proc. Roy. Soc. Lon.,B 124: 56-81. THORSTEINSON, A. J. 1953. The chemotactic responsesthat determinehost specificity in an oligophagousinsect (Plutella maculipennis (Curt.) Lepidoptera). Can. J. Zool., 31: 52-72. TINBERGEN,N. 1951. The studyof instinct. sourceof theirfoodsupply. Arch.Neerland Zool.,10: 171-239. VOGEL, B. 1931. tUberdie Beziehungen zwischen Siissgeschmackund, Nahrwert von Zuckernund Zuckeralkoholen bei der honigbiene. Zeit. vergl.Physiol.,14: 273-347. WAGNER, R. P. 1944. The nutrition of Droso philamulleriand D. aldrichi. Growthof the larvae on a cactus extractand the microorganismsfound in cactus. Univ. Texas Pub. No. 4445: 104-128. WALSH, B. D. 1864. On phytophagic varieties and phytophagous species. Proc. Ent. Soc. Phila., 3: 403-430. . 1865. On the phytophagic varietiesof phytophagous species,with remarkson the unityof colorationof insects. Proc. Ent. Soc. Phila.,5: 194-216. Oxford Univ. Press. WILLIAMS, C. B. 1930. The migration of but-TRAGER,W. 1947. Insect nutrition. Biol. terflies.Oliverand Boyd,Edinburgh. Rev., 22: 148-177. WOLFE, N. J.,R. T. WAREHAM, AND H. T. ScoULLYETT,G. C. 1936. Host selection by MicroFIELD. 1949. Microclimates and macrocliplectron fuscipernis,Zett. (Chalcididae, Hymate of Neotoma,a small valley in central menoptera): Proc. Roy. Soc. Lon., B 120: Ohio. Ohio Biol. SurveyBul., 41: 1-267. 253-291. WOODS,W. C. 1915. Blueberryinsects in B. P. 1928. Insectnutrition UVAROV, andmetabMaine. Bul. MaineAg. Exp. Sta., 244: 249-. olism. Trans. Ent. Soc. Lon., 1928: 255-343. VERWEY,J. 1952. On the ecology of distribution of cockle and mussel in the Dutch Waddensea, their r6le in sedimentationand the 288. E. V. 1943. Introduction to historical plant geography. Chronica Botanica Co., Waltham,Mass. WULFF,