J ournal of Gerontology : PSYCH OLOCICAL 1 9 9 6 ,V o l . 5 l B , N o . 6 , P 3 l 7 - P 3 3 0 SCI ENCES Copyright 1996 by The Gerontological Seiety of Arcrica Interrelations of Age, VisualAcurty, andCognitiveFunctioning Timothy A. Salthouse,Holly E. Hancock,ElizabethJ. Meinz, and David Z. Hambrick School of psychology, Georgia Institute of Technology. It hasrecently beensuggestedthot on ,nan! meanr.resof cognitive -h^ Ihrge Foportion of the age-relatedinfluences functioning h mediatedthrough a singk commonJactor. Thi hypothesis brrn suppoied by the disioviry that much of the age'relaredvariancein diflerent cognitivemeasuresis shared,and is not iiitinct or-independent.Thr1yearlier resultswerereplicatedin thisproiect, and il wasalsodiscoveredthat measuresof corrected visual acuityai processingspeedshareavery-largeproportion of the age-relatedvariancein mcasuresof workingmemory, ^roiiotin1" learning' and conceptidentification. The apparentimptication is that the commonfacir that af,pearsn iontribute to age'relateddffirences in ma-ny.cognitivemeasuresis quite broad and may refuit a relativefy'generalreduction in central nemoussystemfunctioning. pECENT research has established that age-related inr\ fluences on many different cognitive variables are not independent,but instead thatl0%oor more ofthe age-related variance is shared with other variables (Salthouse, l99}b, 1994b, 1996a). This finding suggeststhat adult age differences in cognition are not exclusively attributable to taskspecific processesbut instead are determined at least partially by broader or more general factors. Variations in these broad factors are unlikely to be responsible for all of the observed age differences in cognitive functioning, but it is important to assessand understandthe contribution of anv general factors that might exist because the role of morl specific factors cannot be accurately determined unless the general influences are first taken into consideration. One approachthat can be used to investigatethe nature of the hypothesizedcommon or general factor is to determine which variables "age together" in the sensethat they share large portions of their age-relatedvariance. That is, to the extent that a variable is found to have considerableoverlap of its age-relatedvariance with the age-relatedvariance in other variables, then it can be inferred to be either a causeor a consequenceof the hypothesizedcommon factor. For example, a number of studies have examined measures of how quickly simple comparison or substitution operations can be executed. Nearly everyone achievesperfect accuracy in these tasks if enough time is allowed, and thus performanceis usually assessedin terms of how quickly the tasks can be completed. Because measuresof performance in tasks of this type have been found to share'75Voor more of the age-relatedvariance from a variety of cognitive measures, speed of processing has been postulated to be centrally involved in the hypothesized common factor (Salthouse,1993, 1994b, 1994c, 1996a,I996b). Recently,however,Lindenbergerand Baltes(1994) have reported that measuresof sensory ability also shared large proportions of age-relatedvariancewith severalmeasuresof cognitive functioning in a sample of adults between 70 and 103 years of age. Furthermore, the relations they reported were apparently not merely a consequenceof difficulty registeringthe stimuli, becausesimilar patternswere evident with measuresfrom three different sensory modalities vision, hearing, and balance. Among the possibilities discussedby these authors was that the sensory and cognitive measureswere related becausethey were both indicators of the hypothesizedcommon factor that has been postulatedto contribute to adult age differences in many measures of cognitive functioning. The goal of the current project was to investigatethe role of sensory ability on age-cognition relations in healthy samples of adults from a younger age range - between approximately 18 and 80 years of age - than the sample studied by Lindenberger and Baltes (1994). (SeeAppendix, Note I ). Only measuresof near visual acuity were examined becauseLindenberger and Baltes (1994) found similar relations with measuresfrom each sensorymodality, and vision is the easiestsensory modality to assess.Moreover, visual acuity was assessedwhile the individuals were wearing their normal corrective lenses, becauseLindenberger and Baltes (1994) found that this measure exhibited stiong relations both with age and with measuresof cognitive functioning. Note that, becausevision is assessedwhen the research participantswere wearing corrective lenses,everyonemight have been expected to have close to optimum acuity if the optical corrections were fully effective in remediating any visual defects. However, the research literature contains many reports of age-related declines in corrected visual acuity (e.g., Burg, 1966; Chapanis, 1950; Fozard, 1990; Gittings & Fozard, 1986; Pitts, 1982). There is some difference of opinion as to the primary factors responsiblefor the age-relatedacuity loss, becauseKline and Schieber(19S5, p. 3l) claim that "Much of the slight to moderate loss in static visual acuity accompanying normal aging appearsto be due to changesin the optic media of the eye," whereas Weale (1982, p. 167) suggeststhat optical factors are responsiblefor only some of the declinesin visual acuity, with the rest attributable to loss of neural cells. When acuity is assessedat relatively close viewing distances, as was the casein the presentstudies, reductionsin the effectivenessof P3r7 P3l8 ET AL. SALTHOUSE accommodation probably also contribute to negative relations between age and visual acuity becauseof a decreased ability to focus on near objects. Regardlessofthe reasonsfor the age-related declines in corrected near visual acuity, however, the visual acuity measureis of interestif it is also relatedto measuresof cognitive functioning becauseit might then be another reflection of the hypothesized common factor. The primary analytical strategy in this project involved partitioning the varianceamong age. vision. and cognitive variables to determine how much variance is shared in various combinations. The goal was to find out which variables "age together" by, in effect, examining the correlations between the age-related effects on different variables. That is, the age-relatedeffectscan be expressedas the squareof the correlation(i.e., the covariance),and then the degree of independenceof the relations between age and different variables can be examined by inspection of the overlap of the age-variablecovariances. Commonalityanalysis(Pedhazur,1982)was the principal method used to accomplish the variance partitioning. When there are two predictors(e.9., age and vision) of a measure of cognitive functioning, three variance proportions are of interestin commonality analysis.Two of theseproportions representunique contributionsof age and of vision, respectively. They can be computedwith hierarchicalregression proceduresand correspondto the increment in Rt associated with one predictor variable after the variance in the other predictorvariablehas beencontrolled.The estimatestherefore representthe variance in the criterion variable associated with one predictor that is independent of the other predictor. These unique variance estimateswould be expected to be high if most of the influences of the predictor were distinct from the other predictor, but they would be expectedto be low if most of the influenceswere shared.The third variance estimate representsthe common variance in the criterion variable that is sharedbetween the two predictors, and is not unique to either. It is computed by subtracting the estimateof the unique contribution of a predictor on the criterion variable from the total effects ofthe predictor on that variable. In the current context, this estimate of shared variancecan be interpretedas the contribution ofthe hypothesizedcommon or generalfactor on the age-relatedeffects in the criterion variables. An extension of commonality analysis proposed by Salthouse(1992b, 1994b) was also used to expressthe ratio of shared to total age-related variance in the form of a correlation coefficient. The traditional Pearson productmoment correlation reflects the square root of the ratio of sharedto total variance for all of the variance in the variables, and the partial correlation controlling for age conesponds to the square root of the shared to total ageindependent variance in the variables. In contrast, the quasi-partial correlation is the squareroot of the ratio of the sharedto total age-related variance. It will be high if much of the relation betweenthe variablesis becauseof a common factor associatedwith both variablesand with age, and it will be low if most of the age-relatedinfluences are unique. Commonality and quasi-partial correlation analyseswere conducted both with cognitive measures and with speed measures as the criterion variables. Speed measures are interestingbecauseprevious researchhasrevealedthat speed measuressharea large proportion of the age-relatedvariance with many cognitive measures(e.g., Bors & Fonin, 1995; Bryan & Luszcz, 1996;'Graf & Uttl, 1995;Hertzog, 1989; Lindenberger,Mayr, & Kliegl, 1993;Nettelbeck& Rabbitt, 1992; Salthouse, 1992a, 1993, 1994a, 1994c, 1996a, 1996b;Schaie,1989, 1990). Analyses from three separatedata setsare reported in this article. Two data setswere from studiesconductedfor other purposes, but some of those data were amenable to the presentanalysesbecausethe participantsspanneda wide age rangeand measuresof visual acuity and speedwere obtained from every participant. The other tasks in thesestudieswere not traditional cognitive tasks and thus only the speedmeasuresfrom those studiesare reported here. The third data set is from a new study with the samefour speedmeasuresas in StudiesI and2 and also threemeasuresof working memory and measuresfrom an associativelearning task and from a cation task. concept-identifi Studies I and 2 The purposeof the analysesin the initial two studies was to investigatethe role of vision on the relations between age and relatively simple measuresof processingspeed. Of particular interest was whether strong negative relations betweenage and correctednear visual acuity would be found in samplesof healthy adultsbetweenapproximatelyl8 and 80 years of age and the degree to which the age-related variance in the measuresof processing speed was shared with the age-relatedvariancein the vision measure. METHoD Subjects.- Participantsin thesestudiesconsistedof 77 and 127adults,respectively,in StudiesI and 2. Descriptive characteristicsof the participantsare summarizedin Table l, where it can be seen that nearly all of them reported themselvesto be in good to excellent health. (More details about the participantsare provided in the complete reports of these studies; Salthouse, Hambrick, Lukas, & Dell, in press; Meinz & Salthouse,1996). Procedure. - Visual acuity was assessedby means of a near-vision eye chart held at a distance of approximately 30 cm in a room with normal (uncontrolled) ambient illumination. The chart (Scalae Typographicae Birkhauseri, Birkhauser Verlag, Basel) containedboth Landolt C and two-digit number stimuli in 10 different font sizes conespondingto Snellenacuity ratiosof .l to 1.0. The assessment consistedof asking researchparticipantsto read the numbers or state the direction of the gap in the C with each type of stimulus, first with the left eye covered and then again with the right eye covered. The Snellenratio correspondingto the smallest font size at which this could be accomplishedwith fewer than two errorsout of the 8 to l6 items at eachfont size was identified as the visual acuity estimate.Participantsused any correctivelensesthey had availableduring the testing, VISION -C OGNITI ON RELATI ONS P319 Table l. Characteristicsof Participantsin Studies I and 2 Study I Variable n Age VoFemale Education Health Rating I Health Rating 2 Health Satisfaction Health-RelatedLimitations CardiovascularSurgery HypertensionMedications Head Injury NeurologicalTreatment Vision - Right Eye Vision - Left Eye Synonym Vocabulary Antonym Vocabulary PerceptualSpeed Letter Comparison Pattem Comparison ReactionTime Digit-Digir Digit-Symbol Mean SD Study2 Age r 't'1 50.5 53.2 15.6 2.1 2.4 2.3 1.6 0.06 o.26 0.05 0.08 0.48 0.50 7.2 6.5 9.7 16.2 742 1552 l; 2.6 0.9 0.9 0.8 0.8 o.25 o.44 0.22 0.2'7 o.22 o.24 2.5 3.1 -.l8 .l'7 .19 .17 .24 .16 .53* .01 -. l0 -.56* -.62* .22 .07 2.4 3.4 -.36* -.54* 203 394 .35* . 6 1+ Mean SD t2'7 45.9 66.l 15.5 l)_ / 1.9 2.1 2.2 1.5 0.04 0 . ll 0.05 0.M 0.5'7 0.54 6.9 6.4 10.0 r'1.2 750 1523 Age r 2.9 0.9 0.8 0.7 0.7 0.20 0.31 0.21 0.20 0.26 0.25 2.6 2.8 -.16 -.06 .05 -.02 .ll 2.8 _.46* -.43* ). t t@ .t-i-) -. ll .32* .02 -.14 -.64* .tl .06 .42* .5'7* y'Vote.' Education is number of yearsof formal educationcompleted, and Health Rating, Health Satisfaction, and Health-RelatedLimitations are ratings on a 5-point scale where lower numbers indicate better health. Responses to the Cardiovascular Surgery, Hypertension Medications, Head Injury, and Neurological Treatment items were Yes/No, and thus the meansconespond to the proportion of individuals reporting a positive response. Vision scores are the averageof the Snellen ratios for the number and Landolt C stimuli. Scoresin the Vocabulary and PerceptualSpeedtests are number of items correct, and scores in the Reaction Time tasks are in msec. *P< .ol. but we have no informationaboutthe recency,or accuracy, of their optical correction. Although this particular visual acuity test has not been widely used in the United States,it has severaladvantages for the currentpurposes.First, and most important, the test is from the same set of acuity tables used by Lindenberger and Baltes (1994) and Baltes and Lindenberger(1995, in press), and therefore we can examine the replicability of their results with a very similar assessmentinstrument. Second,unlike many acuity tests,two typesof stimulus(2digit numbersand Landolt C) are presented,and thereforeit is possible to determine whether the results are specific to a particulartype of stimulus.Third, the stimuli are calibrated in equal Snellenratiosfrom 0.1 to 1.0 in stepsof 0.1, and thus there is a wide range of sensitivity within the normal population. And fourth, the acuity estimatesfrom this test were found to correlate .91 with the estimatesfrom a more traditionalvisual acuity test (i.e., the LighthouseNear Visual Acuity Test, Modified ETDRS with Sloan Letters)in a sampleof 19 individuals. Two of the speedtaskswere administeredwith paper-andpencil procedures. The letter comparison task consisted of the presentationof pairs of three, six, or nine letters, with approximately half of the pairs differing in the identity of one letter. The participant was instructed to write an "S" (for same) or a "D" (for different) on a line between the numbers of the pair and to work as many of the items as possiblewithin 30 sec. The pattern comparisontest was very similar except that the pairs consistedof patterns composed of three, six, or nine line segments.Each test beganwith a pagecontaining severalsampleitems, and then was administered in two separatelytimed (30 sec) sections.The score in each section was the number of items marked correctly minus the number of items marked incorrectly, and the averageof the two scoresservedas the primary performance measur9. The digit-digit and digit-symbol reaction time tasks were administeredon computers. Trials in each task consistedof the presentationof a code table at the top of the computer screenand a pair of probe items in the middle of the screen. In the digit-digit task, the code table contained nine pairs of identical digits and hence was superfluous, but in the digitsymbol task, it contained nine digits each paired with a different symbol. Probe items consistedof pairs of digits in the digitdigit task and pairs of a digit and a symbol in the digit-symbol task. Researchparticipants were instructed to press the " 1" key on the keyboard if the members of the probepair were the same(i.e., either physically identical in the digitdigit task or associationallyequivalent in the digitsymbol task), and to press the "2" key on the keyboard if the membersof the pair were different. A practice block of l8 trials precededthe experimental block of90 trials in each task. Because accuracy averaged over 95Vo, the median reaction time servedas the primary measureof performance in thesetasks. No constraintson viewing distance were imposed in any ET AL. SALTHOUSE P320 of the tasks. However, the visual anglesat a viewing distanceof 45 cm were approximately two degreesfor the letter comparison and pattern comparison stimuli, and four degreesfor the digit-digit and digit-symbol stimuli. ANDDISCUSSION RESULTS Age-VisionRelations The visual acuity scoreswith the Landolt C and with the two-digit number stimuli were highly correlated with one another (i.e., r's > .7), and thus the averageof the two scores was used as the visual acuity estimate for each eye. The vision scoresacrossthe two eyes were also moderately to highly correlatedwith one another(r : .82 in Study l, r : .49 in Study 2), and thus the averageacrossthe two eyes was used as a compositevision score (seeAppendix, Note 2). Estimatedreliability of the compositevision scorewas computed by determining the partial correlation betweenthe scoresfor the two eyescontrolling for age and then boosting that value by the Spearman-Brown formula. The resulting estimateswere .87 in Study I and .59 in Study 2. Because the results of the analysesreported below were very similar with the visual acuity scorein eacheye servingas the vision measure, the aggregation across eyes primarily serves to increasethe reliability of the vision measure(seeAppendix, Note 3). Figure I portrays the relations between age and the composite vision measurein Studies I and 2. It is apparentthat there were strong negative age relations on the corrected near-vision acuity measurein samplesranging from l8 to 80 yearsof age. Regression analyses revealed that the quadratic (agesquared)term was significant in both Study I and Study 2 and was responsiblefor an additional6.6Voof the variancein Study I and an additional 3.0Voof the variance in Study 2. Separateanalyses on the subgroups above and below the median age indicated that the nonlinear effects were attributable to a smaller age relation at older ages. Neither the gendermain effect nor the interaction of Age X Gender was significantin either study. The influence of health measureson the relation between age and vision was examined by conducting a principal componentsanalysison the eight health measures(seeTable l), and then controlling the variancein the componentscores before examining the relationship between age and vision. The principal componentsanalysisof the health variables in Study I indicated that two components had eigenvalues greater than 1.0. The first component had high loadings on all health variables except for reports of head injury and of treatment for neurological disorders and was correlated .29 with age. The secondcomponent had high loadings on the head injury and neurological treatment variables and was correlated -. 14 with age. The R2 associatedwith age in predictionof the compositevision index was .379, and this was reducedto .293 after control of factor I and to .376 after control of factor 2. In Study 2 the principal components analysis revealed three componentswith eigenvaluesgreater than l. The first had major loadings from the self-rated heath variables, the second had a high loading from the cardiovascular surgery variable, and the third had a high loading from the report of neurological treatmentvariable. Correlationsof age with the componentswere .05, -.01, and -. 10, respectively.The age-related variance in the composite vision measure was .510, and it was reducedto .507 after control of the first component;it was reducedto .509 after control ofthe second Study2 Study1 . Y = .901- .0080q,12=.379 0.9 .o 0.9 0.8 o.7 a 0.6 6 a o \ 0.8 . \ - . . \ . . .. . \ \. o.7 . . \ \ . .. . . 0.6 .\\ - . \ (6 E y - 1 . t.049- .010(X),r2 .. 0.5 0.5 f o o a a a a 0.4 c -9 o 0.3 0.3 6 0.2 o.2 0.1 0.1 a .:.\ a a 20 30 40 50 60 70 Age Chronological 80 20 30 40 50 60 70 Age Chronological Figure l. Relation between composite visual acuity score and age in Studies I and 2. Each point representsa different individual. 80 VISI ON-COGNITI ON RELATI ONS component, and it increasedto .516 aftercontrol of the third component. The results of the analysesjust describedsuggestthat the observedrelations between age and vision are not mediated by poorer health, at least as health is assessedwith the relatively crude self-report measuresin these studies. Similar analyseswith control ofthe variable ofyears ofeducation also resulted in little reduction of the age-ielatedvariance in the composite vision measure.That is, after the amount of educationvariable was statistically controlled, the R, for age was reducedfrom .379 to .37| in Study I , and from .5 10io .479 in Study 2. Age-SpeedRelations The initial analysis on the speed measuresconsisted of computing correlations, partial correlations, and quasi_ partial correlations between pairs of speed measures. In Study I the absolute magnitude of the correlations ranged from .34 to .65, the rangefor the partial correlationswas .24 to .59, and the range for the quasi-partial correlations was .71 to .86. In Study 2 the rangesof the absolutevalueswere .27 to .63 for the correlations, .10 to .54 for the partial correlations,and .58 to .91 forthe quasi-partialcorrelations. The relatively large values of the quasi-partial correlations indicate that a substantialproportion of the sharedvariance between pairs of speed variables was also related to age. However, it should be noted that one reasonthe quasi-partial correlationsare larger than the other correlationsis that all of the age-relatedvariance was reliable, whereas some of the total variance and of the age-independentvariance was due to error and hencewas not systematic. A composite speed index was created by subtracting the average z-score for the digitdigit reaction time and digir symbol reactiontime measures(r : .65 in Study I and r = .50 in Study 2) from the average z-score for the letter comparison and pattern comparison measures(r : .49 in Study I and r : .63 in Study 2). Note that the subtraction reflects the fact that the reaction time measuresare scaledin time per item, whereasthe comparison measuresare scaled in items per time. This composite speedindex served as an additional speedmeasurein the subsequentanalyses. Influence of Vision on Age-SpeedRelations Tests for the Age x Vision interactionwere conductedby enteringthe cross-productterm after the ageand vision term's in the multiple regression equations with the five speed measuresas criterion variables. Only one of the interaction tgrms (i.e., on Digit Symbol ReactionTime in Study 2) was significantat the specified(cr : .Ol) significancelevel, and therefore there is little evidence that the relations between vision and speedvaried as a function of age. Table 2 contains commonality estimates of the proportions of variance in the speed measures associated with different predictors. Note that the proportion of variance in the speedmeasuresunique to vision was near zero for all five speed measures. This indicates that there was little ageindependentrelation between vision and speed and implies that almost all of the relation between vision and speedwas attributable to the age variation. The estimatesof the variance unique to age ranged from P32l 46 to 66Voof the total age-relatedvariance in Study l, and from 38 to6l%o in Study 2.The percentageofthe total agerelated variance common to vision averaged43Vo instudy I and 53Vo in Study 2. It can therefore be concluded tirat approximately half of the age-relatedvariance in the current speed measures is shared with a measure of near-vision acuity. Although only about 5O7oof the age-relatedinfluenceson speedwere sharedwith the vision measure,it is nevertheless important to note that there were strong negative relations betweenage and correctednear-visual acuity. and moderate correlations between the visual acuity meisure and speed measuresobtained under high visibility conditions. The next study was thereforeconductedto determinewhether similar, or possibly even larger, relations of vision would be evident with measuresfrom higher-order cognitive tasks. Study 3 Previousresearchhas indicated that processingspeedis a major factor in the age relations on any cognitive measures (e.g., Bors & Forrin, 1995;Bryan &Luszcz,1996; Graf & Uttl, 1995; Hertzog, 1989; Lindenberger,Mayr, & Kliegl, 1993; Nettelbeck & Rabbitt, t992; Salrhouse,1992a, 19i3, 1994a, 1994c, 1996a,I 996b; Schaie, I 989, I 990). The question of interest in this study was whether the age-reiated variancethat is sharedwith speedis uniqueor whetherit is also sharedwith measuresof vision. If the laffer is the case, this would suggestthat a common factor reflectingrelatively broad centralnervoussystemfunctioning may be responsiblefor the mediationof age-relatedcognitive differences. The tasks administeredin this study consistedof the same four speed tasks used in Studies I and Z and, in addition. three working memory tasks and two tasksassessinghigherorder cognitive functioning. Two of the working memory tasks, reading span and computation span, have been used in several previous studies (Salthouse & Coon, 1994; Salthouse& Meinz, 1995). The zback task was basedon a task originally described by Kay (in Welford, 1958) and Kirchner (1958). It consistedof the presentationof a series of randomly selected digits with the participant asked to report the digits n back in the sequence.Values of n equal to 0, l, and 2 were usedin this study. The two higher-order cognitive tasks were associative learning (Salthouse, 1994a) and a computer-administered version of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST; Heaton, Chelune, Talley, Kay, & Curtiss, 1993). These particular cognitive tasks are of special interest because both yield measuresofperseveration responsesthat have been found to increase in frequency with increasing age. Although the increase in perseveration responses with increased age seemswell established,particularly for the WCST, there are two important questions about this phenomenon. First, are perseveration measuresfrom different tasks highly conelated, as would be expected if they reflect a common construct?And second, are the age differences in perseveration responsesmediated by age-related differences in working memory, as might be expected if they are attributable to a failure to effectively process feedback information (cf., Salthouse, 1994a)2 It should be possible to answer these ET AL. SALTHOUSE P322 Table2. CommonalityEstimatesfor SpeedMeasures,StudiesI and2 Criterion Predictor Unique to Vision Unique to Age Common to Age & Vision Total Studyl(n:77) Letter Comparison Age Vision .051 .051 .1 3 1 .051 .1 2 8 .128 .294 .129 005 .065 .065 .r20 .070 .000 .125 .126 .376 .126 .000 .t44 .144 .345 .144 .003 .1 3 0 .1 3 0 .2to .1 3 3 .019 .t42 .t42 .182 .l6l .006 .t22 .122 .1 7 8 .128 .004 .t32 .132 .329 .1 3 6 .000 Pattern Comparison Age Vision 166 Age Vision .055 Age Vision 250 Age Vision 201 DigirDigit ReactionTime Digit-Symbol Reaction Time Composite Study2(n: 127) Letter Comparison Age Vision .080 Age Vision .040 Ag" Vision .056 Age Vision .t97 Age Vision .144 Pattem Comparison Digit-Digit ReactionTime Digit-symbol ReactionTime Composite questions with data from a study in which the participants performed a battery of working memory and associative learning tasks in addition to the WCST. The data in this study were examined with two sets of commonality analyses.The first setof analyseswas identical to those in Studies I and2, with age and vision as predictors of the speedmeasures.The secondset of analysesinvolved threepredictors(i.e., age,vision, and speed)ofthe working memory and cognitive measures.The goal in theseanalyses was to determine whether the age-related variance shared with speed and cognition was the same as the age-related variance shared with vision and cognition. If so, then this result would be consistentwith the common factor interpretation. If not, then separatespeed and vision influences on the age differences in cognition would presumably need to be postulated. METHOD Subjects.- The sample consistedof 197 adults between l8 and g2years ofage. None ofthe individualshad partici- .l)-, .005 -z)J .177 .238 pated in either of the previous studies.Descriptive characteristics of the participants are summarizedin Table 3, where it can be seenthat most of the participantsreportedthemselves to be in good to excellent health, and had attendedcollege for an averageoftwo to three years. Procedure. - All participants performed the following sequenceof tasks in a single sessionof approximately two hours. The tasks included letter compiuison' pattern comparison, synonym vocabulary, antonym vocabulary, digitdigit reaction time and digirsymbol reaction time (in counterbalancedorder), sentencespan, computation span, nback with n equal to 0, l, and 2 (in counterbalancedorder), WCST, and associativelearning. computer-administered pattern comparison, digit-digit comparison, The letter reaction time, and digitsymbol reaction time tasks were identical to those administeredin Studies I and 2. The same vocabulary tests from the earlier studies were also used in this study and consisted of 10 four-alternative multiple choice items for both the synonym and antonym tests. The reading spanand computation spantasks were identi- VISI ON -C OGNITION RELATI ONS P323 Table 3. Characteristics of Participants in Studv 3 Age Group Variable I 8-39 40_59 60_92 Age r n Age VoFemale 67 30.0 (6.4) 64.2 r4.8 (2.3) 2.0 (0.e) 2.2 (0.9) 2.3 (0.8) 1.5 (1.0) (0) 0 0 . 0 3( 0 . 1 7 ) 0 . 0 4( 0 . 2 1 ) 0.09(0.29) 0.69 (0.22) 0.'72(0.20) s.3 (2.8) 4.8 (3.0) 68 s0.5 (6.0) 61.8 ts.t (2.3) 2.1 (0.9) 2.3 (0.8) 2-4 (O.7) 1.7 (0.8) 0 . 0 1( 0 . 1 2 ) 0 . 1 6( 0 . 3 7 ) 0.t2 (0.32) 0.09(0.2e) 0.4s(0.23) o.46(0.2t) 6.8 (3.0) 6.1 (3.4) 62 69.8 (7.0) 46.8 14.8 (3.2) 2.0 (0.9) 2.3 (0.8) 2.3 (0.8) 1.8 (0.9) 0 . 1 3( 0 . 3 4 ) 0.37(0.49) 0. l l (0.32) (0) 0 0 . 3 5( 0 .l s ) 0 . 3 4( 0 . 1 6 ) 1.3 (2.9) 6 .r ( 3 . 3 ) -.03 .00 .ll .o4 .20* .29* .39* .ll -. l5 *.59* -.62* .29* .16 Education Health Rating I Health Rating 2 Health Satisfaction Health-RelatedLimitations Cardiovascular Surgery HypertensionMedications Head Injury Neurological Treatment Visual Acuity - Right Eye Visual Acuity - Left Eye Synonym Vocabulary Antonym Vocabulary Nole.'Educationis numberof yearsof formal educationcompleted,and HealthRating, HealthSatisfaction,and Health-RelatedLimitationsareratingson a 5-point scale where lower numbers indicate better health. Responsesto the CardiovascularSurgery, Hypertension Medications, Head lnjury, and Neurological Treatment items were Yes/No, and thus the meansconespond to the proportion of individuals reporting a positive response.Vision icores are the averageof the Snellen ratios for the number and Landolt C stimuli. Scoresin the Vocabulary tests are number of items correct. *p < .ol. cal to those used in earlier studiesby Salthouseand Coon (1994) and Salthouseand Meinz (1995). Eachconsistedof a practice set of trials followed by two experimental blocks with different items in each set. Trials in the reading span task involved the presentationof a short sentenceaccompanied by a question and three alternative answers. The researchparticipant was instructedto usethe arrow keys on the keyboard to position an arow in front of the correct answer to the questionwhile also rememberingthe last word in the sentence.After selecting the answer to the questions, a prompt appeared, requesting the participant to recall the targetwords by typing them on the keyboard. The number of sentences (and to-be-remembered words) increased to a maximum of nine as long as the participant was correct on both the comprehensionquestion and the recall on at least two of the three trials at eachlist length. The spanestimate was the largestnumberof items at which the participantwas correct on both the comprehensionand the recall on at least two of three trials. The computation span task was very similar to the reading spantask, except that it consistedof arithmetic problems insteadof sentences,and the items to be rememberedwere digits insteadof words. The nback task involved the presentationof a sequenceof l0 to 12 (i.e., n + 10)digits on the computerscreenwith the participantinstructedto type the digit 0, l, or 2 itemsback in the sequence.Each digit appearedfor 1.5 sec, and the appropriateresponsehad to be enteredwithin that interval to be counted as correct. Participantsreceived practice in each of the three conditions (i.e., n : 0, l, and 2) before performing a total of six trials in each condition, with the conditionspresentedin a counterbalanced order (i.e., 0-l-22-l-O). The n : 0 condition was primarily a control condition becausethere was no storagerequirementwhen the digit to be typed was currently on the screen.Performancecould be less than maximum (l00Vo) in this condition becauseof confusionaboutthe instructionsand/ordifficulty in locating the responsekeys and respondingwithin the 1.5-secinterval. The influence of these factors on performance in the otherconditionswasexaminedby computingthe residualsin p r e d i c t i o n otfh e n : I a n dn : 2 scoresfromamultiple regressionequation after controlling for the n : 0 score. However, becausethese residualswere highly correlated with the raw scores(i.e., .83 for n : I and .90 for n : 2), only the raw scoreswere usedin subsequentanalyses. A computer-administered version of the Wisconsin Card SortingTest was usedto presentthe WCST. (The computer program was developedby John L. Woodard, who kindly allowed us to use it in this study). The standardversionof this testconsistsof a setof four stimuluscardsand I 28 response cards, which are to be sorted into the appropriate stimulus categoryaccordingto principles(i.e., on the basisof color, form, and number) that had to be discovered, and which changedthroughout the test. Insteadofpresenting the stimulus and response items as cards, in the computeradministered version they were displayed as boxes on the computer screen.A responsecard was sorted into the appropriate category by typing a number from I to 4 corresponding to the stimulus item below which the response card should be placed. The responsecard then appearedunderneath the stimulus card and both auditory (i.e., tones of different frequencies) and visual (i.e., "Right" or "Wrong") feedback was presented. The two measuresof primary interest in this test were the number of categories (out of a maximum of six) successfullycompleted,and the percentageof perseverativeerrors in which the participant continued to respondto a previous category after the sorting principle had changed. The associativelearning task was very similar to the tasks ET AL. SALTHOUSE P324 described by Salthouse (1994a). An initial practice block with two pairs of stimuli was presented, followed by two blocks of six trials each with six symbol pairs as stimuli. Trials in the task consisted of the presentation of a single stimulus item on the left of the screen and a column of six responseitems on the right of the screen.The responsewas selectedby using arrow keys to position an ilrow in front of the designated response item, after which feedback was presented in the form of an auditory signal and visual highlighting of the correct response term. A variety of detailed performancemeasurescan be derived from this task (seeSalthouse,1994a),but the two of primary interestin this study were the percentage of correct responses and the percentageof perseveration responsesin which the same incorrect responseto a stimulus was repeatedon successive trials. As in Studies I and 2, participants viewed the stimuli without constraints;thereforeviewing distancewas not controlled. However, visual angles for the target stimuli at a viewing distanceof 45 cm were approximately 4 degreesfor the items in the digit-digit, digit-symbol, and associative learning tasks, 2 degreesfor the charactersin the reading spanand computation spantasks, l4 degreesfor the digits in the nback task, and 6 degreesfor the individual symbols and "cards" in the computer-administered 24 degrees for the WCST. RBsulrs ANDDISCUSSIoN Age Relations Means, standard deviations, age correlations and estimated reliabilities of the performancemeasuresare summa- rized in Table 4. All variables were significantly related to age except for the WCST perseverativeerror measure, and the reliability estimateswere all in the moderaterangeexcept for the associative learning perseverative erors measure. Becausethe WCST was administeredonly once, no reliability estimates could be computed for the measures in this task. The age relationships on the measuresof performance in the associative learning task were similar to those from two studiesreported in Salthouse(1994a) where the age correlations were -.41 and -.30 for the percentagecorrect measure and .36 and .20 for the perseverationelror measure.The age relations on the WCST measuresin this study were smaller than those in a recent study (Salthouse, Fristoe, & Rhee, 1996)using the traditional card version ofthe test. That is, in the earlier study the age correlations were -.41 for the number of categoriesmeasureand .43 for the perseveration measure.It is not clear whether the smaller age relations in the current study are attributable to differences in the format ofthe test, to sample differences, or to factors related to the other tests administeredprior to the WCST. Other measuresfrom the associativelearning task and the WCST were also examined. Unlike earlier studies (Salthouse, 1994a), the measureof percentageforgetting in associativelearning had a low (r : .10) and nonsignificant correlation with age in this sample. The percentageof conceptuallevel responsesin the WCST had a correlation of -.20 with age, but it was largely redundant with the other WCST measures because it was correlated .91 with the number of categoriesmeasureand -.80 with the percentage of perseverationerrors measure. The correlation betweenage and the WCST perseveration Table 4. PerformanceMeasuresin Studv 3 AgeGroup Variable l 8-39 40-59 60-92 Age r -.46x -.54* Estimated Reliability' PerceptualSpeed Letter Comparison Pattem Comparison 10.5 (2.9) 1 8 l. ( 3 . 6 ) 8.8 (2.9) 1 5 . s ( 3 .l ) '7.3 (2.6) 13.2 (3.6) Reaction Time Digit-Digit Digit-Symbol 698 (148) l30l (317) 786 (l9l) rs72 (329) 852 (184) l80s (476) 3.9 (2.2) 2.6 (r.4) 3.7 (2.0) 2.3 (l.l) 3.0 (2.2) 2.0 (1.2) .21* -.23* .80 NBack 0-Back l-Back 2-Back 8 4 . 7( 1 8 . s ) 66.8 (33.7) 42.r (29.0) ^14.o(22.4) s'7.r (28.'t) 34.9 (22.7) 6t.3 (21.9) 5 1 . 9( 3 1 . 5 ) 32.4 (22.6) _.41* -.25* -.20* .68 .88 .91 Associative Leaming VoConect 7o PerseverationError 4 0 . 3( 1 6 . 3 ) 13.4 (8.4) 30.9 (13.4) 18.6 (e.6) 3 0 . 3( 1 s . 5 ) l7.7 (8.5) -.33* .25* .74 .47 Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Number of Categories 7o PerseverationError 4.o (2.2) 20.3 (12.6\ 3.6 (2.2) 2 0 . 0( 1 1 . 2 ) 2.7 (2.t) 24.0 (r2.O) -.23* .09 Working Memory Computation Span Reading Span .33+ .50* .80 .82 .66 .96 -t) "Estimatedreliability is computed by boosting the partial conelation (controlling for age) between the scoreson the two administrations of the test by the Spearman-Brown formula. *p <.01. VI SI ON-COGNITI ON RELATIONS enor measure was small (r : .09), and the perseverative measures from the associative learning and WCST tasks were weakly related to each other (r : .16). The low correlation between the two perseveration measures provides little evidencefor a common perseverationconstruct. z-scores for the four working memory measures. In both cases, higher scores in the composite measures corre_ spondedto better performance. Hierarchical regression analyses were next conducted with the working memory measuresas the criterion variables and age and the composite speedmeasureas predictors. The age-relatedvariance in the working memory measureswas significantly greaterthan zero for all measureswhen age was the only predictor, but it did not differ significantlf from zero when age was consideredafter control of the composite speedmeasure.For example, the proportions of age-related variance for the composite working memory measurewere .084 for age alone, and .000 for the increment in R2associated with age after control of the speedindex. These results are very similar to thosefrom severalearlier studiesin which processingspeedhas been postulatedto mediate age-related influences on untimed measuresof working memory (e.g., Salthouse, 1992a; l994d; Salthouse & Babcock, t99t; Salthouse& Meinz, 1995). Table 6 contains the results of hierarchical regression analyseswith cognitive measuresas the criterion variables and age and the composite speed and composite working memory measuresas predictor variables. Notice that there was substantialreduction in the age-relatedvariance ofeach criterion variable after control of the composite working SpeedandWorking Memory Table 5 contains correlations, partial conelations, and quasi-partialcorrelationsfor the speedand working memory measures. In all cases, the product-moment correlations, representing shared total variance, were somewhat higher than the partial correlations, representing shared igeindependentvariance. The quasi-partial correlations, repielgnling shared age-relatedvariance, were consistently the highest. This pattern indicates that the variables shared a large proportion of their age-related variance, but much smaller proportions of their total variance or of their ageindependentvariance. Composite speed and working memory variables were formed for later analyses.The compositespeedmeasurewas created by subtracting the average of the z-scores for the digirdigit reaction time and digit-symbol reaction time measuresfrom the averageof the z-scoresfor the letter comparison and pattern comparisonmeasures.The compositeworklng memory measure was formed from the average of the Table 5. Correlations Between SpeedMeasuresand Between Working Memory Measures, Study 3 Pearson Correlation Variables Digit-Digit-DigirSymbol Digit-Digit-l-etter Comparison Digit-Digit-Pattem Comparison Digit-Symbol-L,etter Comparison Digit-Symbol-Pattem Comparison [ftter Comparison-Pattern Comparison Partial Correlation .68 -.40 -.49 -.& -.64 .58 .45 .36 .3l, .41, .40 .77 Computation Span-Reading Span Computation Span-Nback- l. Computation Span-Mack- I Reading Span-Mack-l Reading Span-Mack-2 Mack-l-Mack-2 Quasi-Partial Correlation .63 .90 -.15 -.81 -.91 -.91 .87 _.JU -.39 -.53 -.51 .44 .42 .83 .Ja .t) .28 .37 .37 .'76 .71 .'t9 .79 .96 Table 6. Increment in R'Associated with SuccessivePredictors in Hierarchical RegressionAnalyses, Study 3 Associative [,eamine Equation Predictors 7o Conecl 7o Perseveration Responses No. of Categories Achieved 7o Perseveration Responses I Age .108* .062* .055* .009 2 WorkingMemory Age .279* .033* .124* .024 .231x .010 .136* .000 Speed Age .165* .015 .069* .016 .058* .002 Speed WorkingMemory Age .165* .l 3 l * .068* .062* .018 .058* .080* .001 *p < .ol. .0r9 ET AL. SALTHOUSE P326 memory measure. However, the reduction of age-related variancewas also considerableafter control of the composite speed measure, and the residual age-related variance was nearly the same after control of only the speed measureas after control of both the speed and working memory measures. The finding that a large proportion of the age-related variance in measuresof working memory and higher-order cognitive functioning from tasks without time limits is shared with simple measures of processing efficiency is consistentwith the resultsof numerousrecentstudies(e.g., Lindenberger et al., 1993; Salthouse, 1992a, 1993, 1994a, 1994c.1996a). when age was the only predictor and was .385 after control of the self-rating component, .327 after control of the cardiovascularcomponent, and .299 after control of the neurological component. There was some reduction in the relations between age and vision after control of the health variables, particularly after control of the variance in measures of reports of head injury and treatment of neurological disorder. However, this is a somewhatdifferent pattern than that observedin Study I and may simply reflect sampling variation. There was little reduction of the age-relatedvariance in the compositevision measureafter control of the number of yearsof education(i.e., from .405 to .401). Age-VisionRelqtions The estimatedreliability of the compositevision measure, computed in the same manner describedin Studies I andZ, was .80. The relationbetweenage and the compositevision measureis portrayed in Figure 2, where it can be seen that the parametersof the regressionequation were very similar to those in Studies I and 2. Tests of the quadratic age trend, of the gender main effect and of the Age x Gender interaction indicated that none were significant. The influence of health measureson the age-vision relations was examinedin the samemanneras in Studies I and2. The principal componentsanalysis on the eight health measures yielded three components with eigenvalues greater than 1.0. The components,definedin terms of the variables with the highest loadings (and the correlationsof the components with age), were: self-ratings (.14), cardiovascular (.41), and neurological (-.14). The age-relatedvariance (i.e., R'associatedwith age)in the vision measurewas .405 Influence of Vision on Age-SpeedRelations Tests were conductedfor the interaction of Age x Vision on the speedvariables, but the interaction was not significant for any speed measure. As in Studies I and 2, therefore, there is little evidence that the relation between vision and speedvaries as a function of age. Table 7 contains the commonality estimatesfor the speed criterion measures.Note that there was relatively little variance sharedbetween vision and speedthat was independent of age, but that about one third of the total age-related variancein speedwas independentof vision. The estimates of the common influencein this study were somewhatlarger than those in the earlier studies, but the overall pattern is generallysimilar to that in StudiesI and2. .e € 07 . . . n o r \ . a . a ^ a 0.4 O a \ . oaa - a a a a a o a . a \ o a o a a ..f - . . . . t a o a a a - a a a \aa o \aaa a a aaal \aaa a - a \ a - a a ooa- aaa oa a a - .a . a aa a a a \ o 6 U) . \ . \ . 0.6 (d o Y=.934_.009,r2=.406 a o a a - a a a -a aa a a a l O a a - o a aa |}..a a - \ o a a a a a a o \ -Q a --ao a o a o a a a a 0.1 - 2 0 3 0 4 0 5 0 6 0 7 0 Influence of Vision and Speedon Age-Cognition Relations Interactionsof Age x Vision and Age x Speedwere examined in multiple regressionequationswith the working memory and cognitive measuresas the criterion variables. None of the interactions were significant for any of the cognitive criterion variables, and thus there is no evidence that the relations between vision and working memory or betweenvision and the cognitive measuresvary according to age. Table 8 contains results of the commonality estimates with age, vision, and speed as predictors of the cognitive measures.Notice that a very similar pattern was evident with all measures. The unique contribution of age was quite small, and most of the age-relatedvariance was sharedwith both vision and speed. These results are consistent with the earlier findings that a large percentage of the age-related variancein measuresof working memory and of higher order cognition is sharedwith a measureof speed(also seeTable 6). However, the previous findings are extended by the discovery that vision is also a component of that factor. An average of almost 89Vo of the age-related variance in the working memory and cognitive measureswas shared with both vision and speed. 8 0 9 0 Age Chronological Figure 2. Relation between composite visual score and age in Study 3. Each point representsa different individual. Structural Equation Model The final analysisexamined the fit of a structural equation model with a single common factor postulatedto mediatethe age-relatedinfluences on all speedand cognitive variables' Severalof the measureswere transformed (i.e., the reciprocals of the digirdigit and digirsymbol reaction time values were multiplied by 10,000, and the nback and associative learning percentagecorrect values were divided by l0) to VI SION -C OGNITI ON RELATI ONS Table 7. CommonalityEstimatefor SpeedMeasures,Study 3 (n : Criterion Predictor Unique to Age Unique to Vision P327 197) Common to Age & Vision Total [-etterComparison Age Vision .075 Age Vision .095 Age Vision .0t6 Age Vision .098 Age Vision .096 . 0 r5 PatternComparison .029 t96 t96 291 225 209 209 305 243 Digit-Digit ReactionTime .040 Digit-Symbol ReactionTime . 0 t3 Composite obtain similar variancesof the measuresfor the analysis.A single common factor with relationsfrom age and to all of the variables was then specified, and each variable was examinedto determineif it had a significantrelationdirectly from age. The only variableswith direct relationsfrom age werethe two vision measures.Despitelittle attemptto model relationsamong variables,exceptto allow correlatedresiduals betweenmeasuresderivedfrom the samemethods,the model provided a moderately good {it to the data (1, 1df : 5 9 1: 1 3 5 . 0 6 , S t dR. M R : . O 7 , G F I: . 8 9 , A G F I : . 8 3 , CFI : .94). The model, with significantpath coefficients expressedin standardizedform, is illustratedin Figure 3. General Discussion Two sets of results from the present studies were rather surprising.The first unexpectedresultswere the strong negative relations betweenage and a measureof correctedvisual acuity found in three independentsamples(Figures I and 2). The secondsurprisingsetof resultswas the high proportion of the age-relatedvariancein measuresof speed,working memory, associativelearning, and concept identification that was sharedwith the measuresof vision (Tables 2, 7, and 8). Although similar findings were reportedby Lindenbergerand Baltes(1994), their samplewas composedentirely of older adultsand eventhey suspectedthat the relationsofthe sensory measureswould be much reducedin a younger sample. (But note that these researchershave recently extendedtheir research to a wider age range and found similar results; cf., Baltesand Lindenberger, 1995,in press).Furthermore,many investigatorshave screenedresearchparticipants for vision but have not reportedthat large numbersof potential participants were excluded for this reason, and they still found significant age differencesin many cognitive measures(e.g., Hahn & Kramer, 1995; Hartman, 1995; McCalley, Bouwhuis,& Juola, 1995;Paul, 1996).This raisesthe posiibility that there is somethingunusualabout the current vision assessment,and it is certainly true that corrected visual .034 acuity was not measuredunderoptimum conditionsbecause therewas no control over illumination and no restraintswere usedto ensurethat the viewing distancewas exactly 30 cm. Nevertheless,the measureswere generallyquite reliable as evidentin the reliability estimatesand in the strongrelations with other variables.Moreover, the age relationswere also apparentlynot attributableto declining healthbecausethere was relatively little attenuationof the relationsbetweenage and vision when measuresof self-reportedhealth were controlled.Of course,the rangeof healthstatusexaminedin thesestudieswas likely quite limited comparedto the general populationbecausenearly all of the participantsin the currentstudiesreportedthemselvesto be in good to excellent health. It is also important to note that there was little evidence that the relation betweenvision and either the speedor the cognitivemeasuresvariedas a function of age. This conclusion is admittedlybasedon acceptanceof the null hypothesis, but becausethe pattern of a nonsignificantinteraction betweenage and vision was replicatedacrossthree or more measuresin eachof threeindependentstudies,it can probably be treatedwith someconfidence.The relationsinvolving vision therefore do not seem to be attributable to visual pathologiesemergingonly at middle or late adulthood. Becausecorrelationsamong age, measuresof cognitive functioning, and measuresof visual functioning have been reportedby Clark (1960) and Heron and Chown (1967), the data from those studies were reanalyzed to estimate the amount of age-relatedvariance in their cognitive measures that was sharedwith the vision measures.The assessment of vision in the Clark (1960) study was in terms of "near accommodationdistance" but specificdetailsof the stimuli or viewing distance were not provided. Analyses of the correlationsin the Clark sample of 102 adults between20 and 70 years of age revealedthat this vision measureshared 58.l%oof the age-relatedvariancewith the PMA Reasoning measure and 50.77o of the age-related variance with the PMA Spacemeasure.Heron and Chown (1961) assessed P328 SALTHOUSE ET AL, Table8. CommonalityAnalyseson CognitiveMeasures,Study3 Age Criterion : Computation Span Unique to Age Unique to Vision Unique to Speed Common to Age & Vision Common to Age & Speed Common to Vision & Speed Common to Age, Vision, & Speed Total Criterion = Reading Span Unique to Age Unique to Vision Unique to Speed Common to Age & Vision Common to Age & Speed Common to Vision & Speed Common to Age, Vision, & Speed Total Criterion : Nback-l Unique to Age Unique to Vision Unique to Speed Common to Age & Vision Common to Age & Speed Common to Vision & Speed Common to Age, Vision, & Speed Total Criterion : Mack-2 Unique to Age Unique to Vision Unique to Speed Common to Age & Vision Common to Age & Speed Common to Vision & Speed Common to Age, Vision, & Speed Total Criterion = Associative Leaming, PercentageCorrect Unique to Age Unique to Vision Unique to Speed Common to Age & Vision Common to Age & Speed Common to Vision & Speed Common to Age, Vision, & Speed Total Criterion : WCST, Number of Categories Unique to Age Unique to Vision Unique to Speed Common to Age & Vision Common to Age & Speed Common to Vision & Speed Common to Age, Vision, & Speed Total Vision Speed .006 .02'7 .070 -.006 -.006 -.006 .M9 .o25 .o49 -.006 .025 .M9 .043 .095 .1 3 8 .000 .007 .057 .o02 .008 .002 .041 .013 .(Xl .008 .013 .041 .051 .063 . ll 9 .003 .00t Figure 3. Structural model with a single common factor mediating the age-relatedinfluences on all observed variables. Numbers are standardized coefficients. The variables were: WCST-NC = number of categories achieved in the WCST; AssocPC : percentage correct in associative learning; NB-2 : percentagecorrect in the nback task with n : 2; NB- I : percentage correct in the nback task with n : l; WM-N : working memory with number stimuli (i.e., computationspan);WM-V : working memory with verbal stimuli (i.e., reading span); DSRT : digit symbol reaction time; DDRT : digit-digit reaction time; Patcom : pattern comparison; lrtCom : letter comparison; Vision-R : visual acuity in right eye; and Vision-L : visual acuity in left eye. .l9l -.001 .009 -.001 .054 .016 .054 .009 .016 .054 .065 .070 .2'to .007 .001 .1 8 0 .000 -.003 .000 .035 .015 .035 .039 .051 -.003 .015 .UJ) .227 .001 .026 .o52 .014 .0t2 .014 .081 .o20 .081 .012 .020 .081 .108 .l4l .165 .001 .o23 .045 .o02 .002 .002 .050 .019 .050 .002 .019 .050 .055 .o94 .116 visual acuity with Landolt C stimuli viewed at a distanceof 6 meters with both eyes (uncorrected). Analyses of the relevant correlationsrevealedthat, in their sampleof 300 males, 3l.4%o of the age-relatedvariance in the score on the Raven's ProgressiveMatrices was shared with the vision measure and that 46.OVowas shared in their sample of 240 females. Neither of these studies reported estimatesof the reliability of the vision measures,and thereforeit is possible that the smaller proportions of shared age-relatedvariance than in the current studies are attributable to less reliable assessmentof vision. It is neverthelessimportant to note that similar results indicating that moderateto large proportions of the age-relatedvariance in measuresof cognitive functioning are shared with measuresof visual functioning are apparentin other data sets with different measuresof vision and cognition. Following Lindenberger and Baltes (1994), there seemto be three possible interpretationsof the relation between the vision and cognitive measures.One possibility is that visual deficits are responsible for the cognitive deficits because individuals with low vision may not be able to adequately register the stimuli. Although intuitively reasonable, this interpretationdoesnot seemplausible in the current situation becauseall stimuli were of high contrast and relatively large in terms of visual angle. That is, the smalleststimuli subtended visual angles of about two degrees at a typical viewing distance and should have been easily visible by everyone because all participants had composite vision scoresabove zero. It is also important to note that there was little or no relation between the vision measuresand either the speedor the cognitive measuresthat was independentof age (cf., Tables 2,'1 , and 8). The influenceof vision was VISI ON -C OGNITI ON RELATION S thereforelimited to the aspectsof the speed, working memory, and cognitive tasksthat were affected by increasedage. A secondpossibleinterpretationofthe influenceofvision in thesestudiesis that the visual assessment involves cognitive processes.That is, researchparticipantsneedto comprehend instructions, to maintain concentration, and to have at least a minimum level of motivation to perform well in the visual acuity task. Although it is true that all these aspects are required in the vision test, the cognitive demands are almost certainly substantially less than those in tasks explicitly designedto assesscognitivefunctioning.Ifthe cognitive requirementsin the vision test are responsible for the relations with the other variables, therefore, a very primitive form of cognition must be involved. An interesting, and testable,implication of this interpretationis that the relations to the cognitive measures should be greatly reduced or eliminatedif the visual assessments could be obtainedwith little or no cognitive involvement. A third interpretation of the vision-cognition relations is that the visual acuity measuresare anothermanifestationof a common factor contributing to the age differences in many behavioralvariables.This interpretationdiffers from the first becauseinstead of attempting to attribute causal priority to certain variables, all of the variables are considered to be reflectionsof a common factor that is related to both age and to the variables. To illustrate, although a path model with vision postulated to mediate many of the age-related influenceson the other variables would likely provide a good fit to the data, similar good fits would probably also occur for models based on any variables having moderate to high loadings on the common factor. From this perspective, therefore, it may not be particularly productive to attempt to specify the causal priority among the variables with the currently availableinformation. The key feature of this last interpretation is that the common factor is postulated to contribute to much of the age-relatedinfluenceson a wide rangeof cognitive measures (e.g., Salthouse,1994b;Salthouse, Fristoe,& Rhee, 1996). The novel contribution of the current studies and of the studiesby Lindenbergerand Baltes (1994) and Baltes and Lindenberger(1995, in press)is to suggestthat fairly basic sensory functions are also included within the common factor. That is, the common factor appea$ to reflect primitive central nervous system functions representednot only by measuresof processingefficiency, but also by measures of correctedvisual acuity. However, it should be emphasized that the common factor is not responsible for all observedage-relatedeffects becauseunique or independent age-relatedinfluences were evident on the vision measures in Study 3 (cf., Figure 3), and independentage-related influenceshave been found on measuresof memory in other studies(e.g., Salthouse et al., 1996). It is interestingto consider the implications of the hypothesizedcommon factor for a theorist attempting to accountfor the age differences observed in measuresof working memory or in a cognitive task like associativelearning or concept identification. The theorist could attempt to account for the observedage differences in thesetasks by focusing on fairly specific processes,such as the ability to inhibit irrelevant information, or to shift set when receiving changing feed- P329 back. However, the resultsofthese and other recentstudies indicate that there may be no significant age differences in the to-be-explainedmeasuresafter the researchparticipants are statistically equatedon measuresof speedor measuresof near-visualacuity. These findings imply that the age-related influenceson many cognitive measuresare not independent of the age-relatedinfluenceson measuresof simple processing efficiency and of corrected visual acuity. This in turn indicates that the age-relatedeffects on the cognitive tasks are not restricted to processesspecific to those target tasks. Limiting the explanatory focus to a single task may therefore result in misleading conclusions about the factors responsible for the age-related differences in that task. If, as the results of these and other studies seem to indicate, those differences are not independentof the differences in other tasks, then the theorist may simply be describing one symptom or manifestation of a much broader phenomenon by concentratingexclusively on the results of a single task. Although the researchdescribed above raises at least as many questions as it answers, it does suggestan important priority for future research.That is, a major goal should be to explore the nature of the hypothesized common factor by determining what other combinationsof variablesshareagerelated variance and by determining what variables have independentage-relatedinfluences.The discovery that measuresof sensoryability appearto be involved in the common factor also suggeststhat the range of variables to be examined should be expandedto include noncognitive variables. ACKNowLETTMENTs This researchwas supported by NIA Grant AGR-37 6826 to Timothy A. Salthouse. We would like to acknowledge the valuable contributions of Paul Baltes and Ulman Lindenberger in many phasesof this project and for supplyingcopiesof the visual acuity tests. Address correspondenceto Dr. Timothy A. Salthouse, School of Psychology, GeorgiaInstituteofTechnology, Atlanta, GA 30332-0170. REFERENCES Baltes, P. B., & Lindenberger, U. (1995). Sensorik und Intelligenz: lntersystemische Wechselwirkungen und Veraenderungen im hohen Alter. Akademi e-J ournal, 20-28. Baltes, P. B., & Lindenberger, U. (in press). Emergence of a powerful connection betweensensoryand cognitive functions acrossthe adult life span: A new window on the study of cognitive aging? Psychology and Aging. Bors, D. A., & Forrin, B. (1995). Age, speedof informationprocessing. recall, and fl uid intelligence. Intelligence, 20, 229-248. Bryan, J., & Luszcz, M. A. ( I 996). Speedof information processing as a mediator between age and free recall performance. Psychology and Aging,6,3-9. Burg, A. (1966). Visual acuity as measuredby dynamic and static tests. ' Journal of Applied Psychology, 50, 460-466. Chapanis, A. (1950). Relationships between age, visual acuity, and color vision. Human Biology,22, l-31. Clark, J. W. ( l960). The aging dimension: A factorial analysisof individual differences with age on psychological and physiological measurements. Journal of Gerontology,I 5, 183-187. Fozard,J. L. (1990). Vision and hearingin aging. In J. E. Binen & K. W. Schaie(Eds.), Handbookofthe psychologyofaging (3rded., pp. 150170). San Diego: Academic Press. Gittings, N. S., & Fozard, J. L. (1986). Age changesin visual acuity. Experimental G erontolo gy, 2 1, 423-434. Graf, P., & Uttl, B. (1995). Component processesof memory: Changes acrossthe adult lifespan. SwissJournal of Psychology, 54, ll3-138. Hahn, S., & Kramer, A. F. (1995). Attentional flexibility and aging: You P330 ET AL. SALTHOUSE don't need to be 20 years of age to split the beam. Psychology and Asing, 10,597-609. Hartman, M. (1995). Aging and interference: Evidence from indirect memory tests. Psychology and Aging, 10,659-669. Heaton, R. K., Chelune,G. J., Talley, J. L., Kay, G. G., & Curtiss, G. (1993). Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Manual: Revised and Expanded. Odessa, FL: Psychological AssessmentResources. Heron, A., & Chown, S. M. (1967). Age andfunction. Boston: Little, Brown. Hertzog, C. (1989). Influences of cognitive slowing on age differences in intelligence. Developmental Psychology, 25 , 636-651 . Kirchner, W. K. (1958). Age differences in short-term retention of rapidly changing information. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55, 352-358. Kline, D.W., & Schieber,F. (1985).Vision and aging. In J. E. Birren & K. W. Schaie(Eds.), Handbook of the psychology ofaging (pp.296-331). New York: Van NostrandReinhold. Lindenberger,U., & Baltes, P. B. (1994). Sensoryfunctioning and intelligence in old age: A strong connection. Psychology and Aging, 2, 339-355. Lindenberger,U., Mayr, U., & Kliegl, R. ( 1993).Speedand intelligencein old age.Psychologyand Aging,8,207-220. McCalley, L. T., Bouwhuis, D. G., & Juola, J. F. (1995). Age changesin the distribution of visual attention. Journal of Gerontology: Psychologic a l S c i e n c e s5. 0 8 . P 3 l 6 - P 3 3 1 . Meinz, E. J., & Salthouse,T. A. (1996). Experienceandage effectsonthe recall of visually presented music. Unpublished manuscript. Nettelbeck,T., & Rabbitt, P. M. A. ( 1992).Aging, cognitiveperformance, and mental speed.Intelligence, 16, 189-2O5. Paul, S. T. (1996). Searchfor semanticinhibition failure during sentence comprehensionby younger and older adults. Psychology and Aging, I I , l0-20. Pedhazur, E. J. ( I 982). M ultiple regr ession in behavioral researc h : Explanation and prediction. (2nded.). New York: Holt, Rineha( & Winston. Pitts, D. G. (1982). The effectsofaging on selectedvisual functions:Dark adaptation,visual acuity, stereopsis,and brightnesscontrast. In R. Sekuler, D. Kline, & K. Dismukes (Eds.), Aging and human visual f u n c t i o n( p p . l 3 l - 1 5 9 ) . N e w Y o r k : A l a n R . L i s s . Salthouse, T. A. (1992a). Influence of processing speed on adult age differences in working memory. Acta Psychologica, T9, 155-170. Salthouse,T. A. (1992b). Shifting levelsofanalysis in the investigationof cognitive aging. Human Development, 35, 321-342. Salthouse, T. A. (1993). Speed mediation of adult age differences in cognition. Developmental P sychology, 29, 722-'7 38. Salthouse,T. A. (1994a). Aging associations:Influenceof speedon adult age differences in associative leaming. Journal of Experimental Psy' cholo gy : Learning, M emory and C ognition, 20, 1486- 1503. Salthouse,T. A. (1994b). How many causesare there of aging-related decrements in cognitive functioning? Developmental Review, 14, 413-437. Salthouse,T. A. (1994c). The nature ofthe influence ofspeed on adult age differences in cognition. Developmental P sychology, 30, 240-259. Salthouse, T. A. (1994d). The aging of working memory. Neuropsychology,8, 535-543. Salthouse,T. A. (1996a). General and specific speedmediation ofadult age differences in memory. Journal Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 5tB. P30-P42. Salthouse, T. A. (1996b). The processing speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. P sychological Review, I 03, 403-428. Salthouse,T. A., & Babcock, R. L. (1991). Decomposingadult age differencesin working memory. DevelopmentalPsychology,27,'163-'776. Salthouse,T. A., & Coon, V. E. (1994). Interpretationof differential deficits: The caseof aging and mental arithmetic. J ournal of Experimen' tal Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition,20, 1172-1182. Salthouse,T. A., Fristoe, N., & Rhee, S. (1996). How localizedare agerelated effects on neuropsychological measures?Neuropsychology, 10, 272-285. S a l t h o u s eT, . A . , H a m b r i c k , D . 2 . , L u k a s , K . , & D e l l , T . C . ( I n p r e s s ) . Determinants of adult age differences in synthetic work performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied. Salthouse,T. A., & Meinz, E. J. (1995). Aging, inhibition, working memory, and speed. Journal ofGerontology: Psychological Sciences, 508, P29'7-P306. Schaie, K. W. (1989). Perceptual speedin adulthood: Cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses.P sychology and Aging, 4, 443-453. Schaie, K. W. ( 1990). Correction to Schaie ( 1989). Psychology and Aging, 5, l'71. Weale, R. A. (1982). Senile ocular changes,cell death, and vision. In R. Sekuler, D. Kline, & K. Dismukes (Eds.), Agrng and human visual f u n c t i o n( p p . l 6 l - l 7 l ) . N e w Y o r k : A l a n R . L i s s . Welford, A. T. (1958). Ageing and human skill. London: Oxford University Press. ReceivedOctober I 3, ],995 Accepted April 8, 1996 Appendix Notes l. These researchershave recently extended their sample by adding adults between 25 and 7O years of age (Baltes & Lindenberger, 1995, in press)and have found resultsquite similar to those reported in the Lindenberger and Baltes (1994) study. 2. The correlations were much lower in Study 2 becausesix of the participantsin this study were effectivelyblind in one eye (i.e., they could not report the identity of the items in the largest font size in the vision chart). Elimination of the data from individuals with scores of zero in one eye increased the correlation betweenthe measuresin the two eyes from .49 to .67. For these individuals the eye with the nonzero score was used as the compositevision score. 3. The use of the average score from the two eyes assessed separatelyrather than the scoreof the best eye, or the score from an assessmentwith both eyes together, probably contributed to somewhat lower acuity estimatesthan those reported by other investigatorsbecausethis measureemphasizesthe worst rather than the best measureof acuity. _l I