Predictors of Past Tense Marking by Lexical Aspect Category Brian K. Weiler C. Melanie Schuele Vanderbilt University School of Medicine INTRODUCTION PARTICIPANTS METHODS RESULTS By the age of five, children with typical language skills mark finiteness on past tense at levels approaching mastery (Rice & Wexler, 1996). Prior to mastery, young children sometimes produce bare verb stems in contexts where inflected forms are obligatory in the adult language (e.g., he jump vs he jumped) . A consideration of lexical aspect may help account for why some past-reference verbs are tense marked while others are not. Lexical aspect relates to the temporal properties of situations that individual verbs describe. Fifty-two preschool children, 37 to 61 months, participated in the initial study. Participants demonstrated typical language skills (see Table 2). They were identified from a broader pool of 100 children. Participants were selected for variable past tense marking across sixteen elicited past tense verb targets (i.e., at least one marked and one omitted in obligatory contexts). All children produced word final /d/ and /t/ on a phonological probe. The children were recruited from four preschools. Demographic information received from 83% of the final participant pool indicated that 98% of the children had at least one parent with a college education and that 98% were Caucasian. All participants were monolingual speakers of mainstream English dialect. In the present analysis, the two verb-level predictors (see Table 5) were defined as follows: Stem-final phonology was identified as either alveolar stop consonant or other. Adult lexical log-frequency values for each inflected verb target were determined through use of the CHILDES Parental Corpus (Li & Shirai, 2000; MacWhinney, 2000). Table 6. Hierarchical Multiple Linear Regression Analysis for Predictors of Past Tense Marking on Achievements Table 1. LEXICAL ASPECT CATEGORIES Category Temporal properties Example utterance Achievement [- durative] [+ dynamic] [+ telic] She blew out the candle. Accomplishment [+ durative] [+ dynamic] [+ telic] He tied his shoes. Age in Months SD 48.94 7.01 PPVT-III SS 110.48 11.31 PLS-4 Total SS 116.61 13.07 METHODS The study involved secondary analysis of data collected for an investigation of complex syntax in typical language learners (Schuele, 2006). Child responses to 16 verb targets from the past tense probe of the Rice/Wexler Test of Early Grammatical Impairment (TEGI; Rice & Wexler, 2001) were analyzed. The 16 TEGI verb targets were selected for their lexical aspect categorization as either Accomplishment or Achievement using Shirai and Anderson’s (1995) coding scheme (see Table 3). Category assignment by the first author and a graduate assistant met complete independent agreement. The likelihood of children’s use of past tense morphology in obligatory contexts may be explained, in part, by the additive influence of several properties of the target verb (Owen, 2010). For example, Marchman, Wulfeck, and Ellis Weismer (1999) found that children were more likely to omit past tense morphology on verbs with alveolar stop consonant bare stem phonology and low frequencies for adult past tense use. Past tense morphology also has been shown to be influenced by a combination verb-level phonological (obstruent vs. non-obstruent coda) and lexical aspect (telic vs. atelic) factors (Johnson & Morris, 2007). PURPOSE The current study is a retrospective analysis of past tense marking among typical preschoolers who have not mastered past tense morphology (Weiler & Schuele, 2011). We examined the predictive nature of two factors known to impact variable tense marking, stemfinal verb phonology and adult lexical frequency, on elicited past tense productions across lexical aspect categories differing on the durative temporal property. The purpose of this analysis was to explore whether verb-level predictors of past tense marking behave similarly or differently relative to lexical aspect. clean, climb, made, write, paint, pick, eat, brush, plant, tie Table 4. CREDITED CHILD RESPONSES Correct regular or irregular past He kicked. She made. Over-generalization of regular past She maked. Over-regularization of irregular past She maded. Regular past over-marked He jumped-ed blow 1.785 91.8% make 3.066 92.6% jump 2.009 90.2% tie 1.881 91.8% catch 2.340 87.9% pick 2.286 90.9% kick 1.447 82.1% climb 1.949 88.1% give 2.816 70.7% clean lift* 1.663 15.6% brush She catched-ed 1.322 81.0% -68.94 9.38 -.965** Stem final phonology -72.84 9.44 -1.019** Adult lexical logfrequency value -9.357 7.72 .312 77.1% paint* 1.623 63.0% plant* 1.279 58.8% 1.869 Note. R2 = .931 for Step 1; Δ R2 = .023 for Step 2. **p < .005. Table 7. Hierarchical Multiple Linear Regression Analysis for Predictors of Past Tense Marking on Accomplishments 52.8% β B SE B Stem final phonology -24.56 4.94 -.869** Stem final phonology -22.455 3.93 -.795** 9.25 3.73 .346* Step 1 80.5% 2.544 Step 2 Adult lexical logfrequency value Note. R2 = .756 for Step 1; Δ R2 = .114 for Step 2. *p < .05. **p < .005. DISCUSSION Figure 1. Overall Past Tense Marking by Lexical Aspect Category 100 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 Retrospective analysis of findings from a prior study of elicited past tense productions of preschoolers with variable tense marking (Weiler & Schuele, 2011) using multiple linear regression analysis showed that stem-final phonology significantly predicted variance associated with percent correct past tense marking for verbs within both lexical aspect categories. Consistent with prior studies (e.g., Marchman, Wulfeck, & Ellis Weismer, 1999), all verbs, regardless of lexical aspect, with alveolar stop consonant stem endings were less likely to be marked for past tense. Such verbs may be more susceptible to omissions of past tense markers since there are bare stem verbs ending in alveolar stop consonants that are legitimately zero-marked for past tense (e.g.. hit, bid, cut). 20 After accounting for stem-final phonology, only variance in Accomplishment past tense accuracies was predictable from adult lexical log-frequency values. This finding should be interpreted cautiously given that it was powered at .56 (alpha = .05). Comparable partial R2 values over a corpus of fifteen verbs, five more than the present analysis, would reach power at .80. 10 0 Achievement Accomplishment Figure 2. Percent Past Tense Variance Explained by Predictors ** ** Results of this study suggest differential adult input influences on past tense development relative to lexical aspect category. If, as Brown and others observed, early-emerging past tense use is primarily restricted to Achievements, then past tense marking for Accomplishments may be more dependent on adult input as children begin to more flexibly apply past-tense morphology to less prototypical contexts (Olsen & Weinberg, 1999). ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS Data collection was supported by NIH/NIDCD (PI: Schuele). Completion of this study and preparation of this poster were supported by a Preparation of Leadership Personnel grant (H325D080075; PI: Schuele), U.S. Department of Education. LIST OF CITED REFERENCES AVAILABLE ON REQUEST http://www.mc.vanderbilt.edu/languagelab Poster Presented at the Symposium on Research in Child Language Disorders, Madison, WI June 2012 *p < .05. **p < .005 Irregular bare stem over-marked 1.833 Percent Marked eat* write* Task: The TEGI past tense probe is comprised of picture sets used to elicit regular and irregular past tense verbs. For each item, an examiner showed the child two pictures, one of an action in progress and the other of the same action completed. The examiner described the first picture using the target verb in a present progressive context (e.g., Here the boy is cleaning her room.). Then, the child was asked to describe the second, completed action picture using the prompt, Now he/she is done. Tell me what he/she did. The child was prompted to respond with a complete sentence, including a subject. Scoring: For the purposes of the initial study, only those responses containing the target verb in an obligatory context for past tense were coded for analysis (see Table 4). Correctly marked regular and irregular verbs were given credit. Verbs taking irregular past tense forms (e.g., blow) that were marked with regular past tense overgeneralizations of either the bare stem (e.g., blowed) or the past tense form itself (e.g., blewed) also were given credit. Kuczaj (1977) found that such past tense overgeneralization errors are produced by children who have already demonstrated stable control of the regular past tense rule. Responses containing the bare stem of the target verb in an obligatory context for past tense (e.g., She clean her room.) were not given credit. Interscorer reliability on past tense marking for approximately one quarter of the scored responses was 90%. Percent Marked Percent Marked for Past Tense kick, jump, catch, blow, lift, give Inflected Accomplishment Adult Frequency * = alveolar bare stem verb Percent Variance Accounted for by Predictors A comparison of past tense marking for Achievements versus Accomplishments in an elicitation probe among typical preschoolers (n = 52) who had not mastered past tense morphology revealed no difference in omissions where inflected forms were obligatory (χ2(1) = .156, p > .50; Weiler & Schuele, 2011). This finding suggests that the instantaneous temporal property of Achievements implicated in facilitating early-emerging past tense productions is not facilitative of later preschool-aged past tense. Accomplishment Inflected Adult Frequency Achievement Table 3. VERB TARGETS BY CATEGORY Achievement Stem final phonology Step 1 Table 5. Verb Properties and Accuracies by Lexical Aspect Type Table 2. PARTICIPANT CHARACTERISTICS Mean SE B Step 2 * Prior work (e.g., Brown,1973; Bloom, Lifter, and Hafitz,1980; Shirai and Anderson,1995) indicates that early emerging spontaneous past tense productions frequently occur with Achievements. This is speculated to be due to the prototypical way in which verbs describing Achievements characterize visible events completed rather instantly. Within a lexical aspect categorical framework, such events, characterized by their defined endpoints and sudden durations, are labeled Achievements. Accomplishments, on the other hand, have defined endpoints but gradual durations (see Table 1). β B