637503 research-article2016 AUT0010.1177/1362361316637503AutismEditorial Editorial The importance of characterizing intervention for individuals with autism The estimated annual cost of caring for individuals with autism is US$268 billion (Leigh and Du, 2015). What is this buying? Beyond a general knowledge of setting and type of service provided (e.g. educational, mental health, speech therapy, and occupational therapy), we know very little about the type and quality of interventions being delivered. For several reasons, it is imperative that we be able to accurately and efficiently characterize the treatment that children with autism receive in their communities. One reason for this urgency links treatment to our basic understanding of mechanism and subtype in autism. Most efforts to identify different kinds of autism, using either behavioral or biological measures, have been frustratingly disappointing. Yet there is still growing consensus that autism really comprises a set of phenotypically linked disorders, and that if we could distinguish among them, we could improve our understanding of their basic biology, leading to more targeted treatments and supports. An important unexplored strategy for subtyping autism may be to examine response to treatment. Thus far, we have examined predictors of treatment success; however, these have primarily been relatively gross measures of intelligence and language use (e.g. Ben-Itzchak and Zachor, 2011). A few studies have examined specific behaviors relating to response to specific, well-characterized treatments (e.g. Schreibman et al., 2009; Sherer and Schreibman, 2005; Yoder and Stone, 2006). However, these studies have not led to phenotypic descriptions that provide a better understanding of either the biology that may subtype the disorder or methods to prescribe treatment a priori (Stahmer et al., 2010; Vivanti et al., 2014). If we wish to examine this question, we will either have to standardize treatment for a very large and heterogeneous sample of individuals with autism—a very costly endeavor—or we will have to be able to measure the active ingredients of treatment in the schools and clinics in which treatment is delivered. The second reason for characterizing intervention is the need to identify active mechanisms, with the goal of refining treatments (e.g. Kasari and Lawton, 2010; Schreibman et al., 2015). Due to the complexity of treatment of autism, many evidence-based interventions are packages composed of several components. Each of these components has a cost associated with implementing it, including the time it takes to train providers to accurately use each Autism 2016, Vol. 20(4) 386­–387 © The Author(s) 2016 Reprints and permissions: sagepub.co.uk/journalsPermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/1362361316637503 aut.sagepub.com component. Evidence-based interventions with varying brand names may utilize similar components even when differing in overall theoretical orientation (Schreibman et al., 2015) and may lead to similar outcomes when implemented with high fidelity (Boyd et al., 2014). However, these interventions may also differ in important ways. Therefore, it is important to examine the relative contribution, mechanism of action, and necessity of each component of an intervention package to achieve optimal child outcomes (Lerner et al., 2012; Sanetti et al., 2009; Schulte et al., 2009). We know that community providers often pick and choose specific components of an intervention to fit their clients and setting rather than use the package as a whole (Stahmer et al., 2005). Examining the impact of individual components of an intervention will help determine whether their methods of modifying and combining treatments are likely to be both effective and efficient. If we could measure the delivery of each component accurately, we could dismantle components that are mostly associated with positive outcome. The third reason to characterize interventions is that we can measure the quality of care individuals with autism receive. The service delivery infrastructure for individuals with autism has not kept pace with the dramatically increasing number of children diagnosed. Service providers rush to hang out shingles saying that they treat autism, but there are few licensure and credentialing practices in place. Although this has increased access to care, there continues to be extreme variability in service intensity and quality. Even if we measure the number of service hours, and perhaps the “type” of service, which may often include educational services, speech and language therapy, occupational therapy, parent-implemented intervention, and social skills groups, there is a huge range in the quality of intervention within these services. Families and payers have few ways of assessing the quality of the care individuals with autism receive. Similar to quality assurance and improvement procedures that have been put in place to assess the care provided for other health conditions, we need accurate measures that can be implemented with relatively little time burden to assess quality of care. The measures currently available are narrow in scope, either focused on a specific intervention or a specific service setting. Typically, intervention developers will create a Downloaded from aut.sagepub.com at UNIV CALIFORNIA DAVIS on April 20, 2016 387 Editorial protocol to guide implementation or a tool for assessing fidelity of implementation. Developing accurate, low-cost fidelity measurement is in many ways more challenging. The process of measuring fidelity involves several elements: (1) identifying important treatment components, or “key ingredients”; (2) developing an instrument that allows for valid and reliable measurement of these components; and (3) identifying a system for assessing the components, including methods for providing feedback to providers (Schoenwald et al., 2011). The fidelity process must measure the key ingredients of an intervention and be both psychometrically sound (effective) and usable in clinical care (efficient). Researchers in children’s mental health have validated fidelity measures for clinicians and supervisors in community settings for individual evidence-based practices (e.g. Multi-Systemic Therapy; Schoenwald and Henggeler, 2003); however, most methods of rating fidelity in autism have primarily been used for research. There have been some important, recent efforts to characterize program quality without focus on a specific intervention. For example, the Autism Program Environment Rating Scale (APERS), developed by the National Professional Development Center for Autism, is a tool to guide professional development and coaching efforts in educational settings. However, measures developed so far are typically setting specific, time consuming, and require significant clinical expertise to ensure accuracy (e.g. Hume et al., 2011; Mandell et al., 2013). Developing methods for measuring quality indicators in usual care for ASD across service systems (i.e. education, mental health, and affiliated care) and interventions (e.g. behavioral, developmental, and environmental) that are efficient and accurate will require a targeted and coordinated effort among intervention researchers and community collaborators. References Ben-Itzchak E and Zachor DA (2011) Who benefits from early intervention in autism spectrum disorders? Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 5(1): 345–350. Boyd BA, Hume K, McBee MT, et al. (2014) Comparative efficacy of LEAP, TEACCH and non-model-specific special education programs for preschoolers with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 44(2): 366–380. Hume K, Boyd B, McBee D, et al. (2011) Assessing implementation of comprehensive treatment models for young children with ASD: reliability and validity of two measures. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 5(4): 1430–1440. Kasari C and Lawton K (2010) New directions in behavioral treatment of autism spectrum disorders. Current Opinions in Neurology 23(2): 137–142. Leigh JP and Du J (2015) Brief report: forecasting the economic burden of autism in 2015 and 2025 in the United States. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 45(12): 4135–4139. Lerner MD, White S and McPartland JC (2012) Mechanisms of children in psychosocial interventions for autism spectrum disorders. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience 14(3): 307–316. Mandell DS, Stahmer AC, Shin S, et al. (2013) The role of treatment fidelity on outcomes during a randomized field trial of an autism intervention. Autism 17(3): 281–295. Sanetti LM, Hagermoser A and Kratochwill TR (2009) Toward developing a science of treatment integrity: introduction to the special series. School Psychology Review 38(4): 445–459. Schoenwald SK and Henggeler SW (2003) Introductory comments (Current strategies for moving evidence-based interventions into clinical practice. Special series). Cognitive and Behavioral Practice 10: 275–323. Schoenwald SK, Garland AF, Chapman JE, et al. (2011) Toward the effective and efficient measurement of implementation fidelity. Administration and Policy in Mental Health and Mental Health Services Research 38(1): 32–43. Schreibman L, Dawson G, Stahmer AC, et al. (2015) Naturalistic developmental behavioral interventions: empirically validated treatments for autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 45(8): 2411–2428. Schreibman L, Stahmer AC, Bartlett VS, et al. (2009) Brief Report: toward refinement of a predictive behavioral profile for treatment outcome in children with autism. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 3: 163–172. Schulte AC, Easton JE and Parker KJ (2009) Advances in treatment integrity research: multidisciplinary perspectives on the conceptualization, measurement, and enhancement of treatment integrity. School Psychology Review 38(4): 460–475. Sherer MR and Schreibman L (2005) Individual behavioral profiles and predictors of treatment effectiveness for children with autism. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 73(3): 525–538. Stahmer A, Collings NM and Palinkas LA (2005) Early intervention practices for children with autism: descriptions from community providers. Focus on Autism & Other Developmental Disabilities 20(2): 66–79. Stahmer A, Schreibman L and Cunningham AB (2010) Towards a technology of treatment individualization for young children with autism spectrum disorders. Brain Research 1380: 229–239. Vivanti G, Prior M, Williams K, et al. (2014) Predictors of outcomes in autism early intervention: why don’t we know more? Front Pediatrics 2: 58. Yoder P and Stone WL (2006) A randomized comparison of the effect of two prelinguistic communication intervention on the acquisition of spoken communication in preschoolers with ASD. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research 49: 698–711. Aubyn C Stahmer1, Jessica Suhrheinrich2 and David S Mandell3 1MIND Institute and Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, School of Medicine, University of California, Davis, USA 2Child and Adolescent Services Research Center and Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, University of California, San Diego, USA 3Center for Mental Health Policy and Services Research & Department of Psychiatry and Pediatrics, Perlman School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, USA Downloaded from aut.sagepub.com at UNIV CALIFORNIA DAVIS on April 20, 2016