Psychology Cognitive Psychology Intermediate 1 and 2 7112 Summer 2000 HIGHER STILL Psychology Cognitive Psychology Intermediate 1 and 2 Support Materials *+,-./ CONTENTS Statement of standards Intermediate 1 Intermediate 2 Guidance for teachers Integration with other units Recording student attainment Approaches to learning and teaching Learning environment How to use the pack INFORMATION FOR STUDENTS OUTCOME 1 The Study of Cognitive Psychology Student information and activities on Key concept: Perception Key concept: Memory Key concept: Language INFORMATION FOR STUDENTS OUTCOME 2 Research Methods in Cognitive Psychology Student activities on Research Methods General References Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 1 STATEMENT OF STANDARDS COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY INTERMEDIATE 1 OUTCOME 1 Describe, in brief, key concepts in cognitive psychology. Performance criteria (a) Key concepts in cognitive psychology are identified correctly. (b) Key concepts in cognitive psychology are described simply and accurately. (c) The applications of these key concepts to different situations in everyday life are described simply and accurately. Note on the range of the outcome Key concepts: perception; attention; memory; language and thinking. Evidence requirements To demonstrate satisfactory attainment of this outcome, candidates should produce written or oral responses to cover all performance criteria. They are required to do so for any three key concepts from perception; attention; memory; language and thinking. Written/oral responses will typically be short answers and restricted responses to specific questions. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 2 OUTCOME 2 Describe, in brief, the main research methods used in cognitive psychology. Performance criteria (a) The main research methods used in cognitive psychology are identified correctly. (b) The main research methods used in cognitive psychology are described simply and accurately. (c) Examples of research used in cognitive psychology are described simply and accurately. Note on the range of the outcome The main methods of research used in cognitive psychology: experimental, survey, interview, observation, case study. Evidence requirements To demonstrate satisfactory attainment of this outcome, candidates should produce written or oral responses to cover all performance criteria. They are required to do so for two of the following methods of research: experimental and one other from survey, interview, observation or case study. Candidates should give two examples of research for each method. Written/oral responses will typically be short answers and restricted responses to specific questions. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 3 STATEMENT OF STANDARDS COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY INTERMEDIATE 2 OUTCOME 1 Describe key concepts in cognitive psychology. Performance criteria (a) Key concepts in cognitive psychology are described accurately. (b) The applications of these key concepts to different situations in everyday life are described accurately. Note on the range of the outcome Key concepts: perception; attention; memory; language and thinking. Evidence requirements To demonstrate satisfactory attainment of this outcome, candidates should produce written or oral responses to cover all performance criteria. They are required to do so for any three key concepts from perception; attention; memory; language and thinking. Written/oral responses will typically be short answers and restricted responses to specific questions. OUTCOME 2 Describe the main research methods used in cognitive psychology. Performance criteria (a) The main research methods used in cognitive psychology are described accurately. (b) Examples of research used in cognitive psychology are described accurately. Note on the range of the outcome The main methods of research used in cognitive psychology: experimental, survey, interview, observation, case study. Evidence requirements To demonstrate satisfactory attainment of this outcome, candidates should produce written or oral responses to cover all performance criteria. They are required to do so for two of the following methods of research: experimental and one other from survey, interview, observation or case study. Candidates should give two examples of research for each method. Written/oral responses will typically be short answers and restricted responses to specific questions. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 4 ADVICE FOR TEACHERS / LECTURERS INTEGRATION WITH OTHER UNITS Cognitive Psychology in an optional unit of Intermediate 1 and Intermediate 2 Psychology. These units have close links with the introductory units in the Intermediate Psychology courses. They may be integrated effectively with the introductory units. The range of psychological key concepts, applications of these key concepts to different situations in everyday life and research methods covered in these optional units are directly, and indirectly, relevant to the approaches and research methods covered in each introductory unit. Cognitive Psychology can be offered as a freestanding unit at both Intermediate 1 and Intermediate 2 levels. UNIT CONTENT Both the Intermediate 1 and Intermediate 2 ‘Cognitive Psychology’ units have two outcomes: INTERMEDIATE 1 INTERMEDIATE 2 Outcome 1 Outcome 1 Describe, in brief, key concepts in cognitive psychology. Describe key concepts in cognitive psychology. Outcome 2 Outcome 2 Describe, in brief, the main research methods used in cognitive psychology. Describe the main research methods used in cognitive psychology. The content of these units is similar across the two levels. They can be summarised as follows: • • • • Key concepts: perception; attention; language; memory and thinking. Applications of these key concepts to different situations in everyday life The main methods of research used in cognitive psychology: experimental, survey, interview, observation, case study. Examples of research in developmental psychology which uses these methods. For further information and details about unit content, please refer to the Psychology Arrangements Document. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 5 RECORDING STUDENT ATTAINMENT A recording proforma for teachers/lecturers to complete for individual candidate attainment is available in the Unit Assessment pack. Two further proformas can be found in the Appendix of this pack: • • Candidate’s record of progress - for individual candidates to have a record of their attainment. Internal Assessment Record - to record their internal assessment results of the whole student group. Teachers/lecturers may devise their own alternative system for recording student attainment. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 6 APPROACHES TO LEARNING AND TEACHING In delivering these units, it is useful if teachers/lecturers achieve a balance between teacher/lecturer exposition and experiential learning. It is important to recognise that learners acquire and process information in a number of ways to help them learn. These include visually, aurally, in discussion or exchange with others, during group based problem solving activities, and during solitary reflection. Students should be encouraged from the beginning to draw on their own experiences, perceptions, and their previous and current learning. Personal experience of interacting with a variety of people, and in a number of different situations is an invaluable source of knowledge and is highly relevant to developmental psychology. The sharing of experiences and insights will promote general awareness that developmental psychology assists self-understanding and an understanding of humans in a variety of contexts. Students should also be encouraged to gather and use information about different people’s actions, thoughts and feelings and to consider how these affect themselves and others. Relevant quality newspapers and/or magazine articles and video/film productions are useful resources which bring developmental psychology to life so that it can be shared by comparatively large groups of people at any one time. This remains appropriate even when the material is fictional, provided it presents us with a true picture of the human condition which is not deliberately sensationalised. In delivering this unit, it is appropriate that a multicultural approach is taken since the learning needs of individuals vary according to their cultural background. Case studies, role-play and simulations should incorporate characters and elements from different social and cultural backgrounds wherever possible. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 7 UNIT INDUCTION At the beginning of the unit ‘Cognitive Psychology’, teachers/lecturers should ensure that students are clear about it’s nature and purpose. Induction for this unit should last about two hours and should include an introduction to the content of the unit, provide a programme of work and explain the arrangements for assessment and reassessment. At this point students can be given the Candidate’s Guide from the Unit Assessment Pack. This helps explain what the unit is about and how it is assessed. In order to allow students to make a confident start, reference should be made to links with previous or other current learning with which they are familiar. It is also important to discuss and explore the nature of the Course or Group Award being undertaken by the group. It may be necessary to include induction exercises, particularly if the group is a new one. The type and number of exercises used will however depend on the nature of the particular group, their familiarity with each other and with the teacher/lecturer concerned. LEARNING ENVIRONMENT The expertise of the teacher/lecturer is invaluable in developing skills in, approaches to, and insights about the subject of cognitive psychology. Teachers/lecturers should aim to create a relaxed and enjoyable learning environment which is both motivating and supportive. In order that a ‘people’ perspective is always present the following conditions should be met: • the provision of a learning climate in which students feel supported and able to express their thoughts and ideas. • a teaching style which promotes a supportive learning climate. • teaching and learning methods which draw on students’ past and present learning experience and which enable them to integrate new ideas and skills during their interactions with others. Further guidance can be found in the Psychology Subject Guide. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 8 HOW TO USE THIS PACK Purpose of the pack This pack is designed to provide guidance and support materials to help teachers/lecturers in the delivery of the unit. The student information and activities are designed to be used by teachers/lecturers in whatever way suits their preferred style of delivery and the needs of their particular student group. This pack has not been designed for open learning purposes. Answers to exercises and worksheets will be provided and facilitated by the teacher/lecturer. The student activities in the pack will require to be followed up and brought together by the teacher/lecturer in whatever way is most appropriate. The student activities in this pack cover the two learning outcomes and their performance criteria at both Intermediate 1 and Intermediate 2 levels. The material is presented in such a way that Outcome 1 (Key concepts) is covered first. Outcome 2 (Research methods) is then covered. This sequence of delivery is by no means compulsory and may be rearranged at the discretion of the teacher/lecturer responsible for delivering the unit. Using the materials The student activity and information materials in the pack have been identified as either: • • Information or Student Activity. The essential knowledge required for the unit has been covered on those pages which have headed Information. This is particularly useful as handout material. These information sheets could also be used as the focus of input by the teacher/lecturer and to develop ideas further as part of question and answer sessions and group discussions. These information sheets can be photocopied as a separate pack should the teacher/lecturer prefer to use them either as teaching notes or as separate handout materials. Alternatively, the materials could be assembled into smaller topic packs. All worksheets, assignments, exercises and group activities have been covered on those pages which are headed Student Activity. These general activities have been developed to include exercises for individuals, pairs, triads and small groups to conduct. Teachers/lecturers may well wish to alter the way in which these activities are carried out according to the needs of their particular group. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 9 Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 10 INFORMATION FOR STUDENTS - OUTCOME 1 INTRODUCTION For this outcome you will learn about the following three cognitive psychology key concepts: • perception • memory • language By the end of this outcome you should know and understand what each one of these key concepts involves and be able to describe it. You should also know the names of some of the famous cognitive psychologists associated within each key concept. Finally, it is expected that you will be able to recognise and state how each key concept is applied in different situations in everyday life. For the purposes of the Intermediate 1/2 cognitive psychology SQA particularly emphasise: • perception • memory • language OVERVIEW OF UNIT: ‘COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY’ Cognitive psychology concerns the study of our cognitive processes. Cognitive processes refer to all the ways in which we obtain and use information from our world in order to operate successfully within it. In this support pack we will first explore cognitive psychology generally in an attempt to understand what cognition means. Secondly, we will look at three key concepts which enable us to gain and use knowledge of the world around us. These key concepts are taken from the whole range of cognitive processes which include perception, memory, attention, language and thinking. All of these are inferred and cannot be observed directly. This unit will explore three of these different aspects of cognition as key concepts i.e. perception, memory and language and how they might apply in everyday situations e.g. the application of perception and/or attention to buying behaviour. Choice of other cognitive processes studied is at the discretion of the teacher /lecturer and students. A variety of methods of enquiry are used in cognitive psychology. This unit will therefore provide knowledge about the experimental method in particular and; in addition; the survey method. Choice of other methods studied is at the discretion of the teacher /lecturer and students. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 11 THE STUDY OF COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY Cognitive psychology is the study of the processes that we use: ‘to take in information coming to us from our outside world; to make sense of this information and to use our interpretation of this information to act upon, and in, our external world.’ A knowledge of cognitive psychology is useful in that we tend to react to stimuli: events/objects/people: on the basis of what we each believe is happening. While all healthy human beings share the same cognitive processes - we each reach a conclusion as to what is happening (and thus how to behave/react), on the basis of our individual cognitive processes and meaningful past experiences using them. This can mean that two people can respond totally differently to the same thing. A good example of this would be two women, one of whom has experienced domestic violence. When they see a man walk towards them in the street they both see a man, but their individual thoughts; feelings and behaviours concerning him will be different. The theme of similar but different environmental experience affecting each individual’s own thoughts, feelings and behaviours runs through cognitive psychology as does the importance of realising the inter-relationships between all our cognitive processes (perception: attention: language; memory and thinking). As was stated in the Unit Overview the study of what goes to make up cognitive psychology i.e. cognitive processes is difficult. This is because our cognitive processes do not exist in reality. They are hypothetical constructs. If something does not exist in reality it is extremely difficult, if not impossible, to measure or observe it directly. For a good part of the 20th century psychology placed a great emphasis on experimentation to generate objective empirical data (hard scientific fact) about the phenomena it was interested in. This proved extremely difficult to do in cognitive psychology. Eventually, mainly due to the influences of Edward Tolman and Jean Piaget, the study of cognitive psychology progressed because cognitive psychologists realised they could study and produce objective empirical data concerning their subject matter by studying it indirectly. It should also therefore be emphasised that what we study in cognitive psychology is inferred. Cognitive psychology thus concerns a study of the following cognitive processes: • perception • attention • memory and • language • thinking. While the study of cognitive processes should not be exclusive to just one, two, three or more processes, and with an understanding that they are each related to one another, SQA emphasises in particular the key concepts of perception; memory and language. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 12 The Philosophical and Psychological Roots of Cognitive Psychology Before turning to these three key concepts of perception; language and memory, it will be useful to try to understand where cognitive psychology has come from. Remembering back to Psychology: An Introduction your teacher/lecturer may have mentioned that the cognitive approach (and thus cognitive psychology) grew out of criticisms of the behaviourist approach. Behaviourism emphasises that psychology should only study actual observable behaviour and that human beings should be understood in terms of all the stimulus-response units of learnt behaviour (via classical and operant conditioning) accumulated to date. There are good reasons for this, but a study of actual observable behaviour alone is not enough to understand why and how it is that we think, feel and behave as we do. The study of the human mind is also important. Due to the great influence of behaviourism, a scientific study of the human mind was difficult until a ‘soft’ behaviourist called Edward Tolman (1930) discovered in his ‘rats in mazes’ experiment that his rats appeared to have done something with their units of learnt behaviour about the maze. When a rat was put into the maze it initially sniffed about and explored it in an erratic fashion. It eventually discovered food placed in a particular position within the maze by Tolman. When later put back into the maze the rat(s) went straight to the food. They did not go down blind alleys, turn back on themselves etc., as when first introduced to the maze. Tolman concluded on the basis of measurement, control and observation, that the rat(s) had formed primitive cognitive maps of the maze in their heads based on their earlier experience i.e. by externalising the cognitive process of thinking. As a result the rats were able to understand and react to this environment better in that they were now aware of blind alleys and cul-de-sacs and were able to anticipate and take short cuts to the food. The behaviourists seemed to be wrong. Tolman’s rats suggest that organisms (what behaviourists call people and animals) do something with previously learned information in order to behave in a more enriched and enlightened way when they encounter the same stimulus again. Results of a study by Saaranin (1973) suggests that humans do the same thing. The human equivalent of a cognitive map is called a neural network. Saaranin got American college students to draw maps of their campus. Students tended to enlarge those buildings which were most important to them and shrink those less important. They were often found to be completely wrong when describing campus areas that were not as familiar to them. Similarly, Briggs (1971) discovered, on asking people to judge how far they thought one landmark was from another, that they tended to underestimate the distance between familiar landmark objects and over-estimate the distance between unfamiliar landmarks. This is further illustrated when as adults we return to a once familiar childhood haunt i.e. a Scottish coastal summer holiday resort and find it is a lot smaller and far less exciting than we imagined. The perception of, for example Girvan, for a Glaswegian whose only experience of the place is during his primary school years is dramatically brought into focus when returning to the town 30 years later. It just is not the same. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 13 Cognitive psychology thus concerns an understanding of what are called our internal mental structures and processes. We will look at our senses in order to understand the structure and role of the five higher level cognitive processes of perception; attention; language; memory and thinking. Each of our cognitive processes can be studied individually but to understand cognitive psychology completely, their inter-relationship with each other should be emphasised. Questions 1. What do you understand by the term ‘cognitive psychology’? 2. What do you understand by the term ‘cognitive processes’? 3. Why did the behaviourists not like the study of cognitive psychology? 4. How did cognitive psychologists overcome this criticism by the behaviourists? 5. What two research methods do you think cognitive psychologists use to investigate cognitive processes? 6. Why is it that despite seeing the same 90 minutes of football a Rangers fan and a Celtic fan will report entirely different versions of it? Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 14 KEY CONCEPTS IN COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY: PERCEPTION Perception, a key cognitive process, can be defined as an active cognitive process which entails us processing, organising and interpreting information coming to us from our outside world. SQA ask that we consider in relation to the study of perception: • sensation and perception • illusions • perceptual constancy • factors affecting perception Perception is the way in which we selectively monitor what is happening around us. In order to do this successfully we are equipped with at least five senses! Our senses work independently and together to help give us as much information about our external environment as possible. This is crucial to survival. Of great interest to cognitive psychologists in their study of perception is our visual system, if only to try and solve the mystery of why we experience our world in three dimensions. The images falling on our retinas are two-dimensional much like a photograph. You should be able to explore this further when looking at visual illusions, and another ability we have called perceptual constancies. Finally our perception of our world is not always accurate. This shall be explored when examining the influence of learning, experience and motivation on perception. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 15 Perception: Introduction As usual in psychology we try to first define the concept we are looking at. The study of perception is no exception. ‘Perception is...the process of assembling sensations into a usable mental representation of the world...which...creates faces, melodies, works of art, illusions etc.’ Coon (1983) ‘Perception is not determined by stimulus patterns, rather it is a dynamic searching for the best interpretation of the available data...perception involves going beyond the immediately given evidence of the senses.’ RL Gregory (1966) We should be careful however for: ‘To perceive seems effortless. To understand perception is nevertheless a great challenge’. Dodwell (1995) It may now be said that: Perception is the process by which we take in raw sensations from our environment using our senses and interpret these sensations using our past knowledge and understanding of the world in order that the sensation or what we are sensing, becomes meaningful to us. An understanding of how we do this has intrigued cognitive psychologists for decades. To date three explanations have emerged, each enriching our knowledge of perception. Three Explanations of Perception: 1. The Ecological View (Gibson; Gestalt - ‘organised wholes’): where it is said we perceive most clues from our environment directly and without interpretation (we perceive ‘exactly’ what we see/hear/smell/touch and taste). The ecological view is popularly known as bottom-up processing. 2. The Constructivist View (Gregory; Bruner): where it is said our perceptual system must often make a reality indirectly out of bits of sensory information due to the absence of other information. The constructivist view is better known as top-down processing. 3. Top-Down/Bottom-Up Symbiotic View (Neisser) which says we use the most appropriate of the above processes depending on the situation we find ourself in. The two processing models work together. When one type of perceptual process or aspect of it is impaired, the other process, or an aspect of it, ‘fills’ in or compensates to give us, at the end of the day, as much an understanding of the stimulus as possible. Symbiotic processing is best explained in the area of visual perception using Marr’s Computer AnalogyZKHUH KH VD\V ZH H[WUDFW YLVXDO information from an image/object/event in four stages and put it all together again in the brain in what he calls a symbolic representation of the stimulus. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 16 Whether you advocate a bottom-up (ecological view), top-down (constructivist view) or symbiotic explanation of perception, it is interesting to note the influence of the nature-nurture debate to your position. Most of the theorists above suggest that what we perceive or ultimately understand from what our senses are telling us is the result of learning and experience (empirical basis to our perceptions). The influence of Gestalt psychology in the early years of research into this topic does raise the nativist position; which says our perceptual ability is innate, and needs little if any ‘learning’ to enhance it. At the end of the day, we can say that perception is influenced by our innate abilities as human beings. Also, the cognitive apparatus we are born with is used to make meaningful sense of the many sensory experiences we encounter. What we perceive (or understand) that we are sensing is also strongly influenced by what we have learned from past experience of the same, or similar, sensations. Perception is further influenced by expectations, culture and motivation. Sensation and Perception In our original working definition of perception we said that perception concerns sensing plus our interpretation of this sensation based on meaningful past experience of it. Central to perception must therefore include a knowledge of how we receive information from our environment in the first place. This is of course via our senses. We use our traditional five senses to receive information from our external world i.e. sight, hearing, touch, taste, smell. Psychology is also able to tell us we have at least one other sense which is called our KINAESTHETIC sense. This is a sense from within our own body which tells us about movement, or the feel of our muscles or joints. Our kinaesthetic sense tells us about balance. Our Visual Sense Our sense of vision is in many respects our key sense. Vision, our visual system and visual perception is the most studied information process of all by cognitive psychologists, so much so, it is looked at in more depth in a later section entitled ‘Our Visual System’. Our sense of vision comes to us via our eyes, the structure and location of which allow us to perceive our world in three dimensions: sense colour, sense depth, etc. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 17 Our Tactile Sense Touch, is one of our five senses, allowing for our perception of pressure, touch, temperature (principally temperature change), pain, and hair movement. We experience touch using sensory cells called receptors which are nerve endings in the skin. Touch receptors are either free ending (in the dermis and around hair follicles) or encapsulated (branched or coiled, enclosed in a capsule). Receptors in our skin respond to a specific type of stimulus and are not evenly distributed over the body. The sensitivity of fingertips, for example, results from a large number of touch receptors we have at this extremity. Once a receptor is stimulated, it sends nerve impulses to the brain, which locates and identifies the stimulus involved and assesses its significance. The more intense the stimulus, the greater the frequency of the nerve impulses. The skin's sensory system is important in alerting the body to changes in its external environment. Potentially harmful stimuli may cause pain resulting in either protective reflex actions (e.g. dropping a hot object), or storing a memory to remind you to avoid future similar hazards. The perception of pain is unusual as it is also strongly affected by the emotions and the circumstances in which it is experienced. Our Gustatory Sense Taste is effected by the contact of soluble substances on our tongue Although humans can distinguish between a wide range of flavours, the sensation of taste is actually a response to a combination of several stimuli, including texture, temperature, and smell, as well as taste. In isolation, the sense of taste can only identify four basic flavours: sweet, salt, sour, and bitter, with individual taste buds particularly responsive to one of these. The 10,000 or so taste buds found in humans are distributed unevenly over the top of the tongue, creating patches sensitive to specific classes of chemicals which give the taste sensations. Sweet and salt are usually at the tip of the tongue, sour at the edges, and bitter at the base. Chemicals from food are dissolved in the moisture of the mouth and enter the taste buds through pores in the surface of the tongue where they come into contact with sensory cells. When a receptor is stimulated by one of the dissolved substances, it sends nerve impulses to the brain. The frequency of the repetition of the impulse tells the brain how strong a flavour is and the type of flavour is probably registered by the nerve cells which responded. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 18 Our Auditory Sense The Ear is our organ of hearing and balance. It is composed of three parts – external, middle, and internal - the greater part of which is enclosed within the temporal bone. The external ear is that portion of the hearing apparatus lateral to our eardrum, or tympanic membrane. The eardrum comprises the external flap of the ear (the auricle, or pinna), and the external auditory canal, which is 3 cm (1.25 inches) in length. The middle ear, on the inner side of the eardrum, houses our mechanism for the conduction of sound waves to the internal ear. It is a narrow passage, or cleft, that extends vertically for about 15 mm (0.6 in) and for about the same distance horizontally. The middle ear is in direct communication with the back of the nose and throat by way of the eustachian tube, which allows for passage of air into and out of the middle ear. Across, the middle ear is a chain of three small, movable bones called the ossicles: the malleus, or hammer handle; the incus, or anvil; and the stapes, or stirrup. The ossicles connect the eardrum acoustically to the fluid-filled internal ear. The internal ear (or labyrinth) is the part of the temporal bone containing the organs of hearing and balance to which the filaments of the auditory nerve are distributed. It is separated from the middle ear by the fenestra ovalis, or oval window. The internal ear consists of membranous canals housed in a dense portion of the temporal bone and is divided into the cochlea (Greek, ‘snail shell’), the vestibule, and three semicircular canals. All these canals communicate with one another and are filled with a gelatinous fluid called endolymph. The disposition and orientation of endolymph also helps us experience our sense of balance. The Ear and Balance The semicircular canals and the vestibule are concerned with our sense of equilibrium, or balance. Hairs in these canals, similar to those that form the organ of Corti, respond to changes in the position of the head. The three semicircular canals extend from the vestibule approximately at right angles to each other, providing sensory organs to record movements of the head in each of the three planes of space: up and down, forwards and backwards, and to the left or right. Lying over the hair cells in the vestibule are crystals of calcium carbonate, known technically as otoliths and popularly as ear sand. When the head is tilted, the otoliths shift, and the hairs beneath respond to the change in pressure. The eyes and certain sensory cells in the skin and internal tissues also help to maintain equilibrium, but when the labyrinth of the ear is damaged or destroyed; disturbances of equilibrium invariably follow. With eyes closed, a person with a disease or disturbance of the internal ear may be unable to stand without swaying or falling. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 19 How do we hear? Sound waves, which are actually changes in air pressure, are carried through the external auditory canal to the eardrum, causing it to vibrate. These vibrations are communicated by the ossicular chain in the middle ear through the oval window to the fluid in the inner ear. The movement of the endolymph stimulates the movement of a set of fine hair-like projections called hair cells as the cochlea vibrates. Collectively these projections are called the organ of Corti. The hair cells transmit signals directly to the auditory nerve which carries information to the brain. The overall pattern of response of the hair cells to vibrations of the cochlea encodes information about sound in a way that is interpreted by the brain's auditory centres. The range of hearing, like that of vision, varies in different people. The maximum range of human hearing includes sound frequencies from about 16 to 28,000 cycles per second. The least noticeable change in tone that can be picked up by the ear varies with pitch and loudness. A change of vibration frequency (pitch) corresponding to about 0.03 per cent of the original frequency can be detected by the most sensitive human ears in the range between 500 and 8,000 vibrations per second. The ear is less sensitive to frequency changes for sounds of low frequency or low intensity. The sensitivity of the ear to sound intensity (loudness) also varies with frequency. Sensitivity to change in loudness is greatest between 1,000 to 3,000 cycles, where a change of one decibel can be detected—and becomes less when sound-intensity levels are lowered. The variation in the sensitivity of the ear to loud sounds causes several important phenomena. Extremely loud tones produce in the ear entirely different tones that are not present in the original tone. These subjective tones are probably caused by imperfections in the natural function of the middle ear. The harshness in tonality caused by greatly increasing sound intensities, as when a radio volume control is adjusted to produce excessively loud sounds, results from subjective tones produced in the ear. The loudness of a pure tone also affects its pitch. High tones may increase as much as a whole musical-scale note. Low tones tend to become lower as sound intensity increases. This effect is noticeable only for pure tones. Because most musical tones are complex, hearing is usually not affected to an appreciable degree by this phenomenon. In sound masking, the production in the ear of harmonics of lower-pitched sounds may deafen the ear to the perception of higher-pitched sounds. Masking is what makes it necessary to raise one's voice in order to be heard in a noisy place. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 20 Our Olfactory Sense Smell, is one of the five senses by which odours are perceived. The nose, equipped with olfactory nerves, is the special organ of smell. The olfactory nerves also account for differing tastes of substances taken into the mouth, in that, most sensations that appear introspectively to us as tastes are in essence really smells! Sensations of smell are difficult to describe and classify, but useful categorisations have been made by noting the chemical elements of odorous substances. Research has pointed to the existence of seven primary odours—camphor-like, musky, floral, peppermint-like, ethereal (dry-cleaning fluid, for example), pungent (vinegar-like), and putrid—corresponding to the seven types of smell receptors found in the olfactory-cell hairs. Olfactory research also indicates that substances with similar odours have molecules of similar shape. Recent studies suggest that the shape of an odour-causing chemical molecule determines the nature of the odour of that molecule or substance. These molecules are believed to combine with specific cells in the nose or with chemicals within those cells. This process is the first step in a complex series that continues with the transmission of impulses by the olfactory nerve and ends with the perception of odour by the brain. Our internal and external senses interact with each other because we are constantly linking together information obtained from differing sensory MODES e.g. seeing, hearing, etc. This involves us in what is called cross-modal transfer - where information gained using one mode e.g. sight is applied to information from another sensory mode e.g. hearing. Cross-modal transfer gives rise to a richer array of sensory information upon which we base our interpretation of our own realities (world) - but by which we often become confused. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 21 Characteristics of our Sensory System Our six modalities (or senses) have certain common characteristics. Before looking at what these are please complete both the common and scientific name for each: 1 Apparatus Sense Eye Visual 2 3 4 5 6 • • • • • each respond to a particular form of energy or external information e.g. light waves, sound waves, skin pressure, etc. each have a sense organ or ‘accessory structure’ which is the first port of call for any incoming information on the road to processing and full understanding of the perceived stimuli each accessory structure has sense receptors called ‘transducers’. These are specialised cells which are sensitive to particular kinds of energy. It is as the stimuli impinges on these transducers that the conversion of the stimuli into electrical nerve impulses occurs. This electrical activity is the only kind of energy that can be processed and understood by our brains. each sensory modality involves a different part of the brain. We are here able to interpret messages received from our sensory receptors which gives us the experience of conscious awareness of an object; a person; a word; a sound; a taste etc. a certain minimum stimulation of a sense receptor is needed before we can become consciously aware of the sensory experience which is happening. These minimum requirements are called absolute thresholds, which are based on a value given to a stimulus when we can detect it 50% of the time. The threshold at which we can notice a stimulus differs among and between people, and can be affected by an individuals physical state, time of day, motivation, the way the stimulus is presented etc. This is the area of psychophysics within psychology (the interface between the physical stimulus and subjective experience of it) which is of great importance to the development of psychology as a subject in its own right (see Wilhelm Wundt, 1879). Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 22 Our Visual System Vision and visual perception is by far the largest area of investigation undertaken by cognitive psychologists. A full understanding needs a little introduction to the eye. The eye, according to Ornstein (1975) is ‘the most important avenue of personal consciousness’. We receive around 80% of our information about our world via our visual system. As was said previously, sight or our sense of vision is probably the most studied of all our senses due to the vast amount of work which has been done in the area of visual perception. A basic understanding of the structure and function of the eye is therefore of relevance - if only to give us a clue as to how and why we receive 2-D type photographic images on our retinas but interpret these two dimensional images in three dimensions. A knowledge of our visual system is also important to our understanding of how and why it is we can perceive colour and depth in our world and why it is we can see in the dark, but not as well as cats and other nocturnal animals! Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 23 Structure and Function of the Human Eye The pupil: is the small black circle at the centre of each of our eyes. The pupil controls the amount of light taken in by the eye. In dark conditions our pupil dilates to its maximum size in order to maximise the amount of light entering the eye and thus our ability to see (not too well) in the dark. In light conditions our pupil also dilates, or ‘shrinks’, in response to the intensity of light we experience. Pupil size is controlled by our autonomic nervous system (ANS) [controls organs and glands]. The ANS is linked to our central nervous system (CNS) [brain and spine]. Interestingly the ANS has two branches: i. ii. the parasympathetic branch which in this instance changes pupil size in response to illumination, and, the sympathetic branch which dilates the pupil under conditions of strong emotional arousal. The two branches of the ANS are self-regulating rather than under our conscious control. Regulatory control of the ANS is directed by the hypothalamus found in the brain. Both branches of the ANS are what psychologists call antagonistic to one another. In the eye, dilation of the pupil is controlled by the iris (the coloured part of our eye) via the ciliary muscles which are found in the iris. The lens of the eye is held in place by suspensory ligaments. Much like a camera the lens focuses light on the retina as an inverted or upside down image. The shape the lens forms as it focuses light energy on the retina is controlled by ciliary muscles. The lens thickens and increases in curvature when focusing on nearby objects and becomes flatter when we are focusing on objects far away. The retina which is found at the back of the eye, upon which images we see are thrown, has three layers. Each of the layers helps explain certain human visual abilities. The first layer of the retina contains what are called rods and cones. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 24 Rods and cones are photosensitive transducer cells which convert light energy into electrical nerve impulses. Our 120M rods help us see in ever decreasing light. Our 7M cones allow us to experience chromatic (colour vision). Different cone types respond to the three primary colours of red; green and blue. This is because of the different wavelengths each of these colours has. Mixtures of red, green and blue allow us to experience all the colours found in the colour spectrum. Bipolar cells, a second layer in the retina, are connected to our rods and cones and help relay information to the visual cortex in our brain. Finally in the retina we find a third layer called ganglion cells. Ganglion cell fibres (axons) help form the beginnings of the optic nerve. Three types of ganglion cell ‘fire’ in response to the contours and movement of objects in our visual array or field of vision. The visual pathway from each eye to the visual cortex in our brain is called the optic nerve (see diagram on next page). Each optic nerve converges and crosses over at the optic chiasma, thus information from our right eye goes to the left visual cortex and information from our right eye goes to the left visual cortex. At the visual cortex, we find three other types of cell structure. These again give those with an interest in cognitive processes in general and visual perception in particular clues as to why we can see as we do. These three cell structures are as follows: Simple cells which have been found to respond to simple features of a stimulus i.e. straight lines, edges, slits, etc. when found in a particular orientation, or way-up, in our visual field. Complex cells which are found to respond to lines of particular orientation wherever found in our visual field, and, Hypercomplex cells which deal with the length of visual stimuli. Our Visual Pathway: Note how information from our right visual field goes to the left hand side of our brain, and information from our left visual field goes to the right hand side of our brain. This is one of the clues as to why we perceive our world in three dimensions. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 25 Where all sensory information ends up to be processed and understood This is of course in our brain, the organ concerned with consciousness. Consciousness is our awareness of our self and of our environment. It involves our senses and bodily structure plus perception; attention; language; thinking and memory. Human consciousness is what makes us particularly unique as organisms. The human brain is a relatively small structure, weighing about 14 kg (3.1lb) and making up about 2 per cent of total body weight. It is contained within the skull, which acts as a protective casing. Although the brain is only a small proportion of overall body weight, information received about the outside world and from the rest of the body converges at the brain to be processed. Sensations ultimately arrive at the human brain to be processed, given meaning (understood) and acted upon. We first begin to perceive what a particular sensation is (and therefore what it is we are experiencing) on the basis of how good, bad or indifferent our various senses are individually and collectively (cross modal transfer). How we reach individual understanding of what particular sensations mean for us is further based on some innate abilities and any previous past experiences we have had in connection with the stimulus concerned. These aspects of perception will now be addressed. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 26 FACTORS AFFECTING PERCEPTION: EXPLAINING OUR PERCEPTUAL WORLD The principles of perceptual organisation • gestalten • figure & ground: the answer to illusions? • Perceptual constancies, with particular reference to, depth perception Gestalt psychology and gestalten As was stated at the beginning of our cognitive psychology course, one way of looking at perception is from the point of view of Gestalt psychology (Kohler; Koffka and Wertheimer). It is easily understood. Gestalt psychology, which was in vogue in Germany in the early part of the 20th century, believes we have an innate disposition to see objects using our inbuilt principles of grouping, or Gestalten. What gestalten means is our innate ability to construct our world in terms of organised ‘wholes’. The principles behind gestalten are captured by their Law of Pragnänz which is us, the organism’s, innate way of perceiving things in terms of symmetry; uniformity and stability. Individual Gestalten are Proximity Objects that are close together are perceived by us as a ‘whole’ e.g. a) b) . . . . . . ....................... How do you perceive a) and b) above? Similarity Similar objects are normally perceived by us as belonging to the same group Above, what similar objects do you perceive as a group which do not exist?! Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 27 Continuity Sensations appearing to create a continuous form are perceived by us as belonging together e.g. a fence with slats missing is still perceived as a fence. We organise sensations appearing together to form a continuous whole e.g. x x x x x x x x x x Do you perceive the above as a square shape made up of X’s, or 10 separate and individual X’s? Closure Where we, at an unconscious level, fill in contours/gaps in stimuli to form a complete whole in order to make perceptual sense of it NB the importance of previous past experience and perception. This is illustrated below. What do you make of this stimuli? Your teacher/lecturer should explain the significance of this simple demonstration. The whole is greater than the sum of its parts Texture This is another principle of gestalten where objects of the same texture are perceived as belonging to the same group e.g. grains of sand and pebbles at the seaside form for us a ‘beach’. Simplicity Where we have a tendency to group stimulus features together in a way that provides the simplest interpretation of the world for us e.g. houses as opposed to their make-up e.g. windows, doors, roofs, walls and ceilings. The notion of simplicity does have a link with the social psychological phenomenon of stereotyping and attribution theory. Common Fate This strange term means the principles where individual objects moving together at the same rate are perceived by us as a group. We group, by common fate, flocks of seagulls and swarms of wasps. We do not perceive each individual in the flock or swarm. We innately organise the stimuli into a ‘whole’ in order to perceive and understand it. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 28 The principles behind gestalten and gestalt psychology, which are biological in origin, go some way to explain perception. Perception is more than mere ‘seeing’. We often find in psychology that abilities which seem to be innate have survival value for us. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 29 Figure and Ground: The Answer to Illusions? What do you make of the visual stimuli below? This stimuli is known in psychology as Rubin’s Vase and is often used to demonstrate illusions. As you might be able to ‘see’ from Rubin’s Vase our perceptual processes try with this 2D image by putting some aspect of the stimuli to the ‘front’ or foreground (figure) and another aspect to the meaningless background (ground). Some part of the image always stands out as ‘figure’ and some other part ‘ground’. With Rubin’s vase the principle is reversible. What is meant here is that you will either perceive a vase to the front or two faces squaring up to each other! What you perceive (above, a vase or faces) is related to 1 our expectations 2 our culture 3 our experience of the stimuli 4 our motivation. Please answer the questions which follow concerning the effect expectations, culture and experience (in particular) have on perception. Perceptual Set? Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 30 What do you make of the stimuli below? Give an explanation for your answer. E A D 13 A 16 15 14 B 12 the cat sat on the map and licked its whiskers Questions 1 Whom do you see in the image below? If you see anyone at all (!) is it as a consequence of expectations, culture or experience? Give reasons for your answer. 2 Without looking back to the previous page, what is the saying in the triangle? Please now look back and read what is inside the triangle carefully. What does it actually say? Why were you so sure about what you thought it said the first time round? How was your perception affected? The general effect of expectations; culture and experience is called perceptual set. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 31 3. Whom do you perceive in the image below? What factors, expectations, culture and/or experience could influence your perception of who this is? Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 32 4. The most famous illusion in psychology? What do you make of the picture above? Young Woman or Old? That depends on your interpretation. Young people tend to see a young girl; older people, an elderly lady. With effort, you can switch from one to the other: the young woman's chin becomes the old woman's nose; the old woman's mouth, a band on the neck of the young woman. You will find that here expectations, motivation, culture and experience do not help you if you first see a young woman then an old woman, then a young woman then a ...etc! By American psychologist E.G. Boring Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 33 Motivation and Perception Motivation, in psychology, concerns what prompts us to action or to behave in a particular way. Motivation can effect our perception of objects, events, people stimuli etc. Motivation in psychology is a study all in itself. Cognitive psychology understands that our motivation to behave in a particular way is affected by two things. Our • internal biology and • external environment. Sometimes our internal biology and external environment come together to make us think, feel and behave in a particular way. The interaction of our biology and environment as they motivate us to be more perceptually aware produce interesting and - expensive - results. When our body needs fuel (i.e. food) it tells us so when we experience hunger pangs. This is our body’s internal signal or cue to us to eat. Our biology affects our perception in that when we are hungry and experience hunger pangs, we perceive food much more vividly. Do not go food shopping in a modern supermarket if you are hungry. The fruit appears more appealing, the home-baking more delicious, the meat and fish more tasty looking! We perceive the colour of food more. We perceive the smell of food more. Perception of food is heightened by internal bodily factors. External factors like the clever use of lighting to illuminate fruit colouring more and the aroma of the bakery in the supermarket constantly baking bread and cakes also influence and motivate us to perceive food more when hungry. The internal and external factors of motivation will greatly influence your perception of food and in this situation you may end up buying and spending much more than you needed! Supermarkets of course are aware of this. A knowledge of cognitive psychology can affect your waistline, wallet and purse. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 34 Perceptual Constancy So far we have identified that perception depends upon bodily structures and processes; our innate principles of gestalten and us imposing three dimensional meaning on two dimensional visual experience on the basis of expectations, culture and experience; sometimes wrongly! Perception is influenced by all these factors working individually and together. Where this all ultimately happens is within the human brain centring on the hypothalamus in particular. We also have a perceptual ability (called together perceptual constancies), which helps us to perceive our world. Where our perceptual constancies of size, shape, brightness and depth come from is not fully understood. Cognitive psychologists disagree as to whether they come about as a consequence of our biology and genetic inheritance (nature) or as the result of learning and experience (nurture). As far as depth constancy is concerned, there is good psychological evidence to suggest that depth perception is innate. Perceptual constancy is our ability to perceive sameness of visual stimuli even when the sensory evidence is to the contrary. We have a perceptual constancy in the four areas below which also help us make sense of our perceptual world. 1 2 3 4. Size Shape Brightness Depth Perceptual constancies occur when our brains correct or modify our rapidly changing sensory inputs to give us a more constant perception of the world. For example, size constancy ensures that as we watch a friend walk off into the distance, although the image of the person projected onto our retina is rapidly decreasing in size, we do not perceive that our friend is actually shrinking! The knowledge that as the proximal stimulus (the internal sensory image) changes, the distal stimulus (the external object being perceived) does not, allows us to correct our sensations and maintain constant perceptions. Cognitive psychology has over the years had three theories about perception. It is in the area of perceptual constancies that much supporting evidence to which theory is the more acepted has emerged. These theories are summarised overleaf. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 35 Three Theories of Perception 1 Bottom-Up (Ecological View): (Gibson; Gestalt - ‘organised wholes’): where it is said we perceive most clues from our environment directly and without interpretation (we perceive ‘exactly’ what we see/hear/smell/touch and taste). The ecological view explains perception as being ‘bottom-up’ - our world impinges on our senses which processes the information to the brain which is directly interpreted and understood. Bottom-Up theory cannot explain illusions. 2 Top-Down (Constructivist View): (Gregory; Bruner) who say our perceptual system must often make a reality indirectly out of bits of sensory information due to the absence of other information. They are top-down in their explanation of perception. Higher level cognitive functions play a part. 3 Top-Down/Bottom-Up (Symbiotic View): (Neisser: Marr) who say we use both top-down and bottom-up information processes in the perception of our reality. Nature and nurture both play a part. Both enhance perception individually and collectively. When one perceptual process, or aspect of it is impaired, the other process, or an aspect of it, ‘fills in’ or compensates to give us at the end of the day the best individual understanding of the image/object/event as possible. Symbiotic processing is best explained in the area of visual perception using Marr’s Computer AnalogyZKHUH KH VD\V ZH H[WUDFW YLVXDO LQIRUPDWLRQ IURP DQ image/object/event in four stages and put this all together again in the brain in what he calls a symbolic representation of the stimulus. Marr’s computational model as an explanation of visual perception is illuminating in that it helps emphasise the role of both bottom-up and top-down processing independently, collectively and integratively of each other. For a brief overview of Marr’s Computer Analogy see over. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 36 MARR’S COMPUTER ANALOGY Marr’s (1982) 4 Module Computational Theory Of Vision In an attempt to understand the complex make-up of our visual perception David Marr (1982) thinks neural activity transforms sensory (essentially visual) stimulation into our experience of reality. This is done gradually, by extracting and deconstructing specific information from the object we ‘see’, in four stages and then putting all this information together again in our attempt to recognise and understand what is we are (visually) perceiving. This he calls a symbolic representation. 1. The image or grey-level description Represented by the intensity of light at each point in the retinal image. This allows us to discover the boundaries of and regions in the image. Marr thinks our ability to identify boundaries and regions on this basis is the beginning of visual perception. 2. Primal sketch Here Marr says we go on to identify surface markings, object boundaries and markings using the gestalt principles of grouping. 3. 2½-D sketch A third stage where in the deconstruction of an image we give it depth and orientation. It is not yet 3-D. Object recognition needs the input matched against memory so that non-visible points are accounted for (perceptual constancy). 4. 3-D Model representation The nature and construction of the object is at this final stage confirmed/denied using higher level top-down processing functions and abilities. This gives rise to a symbolic representation of our visual reality and is for Marr true object recognition. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 37 PERCEPTUAL ORGANISATION: FACTORS INFLUENCING PERCEPTION 1. Size Constancy As previously explained, size constancy refers to the fact that although retinal images of objects get smaller as the object recedes into the distance, we perceive that the object does not change in size. Taking size constancy into account the constructivist view of perception maintains that size constancy develops because we learn through experience that objects do not actually shrink as they move away from us. Some cross-cultural evidence is consistent with this view, in that sometimes people from the dense jungle or heavily wooded regions, who are not accustomed to viewing objects at a distance, mistakenly perceive distant objects as being very small. Ecological direct-perception theory on the other hand maintains that size constancy occurs as a direct result of the information taken in by our senses and that failure to conserve size results only when the situation does not provide us with enough direct sensory information. 2. Shape Constancy Shape constancy, is our ability, innate or learned, to perceive the shape of an object as being constant even although our retinal image of the object is changing. An example of this would be the chalkboard at the front of your class. Regardless of where you are sitting we all perceive the chalkboard as a rectangular shape despite the fact that we all have different retinal images of it depending upon where we are sitting. Constructivists, or top-down theorists, see shape constancy coming about as a product of learning in our environment whereas bottom-up theorists see shape constancy as being somehow innate and part of the experience of sensation. 3. Brightness Constancy Lightness, or brightness constancy refers to our perceptual ability to adapt to the situation where the illumination (brightness) of an object changes but we continue to perceive it’s brightness and colour as the same or constant. A white sheet of paper first perceived in bright sunlight will still appear white and of approximately the same shade when later perceived by us under the shade of a tree. Constructivists (top-down theory) explains brightness constancy in our learnt knowledge that objects do not change their ‘brightness’ as lighting conditions change. Ecological theory takes a bottom-up explanation of brightness constancy. It says that enough information is present in the sensory experience itself to allow us to maintain a constant (lightness) perception of the object. Which is more accurate is very difficult to ascertain. Because babies cannot communicate very well when born, we have little way of knowing if these constancies are present from birth, or learned (however quickly), as the result of experience. We may find a more accurate explanation of the origins of perceptual constancies in an examination of depth perception. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 38 Depth Perception An awareness of depth perception will help us understand why it is we visually sense the world in two dimensions (like a photograph) but perceive what we see in three dimensions! This is demonstrated below: This is a 2D diagram called an Impossible Triangle. It is obviously two-dimensional. Are you currently experiencing it in three dimensions? If so, why? Shut one eye. Do you still experience this in 3D? If you are this definitely contradicts direct perception bottom-up theory as an explanation perception. Why is this the case? Without depth perception we would find (among other things) walking, reaching driving and playing games difficult. We see depth in our visual world because of i) monocular depth cues and ii) binocular depth cues. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 39 Monocular depth cues Monocular depth cues come about due to seeing the world with one eye; or our two individual eyes singularly...think about it!! Even if we visually experience our world with one eye, and the image which is striking our retina is definitely two-dimensional, where things are in our visual field allow us understand or perceive our world in three dimensions. Monocular depth cues include things like interposition, linear perspective and relative size. • Interposition One monocular depth cue we call Interposition. Interposition is the monocular cue we use to perceive depth when we see a scene where one object is partially obscuring another. The object we can fully see we perceive as nearer than the partially obscured object - which we perceive as behind. If your teacher or lecturer sits down behind their desk we can adjudge that the desk is nearer you than they are: you can fully see the desk and only the top half of their body. The interposition of the desk and the teacher/lecturer is here a monocular depth cue. • Linear Perspective Linear perspective is another monocular depth cue. If we see two parallel lines converge into one another in the distance, this tells us about depth. This is easily demonstrated in a railway station. When it is safe look down at the railway lines. They are parallel to one another. Now look up the track and you will see the rails converge (come in on one another). This is linear perspective. If we see this happening this is a monocular cue to depth or distance. Linear perspective can even work in two dimensions i.e. Which of the two horizontal lines above is larger? The one at the top or the one at the bottom? Measure them. What do you find? Why did you perceive what you did? Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 40 • Relative size Relative size is another cue for monocular depth. Relative size occurs when we see two objects, like two houses, against their backgrounds in our visual field. We see in two dimensions, that one house is smaller than the other. Indeed the 2D visual information we receive about the smaller of the two houses would appear to suggest it is the size of a match-box! This is not the case. We perceive on the basis of relative size that the visually smaller house is further away from us. The larger of the two we perceive as nearer. If you are out hillwalking you become absolutely certain that the smaller a hill is when looked at in comparison to others in your visual field the further away it will be! Binocular Depth Cues Binocular depth cues are easy to understand. Binocular depth cues come about due to the fact we have two eyes. Binocular cues to depth in our visual field result because each of our eyes receives a slightly different picture of the same scene. Our nose sees to that. The dual and overlapping picture we get as a result is called stereopsis. Stereopsis give us binocular cues to depth because most of us enjoy binocular (two eye) vision. We can lose the ability for binocular depth cues to our visual world due to a blow to the head (that gives us double vision) or damage to the eye due to strabismus (squints), etc. Sports people generally have excellent binocular vision. You will find that professional sports people whose game involves a ball of some description are usually excellent at other ball sports they take as a ‘hobby’. The world famous snooker player, Stephen Hendry is an excellent golfer. The goalkeeper Andy Goram has Scottish caps for both football and cricket. Ian Botham played both professional cricket and professional football. If we have two eyes, when objects get closer to us, each eye turns inwards. As objects, or percepts, move further away each eye turns outwards. The brain interprets this as a binocular cue to how near or how far the percept (image/object) is from us. This inward and outward movement of our eyes in response to how near or how far a percept is from us is called binocular convergence. Because each eye has a slightly different view of the same visual world, this similar, but different information is also used to judge depth. The closer each retinal image (or picture) is to one another, the nearer our brain interprets an object is to us. This is called binocular disparity. We therefore achieve depth perception due to monocular and binocular depth cues. Monocular depth cues comes from our visual environment. Binocular depth cues arise because we have two eyes. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 41 The biology of the human body gives rise to binocular depth cues. Our external environment gives us monocular depth cues. Interposition Monocular Depth Cues Relative Size Linear Perspective Depth Perception Binocular Convergence Binocular Depth Cues Binocular Disparity Perception: Conclusion Perception is our way of successfully monitoring what is happening around us in our environment. In order to do this we firstly use our senses, of which there are at least five. All our senses have physiological properties which convert one type of external energy into electrical energy. Cross-modal transfer (the co-ordination and transfer) of sensory information allows us to very quickly sense what is happening around us. Our senses send electrical impulses to our brain, the intensity and degree of signals being translated by our brain ‘telling’ us what it is we are experiencing. Our perception of sensory experience often involves, and is influenced by, all our cognitive processes: perception; attention; language; memory and thinking. Perception is selectively sensing something plus the brains ability to integrate, interpret and understand this sensory input on the basis of meaningful past experience - which forms, affects and influences attention; language; memory and thinking. Cognitive processes are individually and collectively our higher level (brain) functions. They influence perception as perception influences them. Three theories exist to explain perception. One advocates a ‘nature’ explanation of perception; the other advocates the contribution of ‘nurture’. The third takes an essentially nature and nurture position on the explanation of perception. Bottom up theorists like JJ Gibson say we interpret experiences directly using our senses alone. We perceive our reality directly. Our senses analyse stimuli into basic features i.e. the biology and physiology of the eye allows features of objects such as colour; motion; orientation; light and darkness to be interpreted by the brain as ultimately meaning something. Bottom-up theory is lacking. It cannot explain, for example why we perceive illusions. Direct visual evidence of reality is in conflict with our perception of reality. Another theory of perception, top-down processing, as put forward by for example RL Gregory may help better explain perception. Top-down processing says we make our perceptual reality out of bits of sensory information. We perceive our reality indirectly. This process is guided by higher level functions and psychological factors like expectation; culture; experience and motivation. Perception for Gregory is therefore top-down. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 42 A kind of compromise theory is a third theory of perception called symbiotic processing. Those who favour an explanation of perception on the basis of symbiotic processing would include Neisser and Marr. Symbiotic processing theory sees a place for both bottom-up (from the senses) and top-down (from the brain) theories of perception. When one type of perceptual process, or aspect of it is impaired, the other process, or an aspect of the other process ‘fills in’ or compensates to give us, at the end of the day, as good an understanding of the stimulus as possible.6\PELRWLF processing is best illustrated in the area of visual perception using Marr’s Computer DUU¶V &RPSXWHU $QDORJ\ VD\V ZH WDNH YLVXDO LQIRUPDWLRQ IURP DQ Analogy 0 image/object/event in four stages and put it all together again in the brain in what he calls a symbolic representation of the stimulus. Marr’s computational model as an explanation of visual perception is helpful in that it helps emphasise the role of both bottom-up and top-down processing integratively and independently. TopDown/Bottom-Up processes working together both contribute, in an enriched way, to our understanding of the fascinating topic of perception. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 43 Glossary: Perception attention: our higher level cognitive process which allows us to focus on selected aspects of our environment. absolute thresholds: the value given to a stimulus when we can detect it 50% of the time. The threshold at which we can notice a stimulus e.g. the smell of food differs among and between people, and can be affected by an individuals physical state; time of day; motivation; the way the stimulus is presented etc. auditory sense: this sense comes to us via our ear. Our ear is our organ of hearing and balance. It is composed of three parts - external, middle, and internal - the greater part of which is enclosed within the temporal bone. Within the inner ear the disposition and orientation of endolymph helps us experience our sense of balance. autonomic nervous system (ANS): part of our body and bodily processes which controls organs and glands. It is linked to our central nervous system (CNS) or brain and spine. binocular convergence: occurs as our eyes turn inwards when something gets closer to us. It is a binocular depth cue. binocular disparity: another binocular cue to depth: it occurs because we get two slightly different and overlapping images of the same scene. This information when processed in the visual cortex is a valuable binocular depth cue. binocular depth cues: the clues to depth we get because of the unique biological and physiological properties of our eyes. bipolar cells: a second layer in the retina, these cells are connected to our rods and cones and help relay information to the visual cortex in our brain. brain: the organ concerned with consciousness. Consciousness is our awareness plus perception; attention; language; thinking and memory) and the overall control of the body. cognitive maps: an animal. such as a rats, ability to actively learn from it’s experiences. Edward Tolman (1930) in his rats in mazes experiment discovered that rats formed these in order to make them more efficient and effective in their environment. The human equivalent is called a neural network. cognitive processes: a collective term for perception; attention; language; memory and thinking. complex cells: these cells in our visual cortex which respond to lines of particular orientation of a stimulus wherever found in our visual field. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 44 cones: photosensitive transducer cells which convert light energy into electrical nerve impulses. Around 7M of these allow us to experience chromatic (colour vision). Different types respond to the three primary colours of red; green and blue. This is because of the different wavelengths each of these colours have. Various mixtures of red, green and blue allow us to experience all the colours found in the colour spectrum. constructivist View: a theory of indirect perception put forward by RL Gregory and Jerome Bruner which says our perceptual system must often make a reality indirectly out of bits of sensory information due to the absence of other information. The constructivist view is better known as top-down processing. ecological View: a theory of direct perception put forward by JJ Gibson and the Gestalt school which says where it is said we perceive most clues from our environment directly and without interpretation (we perceive ‘exactly’ what we see/hear/smell/touch and taste). The ecological view is popularly known as bottomup processing. experimental method: the scientific practice of gathering hard empirical data characterised by the experimenter’s ability to control and alter variables. figure & ground: a perceptual ability where we allocate one aspect of a stimulus to the foreground (front: figure) and another aspect of it to the background (ground). ganglion cells: the third layer of the retina these cell fibres (axons) help form the beginnings of the optic nerve. Three types of this type of cell ‘fire’ in response to the contours and movement of objects in our visual array or field of vision. Gestalt psychology: a school or an approach founded in Germany in the early part of the 20th century by Kohler; Koffka and Wertheimer. Gestalt psychology, believes we have a innate disposition to see objects using our inbuilt principles of grouping, or Gestalten. What gestalten means is our innate ability to construct our world in terms of organised ‘wholes’. The principles behind gestalten are captured by their Law of Pragnänz which is us, the organism’s, innate way of perceiving things in terms of symmetry; uniformity and stability. gestalten: innate principles of grouping which human beings have which help us organise our perceptual world. Individual gestalten are - proximity (objects that are close together are perceived by us as a ‘whole’; similarity (similar objects are perceived by us as belonging to the same group): continuity (sensations which create a continuous form are perceived by us as belonging together): closure (where we fill in contours/gaps in stimuli to form a complete whole in order to make perceptual sense of it): texture (objects of the same texture are perceived as belonging to the same group): simplicity (where we group stimulus features together in a way that gives us the simplest interpretation of our world): common fate ( where individual objects moving together at the same rate are perceived by us as a group): gustatory sense: taste, another of the five senses, affected by the contact of soluble substances on our tongue Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 45 hypercomplex cells: these cells found in our visual cortex deal with the length of visual stimuli. hypothetical constructs: a term given to names psychologists give to things which do not exist in reality. You cannot yet go into a chemist and ask for 250mg of ‘id’! introspection: asking people to think about, describe and explain the affect a stimulus has on them. It is subjective self-report which is not a very scientific method of enquiry. interposition: a monocular depth cue It is the monocular cue to depth we use when we see one object partially obscuring another. The object we can fully see we perceive as nearer than the partially obscured object - which we perceive as behind. iris: the coloured part of our eye. kinaesthetic sense: a ‘sixth’ sense from within our own bodies which tells us about movement, or the feel of our muscles or joints. This sense tells us about balance. language: a very important cognitive process which allows us to communicate with others. It depends on biological maturation of the brain i.e. Wernicke’s area, Broca’s area. lens: the part of our eye which operates like a camera held in place by suspensory ligaments. It focuses light on the retina as an inverted or upside down image. linear perspective: another monocular depth cue. If we see two parallel lines converge into one another in the distance this cue tells us about depth. memory: this cognitive process allows us to keep and remember information about events and experiences in our life. monocular depth cues: the clues to depth we get from our external environment. motivation: concerns what prompts us to action or to behave in a particular way. It can effect our perception of objects, events, people stimuli etc. as a result of both our internal biology and external environment. nature-nurture debate: a argument concerning whether we can be explained wholly from the point of view of our biology or genetics or whether we are as we are as the result of experiences in our environment i.e. the way we are brought up. In many instances in psychology where the debate arises e.g. intelligence or personality neither of the two views are correct. Our intelligence and our personality are the consequence of our genetics and environment working together. neural network: formed by human beings as a result of their experiences in their environment. It is the outcome of all our cognitive processes working together as influenced by our experiences. The animal equivalent, called a cognitive map, was discovered by Edward Tolman (1930) in his rats in mazes experiment. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 46 olfactory sense: smell, is one of the five senses by which odours are perceived. The nose, equipped with olfactory nerves, is the special organ of smell. optic chiasma: where each optic nerve converges and crosses over. Information from our right eye goes to the left visual cortex and information from our right eye goes to the left visual cortex. optic nerve: the visual pathway from each eye to the visual cortex in our brain. organisms: behaviourist term for humans and animals. perception: a key cognitive process, is our way of successfully monitoring what is happening around us in our environment using our senses, innate abilities and previous past experience to make sense of our world. perceptual constancies: those abilities we have to make our world constant i.e. size, shape, brightness and depth. They arise due to our biology; genetics and experience. perceptual set: is unconsciously used by us to perceive what we perceive as a result of expectations; culture; experience and motivation ie the cat sat on the map. psychophysics: a topic within psychology researching into the relationship and interface between a physical stimulus and our subjective experience of it. First popularised by see Wilhelm Wundt, 1879 with his study into perception using introspection. pupil: is the black circle at the centre of each of our eyes. The pupil controls the amount of light taken in by the eye. It gives us our ability to see (not too well) in the dark. relative size: another monocular depth cue. We apply it when we see two objects, like two houses, against their backgrounds in our visual field. We see in two dimensions, that one house is smaller than the other. We perceive however that in reality this is not the case. retina: this structure is found at the back of the eye upon which the images we see are thrown. rods : photosensitive transducer cells which convert light energy into electrical nerve impulses. Over 120M of them help us see in ever decreasing light. Rubin’s Vase: a famous illusion in psychology which demonstrates the perceptual ability of figure-ground: even in two dimensions. Our perceptual processes try with this 2D image to put some aspect of the stimuli to the ‘front’ or foreground (figure) and another aspect to the meaningless background (ground). What you perceive above, a vase or faces is related to expectations; culture; experience of the stimuli and motivation. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 47 sensation: a sensory process explaining how we receive information from our environment in the first place. Senses: the means by which we receive information from our external world i.e. sight, hearing, touch, taste, smell. simple cells: these cells in our visual cortex respond to simple features of a stimulus i.e. straight lines; edges; slits etc. when found in a particular orientation, or way-up, in our visual field. stereopsis: the dual and overlapping visual picture we get when seeing. It is responsible for binocular vision. stimuli: an event or feeling which prompts some response from us i.e. swearing when you burn your finger! survey: a research method often used in social psychology to gather data about attitudes etc. based on replies to standardised written questions. tactile sense: touch, is one of our five senses, allowing for our perception of pressure, touch, temperature (principally temperature change), pain, and hair movement. thinking: a complex ‘whole-brain’ cognitive process which allows us to problemsolve in our environment. Tolman: a soft behaviourist famous for his rats in mazes experiment (1930) which led to the belief that humans are active, as opposed to passive, learners. Top-Down/Bottom-Up Symbiotic View: a theory of perception which brings together aspects of top-down and bottom-up theories of perception. Advocated by the likes of Neisser it says we use the most appropriate of the above two processes depending on the situation we find ourself in. The two processing models work together. When one type of perceptual process, or aspect of it is impaired, the other process, or an aspect of the other process compensates. vision: in many respects our key sense. It is the most studied information process by cognitive psychologists. This sense comes to us via our eyes, the structure and location of which allow us to perceive our world in 3 dimensions; sense colour; sense depth etc. percepts: the objects/images/events we perceive. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 48 STUDENT ACTIVITY : MEMORY 1. What do you think the study of cognitive psychology is all about? Identify by ticking in the boxes below what a student of cognitive psychology might study in relation to Memory: the senses coding, storage, retrieval episodic memory psychophysics selection and attention memory for facts selection and attention meaning of stimuli motivation acoustic codes visual illusions chunking learning strategies forgetting [ [ [ [ [ [ [ [ [ [ [ [ [ [ Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) ] ] ] ] ] ] ] ] ] ] ] ] ] ] 49 2. Please fill in the blank spaces from the boxed word list below: monitoring what is happening around us in our environment higher level perception; attention; language; memory and thinking directly indirectly senses Bottom up theorists culture; experience and motivation symbiotic processing selectively sensing something top-down processing electrical energy illusions Cross-modal transfer conflict integrate, interpret meaningful past experience bits of sensory information ‘fills’ in or compensates four stages symbolic representation colour; motion; orientation; light and darkness computational working together. Perception is our way of successfully In order to do this we firstly use our , of which there are at least five. All our senses have physiological properties which convert one type of external energy into . (the co-ordination and transfer) of sensory information allows us to very quickly sense what is happening around us. Our senses send electrical impulses to our brain, the intensity and degree of signals being translated by our brain ‘telling’ us what it is we are experiencing. Our perception of sensory experience often involves, and is influenced by, all our cognitive processes: . Perception is plus the brains ability to and understand this sensory input on the basis of which forms, affects and influences attention; language; memory and thinking. Cognitive processes are individually and collectively our (brain) functions. They influence perception as perception influences them. Three theories exist to explain perception. One advocates a ‘nature’ explanation of perception; the other advocates the contribution of ‘nurture’. The third takes an essentially nature and nurture position on the explanation of perception. using our like JJ Gibson say we interpret experiences senses alone. We perceive our reality directly. Our senses analyse stimuli into basic features i.e. the biology and physiology of the eye allows features of objects such as to be interpreted by the brain as ultimately meaning something. Bottom-up theory is lacking. It cannot explain, for example why we perceive . Direct visual evidence of reality is in with our perception of reality. Another theory of perception, , as put forward by for example RL Gregory helps better explain perception. Top-down processing says we make our perceptual reality out of . We perceive our reality . This process is guided by higher level functions and psychological factors like expectation; . Perception for Gregory is therefore top-down. A kind of compromise theory is a third theory of perception called . Those who favour an explanation of perception on the basis of symbiotic processing would include Neisser and Marr. Symbiotic processing theory sees a place for both bottomup (from the senses) and top-down (from the brain) theories of perception. When one type of perceptual process, or aspect of it is impaired, the other process, or an aspect of the other process to give us, at the end of the day, as good an understanding of the stimulus as possible. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 50 Symbiotic processing is best illustrated in the area of visual perception using Marr’s Computer Analogy 0DUU¶V &RPSXWHU $QDORJ\ VD\V ZH WDNH YLVXDO LQIRUPDWLRQ IURP an image/object/event in and put it all together again in the brain in what he calls a of the stimulus. Marr’s model as an explanation of visual perception is helpful in that it helps emphasise the role of both bottom-up and top-down processing integratively and independently. Top-Down/Bottom-Up processes both contribute: ultimately in an enriched way: to our understanding of the fascinating topic of perception. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 51 3. Perception Gigantic Wordsearch Solve the clues to the Gigantic Wordsearch that follows. Put a line through the correct word or phrase in the wordsearch and write your answer beside the clue. CLUES Photosensitive transducer cells which convert light energy into electrical nerve impulses. Over 120M of these help us see in ever decreasing light. The means by which we receive information from our external world i.e. sight: hearing: touch: taste: smell. Where each optic nerve converges and crosses over. Information from our right eye goes to the left visual cortex and information from our right eye goes to the left visual cortex. Part of our body and bodily processes that controls organs and glands. In many respects our key sense. It is the most studied information process of all by cognitive psychologists. This sense comes to us via our eyes, the structure and location of which allow us to perceive our world in 3 dimensions; sense colour; sense depth, etc. Touch is one of our five senses, allowing for our perception of pressure, touch, temperature (principally temperature change), pain, and hair movement. A theory of indirect perception put forward by RL Gregory and Jerome Bruner that says our perceptual system must often make a reality indirectly out of bits of sensory information due to the absence of other information. The constructivist view is better known as top-down processing. A ‘sixth’ sense. This sense tells us about balance. The value given to a stimulus when we can detect it 50% of the time. A topic within psychology researching into the relationship and interface between a physical stimulus and our subjective experience of it. First popularised by Wilhelm Wundt, 1879 with his study into perception using introspection. A key cognitive process, is our way of successfully monitoring what is happening around us in our environment using our senses, innate abilities and previous past experience. A very important cognitive process which allows us to communicate with others. Taste, another of the five senses, affected by the contact of soluble substances on our tongue A collective term for perception, attention, language, memory and thinking. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 52 The scientific practice of gathering hard empirical data characterised by the experimenter’s ability to control and alter variables. A research method based on replies to standardised written questions. An event or feeling which prompts some response from us A term given to names psychologists give to things which do not exist in reality. Our cognitive process which allows us to focus on selected aspects of our environment. Problem-solving. A soft behaviourist famous for his rats in mazes experiment (1930). An animal’s ability to actively learn from it’s experiences (e.g. a rat). Behaviourist term for humans and animals. Formed by human beings as a result of their experiences in their environment. It is the outcome of all our cognitive processes working together as influenced by our experiences. The animal equivalent, called a cognitive map, was discovered by Edward Tolman (1930) in his rats in mazes experiment. A theory of direct perception put forward by JJ Gibson and the Gestalt school which says where it is said we perceive most clues from our environment directly and without interpretation. The ecological view is popularly known as bottom-up processing. An argument arising out of whether our genetics or our environment influences us the most. How we receive information from our environment in the first place. Brain and spine. The coloured part of our eye The part of our eye which operates like a camera held in place by suspensory ligaments. It focuses light on the retina as an inverted or upside down image. Photosensitive transducer cells which convert light energy into electrical nerve impulses. Around 7M of these allow us to experience chromatic (colour vision). A second layer in the retina, these cells are connected to our rods and cones and help relay information to the visual cortex in our brain. The third layer of the retina these cell fibres (axons) help form the beginnings of the optic nerve. The visual pathway from each eye to the visual cortex in our brain. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 53 These cells in our visual cortex respond to simple features of a stimulus i.e. straight lines; edges; slits etc. when found in a particular orientation, or way-up, in our visual field. The organ concerned with consciousness. Consciousness is our awareness plus perception; attention; language; thinking and memory) and the overall control of the body. This structure is found at the back of the eye upon which the images we see are thrown. Innate principles of grouping which human beings have which help us organise our perceptual world. A perceptual ability where we allocate one aspect of a stimulus to the foreground (front:) and another aspect of it to the background (background). Those abilities we have to make our world constant even although sensory evidence is to the contrary i.e. size, shape, brightness and depth constancies. They arise due to our biology; genetics and experience. A school or an approach founded in Germany in the early part of the 20th century by Kohler; Koffka and Wertheimer. Edward Tolman (1930) in his rats in mazes experiment discovered that rats formed these in order to make them more efficient and effective in their environment. The human equivalent is called a neural network. These cells found in our visual cortex deal with the length of visual stimuli. This sense comes to us via our ear. A famous illusion in psychology which demonstrates the perceptual ability of figure-ground: even in two dimensions. A theory of perception which brings together aspects of top-down and bottom-up theories of perception. Advocated by the likes of Neisser it says we use the most appropriate of top-down/bottom-up processes depending on the situation we find ourself in. These cells in our visual cortex respond to lines of particular orientation of a stimulus wherever found in our visual field. Smell, is one of the five senses by which odours are perceived. The nose, equipped with olfactory nerves, is the special organ of smell. Is unconsciously used by us to perceive what we perceive - sometimes contrary to direct sensory experience. This phenomenon happens as a result of expectations; culture; experience and motivation i.e. the cat sat on the map. Concerns what prompts us to action or to behave in a particular way. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 54 It can effect our perception of objects, events, people stimuli etc. as a result of both our internal biology and external environment. The clues to depth we get from our external environment. The clues to depth we get because of the unique biological and physiological properties of our eyes. A monocular depth cue. It is the monocular cue to depth we use when we see one object partially obscuring another. Another monocular depth cue. If we see two parallel lines converge into one another in the distance this cue tells us about depth. The dual and overlapping visual picture we get when seeing. It is responsible for binocular vision. The objects/images/events we perceive. Occurs as our eyes turn inwards when something gets closer to us. It is a binocular depth cue. Another binocular cue to depth: it occurs because we get two slightly different and overlapping images of the same scene. This information when processed in the visual cortex is a valuable binocular depth cue. Another monocular depth cue. We apply it when we see two objects, like two houses, against their backgrounds in our visual field. We see in two dimensions, that one house is smaller than the other. We perceive however that in reality this is not the case. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 55 Perception: Gigantic Wordsearch perceptionexperimentalmethodsurveystimuli languagememorycognitiveprocessesattention hypotheticalconstructsthinkingTolman cognitive mapsorganismsneuralnetwork TheEcologicalViewsenseskinaestheticsenseiris nature-nurture debateTheConstructivistView sensationlensgustatorysenseauditorysense Top-Down/Bottom-UpSymbioticViewretina olfactorysensepsychophysicsintrospectionpupil absolutethresholdstactilesenserelativesize rodsconesautonomicnervoussystem(ANS) opticchiasmabinoculardisparitybipolarcells perceptualsetcentralnervoussystem(CNS)brain stereopsisganglioncellsopticnervesimplecells complexcellsfigure&groundlinearperspective perceptualconstanciesbinocularconvergence Gestaltpsychologyhypercomplexcellsgestalten perceptsmotivationRubin’sVaseinterposition monoculardepthcuesbinoculardepthcues Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 56 4. Quick Quiz 1. Identify our five cognitive processes. 2. What is perception? 3. How do we detect information in our environment? 4. What other names can you give for our visual; tactile; gustatory; olfactory and auditory senses? 5. What is the only type of energy our brains understand? 6. What properties do our senses have which is important in the above respect? 7. What do you call the property where all our senses operate individually and together to give our brain as much information about our world as possible? 8. Give an alternative name for the direct theory of perception? 9. Give an alternative name for the indirect theory of perception? 10. Which theory is the more acceptable to our understanding of perception? Give a reason for your answer. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 57 5. Please answer the following questions a) What is meant by perception in cognitive psychology? You should • define perception as an active cognitive process which has us processing, organising and interpreting information coming to us from our outside world • say how we do this i.e. via our senses • give an example • say what you think perception does for us i.e. it’s function is to give us an accurate ‘picture’ of the world around us in order that we can operate successfully within it, and that sometimes this accurate picture can be wrong • refer to illusions and/or perceptual constancies and/or factors influencing perception. b) Read the following scenarios: A girl looking at her watch. A boy reading a book. A mountaineer climbing a cliff-face. A child working out a jig-saw. Choose one of these and describe the role of perception in this situation. Your answer must • mention what perceptual process is being used in this situation (clue: it begins with a ‘P’!) • refer to the situation directly i.e. ‘In telling the time....’ • identify one/some of the senses being used, and make mention of what these senses are doing for us • mention what happens when this information reaches our brain. Are other cognitive processes involved in this activity? If so what are they and why? • mention the influence of culture/experience/expectations/motivation - but only if appropriate to the situation chosen. c) One group of people were shown pictures of capital letters, another group were shown pictures of numbers. They were then shown the stimulus below and asked what they saw. What cognitive process is this a study of? What method of research is being used? What do you think participants reported seeing? What factors affected the participants’ perception of this stimulus? Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 58 KEY CONCEPTS IN COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY: MEMORY Memory, another of our cognitive processes, can be defined as a system for dealing with and organising incoming information coming to us from our outside world. It is the highest level function we have which allows us to keep and hold information about events and experiences that have happened to us in the past. The cognitive organisation of the things we remember and can recall is our memory. SQA ask that we consider in relation to the study of memory: • forms of remembering • episodic and semantic memory • encoding (registration), storage and retrieval • short-term memory and long-term memory. You should become familiar with the different forms which memory can take and that memory, similar to perception should be seen as an active process. We do things with our memories, individually and together, to operate in our environment. Memory thus acts in concert with perception and attention. We cannot attend to something unless we perceive it, and cannot perceive something unless we attend to it. Whether we perceive and attend to things often depends upon just how meaningful a stimulus is to us. Memory will play it’s part here. We have two types of memory. The first is episodic memory: which is memory of personal events, experiences and episodes. the second is semantic memory: which is memory for facts, the detail of which often depends on just how meaningful the fact was for us at the time. How we code, store and retrieve memories is of interest to us in the study of memory as is the breakdown in coding, storage or retrieval which results in the phenomenon we call ‘forgetting’. Memory does not just happen in one part of the brain. Different parts of the brain are responsible for different types of memory. With deliberate parallels found in computing these types of memory have been identified as short-term memory and long-term memory. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 59 Cognitive Psychology: Memory When You Are Old By William Butler Yeats WHEN you are old and grey and full of sleep, And nodding by the fire, take down this book, And slowly read, and dream of the soft look Your eyes had once, and of their shadows deep; How many loved your moments of glad grace, And loved your beauty with love false or true, But one man loved the pilgrim Soul in you, And loved the sorrows of your changing face; And bending down beside the glowing bars, Murmur, a little sadly, how Love fled And paced upon the mountains overhead And hid his face amid a crowd of stars.1 Memory is another cognitive process which we are asked to look at. The others are of course perception, attention, thinking and language. Memory is a cognitive process closely associated with, and, related to perception and attention. One need only consider ‘eye witness testimony’ to appreciate this. Because people never lie in court (!), cognitive psychologists who have lost their grip on reality might claim that it is their interface which is why differences in evidence arise. We perceive and attend to things on the basis of just how meaningful it is to us. The things in life which mean something to us are affected by our biology, culture, expectations and motivation. Would you look twice at a smartly dressed young man in a car park with his briefcase open on the top of a sports car? On the other hand, what would you think about a grubby, shell-suited young man lurking beside the vehicle? Why is this the case? In a recent BBC documentary into criminality, the smartly-dressed young man was a professional car thief. His briefcase covered his very successful efforts to get the lock out from the sports cars boot. He took the lock back to his own car. On the boot lock is a key code. As a result of breaking into a locksmiths (!) he had a tool which made a replica boot lock key - which obviously opens the car doors and turns the ignition. This he duly did and a top of the range BMW disappeared! 1 While a beautiful and meaningful poem in it’s own right ‘When You Are Old’ reminds the author of many semantic and episodic long-term memories he has concerning himself, his immediate and extended family, his collective unconscious, his friendships, his education, his neural networks, his politics, his personality, outlook etc. In tribute to my dear old dad, Arthur Keegan RIP. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 60 Memory may best be described as an information processing system for dealing with and organising incoming information. There are two traditions into memory research in psychology. The first was started by Ebbinghaus (1885) who used the experimental method to investigate, in particular, STM. He felt memory to be a ‘factual record’. The alternative tradition was established by Bartlett (1932) who saw meaning as important to memory. he saw memory as an active, dynamic cognitive process. Memory has different forms which are related to the type of stimulus we encounter i.e. episodic and semantic memory. Psychology sees memory as a hypothetical construct - where it’s findings based are used, and will continue to be used, by computer companies. At present it is generally accepted that the three main processes of memory involve encoding (also called registration), storage and retrieval. Any breakdown in encoding, storage or retrieval, either putting material into memory, or trying to get it out, is the reason why we ‘forget’. While cognitive psychologists take an information processing approach to memory see parallels with computers, they do not think a computer operates in exactly the same way as human memory...yet. This further interests the likes of Microsoft, as does the capacity and links between our short-term and long-term memories. What is Memory? ‘Memory; a cognitive process important to learning, thinking and problem solving, is the information process we use to store and retrieve information in our brain.’ GA Keegan (2000) Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 61 Where is Memory? Where memory occurs is a bit of a mystery. Research suggests some aspects of memory are associated with particular areas of our brain. It has been found we store and retrieve memories of/for sounds in our temporal lobe. We remember what we see using our visual cortex. We recognise and remember patterns using our parietal lobe. We store and retrieve our memories of faces in our frontal lobe. Memory is, thus, a whole-brain cognitive process. Memory of an event relies on widespread regions of the brain working together to create a whole (or gestalt). Thus, aspects of a particular memory are stored by us in our brains as separate sense modalities that have to be put together anew each time we come across a previously experienced stimuli (see Forms of Memory). Our perceptions first occupy an immediate, iconic memory (visual sensory memory), then move to short-term memory, from which some transfer to longterm stores (see Storage). Recall of what you are sensing depends upon past perceptual experience of it. Our perception of what it is we are sensing is often coloured by the memory we have of it. This is connected to the feelings that accompanied the event at the time. Dull events are hard to remember, life-changing ones are easy. Meaning is once again central to yet another cognitive process. Memory helps us ‘make sense’ of our reality based upon just how meaningful a stimulus is for us - either at that time or at some time in the past. What we remember, and how detailed and deeply we remember it, is kind of subjective for us in many ways. Where in the brain memory happens is problematic. Studies have shown that damage to the hippocampus (thought to be the centre of our emotions and autonomic nervous system) impairs episodic memory (personal events, experiences and episodes), but allows us to still deal with everyday life, The hippocampus is linked by nerve fibres to our cortex where visual, auditory motor memories, etc. are located - though’ not all together. Memories of whole events involve different parts of the brain (EEG & MRI). To remember or retrieve a memory we have to bring all the relevant ‘bits’ of it together. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 62 Forms of Remembering: Encoding (Registration): Storage & Retrieval of Memories Encoding or Registration The process we use when putting information into our memory is called encoding. Taking all our senses and sensory modalities into account this makes sense! We store/remember using the most relevant code dependent upon the type, or manner, of stimulus we come across. 1. Acoustic codes represent particular events stored and remembered as a sequence of sounds 2. Visual codes represent visual information stored and remembered as forms or images 3. Semantic codes get us involved in remembering things on the basis of their meaning for us. Holding memory over time is called storage. We come to store memories in three main ‘files’ each with a far greater capacity compared to what came before. These are called sensory register: short term memory and long term memory (stores). Forms of Remembering: Storage Sensory: Short- and Long-term Memory Storage Sensory Register which includes iconic memory holds information from all the senses, but only for a fraction of a second. It’s role makes us aware of our surroundings on a ‘real-time’ basis and helps us decide whether a particular stimulus is worth bothering about and thus involve a range of further, deeper and more extensive information processing. Sensory register is more a subconscious state of awareness of an event than a storage facility as such. Sensory register makes us aware of both visual and acoustic aspects of our world - but only for a fraction of a second. We would know it more as our awareness of the sensation of a breeze blowing off our cheek. A baby in a pram would utilise it’s semantic register more than an adult; why might this be? Short Term Memory (or ‘Working Memory’) receives the information that was perceived and selectively attended to in sensory register or retrieved from long term memory if no further processing occurs. STM has a duration of around 20-30 seconds. STM has two functions. STM helps us • to form and update a picture of our world on a minute-by-minute basis, and • to think and solve problems. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 63 Things you didn’t know about STM Encoding: cross-culturally people tend to use acoustic codes to encode information into STM. Visual codes tend to decay faster than acoustic codes. Storage Capacity: The immediate memory span is the number of items you can recall perfectly after one presentation of the stimulus. STM is between 5-9 chunks of information long (Miller, 1956). The Power of Chunking: the capacity and power of our STM can be increased by trying to remember bigger and bigger ‘chunks’ of information. Efficient chunking needs STM and LTM working together. This can be illustrated easily. Please take 30 seconds to memorise the following letter string sruoyylerecnis Now cover up the letter string - and without cheating - write down as many letters in sequence as you can remember. Because our STM capacity is only 7+2 units of information long, you will be hard pushed to get it right first, second or possibly even third time. However....think about the power of chunking and look at the stimulus again. Can you see how it could be committed to memory in two chunks? Duration of STM: Browne-Peterson procedure research results indicate that the duration of our STM is 20 seconds long and also that repeating something that has to be remembered over and over again (maintenance rehearsal using focussed attention) information can be held in STM almost indefinitely. You would not be able to do much else however: this is related to our more natural state of divided attention. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 64 Forms of Remembering: Storage Long Term Memory: 1. Encoding: Encoding information into our LTM is the consequence of a deep level of conscious processing and usually involves some form of semantic coding (you never forget what you were just doing when you hear a parent dies and events thereafter, but you are never entirely sure what you wrote in a well-prepared-for exam or what happened at your well deserved pub post-mortem - especially the next day...if of course you are over 18!). Visual codes are also utilised to encode LTM’s. Research of late has indicated that smells and tastes are also stored in LTM. This shows how flexible LTM is. Dual-coding theory (elaborated by Craik & Tulving, 1975) recommends we use both acoustic and visual codes to help us commit important things to memory. If you want to do well in your Intermediate 2 psychology assessments dualprocessing theory would help by reminding you to pay attention to what your teacher lecturer is saying and also to read these notes carefully. Doing the student activities will enhance performance even more. Your teacher/lecturer should explain why this is. This is related to Bradshaw & Anderson (1975) in their memory recall task where they who got subjects to try and remember events which occurred to famous people e.g. ‘At a critical point in his life Mozart made a journey from Munich to Paris’. The independent variable was the emphasis (control group: absence) of semantic meaning i.e. one group were given detail of why an event was personally important to Mozart, the other group were not. The experimental group (those who got the ‘treatment’ of personal meaning, remembered more than the control group (those who did not get the ‘treatment’). The control group’s memory task was as about exciting as watching paint dry! All Mozart’s comments about towns and villages along the route made the experimental group’s ability to remember more detail that much easier. The independent variable: the one the researcher is changing or manipulating. The dependent variable: that which the researcher would measure or observe. Measure in this instance was memory recall score. This explains why we can remember more vividly, news items which have both visual and semantic content in them. They help the news item to become more meaningful for us. 2. Storage Capacity : Most theorists believe there is no limit to the amount of information that can be stored in LTM. It is the biggest hard disk in existence. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 65 Episodic and Semantic memory Types of Memory In general, memories are less clear and detailed than perceptions but occasionally a remembered image is complete in every detail. Often called ‘photographic memory’ this phenomenon is as a result of eidetic memory (or eidetic imagery) found in 1:20 children. They use it to spell out an entire page of writing in an unfamiliar language that they have only seen for a short time. They have no way of remembering it on the basis of meaning. It is gobbledegook to them. Only the few possess eidetic memory in adulthood. Episodic Memory, also known as ‘procedural memory’, is according to Tulving (1972) an autobiographical memory containing our personal memories of past events, people, objects, etc (i.e. have a spatio-temporal significance for us). They are remembered by us on the basis of when and where they occurred. They have a subjective reality. (They are remembered on the basis of just how meaningful it was for us). Semantic Memory, on the other hand is our store of general factual knowledge about the world including concepts, rules and language. It is a ‘mental thesaurus , organised knowledge about words and other verbal symbols, their meanings and referents.’ Tulving (1972) Semantic memory is influenced by just how meaningful general factual knowledge was made for us at the time. Differences Between Episodic and Semantic Memory Episodic Memory Semantic Memory Uses sensory experience as data Uses comprehension as data Units of information are episodes/events Units of information are concepts; ideas and facts Organised as time-related Organised as concepts Emotional content very important Emotional content less vital High chance of forgetting Low chance of forgetting Source: Tulving, 1983 Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 66 Forms of Remembering Why do we remember..... Four different forms or types of remembering are distinguished by psychologists: Ebbinghaus identified: • recollection • recall • recognition and • relearning. Recollection involves the reconstruction of events or facts on the basis of partial cues. These serve as reminders to us of a past event or fact. Recollection would occur when watching e.g. a scene in Eastenders where the situation reminds you of something similar which you have experienced. Your mind wanders off and you build up a memory of this actual event before returning, 5 minutes later, to your favourite soap. Recall, in its purest form is our active and unaided remembering of something from the past. There are two types of recall used by us all the time. Cued Recall: is where we come across a memory trigger in our environment which reminds us that we have to do something. We often remind ourself to buy a particular item at the time we run out of it. When we are in the supermarket we see the item on the shelf. This triggers the memory that we have indeed run out of the item at home. We had reminded ourself at the time that we would buy it the next time we saw it. This is cued recall. Tying a knot in a hanky would be another form of it. Serial Recall: is where we have to bring out of our different memory stores aspects of a particular memory and puts it all together in a set sequence. This is particularly relevant in the situation of a group having to play and sing a song. They have to bring together out of long-term memory, the words (which have been encoded visually); the music (which has been encoded acoustically) and the meaning of the song (which has been encoded semantically). Not only that, the band have to individually and collectively produce the words, tune and emphasis in a set sequence. This is serial recall. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 67 Serial recall Cued recall Recognition is our ability to correctly identify previously encountered perceptions or internal experiences as familiar. Recognition is when you are standing in Central Station in Glasgow, for example, and recognise a face from your past. Recognition is realisation of a previously encountered experience - often buried deep in LTM. Relearning may show evidence of the effects of memory. Material that is familiar is often easier to learn a second time than it would be if it were unfamiliar, relearning suggests there might be another different type of memory. A short-cut to a hard-drive perhaps? .....And why do we forget? Forgetting or Faulty Retrieval The course of forgetting over time has been studied extensively by psychologists. Most often, rapid forgetting occurs at first, followed by a slower rate of loss. Improvement in the amount of material retained can be achieved by practising active recall during learning, periodically reviewing the material, and overlearning that is, relearning the material beyond the point of bare mastery. This would involve reading your psychology notes over and over again as often as you can. Rehearsal again plays it’s part in efficient and effective memory recall. It is painful but this process relates memory ability to comprehension. If you look back at Tulving (1983) you will be able to see why this is important. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 68 Four traditional explanations of forgetting exist. One is that memory traces fade naturally over time as a result of organic processes occurring in the brain. Memory - and ‘forgetfulness’ - can also be affected by brain damage, accident or illness. A second is that memories become systematically distorted or modified over time. A third is that new learning often interferes with, or replaces, old learning. Finally, some forgetting may be motivated by emotional needs and wishes—as when unpleasant childhood experiences are repressed or denied. Here is a man making a speech. What things could he do when preparing for the speech to help him make it seem easy on the day? Memory: Conclusion Memory; a cognitive process important to learning, thinking and problem solving is the information process we use to store and retrieve information in our brain. Memory takes different forms which are related to the type of stimulus we encounter i.e. episodic and semantic memory. Episodic memory is personal to us. It is autobiographical in content. Semantic memory is our memory for facts. Often what we remember; in either semantic or episodic form; is related to just how meaningful an event or a stimulus is to/for us. Past memories will influence our meaningful perception and attention of the present. As an information process memory is also studied from the point of view of how we code, store and retrieve our memories. We (en)code, or register, memories of stimuli on the basis of particular aspects of it. We store memories on the basis of how things sound (acoustic code), how things look (visual code) and what things mean to us (semantic code). We have three memory stores available to us. Semantic memory is ‘memory’ of our awareness of an event. Short-term memory is our working memory which can hold 7+2 units or chunks of information at any one time and lasts around 20 seconds. Short-term memory helps us to form and update a picture of our world on a minuteby-minute basis, and to think and solve problems. Our main memory store is longterm memory. We encode long-term memories on the basis of semantic and visual aspects of the stimulus. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 69 Faulty retrieval or ‘forgetting’ happens due to a breakdown in our ability to encode, store or retrieve memories. Memory and forgetting is a whole-brain cognitive process. Different parts of the brain are responsible for different aspects of ‘memory’. Someone may therefore suffer damage to the part of the brain responsible for short-term memory, and be unable to recollect what they had for breakfast; but on the other hand will be able to vividly remember the day the Second World War ended. We can use our knowledge of cognitive processes in general and memory in particular to devise strategies to aid learning. Cued recall and using rehearsal for serial recall are examples of this. While psychology sees memory as a hypothetical construct, findings are used (and will continue to be used) by computer companies. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 70 Glossary: Memory acoustic codes: this is an aspect of storage where particular events are stored and remembered as a sequence of sounds. capacity: the size of a memory store. Sensory register is immediate and momentary; short-term memory can handle 7+2 units or chunks of information at any one time; long-term memory is infinite in size. What we remember from our long-term memory will depend on it’s visual and acoustic properties and just how meaningful the image/object/event was for us at the time. coding: process by which we encode (commit) things to memory. We do this on the basis of the acoustic and visual properties of a stimulus and just how much this stimulus means to us. cued recall: is where we come across a memory trigger in our environment which reminds us that we have to do something. dual-coding theory: put forward by Craik & Tulving, 1975 who say we use both acoustic and visual codes to help us commit important things to memory. eidetic memory: often called ‘photographic memory’ this phenomenon is as a found in 1:20 children, who, using it could spell out an entire page of writing in an unfamiliar language that they have only seen for a short time. Only the few possess eidetic memory in adulthood. episodic memory: also known as ‘procedural memory’, is according to Tulving (1972) an autobiographical memory containing our personal memories of past events, people, objects etc. encoding: the process we adopt when coding memories. We encode on the basis of the acoustic and visual properties of a stimulus and how much it means to us. Cross-culturally people tend to use acoustic codes to encode information into STM. Visual codes tend to decay faster than acoustic codes hippocampus: the centre of our emotions and autonomic nervous system, damage to this part of the brain impairs episodic memory (personal events, experiences and episodes), but allows us to still deal with everyday life, It is linked by nerve fibres to our cortex where it is visual and auditory memories are located - though not together. Memories of whole events have been found to involve different parts of the brain iconic memory; another name for sensory register. long-term memory: our memory for personal events (episodic memory) and facts (semantic memory). LTM of a stimulus is found in various parts of the brain in different types of long-term memory stores. Storage depends on the different properties of the stimulus (acoustic; visual and personal meaning). meaning: is central to memory. Our memory helps us ‘make sense’ of our reality based upon just how meaningful a stimulus is for us - either at that time or at some time in the past. What we remember, and how detailed and deeply we remember it, is kind of subjective for us in many ways. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 71 memory: is a higher level cognitive process, which is our system for dealing with and organising incoming information coming to us from our outside world. It is the higher level function we have which allows us to keep and hold information about events and experiences that have happened to us in the past. The brain’s organisation of the things we remember and can recall is our memory. recall: a form of remembering which is the active and unaided remembering of something from our past. recognition: a form of remembering which allows us to correctly identify previously encountered perceptions or internal experiences as familiar. recollection: a form of remembering which involves the reconstruction of events or facts on the basis of partial cues, which serve as reminders to us of a past event or fact. relearning: a form of remembering which show evidence of the effects of memory. Material that is familiar is often easier to learn a second time than it would be if it were unfamiliar, relearning suggests there might be another different type of memory. In computing terms it is a form of remembering which is a short-cut to our LTM ‘hard-drive’. retrieval: of memory. This is related to what we remember and what we forget. Forgetting occurs due to either faulty encoding, storage or retrieval. semantic codes: is the code we use to remember things on the basis of their meaning for us. semantic memory: is our memory of general factual knowledge about the world including concepts, rules and language. It is a ‘mental thesaurus , organised knowledge about words and other verbal symbols, their meanings and referents.’ (Tulving (1972)). It is influenced by just how meaningful general factual knowledge was made for us at the time. sensory register or iconic memory holds information from all the senses, but only for a fraction of a second. It is more a subconscious state of awareness of an event than a storage facility. We would know it more as our awareness of the sensation of a breeze blowing off our cheek. serial recall: is where we have to bring out of our different memory stores aspects of a particular memory and put it all together in a set sequence. short term memory: (or ‘Working Memory’) receives the information that was perceived and selectively attended to in sensory register or retrieved from long term memory if no further processing occurs . STM has a duration of around 20-30 seconds. STM has two functions. STM helps us to form and update a picture of our world on a minute-by-minute basis, and to think and solve problems. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 72 storage: our brains capacity to hold memories over time. We come to store memories in three main ‘files’ each with a far greater capacity compared to what came before. These are called sensory register: short term memory and long term memory (stores). storage capacity : term referring to the size of our three different memory stores. Short-term memory has a capacity of 7+2 units or ‘chunks’ of information for up to 20 seconds at a time. Long-term memory has unlimited capacity and we can retrieve memories from LTM many years after experiencing the event...sometimes. Most theorists believe there is no limit to the amount of information that can be stored in LTM. It is the biggest hard disk in existence. visual codes: represent visual information stored and remembered as forms or images Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 73 STUDENT ACTIVITY : MEMORY 1. What do you think the study of memory is all about? Identify by ticking in the boxes below what a student of cognitive psychology might study in relation to Memory the senses coding, storage and retrieval episodic memory psychophysics memory for facts selection and attention meaning of stimuli motivation acoustic codes visual illusions chunking learning strategies forgetting [ [ [ [ [ [ [ [ [ [ [ [ [ Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) ] ] ] ] ] ] ] ] ] ] ] ] ] 74 2. Please fill in the blank spaces from the boxed word list below: episodic and semantic sound semantic code working memory 20 forgetting store and retrieve visual awareness cued 7+2 long-term memory learning thinking and problem solving semantic and visual learning autobiographical meaningful code, store and retrieve whole-brain Memory; a cognitive process important to , is the information process we use to information in our brain. Memory takes different forms which are related to the type of stimulus we encounter i.e. memory. Episodic memory is personal to us. It is in content. Semantic memory is our memory for facts. Often what we remember; in either semantic or episodic form; is related to just how an event or a stimulus is to/for us. Past memories will influence our meaningful perception and attention of the present. As an information process memory is also studied from the point of view of how we our memories. We code memories of stimuli on the basis of particular aspects of it. We store on the basis of how things : acoustic code; how things look: code and what things mean to us: . We have three memory stores available to us. Semantic memory is ‘memory’ of our of an event. Short-term memory is our which can hold units or chunks of information at any one time and lasts around seconds. Short-term memory helps us to form and update a picture of our world on a minute-by-minute basis, and to think and solve problems. Our main memory store is . We encode long-term memories on the basis of aspects of the stimulus. Faulty retrieval or ‘ ’ happens due to a breakdown in our ability to code, store or retrieve memories. Memory and forgetting is a cognitive process. Different parts of the brain are responsible for different aspects of ‘memory’. Someone may therefore suffer damage to the part of the brain responsible for short-term memory, and be unable to recollect what they had for breakfast; but on the other hand will be able to vividly remember the day the Second World War ended. We can use our knowledge of cognitive processes in general and memory in particular to devise strategies to aid . recall is an example of this. While psychology sees memory as a hypothetical construct findings are used, and will continue to be used, by computer companies. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 75 3. Memory: Not-So Gigantic Wordsearch Solve the clues to the Not-So Gigantic Wordsearch that follows. Put a line through the correct word or phrase in the wordsearch and write your answer beside the clue. This is an aspect of storage where particular events are stored and remembered as a sequence of sounds. The size of a memory store. Sensory register is immediate and momentary; short-term memory can handle 7+2 units or chunks of information at any one time; long-term memory is infinite in size. What we remember from our long-term memory will depend on it’s visual and acoustic properties and just how meaningful the image/object/event was for us at the time. This is where we have to bring out of our different memory stores aspects of a particular memory and put it all together in a set sequence. Information stored and remembered as forms or images Process by which we encode (commit) things to memory. We do this on the basis of the acoustic and visual properties of a stimulus and just how much this stimulus means to us. Theory put forward by Craik & Tulving, 1975 who say we use both acoustic and visual codes to help us commit important things to memory. Often called ‘photographic memory’ this phenomenon is as a found in 1:20 children, who, using it could spell out an entire page of writing in an unfamiliar language that they have only seen for a short time. Only the few possess eidetic memory in adulthood. Also known as ‘procedural memory’, is according to Tulving (1972) an autobiographical memory containing our personal memories of past events, people, objects etc. The process we adopt when coding memories. We encode on the basis of the acoustic and visual properties of a stimulus and how much it means to us. Cross-culturally people tend to use acoustic codes to encode information into STM. Visual codes tend to decay faster than acoustic codes The centre of our emotions and autonomic nervous system, damage to this part of the brain impairs episodic memory (personal events, experiences and episodes), but allows us to still deal with everyday life. It is linked by nerve fibres to our cortex where it is visual and auditory memories are located - though not together. Memories of whole events have been found to involve different parts of the brain iconic memory; another name for sensory register. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 76 Our memory for personal events (episodic memory) and facts (semantic memory). LTM of a stimulus is found in various parts of the brain in different types of longterm memory stores. Storage depends on the different properties of the stimulus (acoustic; visual and personal meaning). This is psychological in nature and is central to memory. Our memory helps us ‘make sense’ of our reality based upon just how meaningful a stimulus is for us - either at that time or at some time in the past. This a higher level cognitive process, which is our system for dealing with and organising incoming information coming to us from our outside world. It is the higher level function we have which allows us to keep and hold information about events and experiences that have happened to us in the past. A form of remembering which is the active and unaided remembering of something from our past. A form of remembering which allows us to correctly identify previously encountered perceptions or internal experiences as familiar. This is where we come across a memory trigger in our environment which reminds us that we have to do something. A form of remembering which involves the reconstruction of events or facts on the basis of partial cues, which serve as reminders to us of a past event or fact. A form of remembering which show evidence of the effects of memory. Material that is familiar is often easier to learn a second time than it would be if it were unfamiliar, relearning suggests there might be another different type of memory. In computing terms it is a form of remembering which is a short-cut to our LTM ‘hard-drive’. This is related to what we remember and what we forget. Forgetting occurs due to either faulty encoding, storage or retrieval. This is the code we use to remember things on the basis of their meaning for/to us. This refers to our memory of general factual knowledge about the world including concepts, rules and language. It is a ‘mental thesaurus , organised knowledge about words and other verbal symbols, their meanings and referents.’ (Tulving (1972)). Known also as iconic memory this holds information from all the senses, but only for a fraction of a second. It is more a subconscious state of awareness of an event than a storage facility. Known also as ‘Working Memory’ this receives the information that was perceived and selectively attended to in sensory register or retrieved from long term memory if no further processing occurs. STM has a duration of around 20-30 seconds. STM has two functions. STM helps us to form and update a picture of our world on a minute-by-minute basis, and to think and solve problems. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 77 This is our brains capacity to hold memories over time. We come to store memories in three main ‘files’ each with a far greater capacity compared to what came before. These are called sensory register: short term memory and long term memory (stores). A term referring to the size of our three different memory stores. Short-term memory has a capacity of 7+2 units or ‘chunks’ of information for up to 20 seconds at a time. Long-term memory has unlimited capacity and we can retrieve memories from LTM many years after experiencing the event...sometimes. Most theorists believe there is no limit to the amount of information that can be stored in LTM. It is the biggest hard disk in existence. Not so Gigantic Wordsearch Memory codingencodingrestoragecapacity iconicmemorysensoryregister dual-codingtheorymeaning long-termmemoryvisualcodes semanticcodesmemorycapacity episodicmemoryeideticmemory acousticcodesrecallrecognition recollectionhippocampusstorage retrievalsemanticmemoryserial learningshorttermmemorycued Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 78 4. Quick Quiz 1. What do you understand by the cognitive process called memory? 2. Why is memory important? 3. Define memory. 4. What two forms do memories take? 5. What might influence the contents of these forms of memory? Give an example of the affect of this. 6. On what bases do we code, store and retrieve information? 7. What types of memory stores do we have? 8. Give a description of each of these memory stores. 9. Why do we ‘forget’? Give an example of why we might forget something. 10. What is ‘serial recall’? Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 79 5. Please answer the following questions: a) What is meant by memory in cognitive psychology? • • • • • Your answer should cover the forms of remembering that have been identified by cognitive psychologists give some everyday examples in your answer. Is one form better than another? explain what is meant by episodic and semantic memory how do the three basic processes of memory (coding, storage and retrieval) combine to make our memory more effective? what are the similarities and differences between short term and long term memory? b) Read the following scenarios: A man selecting something in a supermarket A group on stage just about to start a set Someone keeping a daily diary Your Headteacher or Principal making a speech Choose two of these and describe the role of memory in this situation: • your answer must mention which perceptual process is being used and why it is relevant to/necessary in the situation(s) chosen • refer to the situation directly • give detail about any particular aspects of memory identified in the situation(s) • give detail about how memory can be influenced in the situation(s) chosen. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 80 KEY CONCEPTS IN COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY: LANGUAGE Language is the last of three cognitive process we will examine. You will become familiar with the range of functions with which language is associated and develop an appreciation of the importance of linguistic functioning to the human species in particular. The influence that language has on cognition generally will be emphasised and close links with memory and thinking explored. Modern day issues such as sexism in language and emotive language will be effectively utilised during the teaching of this concept. You will become familiar with the fact that other species have effective communication systems and you will be introduced to attempts to teach human language to chimpanzees and other higher level species. The limitations of animal language will be acknowledged and the distinctive features of human language emphasised. To this end SQA particularly emphasise in the study of language: • • • humans and language different language styles – social, cultural influences animals and language Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 81 KEY CONCEPTS IN COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY: LANGUAGE Mike Cardwell in the excellent ‘Complete A-Z Psychology Handbook’ defines language as ‘an agreed set of symbols that enable us to convey meaning and converse with other members of the same culture that share the same language’. While the Collins Dictionary and Thesaurus see language as, A system for the expression of thoughts, feelings etc by the use of spoken sounds or conventional symbols. The faculty for the use of such systems, which is a distinguishing characteristic of man... The language of a particular nation or group. The specialised vocabulary used by a particular group. Agreed definition is difficult. Language has been studied from the beginnings of time. Is there such a thing as one agreed language? We learn spoken language at home. We are then sent to school to learn to read, write and converse better in language! How we speak in the home will hopefully not be the language we use with our peers. Similarly the language some of us use with our peers will not be the same when we are talking to small children. We learn foreign languages. We learn the language of academic subjects which allow us to converse with others who use the same language i.e. mathematics; music; chemistry; physics; psychology, etc. While we all share the same spoken language, why is it that the language coming from a particular person is perceived as more important than another? Language is power. A knowledge of language from the perspective of cognitive psychology should equip you to be more effective in your interpersonal relationships. The psychological study of language is heavily influenced by British philosophy from the establishment of the British empirical tradition by John Locke in the 17th and 18th Centuries and continued by George Berkeley, David Hume, John Stuart Mill, GE Moore and Bertrand Russell in the 19th and 20th centuries. These English (and Scottish!) philosophers gave birth to what is nowadays called analytic or linguistic philosophy. As cognitive psychology has it’s roots in philosophy this influence sees us concentrate on the development, formation and construction of language; it’s structure and function. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 82 LANGUAGE: HUMANS AND LANGUAGE The psychology of language concerns itself with 4 main areas - some of which are challenging in interest and content. • • • • Elements of language Understanding speech Learning to talk - stages of language development The acquisition of language 1. The Elements of Language From an information-processing point of view, language has two basic elements: symbols and grammar. For psychologists, ‘Grammars represent the tacit knowledge that native speakers have about their language, which includes knowing how to form grammatically acceptable sentences, knowing what they mean and knowing how to pronounce them’ (Carroll, 1986). We form sentences using words, which themselves are made up two elements. • • Morphemes, a combination of meaning and and Phonemes, which are the way words/parts of words sound (phonology). This can be demonstrated below i.e. ‘Port’ sounds better and is sequentially more grammatically correct than ‘Plort’ or ‘Plorb’. Rules of syntax determine the way our words go together to form sentences e.g. if asked to complete the following sentence below, rules of syntax determine that we must choose a noun (a noun is of course a class, general category or particular thing!). ‘The psychology lecturer played with ...?’ The answer being of course, ‘bits of paper’ or ‘elastic bands.’ Another important syntactic rule is to do with word order (see Learning to Talk: Stages of Language Development). ‘John loves Mary’ has a different meaning than ‘Mary loves John,’ both of which are grammatically correct but semantically (meaning) different. Semantics are the set of rules governing the meaning of words. Semantics influence our understanding of something that is being communicated to us. We often put semantic interpretation on what is being said to/about us on the basis of what we think the person means. What things mean to us is based on previous past experience. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 83 What is being said here is that the string of words we use to put a sentence together has both a: a) surface structure and a b) deep structure This is a neat way of explaining i) ii) iii) how we mean what we say don’t often mean what we say, and what can happen when others interpret what they think we meant about what we said! Take the following example: ‘If you don’t shut up I will kill you.’ This sentence is grammatically and syntactically correct. Would you do what you say you mean to do? If you were the victim of a recent assault and overheard the above, would this experience colour your interpretation of what is going on? Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 84 Language: Humans and Language Υνδερστανδινγ Σπεεχη As demonstrated above, the topic of this section is very definitely the province of psychology - the scientific study of the mind and behaviour. We use the following strategies and abilities in our attempt to understand and act appropriately to what is being communicated to us. • Perceiving Words & Phrases The gaps we hear between spoken words are not real but perceived because of topdown higher level mental processing. Syntax, memories, and knowledge of the world help us understand and remember verbal and written communication. As a result when we are engaged in a conversation with someone we very often perceive, or anticipate, what they are going to say next and jump in with our tuppence worth. Occasionally our anticipated perception of words and phrases is wrong: and this often gets us into bother! In the absence of language percepts (cues which surround language), we are very careful to wait until we hear a full sentence from people who ‘phone us before we pitch in. Not so in interpersonal interaction. • Using Context and Scripts An understanding of language depends on constructing information based partly on the context in which the information is encoded. Put simply the situation in which the language happens helps understanding of what it means. ‘The Full Monty’ now means more than just a film title to many of us!! Context of language is created by the situation, and by personal factors such as education and culture. Where the same language is appropriate in one context but inappropriate in another In 1999 the eminent Scottish QC, Donald Findley, found himself at a Rangers FC league, Scottish and League Cup celebration. The party, attended by other Board members, players, officials, and supporters was in full swing when Donald, the then Vice-Chairman of Rangers got up to sing The Sash. His rendition was very well received. Within the context of the situation, Rangers’ supporters might have seen the song as appropriate, if a little unwise for someone in Donald’s position. Someone took a video of the proceedings which was released to the media. When taken out of the context within which the events happened, the video caused the lawyer a lot of criticism. He resigned from his much cherished position at Rangers. He was heavily fined by the Faculty of Advocates. However, he received hundreds of letters in support for his behaviour, a number of which came from Celtic fans who had a certain sympathy for his plight. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 85 Scripts also help us understand the language being used in a particular situation. Scripts are our own ideas; thoughts, images of a familiar activity, event or sequence. A script can also be called a mental representation. Awareness of the script which goes with a particular activity helps us avoid making embarrassing blunders. Knowledge of the script helps us understand in a more meaningful way what is being communicated to us. The ‘script’ is one reason why we show respect when a hearse passes by us. • Conventions and non-verbal Cues Conventions (customs and etiquette), often built up over years, often govern the meaning of conversations. They are a sign of the power within which the language is situated. This ranges from more obvious examples of the Queen’s Speech through to less obvious, but more pertinent, language conventions e.g. ‘SQA-speak’. We perceive conventions as important and we sometimes give the convention-delivered communication over-inflated credibility and respect. Non-verbal cues like body language, situation and culture also play a part in the understanding of the situation and language used (and to use). The language of an interview is hide-bound by ‘NVCs’. Taking all of the above into account, • • • • • perception context scripts conventions NVCs What do you understand by the following? Πσψχηολογψ: τηε σχιενχε οφ µινδ ανδ βεηαϖιουρ. Now the hard bit! Give reasons for your answer below: Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 86 Language: Humans and Language: Learning to Talk: Stages of Language Development At about four months of age, babies begin to repeat simple syllables (Ba...ba...ba; Da...da...da). This is called babbling. At nine months, babies stop uttering noises that are not within their environmental ‘frame of reference’. This is the beginning of their perception that only particular sounds mean something to others. Real words follow at 12-14 months, usually proper names and objects - Daddy; Mummy, Teddy etc. At this one word stage they use only one word at a time, and overextend its use to mean more than one object (see Piaget’s Theory of Cognitive Development Sensori-Motor Stage). First sentences come at 18-24 months as two word pairs e.g. ‘There’s bus’, ‘Big horse’ etc. These first sentences are brief and telegraphic. Next come three word sentences with subject -verb-object sequences e.g. ‘Me want Barbie’, ‘I want Power Ranger.’ Word endings begin to emerge but are often misplaced e.g. ‘I going to nanna’s.’ These rules we learn from others around us. Finally by age five comes the use of adjectives, and fairly complex sentences full of progressively correct syntax, surface and deep structural meaning. When a six year old was asked what she wanted Santa to bring her for Christmas, she looked deeply thoughtful for a moment and then said, ‘I don’t mind what presents Santa brings me as long as there are lots of them.’ And there were! Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 87 Language: Humans and Language: How is Language Acquired? Conditioning, imitation and rules have all been found to play a part. Before these environmental influences begin our biology must come into play. We have a biologically determined critical period for language acquisition. This was first brought to psychology’s attention by Noam Chomsky. Chomsky, is an American linguist, writer, teacher and political activist. He founded what is both brilliantly and absurdly called transformational-generative linguistic analysis! What Chomsky means by this is that as well as the rules of grammar specific to individual languages, there are also universal rules common to all languages. This shows that the ability to form and understand language is innate to all human beings. Chomsky believes that children the world over are born with an innate language acquisition device to help them cope with the complexities of language. There appears to be a critical period for language acquisition. The theory lies within the biological approach in psychology and is beyond the scope of this course, but suffice to say it is all down to the epigenic maturation of those parts of the brain that deals with language. Once this critical period is reached (11/12 according to Lennenberg (1964 & 1967), little can be done to kick-start language and its attendant potentialities for the individual. Our ability to switch on to the acquisition of language in early childhood is uncanny. Bilingualism need only prove this point. An understanding of language from the psychological side of things must give students a notion as to its importance. Never again will ‘Oh no, it’s English again’ mean quite the same thing for you. Not getting to grips with language in a complex and meaningful way, may affect more than your examination mark! Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 88 Language: Different language styles – social, cultural influences This is a particularly controversial area of psychology. In 1973, Basil Bernstein a sociologist, suggested that we used two types of language code: • • restricted code and elaborated code Termed Bernstein’s verbal deprivation hypothesis whatever code you used is largely influenced by your class and socialisation. Working-class people use restricted code (the language of the streets in many ways), and middle and upper class people use elaborated code (like the Queen or Jeremy Paxman!). Elaborated code gives better more enriched meaning and understanding to complex situations, issues, events, objects, experiences, etc. You have to learn elaborated code. Elaborated code is learned and used in middle-class homes, schools, communities, all the time. Elaborated code is the language of power. Elaborated code is the key to personal advancement. Elaborated code encourages thinking in terms of general principles (hypothesis testing, abstract thought, creativity, etc). Also according to Bernstein, thinking in terms of restricted code narrows your potential, opportunities and life chances. The theory carries with it an implication that working-class people are verbally deprived and ultimately unable to think in the abstract. The verbal deprivation theory was challenged by Labov (1972). In a series of studies where researchers chatted informally and colloquially with children about tasks they wished them to do. They discovered there was no relationship between use of elaborated code and conceptual ability (abstract thinking and problem solving). Labov also says that when you look at non-standard English users they linguistically conform to the same rules of syntax, grammar as elaborated code users. Non-standard English users in supportive environments have been found to be as competent and sophisticated in their thinking as anyone else. Labov feels that Bernstein is trying to push an agenda - on a false premise - where one type of language is seen as ‘better’ than others. If language falls into just two codes, both have survival value for us. There is a place for both. In Scotland, it is not a particularly good idea when you find yourself confronted with someone snarling, swearing and squaring up to you to say ‘Now look here my man....’ On the other hand in an interview situation for a job in a bank, it is equally crazy to continually use the phrase ‘’Know what I mean, man...?’ The controversies of the debate aside it is worth mentioning that the code we do use when communicating with others can influence how people perceive us and, thus, think, feel and react to us. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 89 Accent and Social Judgements Giles (1973) was interested in whether we were influenced in the formation of our opinions based on the accent used by the communicator. He got a group of 17 year olds to indicate their views on capital punishment. Seven days later they were presented with an argument against capital punishment delivered in either received pronunciation, a South Welsh, Somerset or Birmingham accent (the independent variable). The participants were then asked to rate the quality of the argument presented (the dependent variable). Their ratings showed a statistically significant correlation with the social status of the accent. The more prestigious the accent, the better the argument was thought to be presented. This is related to the work of Rosenthal and Jacobson (1968). They discovered that teachers unconsciously rate children on the basis of speech, appearance, etc. They attribute a lower intellect to children they perceive as low on social status. They are attracted to children they perceive on a higher (or same as them) social status. Academically they expect less of the lower class child. This is called a self-fulfilling prophecy and influences how teachers may think, feel and behave towards children in their charge. Emotive Language Emotive language is language which conveys a judgement or transmits strong emotions about something which can influence the listener in a particular way (Thouless, 1974). Prior to democratic majority rule in his country a South African once commented to the author: who supported Nelson Mandela’s military wing of the African National Congress Umkhonto We Sizwe: that ‘Geography makes all the difference between one man’s terrorist and another man’s freedom fighter.’ The use of freedom fighter conveys a positive, romantic image or message, the use of terrorist conveys a negative dark image or message. In the 19th century the following phrase was often seen hanging in a picture frame: A Woman’s Place Is In The Home. In the context of the time this was acceptable. Why is this not the case today? Of what is the above an example? Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 90 Language: Animals and Language There is a touch of the Doctor Dolittle’s about this topic! We have always been fascinated by the mystery of animal communication; whether/what they experience as meaningful consciousness and whether higher level primates like apes and chimpanzees can be taught to communicate using human language. This is to what we shall now turn! Vocalisation As far back as 1933, psychologists, Kellog and Kellog tried to get their young chimpanzee Gua to talk English. Gua was raised alongside the Kellog’s baby of the same age. They observed Gua was as quick to learn as their baby, but in it’s six years with them Gua only learnt three words in total. In a similar situation in the Hayes’ study in 1950, their chimpanzee Vicki was reported to master only four words which included papa, mama, up and cup. Why could this be? Both Gua and Vicki were adjudged, language apart, to be using similar high level cognitive processes to babies and children of the same age. Without putting too fine a point on it, these researchers were barking up the wrong tree. Chimps do not possess the physiology or biology necessary to vocalise correctly. They do not possess the bodily and, consequentially, the mental structures to speak. This was realised by Gardner and Gardner (1969) in what is known as the Washoe study. Gardner and Gardner noticed from natural observation that chimpanzees used a lot of visual signals to communicate with each other. They taught their young chimp Washoe to talk using Ameslan (American Sign Language). Taught via modelling (copying and being praised), and initially having to have her hands moulded into the correct sign shape, Washoe developed the ability to communicate. By 20 months she was forming her own two word sentences. By age four she was able to sign 132 different words, including nouns, pronouns, adjectives and verbs. In comparison to a human four year old, who by this age has mastered 4000+ words, this is not too impressive: but in comparison to other chimpanzees Washoe was a star! To confirm or otherwise Washoe’s ability the Gardner’s used what is called a doubleblind technique. Independent observers, one of whom could only see the picture Washoe had to learn the sign for, and the other who could only see Washoe make a sign confirmed Washoe got it right on 72% of occasions. As the years rolled out it became evident that Washoe was an active information processor of language, using it in an proactive manner in conjunction with memory and thinking to communicate her wishes, desires and intentions. She was not producing passive stimulus-response Ameslan language units. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 91 A similar natural observational study was conducted by Patterson (1978) with Koko the gorilla. Koko also learnt Ameslan. After a year another gorilla called Michael joined Koko. Patterson found over the seven year study that both apes used signing to communicate between themselves and with humans. Koko learnt over 400 signs and 20 which she developed herself to signify in a consistent fashion certain things for her. Both Koko and Michael were, like Washoe, actively using signs to communicate. It was not mere stimulus-response units of leant behaviour Koko, it was found, could even use Ameslan to lie herself out of situations. This shows the relationship once more between all the higher level cognitive processes to ensure survival in our environment - even with apes and chimpanzees Species-species knowledge concerning language is limited. There appears to be some success with teaching (sign) language to some higher level primates. What we have discovered emphasises the unique human ability of verbal language as an incredible communication, thinking and survival tool. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 92 Language: Conclusion Language, while difficult to define, is the cognitive process we use to communicate with one another. Language contributes to - and is influenced by - our other higher level cognitive processes of perception, attention, memory and thinking. Language involves using agreed words and symbols which allows us to share and exchange ideas. Language involves both speech and listening skills. Language takes many different forms spoken; written; non-vocal; non verbal etc. Language is used by humans and non-humans alike. This is especially true of dolphins, whales, chimpanzees and apes. All use a form of language to communicate messages/ideas/meanings which helps individual and group survival. Humans are no different. Language is the key to the maximisation of all our abilities; inclinations and potentials whatever they may be. Psychologists like Chomsky believe, that in order to be all we are capable of becoming, the biological trigger called the Language Acquisition Device (LAD) has to be nurtured and enriched before our teenage years. If it is not, he thinks, we lose the ability to acquire speech forever. The LAD essentially withers and dies. In everyday terms we use shared spoken language which allows us to symbolise and convey messages/ideas/meanings to another person: and we use listening to receive messages/ideas/meanings in similar shared symbolic spoken words in return. Often our perception of what is being said to us in influenced by non-verbal language cues such as body language, body posture facial expressions, etc. Language in the written sense is used by us all the time in conjunction with thinking. Written language is a very powerful medium to convey messages/ideas/meanings to another person and indeed a mass audience. An understanding of language from the point of view of cognitive psychology now allows us to perceive it as something more than English. Language is knowledge in the broadest sense. Language is power. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 93 Glossary: Language ameslan: whole body American Sign Language. analytic philosophy: also known as linguistic philosophy. It is a branch of philosophy strongly influenced by the British empirical tradition. These philosophers are interested in the development, formation and construction of language; its structure and function. babbling: the repetition of simple syllables (Ba...ba...ba; Da...da...da) by babies from 4 months onwards. The early beginnings of verbal language. conditioning: behaviourist idea applied to language acquisition. We learn language as a result of associating that one linguistic ‘event’ follows another, e.g. an adjective comes before and describes a noun (i.e. ‘the small psychologist’)! Reward and punishment are also influential, i.e. as a baby we see big people’s excitement when we say ‘Dada’. We will say this same word in the future to further encourage this response. Not so very long ago children and teenagers in Scottish schools got one of the strap on the hand for every mistake in a sentence. Sentences can be big and complex and its use discouraged the same mistake in the future ... rapidly! context: the situation within which a communication is set. An understanding of this helps us understand the communication better. conventions: customs and etiquette. critical period: Noam Chomsky says that we have to acquire spoken language within this period (4 months-14) otherwise we lose the ability to acquire it. deep structure: what a sentence - even if constructed correctly - means to you. This is based on previous past experience. elaborated code: middle and upper-class speech. The code of power. emotive language: language which conveys a judgement or transmits strong emotions about something which can influence the listener in a particular way (Thouless 1974). epigenic maturation: biological maturation necessary before we are able to do something, e.g. walk and talk. grammars: with symbols form human language, this other element of language is our innate knowledge of our own language, how to form grammatically correct sentences in it, what these mean and how to say them properly. imitation: another idea put forward originally by Alfred Bandura concerning the acquisition of language by us. Using this, we observe, copy and model our own use of language. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 94 language: ‘an agreed set of symbols that enables us to convey meaning and converse with other members of the same culture that share the same language’. language acquisition device: biological device/abilities innate to human beings (Chomsky) which help us acquire a key cognitive process essential to our personal survival in the world. morphemes: the part of a sentence which conveys meaning to others. Noam Chomsky: American linguist, writer, teacher, thinker, and political activist founded transformational-generative linguistic analysis which says as well as the rules of grammar specific to individual languages there are also universal rules common to all languages, and this shows that the ability to form and understand language is innate. non-verbal cues: NVCs: body language; body posture; situation and culture which influences our interpretation of the communication . one word stage: second stage of language acquisition at 18-24 months following babbling where babies use only one word at a time, and overextend it’s use to mean more than one thing i.e. all men are Dada! phonemes: the way words/parts of words sound (phonology) when we speak e.g. phonetically (and grammatically) port sounds better than plorb. We instinctively know port to be the correct word to use. scripts: our own ideas/thoughts/images (mental representations) of a familiar situation which influences our current perception/understanding of it. This aspect to language influences our behaviour in the situation i.e. you would not normally laugh during a funeral because of a previous knowledge of this! self-fulfilling prophecy: occurs when others react and behave in a particular way towards you based on how you speak, are dressed, are related to, etc. semantics: the set of rules governing the meaning of words. A knowledge of it will influence our understanding of something that is being communicated to us. sentence: used to convey spoken and written language. It will have a surface structure and a deep structure. surface structure: this concerns whether or not a sentence is grammatically and syntactically correct i.e. if it is constructed correctly. syntax: written and unwritten; conscious and unconscious rules concerning correct language. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 95 restricted code: essentially working-class speech. verbal deprivation hypothesis: put forward by Bernstein who says that whatever language code you use (restricted code or elaborated code) is largely influenced by your class and socialisation process. vocalisation: the biological, physiological and genetic ability to form words/phrases. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 96 STUDENT ACTIVITY : LANGUAGE 1. What do you think the study of language is all about? Identify by ticking in the boxes below what a student of cognitive psychology might study in relation to Language syntax coding, storage and retrieval episodic memory semantics restricted code selection and attention vocalisation phonemes morphemes visual illusions LAD power forgetting [ [ [ [ [ [ [ [ [ [ [ [ [ Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) ] ] ] ] ] ] ] ] ] ] ] ] ] 97 2. Please fill in the blank spaces from the word box list below: perception survival thinking words and symbols non-vocal potentials shared symbolic Chomsky non-verbal a mass audience chimpanzees and apes Language Acquisition Device cognitive to communicate with one another non-humans to acquire speech messages/ideas/meanings Language Language , while difficult to define is the cognitive process we use . Language contributes to - and is influenced by - our other higher level cognitive processes of perception; attention; memory and . Language involves using agreed which allows us to share and exchange ideas. Language involves both speech and listening skills. Language takes many different forms spoken, written, , non-verbal etc. Language is used by humans and alike. This is especially true of dolphins, whales, . All use a form of language to communicate messages/ideas/meanings which helps individual and group . Humans are no different. Language is the key to the maximisation of all our abilities, inclinations and whatever they may be. Psychologists like believe, that in order to be all we are capable of becoming, the biological trigger called the (LAD) has to be nurtured and enriched before our teenage years. If it is not: he thinks we lose the ability forever. The LAD essentially withers and dies. In everyday terms we use shared spoken language which allows us to symbolise and convey to another person: and use listening to receive messages/ideas/meanings in similar spoken words in return. Often our of what is being said to us in influenced by language cues such as body language, body posture facial expressions etc. Language in the written sense is used by us all the time in conjunction with thinking. Written language is a very powerful medium to convey messages/ideas/meanings to another person and indeed . An understanding of language from the point of view of psychology now allows us to perceive it as something more than English. Language is knowledge in the broadest sense. is power. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 98 3. Language Not-So Gigantic Wordsearch Solve the clues to the Not-So Gigantic Wordsearch that follows. Put a line through the correct word or phrase and write your answer beside the clue. Customs and etiquette. Our biological; physiological and genetic ability to form words and phrases. Also known as linguistic philosophy. It is a branch of philosophy strongly influenced by the British empirical tradition. These philosophers are interested in the development, formation and construction of language; it’s structure and function. The repletion of simple syllables (Ba...ba...ba; Da...da...da) by babies from four months onwards. Whole body American Sign Language What a sentence means to you based on previous past experience. Middle and upper-class speech. The code of power. Noam Chomsky says that we have to acquire spoken language within this period (four months-14 months) otherwise we lose the ability to acquire it. Behaviourist idea applied to language acquisition. Language which conveys a judgement or transmits strong emotions about something which can influence the listener in a particular way (Thouless 1974). The situation within which a communication is set. Biological maturation needed before we are able to do something e.g. walk and talk. This other element of language is our innate knowledge of our own language, how to form grammatically correct sentences in it, what these mean and how to say them properly. Another idea put forward originally by Alfred Bandura concerning the acquisition of language by us. Using this, we observe, copy and model our own use of language. ‘An agreed set of symbols that enable us to convey meaning and converse with other members of the same culture that share the same language’. Biological device/abilities innate to human beings (Chomsky) which help us acquire a key cognitive process essential to our personal survival in the world. The part of a sentence which conveys meaning to others. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 99 American linguist, writer, teacher, thinker, and political activist founded transformational-generative linguistic analysis which says as well as the rules of grammar specific to individual languages there are also universal rules common to all languages, and this shows that the ability to form and understand language is innate. NVCs: body language, body posture, situation and culture which influences our interpretation of the communication . Second stage of language acquisition at 18-24 months following babbling where babies use only one word at a time, and overextend it’s use to mean more than one thing i.e. all men are Dada! Written and unwritten; conscious and unconscious rules concerning correct language. The way words/parts of words sound. Our own ideas/thoughts/images (mental representations) of a familiar situation Happens with teachers for example when others react and behave in a particular way towards you based on how you speak, are dressed, are related to, etc. Used to convey spoken and written language. It will have a surface structure and a deep structure. This concerns whether or not a sentence is grammatically and syntactically correct i.e. if it is constructed correctly. The set of rules governing the meaning of words. Essentially working-class speech. Theory put forward by Bernstein who says that whatever language code you use (restricted code or elaborated code) is largely influenced by class and socialisation. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 100 Not so Gigantic Wordsearch: Language languageacquisitiondeviceameslan selffulfillingprophecyNoamChomsky semanticssentencedeepstructure verbaldeprivationhypothesissyntax vocalisationbabblingcontextemotive selffulfillingprophecyNoamChomsky languagecriticalperiodimitation elaboratedcodenon-verbalcues epigenicmaturationgrammars languagemorphemesphonemes conventionsonewordstage analyticphilosophyconditioning scriptsrestrictedcodesurfacestructure Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 101 4. Quick Quiz 1. What do you understand by the cognitive process called language? 2. Why is language important? 3. Try to define memory. 4. Identify the elements of language. 5. How do we understand language? 6. How is language acquired by us? 7. What types of language codes have been identified? Describe them. 8. What is the verbal deprivation hypothesis? 9. What do you understand by the term ‘emotive language’? Give three examples. 10. How successful have we been in teaching language to apes and chimpanzees? Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 102 5. Please answer the following questions: a) What is meant by language in cognitive psychology? Your answer should cover • what purpose language has for us • what language consists of • the different types of language used by us to communicate with each other • how we acquire language. b) Read the following scenarios: You are talking to someone of the telephone You are watching television You are writing a letter You are reading a story to your child Choose two of these and describe the role of language in this situation. Your answer must • mention which perceptual process is being used and why it is relevant to/necessary in the situation(s) chosen • refer to the situation directly • give detail about any particular aspects of language identified in the situation(s) • give detail about how aspects of language can be influenced in the situation(s) chosen. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 103 Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 104 INFORMATION FOR STUDENTS - OUTCOME 2 For this outcome you will learn about two research methods from the following; their characteristics and their applications as used by cognitive psychology • • • • • experimental survey interview observation case study By the end of this outcome you should know and understand what two of these research methods involve and be able to identify and describe each as applied within cognitive psychology research. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 105 RESEARCH METHODS IN COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY All psychological knowledge is based upon evidence which is gathered by psychologists in a variety of ways. Students need to develop basic knowledge about methods and techniques used by psychologists in their research. It is intended in ‘research methods’ to establish the foundations of critical understanding in relation to psychological evidence and to distinguish this from ‘common sense’ opinion which explains human behaviour mainly in terms of subjective and idiosyncratic assumptions and preconceptions. Common to Intermediate 1 and Intermediate 2 cognitive psychology these methods are: a) The experimental method: • purpose of the experimental method • main characteristics of the experimental method – cause/effect, control, variables independent and dependent • types of experiment – laboratory, field, naturalistic, quasi-experiment b) The survey method: • purpose of the survey method • main characteristics of the survey method – questionnaires (open/closed questions), representativeness, response bias, standardised design • types of survey – large scale, small scale, attitude/opinion measurement, rating scales c) The interview method: • purpose of the interview method • main characteristics of the interview method – detailed information, personal opinions/views, individualised design • types of interviews – personal structured, personal unstructured d) The observational method: • purpose of the observational method • main characteristics of the observational method – objectivity, standardised recording using checklists, detail, accuracy, interactive (for participant observation) • types of observational methods – participant observation and non–participant (direct) observation e) The case study method: • purpose of the case study method • main characteristics of the case study method – individualised, detailed, personalised, often retrospective, • types of case study – individual, small group Much of the above has already been introduced in ‘Psychology: An Introduction’. Research methods are further extended in other optional units e.g. developmental psychology, social psychology, etc. This unit will not reinvent the wheel! An overview of research methods will be given and two particular research methods will be emphasised i.e. experiment and the survey. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 106 Ethics in Psychological Research Based on the British Psychological Society (BPS) Ethical Guidelines and Code of Conduct 1985. 1. General considerations: especially as much cognitive psychology investigates people always ensure that the research you do is done from the standpoint of the participants taking part. Research should never be offensive to anyone. This means that you should do nothing which threatens a persons health, well-being, or dignity. You should also be aware that we live in a multi-cultural country of diverse ethnic communities. Research should be considered from a socially inclusive, non-sexist, anti-racist and non-ageist perspective. 2. Consent: wherever possible consent should always be got from participants. 3. Deception: deception is not allowed if participants would be unlikely to cooperate in it’s absence. If in doubt the researcher should seek advice from a teacher; lecturer etc. 4. Debriefing: any research should provide participants with an opportunity to discuss the outcomes of it. This is called debriefing, and allows discussion of the specific purpose of the research; interpretation of the participants particular performance, scores, answers etc, and give them an opportunity to ask questions. 5. Withdrawal from the investigation: all participants should give their permission to take part in your research. They should also be allowed to withdraw at any time if they so wish. 6. Confidentiality: unless subject to Scots law and UK statute e.g. the Data Protection Act confidentiality between participant and researcher should be observed at all times. If in doubt seek advice from your teacher, lecturer etc. 7. Protection of participants: all participants in a piece of research should be protected from any physical or mental harm. 8. Observational research: any observation should observe the privacy and psychological well being of those studied. If consent to be observed is not possible, observations should only occur where it would be normal that those observed would/could be by others. If in doubt consult your teacher or lecturer. 9. Giving psychological advice: sometimes during research, the researcher will be asked their advice concerning a psychological matter which is of concern to a participant. The golden rule is not to give advice if not qualified to do so. If in any doubt you should seek advice from your teacher or lecturer. 10. Colleagues: all of us who study psychology share the above set of ethical principles. It is our duty to encourage others who do psychological research to observe these ethical guidelines at all times. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 107 Research Methods: A brief resume In Psychology: An Introduction we discovered that psychologists use 5 main types of research methods. These are: • the experiment • survey • interview • observation • case study The purpose of research is to generate and discover evidence about any relationship between, or probable cause of, psychological phenomena. Research methods give us a systematic set of investigative tools to allow us to do this. Cognitive psychology particularly uses the experiment but also makes use of surveys, interviews, observations and case studies. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 108 All you want to know about: The Experiment • an experiment involves the manipulation of an independent variable (IV) so that we can measure or observe the effect this has on a dependent variable (DV). • An experiment will observe the experimental method which is characterised by the establishment of a cause and effect relationship between an independent and dependent variable; the ability of the researcher to control the environment where the experiment is being conducted and his/her awareness and adeptness to anticipate and control variables not relevant to the experiment. • Experiments are of three main types; laboratory; field; and quasi-experiment. The difference between the three is the location and thus the amount of control the researcher has over variables. • The laboratory experiment sees the manipulation of an independent variable and measurement/observation of the dependent variable and takes place in a closed heavily controlled setting. • The field experiment also sees the manipulation of an IV and measurement/observation of the DV but takes place outwith the laboratory, indoors or out. • The quasi-experiment also occurs away from the laboratory but here there is no manipulation of any independent variable as the IV is already ‘in place’. Measurement/observation of a DV does however follow. An example of a quasiexperiment would be where a psychologist was interested in finding out if there was a relationship (correlation) between speeding restrictions and road traffic accidents in two adjacent countries who were similar in all other respects apart from the legal driving speeds allowed. The independent variable - speed limits is already in place and out of the control of the experimenter. • Variables are things that can creep into an experiment and give rise to alternative explanations for a cause/effect relationship or correlation. These nuisance or extraneous variables (coming from outside) are of two main types - confounding and random. Confounding variables can be controlled with careful thought. Random variables cannot be controlled. • The experiment has advantages of better cause and effect evidence in the form of factual, or empirical, data: precision measurement and control over extraneous variables. • Disadvantages associated with the experiment are that they can often lack ecological validity (are not ‘real’ compared with normal, everyday behaviour); participants, and the experiment can suffer from what are called demand characteristics (participants behaviour is not consistent with their behaviour in everyday life because they realise that they are taking part in an experiment). Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 109 • Confusingly a natural experiment is often included under the heading of the ‘experiment’ in psychology. In a natural experiment the researcher usually observes an animal or human in their natural environment. There is no manipulation of an independent variable in a natural experiment. The researcher does not, and cannot, have one. A hungry lion in the South African veldt is not known for it’s co-operation with psychologists! • Finally, a laboratory experiment would be very appropriate if as a cognitive psychologist you wanted to investigate changing cognitive abilities like perception; attention; language; memory and thinking as a result of the ageing process; biological deterioration; accident etc. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 110 The Experiment as used in Cognitive Psychology Name of Researcher(s): Beltramini (1992) Aim: To investigate and measure the giving/not giving of a gift (IV) had on customers’ perception of a business (DV). Customers were randomly allocated to the experimental group and the control group. Method: Unrelated Independent Sample Design Field Experiment Results: The experimental group showed a raised perception of the company in comparison to the control group. Neither group however bought significantly more from the company!! Name of Researcher(s): Ebbinghaus (1885) Aim: To investigate and measure size and duration of short-term memory Method: Single Participant Design Field Experiment Results: Ebbinghaus using himself as a subject committed longer and longer lists of nonsense syllables (IV) to memory everyday. Putting himself under a variety of conditions, i.e. a certain period of time, after certain activity etc. he tried to recall them (DV). He found that he could extend the size and duration of short-term memory. Name of Researcher(s): Gibson and Walk (1960) Aim: To investigate, measure and observe if depth perception is innate or learned. Method: Laboratory Experiment (and observation). Results: Using their famous visual cliff, 36 babies were placed on one side of it. Their mothers were placed at the end. The mothers called the babies. If the babies were observed to crawl straight over the glass cliff they were adjudged not to have depth perception. 27 infants moved: 3 crawled over the cliff, 24 refused to cross the cliff to reach mother. Gibson and Walk found that babies of six months plus did not crawl over the deep side. The results suggest infants as young as six months do have depth perception, and that depth perception is probably innate. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 111 All you want to know about: The Survey • A survey is a technique where researchers give the same written questionnaire to a sample of people to find out information about their thoughts, feelings and behaviours regards a variety of diverse attitudes and opinions. • A good survey will be designed well in that it will be characterised by those surveyed being given the same easily understood set of instructions (standardised instructions): it will also have carefully worded (standardised) open and closed questions which are randomly presented in the survey itself (this stops some respondents filling in the survey with the answers they think the questioner wants as opposed to how they themselves think, feel and behave). The survey will be given to a reasonable representative sample of the desired population upon which the researcher wishes to draw conclusions i.e. it would be impossible to anticipate the future voting intentions of the Scottish people if only a representative sample of people from the town of Kilmarnock was surveyed. You would only be able to anticipate how the Kilmarnock Westminster MP or Scottish parliament MSP election result might turn out. • An example of an open question in a survey would be ‘What is the biggest problem facing Scotland today?’ If this was given to 100 different people you could get 100 different answers. Concluding what these 100 people have in common would be impossible. Open questions like this in a survey are to be avoided. They give you too much qualitative information with which you can do very little. To be of any value to the researcher data in a survey should be amenable to measurement. It should be quantifiable. • How you can get measurable answers to questions in a survey is to use what are called ‘closed questions’. Closed questions are questions which offer only one choice of response for the participant. They would include questions on what sex you are, male/female; what age you are etc. Questions like this often appear at the beginning of surveys and give the researcher valuable quantitative information as regards how many men and women took part in the survey, what age they were, what postcode district they lived in etc. Where closed questions fall down is when used to try and get quality detailed answers to complex questions. If you consider the closed question ‘The biggest problem facing Scotland today is the state of Premier League football: Yes/No’ you are forcing people to make a choice whether or not they think that football is the biggest problem and you also do not allow people who genuinely ‘do not know’ to respond thus. You can add up and measure yes/know answers to questions, but if the question doesn’t really apply to peoples’ actual thoughts, feelings and behaviours then what you get is meaningless. • One way round the problem of generating quality answers to restrictive closed questions is to use what is called a Likert Scale. This is named after Renais Likert who designed it’s use at the University of Michigan Institute for Social Research in the 1960s . It is often used in the body of surveys today and is recognisable when you are asked to indicate your strength of feeling about a particular issue on a rating scale. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 112 Taking the above question ‘The biggest problem facing Scotland today is the state of Premier League football’ you would indicate strength of feeling on the matter by circling 1 2 3 4 or 5 on a rating scale. Circling 5 would mean you strongly agree, circling 4 agree, circling 3 don’t know, 2 disagree and 1 strongly disagree. This would allow other questions on unemployment, homelessness, education, the National Health Service, etc. to be asked and answered elsewhere. At the end of the day the researcher can generate statistical measurements of peoples attitudes and opinions on a range of issues which they feel are important in the Scotland of 2000 and beyond. The use of Likert scales of measurement in surveys as a means of yielding both qualitative and quantitative data is relatively obvious and widespread. • In order to avoid what is called response bias (people putting down the answers they think you want; or putting down an answer in response to a trigger in the question i.e. Don’t you agree that..?) a number of strategies should be adopted. You should first do a pilot survey e.g. given the draft survey to a number of people whose job it is to pick faults in it’s construction. Once you have sorted out anomalies and ambiguities you should randomly allocate your questions in the final draft. Make Question 1 question 14, question 2 question 7 etc. • The advantages of surveys are that large amounts of information can be got in a short space of time; they are cheap and they can form good solid ground for future research. • The disadvantages are that you cannot draw any cause and effect conclusions from a survey; poor construction suffers from the GIGO effect (garbage in garbage out) and limited and restrictive questions do not get interesting and creative answers. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 113 The Survey as used in Cognitive Psychology Name of Researcher(s): Harvey(1999) of the University of Massachusetts at Amherst Aim: To investigate whether a mother's employment outside of the home influences childrens cognitive development, self-esteem, academic achievement and behaviour. Method: Longitudinal Survey by the American National Longitudinal Survey of Youth (NLSY). The NLSY has surveyed 12,600 individuals annually from 1979 when they were between 14 and 22 years of age. Beginning in 1986, the children of women in the group were also assessed on, amongst other things, cognitive development, self-esteem, academic achievement and behaviour. Results: Following research from 6 previous surveys Harvey (1999) concluded that a mother's employment outside of the home has no significant negative effects on cognitive development, self-esteem, academic achievement and behaviour. The study found that children whose mothers worked during the first three years of their lives weren't significantly different from children whose mothers didn't work during the same timeframe. However, two minor differences were found to decline over time: Children ages 3 and 4 whose mothers returned to work later showed a higher rate of obedience than the same age group whose mothers returned to work sooner. However, these differences were small and disappeared by the time the children were ages 5 to 6. Children whose mothers worked long hours were found to have slightly lower scores on tests that measure vocabulary and individual student achievement. However, these differences were small and faded over time. Name of Researcher(s): Albert; Savage et al (1999) Aim: To investigate physical and social factors which might influence cognitive decline as a result of getting older. Method: Longitudinal 2 year survey of 1,200 elderly people followed for up to two-and-half years and questioned about cognitive functioning, alcohol intake, smoking, emotional support, education, social life, body mass index, activity levels and cholesterol levels. Results: Length of education and physical activity slows down decline in a person’s cognitive abilities despite natural ageing. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 114 Name of Researcher(s): Davie et al (1972) National Child Development Study Aim: To investigate if reading/language ability is influenced by social class. Method: Survey A survey on behalf of the NCDS was conducted by Davie et al with a large sample of children born in 1958. Data concerning reading ability, social class, sex, geographical area, type of house, numbers living in house, social amenities, etc. was collected, collated and analysed. Results: Severe reading retardation (Coolican; Research Methods and Statistics in Psychology Pg. 131!) correlated with overcrowded homes with poor amenities. Children from Social Class V (unskilled manual workers) were found more likely to live in such homes. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 115 All you want to know about: The Interview • The late Geoff Cooke, the much-respected Course Leader of the Postgraduate Diploma in Careers Guidance University of Strathclyde, often described an interview as a ‘conversation with a purpose’. The interview is a research technique where the researcher collects detailed information about individuals in a one-to-one (individualised) situation using oral questions. An interview is used in psychology often in tandem with a survey to supplement and extend knowledge about the person(s)/psychological issue under investigation. Interviews therefore yield qualitative, as opposed to quantitative, data primarily because of its free-ranging nature. • There are many different forms of ‘interview’ used in psychological research and related occupational areas which fall into two broad categories: the personal structured interview known as a standardised interview and the personal unstructured interview known as a clinical interview. • A personal structured interview will have the researcher use pre-planned, carefully constructed and pre-set questions. • Personal structured interviews - while done on a one-to-one (individualised) basis - are very often ‘standardised’ in that the same questions will be asked to different individuals. Personal structured interviews are lengthy and are aimed at obtaining a detailed understanding of a persons mental processes and behaviours. • A personal unstructured interview is a bit more informal in style. The interviewer will have some sort of aim or objective but there is little use of formal pre planned questions. The discussion between the researcher and interviewee is more free ranging. The questions asked really depend on the last answer given by the interviewee. • Whatever style of interview technique used the interviewer, researcher, psychologist or facilitator will have undergone lengthy training. A clinical psychologist , for example, will have studied for over six years completing undergraduate study in psychology and post-graduate study and supervision as a probationer clinical psychologist. Many careers officers in Scotland will also have studied and trained in interview techniques at postgraduate degree level in order that they give considered careers advice when interviewing young people and adults who wish their assistance. • Advantages of the personal unstructured clinical interview are that they often give rise to spontaneous information which is very valuable in understanding the person being interviewed. Personal structured standardised interviews, on the other hand, allow for generalisations to be made. Clinical interviews give a more realistic insight into the person being interviewed because of their more informal free-ranging nature in comparison to the more formal standardised interview. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 116 • Disadvantages of the more informal personal unstructured clinical interview is that you cannot generalise your findings to a larger population. The personal structured standardised interview often restricts those interviewed to answers to questions asked. People tend to answer questions in an automatic and routine way. The interviewer in the more formal structured interview needs substantial training in order that the interview does not seem too awkward and uncomfortable for those taking part. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 117 The Interview as used in Cognitive Psychology Name of Researcher(s): Barkham (1999); Leeds University Aim: To determine whether short-term cognitive-behavioural therapy helps mildly depressed patients. In their study, they tested what they call a "two-plus-one" treatment model, in which patients received two therapy sessions one week apart, and a third session three months later. Participants were 67 men and 47 women in white-collar jobs. The clients, were classified into three groups as either stressed, subclinically depressed or clinically depressed (at low levels) based upon scores on 2 the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) . Each individual in each of the three groups received the two-plus-one treatment either immediately or four weeks after completing the BDI. Method: Series of Clinical Interviews. Results: 'Two-plus-one' CBT works for mild depression, and this very brief therapy shows considerable promise for successfully treating patients with low-level, or subsyndromal, depression. 2 Hence the reason Beck’s Bier sponsors the brilliant Frasier on Channel 4! Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 118 All you want to know about Observation • Observation is the planned gathering of data on observed behaviour. • The characteristics of observation include an in-depth approach to allow us to objectively record observed behaviours of people using a standardised (planned and systematic) format e.g. time frames in a video or carefully designed checklist. This allows for the accuracy and detail needed to come to any firm conclusion about the reason for the observed behaviour. • Observations often take place in peoples homes; school; workplace or in specially arranged settings such as a laboratory testing or interview room. Observations can be one-off or longitudinal i.e. you observe the same ability, skill or behaviour at different points in the participant’s lifetime. • Observation is of three main types: naturalistic, participant and non-participant. Naturalistic observation (observing in a natural setting e.g. a nursery) is often used in cognitive psychology when children are being studied. Participant observation sees the researcher setting up the situation and then taking part in it. Non-participant observation sees the researcher setting up the situation and then recording what happens unobtrusively using, for example, a two-way mirror. He or she would not take part as such. • There is no manipulation of an independent variable in an observation and there are umpteen extraneous variables that can pollute even the best planned observations. • Advantages of the observational technique include high ecological validity (observed behaviour is more likely to be consistent with everyday life) and the spontaneous behaviour of those observed. • Disadvantages of observation are that no cause and effect connections can be made; observations are difficult to replicate in order to confirm the original results and any observation lacks control from the point of view of the researcher. Observation is a popular research method in both social and cognitive psychology. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 119 Observation as used in Cognitive Psychology Name of Researcher(s): Fagot and Gauvain (1999) Aim: To observe whether a mother’s reactions during their child’s temper tantrums affected later problem-solving abilities. Method: Non-participant Observation Fagot and Gauvain first observed 93 children's temperaments at 18 months old and their mothers' behaviour toward them i.e. favourable comments, smiling, criticising, harsh looks etc. in their homes. One year later, the mothers and children were observed while working on two problem-solving tasks in a laboratory when the children were 30 months old. They found that if the mothers rated their child as having a difficult temperament their child were observed to make more errors on laboratory performance tasks taken at 18 months and at 30 months. Disapproving behaviour by the mother when the child was 30 months increased the child's likelihood of developing learning problems at age five. Also, those mothers who gave disapproving looks, criticised or gave support found their children to achieve lower verbal and maths scores on IQ tests in comparison to those mothers who did little during their childrens tantrums. Results: Fagot and Gauvain found that if mothers were cautious or overly critical of temper tantrums their children’s later cognitive development could be affected. As we know from Developmental Psychology a child inherits a temperament which may give rise to a difficult/moody/withdrawn/physically active baby and toddler. His/her temperament can thus affect how their mother will perceive him/her and this can have an influence on their interaction. This in turn Fagot and Gauvain discovered could hinder the child's problem-solving abilities then and in the future. ‘It appears that children perceived as more difficult were more likely to give up on the problem-solving tasks. Some mothers would intervene and offer help while others would become more demanding that the child finish the task. Ultimately, these children were given less opportunity to discover strategies on their own and learn how to problems solve.’ Fagot and Gauvain (1999) Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 120 Name of Researcher(s): Halliday and Leslie (1986) Aim: To investigate the development of language acquisition in mother-child interactions. Method: Non-participant Observation Halliday and Leslie wanted to investigate Bruner’s idea that children develop more sophisticated language/thinking skills out of necessity! They thought that after a while the child would become less successful in getting what it wants by simply referring to it/requesting it i.e. ‘Sweets!!’; ‘Get me juice.’ etc. From a series of pilot studies they discovered 42 different actions occurring during mother-child interaction e.g. describes/gives information; makes questioning noises; holds/takes hold of; imitates completely; makes an emotional noise; looks at mother etc. They then video-recorded 144 half-hour mother-child interaction sessions and using the pilot study categories scored verbal and non-verbal detail between mother and child Results: Bruner is correct in that children use more and more sophisticated forms of language in their interactions with adults to get what they want! Name of Researcher(s): Shneidman (1963) Aim: To investigate whether language competence and thinking skills influenced person perception. Method: Non-participant Observation using content analysis. Developed during the Second World War to investigate the influence of mass media messages content analysis is ‘the (more or less) systematic, objective and quantitative description of a communication or series of communications’ (Crano and Brewer 1973). Shneidman (1963) using content analysis analysed, on the basis of logical argument presented, the televised speeches made by US Presidential candidates Richard Nixon and John F. Kennedy. Results: Shneidman concluded that language competence and thinking skills were measurably better displayed by John F. Kennedy. These televised debates were a first in American broadcasting history and attracted audiences of millions. Before they took place Kennedy was ranked a long-shot to win the race for the White House. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 121 Despite his apparent youth, vitality and Jackie by his side, Kennedy had a political albatross around his neck - he was a Catholic. Political commentators saw these televised debates as crucial to Kennedy winning the Presidential election. Shneidman believed that the logical argument presented by Kennedy during these debates was a factor to this success. American, EU and UK politics have never been quite the same since!! Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 122 All you want to know about: The Case Study • A case study is an in-depth written investigation into an individual who has been found to be unique in some way. They are normally conducted face-to-face with the individual or with individuals in a small group who share something in common. • Characteristics of the case study are that they are orientated towards the individual concerned and as a consequence can yield great personal detail. The document your GP holds on you which contains all your medical details from birth is an everyday example of a case study. • Case studies are of two main types: retrospective and longitudinal. A retrospective case study is when the individual concerned is asked to reflect on their past. This type of case study is problematic in that the individual can be deliberately selective about what they reveal about themselves and often they lie into the bargain. Another type of case study is the longitudinal case study. This is where you study an individual in depth for a period of time into the future of at least one year. Details here are based upon the individuals future. Longitudinal case studies are difficult to manipulate from the point of view of the participant. A very good example of a visual longitudinal case study is the excellent BBC 7Up series which follows physical, cognitive, social and emotional development of a cross-sectional group of British babies all born within a particular week in 1955 as they journey through life. • Advantages of the case study is that it can give excellent in-depth information about an individual and treats all participants as individuals. • The disadvantages of the case study would be that it’s findings cannot be applied to everyone (or generalised to the whole population). As a result the case study as a research tool lacks scientific validity. • The case study within cognitive psychology might be used to investigate the depreciation of memory as a consequence of biological deterioration of parts of the cerebral cortex. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 123 The Case Study as used in Cognitive Psychology Name of Researcher(s): Gregory and Wallace (1963) Method: Longitudinal Case Study Gregory and Wallace conducted a fascinating longitudinal case study with SB, who, blind almost from birth had his sight surgically restored when he was 52. They studied his new visual ability and development and were also able to gather qualitative data concerning SB’s emotional reactions to his new experiences. They recorded SB’s initial euphoria and later depressions due to his disappointment at what he thought sight might be. They also considered perception and the nature-nurture debate. They thought that as SB had spent a lifetime over-compensating with his other senses for his lack of vision his perceptual learning experiences could not be compared to that of a young child. Result: Generated useful medical, psychological and psychotherapeutic data regards the development of visual ability and perception. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 124 Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 125 STUDENT ACTIVITY Research Methods in Cognitive Psychology Answer the following questions: 1a) The experimental method: • • • What is the purpose of the experimental method? Identify and describe it’s main characteristics. What do you understand by the laboratory, field, naturalistic, and quasi-experiment? b) The survey method: • • • • What is the purpose of the survey method? Identify and describe it’s main characteristics. What sort of things in psychology do surveys measure? How can you measure these things in a survey? c) The interview method: • • • What is the purpose of the interview method? What are it’s main characteristics? What do you understand by the terms personal structured and personal unstructured interview? d) The observational method: • • • What is the purpose of the observational method? What are it’s main characteristics? Describe participant observation and non–participant (direct) observation. e) The case study method: • • • What is the purpose of the case study method? What are it’s main characteristics? What is the difference between a retrospective and longitudinal case study? Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 126 2. For the experiment as used in COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY please complete the following: The Experiment: Please give two examples below: a) Name of Researcher(s): Aim: Method: Results: b) Name of Researcher(s): Aim: Method: Results: Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 127 3. For the survey as used in COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY please complete the following: The Survey: Please give two examples below: a) Name of Researcher(s): Aim: Method: Results: b) Name of Researcher(s): Aim: Method: Results: Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 128 4. For observation as used in COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY please complete the following: Observation: Please give one example below: Name of Researcher(s): Aim: Method: Results: Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 129 5. For the case study as used in COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY please complete the following: Case Study: Please give one example below: Name of Researcher(s): Aim: Method: Results: 6. For the interview as used in COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY please complete the following: The Interview: Please give one example below: Name of Researcher(s): Aim: Method: Results: Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 130 GENERAL REFERENCES Mike Cardwell The Complete A-Z Psychology Handbook 1998 Hodder and Stoughton Collins Dictionary and Thesaurus 1992 Collins Hugh Coolican Research Methods and Statistics in Psychology 1990 Hodder and Stoughton Richard D Gross Psychology - The Science of Mind and Behaviour 3rd ed 1997 Hodder and Stoughton Richard D Gross Key Studies in Psychology 2nd ed. 1994 Hodder and Stoughton Nicky Hayes A First Course in Psychology 3rd ed. 1984 Nelson Nicky Hayes Foundations in Psychology 2nd ed. 1998 ITP Gerard Keegan ‘Psychology at Kilmarnock College: Notes and Materials’ 5th ed. 1999 Kilmarnock College ‘Short-Term and Long-Term Effects of Early Parental Employment on Children of the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth’ Elizabeth Harvey, Ph.D., University of Massachusetts at Amherst. Developmental Psychology, Vol. 35, No. 2 American Psychological Association Volume 30, No. 5 September 1999 ‘CBT and Conventional Psychotherapy’ Professional Psychology: Research and Practice (Vol. 30, No. 5, p. 470-473). 'Predictors of Cognitive Change in Older Persons: MacArthur Studies of Successful Aging' by Marilyn S. Albert, Ph.D., and Cary R. Savage, Ph.D., Harvard Medical School, Kenneth Jones, Ph.D., Brandeis University, Lisa Berkman, Ph.D. and Teresa Seeman, Ph.D., Yale University, Dan Blazer, M.D., Duke University Medical School and John W. Rowe, M.D., Mt. Sinai School of Medicine, in Psychology and Aging, Vol. 10, No. 4, pp 568-589. 'Mother-Child Problem Solving: Continuity Through the Early Years' by Beverly I. Fagot, Ph.D., University of Oregon and Oregon Learning Centre and Mary Gauvain, Ph.D., University of California, Riverside, in Developmental Psychology, Vol. 33, No. 3. Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 131 Psychology: Cognitive Psychology (Int1/2) 132