Revisiting modality effects in children’s acquisition of spatial language: Insights from Turkish and Turkish Sign Language (TİD) (English) The visual-spatial modality of sign languages affords analogue mapping of spatial relations in the real world onto signing space – unlike spoken languages. Would this affordance influence spatial language development by deaf children acquiring sign language? Interestingly, previous studies have reported a hindering effect of modality in deaf children’s acquisition of spatial language due to the morphological complexity of spatial predicates in sign languages and the challenges of simultaneous articulation [1, 2, 3]. However, most of these studies lack controlled comparisons across ages (notably to adults) and modalities, and do not generally include data from typologically different and less studied sign and spoken languages. Furthermore, most of this research has been limited to motion descriptions. To go beyond these shortcomings, we compare elicited descriptions of both static (locative) and dynamic (motion) spatial relations by deaf children acquiring Turkish Sign Language (TİD) natively and hearing children learning Turkish to adult patterns in these languages. Data were elicited from three age groups (4-6 years; 7-10 years; adults) in both languages in Istanbul. In each age group, seven participants described pictures of static spatial relations and five participants described videos showing dynamic events. We examined the frequency of explicit encoding of the spatial relations, the order of mention of Figure and Ground, and the strategy of encoding for each language (e.g. use of spatial lexemes, adpositions, classifier predicates, motion verbs, simultaneity). We compared each child group to the corresponding adult targets on the relevant measures. We observed different developmental trajectories in learning to encode static and dynamic relations. For static relations, for both Turkish and TİD, the youngest group differed from the adults in frequency of encoding the spatial relation and order of Figure and Ground. In some of the strategies used, both hearing and deaf children were adult-like in both age groups. Deaf children preferred classifier predicates and used simultaneous constructions for static configurations and hearing children preferred adpositions over a general locative as frequently as adults [4]. However, in encoding angular spatial relations (left-right, front-back) [5], deaf children were faster in achieving adult TİD patterns than hearing children who encoded angular information in Turkish less frequently than adults. In encoding motion, deaf children lagged behind their hearing peers in the use of many adult-like strategies. Deaf children differed from adults in patterns of FigureGround encoding, using general directional verbs (e.g. GO) over more specific classifier constructions, and avoiding simultaneity – as found in previous research. However, deaf children encoded Manner of motion as frequently as adults, unlike their hearing peers who encoded Manner much less than adults [as in 6]. These findings question the general previous assumption that acquiring spatial language in the visual modality poses difficulties for children, and specify further which aspects of a sign language might be more or less challenging, or even facilitating, for signing compared to speaking children. Findings will also be discussed in relation to universal and language-specific aspects of learning spatial language in typologically different spoken languages [7,8]. References 1. Engberg-Pedersen, E. (2003). How Composite is a Fall? Adult’s and Children’s Descriptions of Different Types of Falls in Danish Sign Language. In: Emmorey, K. (Ed.), Perspectives on Classifiers in Signed Languages: 311-332. Mahwah, N. J. / London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 2. Morgan, G., Herman, R., Barriere, I., & Woll, B. (2008). The onset and mastery of spatial language in children acquiring British Sign Language. Cognitive Development, 23, 1-19. 3. Tang, G., Sze, F., & Lam, S. (2007). Acquisition of simultaneous constructions by deaf children of Hong Kong Sign Language. In: Vermeerbergen, M., Leeson, L., & Crasborn, O. (Eds.), Simultaneity in Signed Languages: 283-316. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. 4. Sümer, B., Zwitserlood, I., Perniss, P. M., & Özyürek, A. (2012). Development of locative expressions by Turkish deaf and hearing children: Are there modality effects? In A. K. Biller, E. Y. Chung, & A. E. Kimball (Eds.). Proceedings of the 36th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development. Boston, MA, Cascadilla Press. 5. Levinson, S. C. (1996). Frames of reference and Molyneux's question. In: Bloom, P., Peterson, M.A., Nadel, L., and Garett, M. (Eds.), Language and Space: 109-170. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. 6. Sallandre, M.-A., Courtin C., Fusellier Souza, Y. & L’Huillier, M.-Th. (2010). L’expression des déplacements chez l’enfant sourd en langue des signes française. LIA 1:1, 41-66. 7. Allen, S., Özyürek, A., Kita, S., Brown, A., Furman, R., Ishizuka, T., & Fujii, M. (2007). Language-specific and universal influences in children's syntactic packaging of Manner and Path: A comparison of English, Japanese, and Turkish. Cognition 102(1), 16-48. 8. Ji, Y., Hendriks, H., & Hickmann, M. (2011). Children’s expression of voluntary motion events in English and Chinese. Journal of Foreign Languages 34 (4), 2–20.