Rev Int Organ (2009) 4:47–72 DOI 10.1007/s11558-008-9050-5 IMF programs and human rights, 1981–2003 M. Rodwan Abouharb & David L. Cingranelli Received: 28 February 2008 / Revised: 6 November 2008 / Accepted: 14 November 2008 / Published online: 19 December 2008 # Springer Science + Business Media, LLC 2008 Abstract We examined the effects of International Monetary Fund (IMF) supervised programs on changes in government respect for physical integrity rights in developing countries between 1981 and 2003. A longer period under an IMF program increased government use of torture and extra judicial killing and also worsened the overall human rights conditions in developing countries. The use of a two-stage model ruled out the possibility that human rights practices would have worsened even if IMF programs had not been in effect. Previous studies of the impacts of IMF programs also found that they had worsened government respect for human rights. However, those studies did not control for the effects of selection. We found preliminary evidence that the worsened human rights conditions persisted even after the reforms in program lending of the late 1990s. Keywords International Monetary Fund . Conditionality . Human rights . Physical integrity rights JEL Codes F33 . F34 . F35 . F53 . F55 . F59 Electronic Supplementary Material The online version of this article (doi:10.1007/s11558-008-9050-5) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users. M. R. Abouharb (*) Department of Political Science, 240 Stubbs Hall, Louisiana State University, Baton Rouge, LA 70803, USA e-mail: roda@lsu.edu D. L. Cingranelli Department of Political Science, Binghamton University, PO Box 6000, Binghamton, NY 13902-6000, USA e-mail: davidc@binghamton.edu 48 M.R. Abouharb, D.L. Cingranelli 1 Introduction Since the 1980s, the directors of the International Financial Institutions (IFIs) have promoted a shift in power from the state to the market. The IFIs have promoted a variety of programs in developing and transition countries to achieve that goal. The commonality amongst these programs is the conditionality used to call upon recipient governments to liberalize and privatize economies in the context of strict budget discipline. The IMF has four main types of agreements that involve policy conditions including “The Stand-By Arrangement (SBA), the Extended Fund Facility (EFF), the Structural Adjustment Facility (SAF), and the Enhanced Structural Adjustment Facility (ESAF)” (Vreeland 2003: 10).1 We collectively refer to these different arrangements as ‘IMF programs’. Such measures were intended to jump start economic growth and free up resources for debt service. However, in most countries, public investment in critical areas (health care, education, infrastructure) declined, growth rates were disappointing and debts mounted to unsustainable levels (Pettifor 2001). There is also some evidence that participation in these programs negotiated with the IFIs has caused the governments of less developed countries to reduce their respect for a wide variety of human rights. From a policy perspective the relationship between exposure to programs and respect for human rights is important, because there is mounting evidence that national economies grow fastest when citizens can exercise their human rights (e.g., Kaufman 2004; Sen 1999). Thus, if program lending undermines government efforts to protect human rights, it also undermines IFI efforts to promote economic growth. This paper explores the relationship between the length of time a government had been under IMF programs between 1981 and 2003 and changes in its respect for human rights. The category of human rights we focus on are physical integrity human rights which include the rights of citizens to be protected from torture, political imprisonment, extra-judicial killing, and disappearance. Previous research on the human rights effects of IMF programs found that loan conditions in less developed countries has worsened government human rights practices (Franklin 1997; Keith and Poe 2000; McClaren 1988; Pion-Berlin 1984). In our earlier work, we demonstrated that during the periods that a country was under a World Bank program, torture, political imprisonment, extra-judicial killing, and disappearances were all more likely to occur (Abouharb and Cingranelli 2006). In another study focusing on the joint effects of IMF2 and World Bank program lending, we showed that overall respect for physical integrity rights declined the longer a country had been under programs negotiated by either the IMF or World Bank (Abouharb and Cingranelli 2007). In our 2007 study, we showed that government respect for economic and social rights and worker rights also worsened the longer a program was in place. This is the first large scale, selection-corrected study exploring the human rights impacts of IMF program lending, and the time period examined in this work (1981– 2003) nearly triples the time period examined in any other study of the human rights 1 The ESAF was renamed to the Poverty Reduction and Growth Facility (PRGF) in 1999. 2 We include all four different types of IMF facilities. IMF programs and human rights, 1981–2003 49 effects of IMF programs. We conducted this study for several reasons. First, none of our previous work had estimated the human rights effects of IMF programs. The formal missions of the IMF and World Bank are different, so the human rights effects of IMF program lending may have been different as well.3 Second, while other scholars had examined the human rights effects of IMF program lending, none had controlled for selection effects. Doing so is important, because it is possible that the countries that stayed in IMF programs for long periods were intrinsically difficult cases. Thus, their poor human rights and economic outcomes might have occurred whether or not they were under IMF programs. We needed to determine the counterfactual—namely, what would have happened to developing countries if the IMF had never intervened. Third, this is the only study of the effects of IMF lending on government respect for particular physical integrity rights such as the frequency of the use of torture. All previous studies of the human rights impacts of IMF programs had examined impacts on an aggregate index of respect for physical integrity rights, the Political Terror Scale. Fourth, in this project, we adopted some important refinements to our method for estimating selection effects to better control for the types of countries that were selected for long periods of program treatment rather than simply controlling for the types of countries that negotiate any program loans with the IMF. Finally, for the first time, we compare the human rights effects of IMF programs before and after some reforms in IFI lending took effect in late 1990s. In recent years the IFIs have been more willing to acknowledge their responsibilities for the bad social consequences of program lending. Thus, based on our previous research and the research of others, we hypothesized that the overall human rights effects of IMF programs had been negative between 1981 and 2003, but had been more positive since the reforms of the late 1990s. In general, the results of this study are consistent with the results of all previous work on the physical integrity rights effects of IFI program lending. We found substantial evidence of negative impacts on government respect for physical integrity rights and no evidence of positive impacts. The more years a developing country government spent under IMF programs between 1981 and 2003, the greater was the use of torture and extra-judicial killing and the worse its score on the Political Terror Scale.4 The negative human rights consequences of IMF program lending were more pronounced during the 1981–1998 period as expected. However, we did also find that countries with more exposure to IMF programs between 1999 and 2003 also experienced an increase in political terror during that period. Though it is probably too soon to make an assessment of reforms in program lending, this finding provides preliminary evidence that reforms in IMF lending practices have not helped to reduce the negative social impacts of program lending. 3 The formal mission of the World Bank, when it was first established as a UN agency, was to address issues of economic development, while the IMF was to focus on helping to correct balance of payments problems. Since 1980, the missions of the two international financial institutions have become more similar, and there is increasing cooperation between them in developing country-specific strategies to promote economic development (Abouharb and Cingranelli 2007; Blackmon 2008; Jayarajah et al. 1996). 4 IMF program lending had no effect on the frequency of a government’s use of political imprisonment or disappearances. 50 M.R. Abouharb, D.L. Cingranelli 2 Theory There are a several reasons why a long period under loan conditionality might lead to worse human rights practices by loan recipient governments. In our earlier work (Abouharb and Cingranelli 2006, 2007) we argued that implementation of programs caused discontent among the poor, whose lives often were affected in negative ways as social programs were cut. Discontent produced rebellion in the form of protests, riots, or even rebellion against the government. Governments often responded with greater coercion. But there are other ways that the implementation of programs could begin a chain of events that eventually caused greater repression by the government. There are also ways that the negotiation of loans with the IMF or dependence on loans could cause a government to become more repressive, even when many of the conditions associated with the programs were never implemented. In the discussion below, we begin with causal linkages that assume some implementation of the provisions of programs, and then we consider some that result in more government repression even if few or no provisions of programs were implemented. Besides the program-hardship-rebellion-repression linkage described in our earlier work another causal argument is that implementation of programs weakens the role of the state as it strengthens the market. A shift in power from the state to the market may cause worsened human rights practices by governments mainly because a strong state and substantial government intervention in the market is essential for the protection of all human rights (Donnelly 2003). The protection of physical integrity rights, for example, requires government expenditures for properly training and adequately compensating judges, police and soldiers. Funds are also needed to establish institutions for monitoring the activities of police and soldiers, so that principals (government leaders) can constrain their discretion. More administrative discretion on the part of police and soldiers is likely to lead to greater abuse of physical integrity rights (Policzer 2004). As other examples, the protection of worker rights in general and women’s economic rights, in particular, requires that government regulate the relationship between workers and employers.5 Besides requiring that the government withdraw from much of their involvement in the market, implementation of programs, by definition, requires that the governments of loan recipient countries adopt new economic policies designed to produce substantial, long-term, behavioral changes in the affected populations. People have a natural tendency to resist making substantial changes in their previous behavior (Davidson 2002). Those required to make such changes may start or join social movements including political parties committed to the opposition of government economic policies. They may organize peaceful or violent protests. One of the tools government may use to overcome such resistance to policies is 5 Some scholars have argued that a weak state is necessary for the protection of physical integrity rights. According to this view, a relatively limited government is fundamental to all human freedoms. Limited government reduces barriers to the functioning of the free market, allowing human beings to pursue their own interests in their own ways and allowing them to pursue opportunities that are likely to be lost if human freedom is restricted (Friedman 1962; Hayek 1984). With regard to the physical integrity human rights examined here, Cranston (1964) has argued that respect for these rights only requires forbearance on the part of the state. IMF programs and human rights, 1981–2003 51 increased coercion in the forms of increased torture, political imprisonment, extrajudicial killing, and disappearances. The three causal linkages described above all require that at least some provisions of programs be implemented, but programs may cause repression of human rights even when they are not implemented at all. It is important to consider these arguments, because research has shown that some governments’ never implement the provisions of their programs and many more do not fully implement those provisions (e.g., Dollar and Svensson 2000; Killick 1996; van de Walle 2001). Indeed, previous research has argued that the numbers and types of conditions associated with programs vary for a variety of political reasons (Dreher and Jensen 2007; Stone 2004). Even if the liberalization and privatization provisions of programs are not fully implemented, negotiation of programs can cause greater government repression by reducing the need for governments to raise revenue through taxes on their populations, angering citizens who prefer that their government not cede sovereignty to foreign powers, and causing citizens’ expectations to outpace improvements in economic conditions. We examine each of these arguments below. The revenue argument begins with the contrast between governments that depend, largely or entirely, for revenue on earned income in the form of broad taxation and those that depend on unearned income from oil, gas and mineral exports, foreign grants or foreign loans (Collier 2003; Moore 1998, 2007). In economies that are based on earned income and are less dependent on loans from international financial institutions, elites rely upon levying taxes across a broad base of their citizenry. In return citizens demand efficient and responsive government. This bargain establishes a legitimate political relationship between rulers and subjects. In states where the government depends on unearned income, elites are financially independent of citizen-taxpayers. This changes the political incentives that they face, and the ways they seek, use and retain power. The mutually beneficial relationship between rulers and subjects may not develop or, if developed already, may break down. More insidiously, those leaders who benefit from unearned income may perceive an effective and watchful civil service and civil society as a threat to the benefits they enjoy (Alexander 2006), and they may take steps to thwart them. As a result, citizens are often poorly served by their rulers, and if the citizens complain, unearned income enables governments to pay for armed forces to keep the citizens in check. Thus, countries whose economies are dependent on loans from international financial institutions or whose revenues are dominated by resource extraction industries tend to be more repressive, corrupt and badly-managed (Moore 2007). Furthermore groups opposed to the government may interpret entering into an agreement with the IMF as a signal of government weakness or as a signal of the government’s willingness to collaborate with western interests. These popular perceptions can, in turn, stimulate acts of rebellion, which may cause increased government repression of human rights. For example, the Shagari Administration in Nigeria, had assiduously avoided entering into an IMF agreement despite great need because the general public’s view towards the IMF was one of “vehement popular antipathy” (Callaghy 1990: 269). The government avoided entering into negotiations with the IMF until winning re-election. Having been safely re-elected, the Administration decided to enter into negotiations with the International Monetary 52 M.R. Abouharb, D.L. Cingranelli Fund. Subsequently, a military coup toppled the civilian administration (Vreeland 2003: 37). In Egypt, the government’s willingness to negotiate with the IMF was portrayed by the Muslim Brotherhood as further evidence that the government was too friendly with the West (Kienle 2001). Finally, establishing an IMF program may cause citizen expectations to outpace improvements in economic conditions. The concept of relative deprivation describes the differences between what an individual expects and whether those expectations are satisfied (Davies 1969; Feierabend and Feierabend 1966; Gurr 1968). The signing of a program between an individual’s own government and the IMF may lead to higher expectations about the future of the nation’s economy. If these expectations are not satisfied, the nation’s citizens may feel relatively deprived. Their reaction may be some form of rebellion against the government. This rebellion may stimulate greater levels of government repression of human rights (Gurr 1968). 3 Previous Research on IMF Programs and Human Rights Sen (1999) contended that increasing people’s ability to exercise their fundamental human rights was critical, in an instrumental way, to the promotion of economic growth. At the time Sen wrote his book, there already were suspicions that structural adjustment policies were not producing economic growth in most developing countries. Moreover, in those cases where economic growth had occurred, often the growth did little to alleviate poverty. Some studies even have argued that IMF programs have had a negative effect on economic growth (Dreher 2006; Przeworski and Vreeland 2000; Vreeland 2003). These studies showed that IMF programs produced less growth in developing countries than would have occurred without any IMF intervention. Vreeland noted that structural adjustment did the most damage to the least well-off in society. It usually reduced the size of the “economic pie” to be distributed, and resulted in a more unequal distribution of the pie itself (Vreeland 2002). Lower levels of economic development have been associated with reduced respect for human rights (e.g., Abouharb and Cingranelli 2007; Davenport and Armstrong 2004; Mitchell and McCormack 1988; Poe and Tate 1994; Poe et al. 1999). Research seeking to untangle the causal relationship between respect for physical integrity rights and economic development is in its early stages. The most comprehensive and methodologically sophisticated study on this topic explored the links among respect for human rights, the quality of governance, and the rate of economic growth (Kaufman 2004). Three possible explanations for the strong positive correlation between average per capita incomes and respect for physical integrity and democratic rights were suggested (Kaufman 2004). More respect for these human rights may exert a powerful causal effect on per capita incomes. Alternatively, higher incomes may lead to improvements in respect for physical integrity and democratic rights. As a final possibility, he speculated that the relationship might be spurious. In other words, there might be another factor that makes countries richer and causes them to have more respect for physical integrity and democratic rights. His statistical tests indicated that more respect for physical integrity and democratic rights caused higher level of respect for economic and social rights. Unfortunately, previous studies have shown that IMF programs have been associated with worsened government respect for IMF programs and human rights, 1981–2003 53 physical integrity rights (Abouharb and Cingranelli 2007; Franklin 1997; Keith and Poe 2000; McLaren 1998).6 Many studies of one, two or a few countries have described the negative consequences of structural adjustment programs (e.g., Structural Adjustment Participatory Review Network 2004). Not many of these case or small-n comparative studies focus explicitly on the human rights effects of IFI conditionality, but most of them describe hardships that structural adjustment conditions caused for the poorest people. There are many web sites maintained by human rights non-governmental organizations that also detail the harmful effects of structural adjustment policies on the least well off in developing countries. Work by David Pion-Berlin (1983, 1984, 1989, 1991) explored the linkages between programs and repression of human rights in Argentina and Peru. His work led us to expect that governments seeking to make major economic changes that hurt the poorest members of society would be likely to resort to coercion including the violation of human rights. Earlier studies also found support for the hypothesis that more respect for democratic human rights led to higher levels of effectiveness of World Bank development projects around the world (Isham, Kaufman, and Pritchett 1997) and higher rates of economic growth (Haggard 1995). However, previous research mainly argues that programs negotiated by the IMF principally with the executive branch of developing and transitioning countries may be undermining democratic systems (Haggard 1995; Stiglitz 2002).7 They may be increasing the level of corruption in loan recipient countries (Alexander 2006). Less democracy has been associated with reduced respect for human rights (e.g., Abouharb and Cingranelli 2006, 2007; Davenport and Armstrong 2004; Mitchell and McCormack 1988; Poe and Tate 1994; Poe 1999). Increased corruption also may lead to more violations of human rights, since the existence of corruption makes it more difficult for politicians (principals) to control the discretion of police and soldiers (agents). Partly in response to such criticisms of the consequences of program lending, the IMF and World Bank instituted a series of reforms in the late 1990s. Those reforms included the introduction of the Heavily Indebted Poor Countries Initiative (HIPC) in 19968 coupled with the implementation of the Poverty Reduction Strategy Papers (PRSP) in 1999. The HIPC Initiative was designed to reduce the debt of poor countries through debt relief. The purpose of the PRSP was to re-direct resources that had previously gone to servicing a country’s debt towards programs aimed at improving social services such as health care and education (Blackmon 2008). No previous study of the human rights effects of IMF programs has compared consequences before and after these reforms. The World Bank has been more willing to address poverty and human rights as societal and developmental issues (Abouharb and Cingranelli 2007; Blackmon 2008; World Bank 1998). As former World Bank President Wolfensohn said in his statement to the Bank’s Development Committee, “There is …widespread recognition of the strong link between human rights and development…The Bank is currently reviewing 6 Only two of those studies controlled for selection effects (Abouharb and Cingranelli 2006, 2007). 7 For a more nuanced view, see Abouharb and Cingranelli (2007). Subsequently, the HIPC was strengthened and renamed “the Enhanced HIPC Initiative” (Blackmon 2008). 8 54 M.R. Abouharb, D.L. Cingranelli its role with a view to making a more explicit link between human rights and our work, while at the same time remaining fully in compliance with our Articles of Agreement” (World Bank 2005). Former World Bank President Wolfowitz also expressed his commitment to this endeavor most recently by emphasizing the role of respect for women’s rights in development (World Bank 2005). The IMF has been slower to address issues of poverty and human rights directly, and is still more likely to focus on macroeconomic issues that affect the poor such as high inflation and slower economic growth (Blackmon 2008). An Assistant Director in the IMF’s Office in Europe contended that human rights advocates should not expect the international financial institutions (IFIs) to impose human rights-related policy conditions on their member countries, because the IFIs do not have the expertise required to make judgments about human rights practices and nothing prevents developing country governments under structural adjustment from incorporating human rights into their poverty reduction agreements (Leite 2001). Responding to criticism that the IMF was ignoring the human rights consequences of its activities, another IMF spokesperson declared that it was not obligated to promote human rights around the world. Speaking before the United Nations Sub commission for the Promotion and Protection of Human Rights, he said that the Fund, in a strict sense, does not have a mandate to promote human rights. He also stressed that the IMF is not “bound by various human rights declarations and conventions” (Capdevila 2001). Several members of the UN Sub commission expressed their disappointment. Yozo Yokota, of Japan, noted that human rights are “peremptory norms” that cannot be ignored in agreements between states or in the operations of international financial institutions (Capdevila 2001). Estimating the human rights effects of program lending by the international financial institutions requires the use of a two-stage econometric model. As explained by Achen (1986), Dreher (2006), Heckman (1988), Przeworski and Vreeland (2000) and Vreeland (2002, 2003) issues of endogoneity, selection, and randomization must be accounted for when assessing the impact of any public policy. Unfortunately, few previous studies of the human rights effects of program lending had controlled for the effects of selection. Perhaps the countries the IMF had worked with had failed because they were intrinsically difficult cases. We needed to determine the counter-factual— namely, what would have happened to developing countries if the IMF had never intervened. One needs to disentangle the impacts of the policy from any prior attributes that may also have an impact (Collier 1991). Were the negative effects of IMF programs on government respect for physical integrity rights practices found in previous research the result of the economic difficulties that made the loan recipient country a good candidate for a program in the first place or were they the consequence of the program itself? Single-stage models cannot provide an answer to that question. 4 Hypotheses There is a substantial literature explaining IMF selection practices.9 The findings of that research suggest that economic, political, conflict, and human rights factors help 9 Thorough reviews of studies examining the selection biases of the IMF can be found in Abouharb and Cingranelli (2007), Stone (2004) and Vreeland (2003). IMF programs and human rights, 1981–2003 55 determine when countries go under IMF programs. Previous research has found that governments are more likely to go under IMF programs if they have greater economic difficulty, greater respect for workers’ rights, an alliance with the United States, larger populations, more authoritarian political institutions, or a previous colonial/dependent relationship with the US, the UK or France (Abouharb and Cingranelli 2006, 2007; Dreher 2006; Joyce 1992; Keith and Poe 2000; Stone 2004; Vreeland 2003). They also were more likely to go under IMF programs before the end of the Cold War and during years when larger numbers of countries were under programs (Abouharb and Cingranelli 2007; Vreeland 2003). The focus of this study is on the second-stage hypotheses concerning the human rights effects of programs. Previous research supports the following hypotheses, which will be tested: H1: The greater the number of years a country has been under IMF-supervised programs, the more frequent will be the government’s use of torture, political imprisonment, disappearances, and extra-judicial killings. H2: The relationship between the number of years under programs and repression of physical integrity rights will be negative prior to 1999. After 1999, because of reforms enacted in the late 1990s, there will be a positive relationship. Since no previous study has examined the human rights effects of IMF programs after the adoption of reforms in lending instituted in the late 1990s, the hypothesized positive relationship between exposure to IMF programs and respect for physical integrity rights after 1999 requires justification. The reforms were not expressly designed to improve the human rights practices of loan recipient governments. They were designed to reduce the potential hardships of debt payment, privatization and liberalization policies on the poorest people in loan recipient countries. There would be no expected positive effect of the reforms if the debt payment, privatization and liberalization policies included in IMF programs were never implemented. In those cases, the act of going under an IMF program could have caused greater government repression by reducing the need for governments to raise revenue through taxes on their populations, angering citizens who prefer that their government not cede sovereignty to foreign powers, and causing citizens’ expectations to outpace improvements in economic conditions. Thus, if the reforms made a difference, they did so because they changed the content of IMF programs or the way these programs were implemented. Other studies—especially those by Poe (2004), Poe and Tate (1994), and Poe, Tate and Keith (1999)—have demonstrated that wealthier countries, more democratic countries, and countries with a British colonial experience tend to have governments that provide more respect for the physical integrity rights of their citizens. Countries with military governments, relatively large populations, relatively large population increases, high levels of domestic conflict, and involvement in interstate war tend to have governments that provide less respect for the physical integrity rights of their citizens. The findings on the human rights effects of interstate war are the least consistent in previous research.10 All of these factors were included as control variables in the second, human rights stage of the analysis. 10 See for example the findings of Landman (2005). 56 M.R. Abouharb, D.L. Cingranelli 5 Research Design This study uses a cross-national, annual time-series dataset comprised of 131 developing countries, with a population of at least 500,000 in 1981, which were in existence for the entire period under examination. Our IMF program implementation measure is a running count of the individual years countries have spent under IMF programs for the entire period. It measures the long-term, cumulative effects of IMF programs. More detail on the construction of this measure is provided below. The data spans the time period from 1981 to 2003. During the period of our study, the IMF negotiated a total of 465 programs to 98 countries in our sample. The unit of analysis is the country year. At the second, human rights impact stage, we investigate whether the number of years under an IMF program between 1981 and 2003 was associated with a change in the frequency of the use of torture, political imprisonment, extra-judicial killing and disappearances by the governments of loan recipient countries. This permits a test of our first hypothesis. To test the second hypothesis, we compare the human rights effects of IMF programs on loan recipients between 1981 and 1998 with the human rights effects on loan recipients between 1999 and 2003. 5.1 Variables The number of years each country was under a program in a particular year is our dependent variable in our first selection stage and in the second, human rights stage of our analysis. Our data-set uses Vreeland’s (2003) measure of when countries were under IMF programs. His data-set separately codes each time a country negotiated an IMF program. He also codes the years that countries were under programs using the IMF Annual Reports to generate this information. Vreeland’s (2003: 167) approach gives a value of ‘1’ for each year a country is under any of the following IMF loans: Stand-By Agreements, Extended Fund Facility (EFF), Structural Adjustment Facility (SAF) and Enhanced Structural Adjustment Facility (ESAF), (whose name was changed to Poverty Reduction and Growth Facility (PRGF) in 1999). We follow his coding scheme as we updated his data through 2003 using the IMF Annual Reports. The commonality amongst all these programs is that each has economic liberalization conditions attached. To be sure, the breadth and depth of the conditions attached change depending upon the particular type of loan and the individual program negotiated by the loan recipient governments and the International Monetary Fund. Our update provided us with information about when countries negotiated these programs and, importantly for this research, the individual years which countries were under IMF conditionality over the period 1981–2003. Using this measure we are able to count the cumulative number of years countries have been under IMF conditionality. This measure is used as the dependent variable in the first stage of our equation estimating the length of time countries are under IMF programs and is also our key independent variable in the human rights stage equations. Our measure, apart from being a cumulative count of the years under IMF conditionality, contains one difference from Vreeland’s approach. Other research has determined that on average it takes eighteen months for any implementation to affect the economy (Jayarajah et al. 1996). For this reason, the year of negotiation was coded as “0” in IMF programs and human rights, 1981–2003 57 our measure indicating whether a country was under program conditions unless a previously negotiated IMF program was still in effect that year. We generated a running count of the numbers of years under IMF programs countries have spent during the period 1981–2003. Thus the number can range from 0 to 22. A value of 22 indicates that a country had been under an IMF program for the entire period.11 To test whether the human rights effects were different before and after IMF reforms were put in place in 1999 we created one “number of years under a program” variable from 1981–1998 and another from 1999–2003. The former variable ranges between 0 and 18, and the latter ranges between 0 and 4. In developing our selection model, we did not seek to contribute to the large literature on IMF selection. There is nothing new included in our selection equation. Our purpose was to create a selection equation that included those variables repeatedly found to be significant in previous research while at the same time ensuring proper identification of the selection and human rights impact equation models. For the current research project this required finding variables that in the first stage of our model predict program participation but are not correlated with human rights violations, and in the second stage of our model predict human rights violations but are not correlated with program participation. Several variables included in the selection equation have never been shown to affect human rights violations such as average foreign currency reserves, alliance with the United States, the number of countries under IMF programs, most types of colonial experiences. Variables included in the second stage human rights impact equation that have been shown to predict human rights violations but not IMF participation were the level of domestic conflict, the level of interstate conflict, and population density. The number of years a country has been under IMF programs is not the same as the number of years that IMF programs have been implemented. Many scholars have noted that conditions included in most IMF programs are not fully implemented (e.g., Dollar and Svensson 2000; Dreher 2006; Dreher and Jensen 2007; Killick 1996). It is conceivable, therefore, that one or more of the countries in our sample was under IMF programs for most of the period of our study, but never fully implemented its program in any of the years it was under those conditions. We considered the possibility of controlling for degree of implementation of programs but decided not to do so. First, some of our theoretical arguments linking years under IMF programs to the level of repression by loan recipients are not contingent on the implementation of all or most provisions of these programs. In addition, there is a wide gap between existing measures of IMF program implementation and our theoretical arguments that do link human rights abuse to implementation of these programs. The measures used to assess implementation have been proxies such as the amount of money drawn from the IMF as a proportion of the amount that could be drawn in a particular year (Killick 1996; See Dreher 2006 for a good review of the literature). When governments drew less than 80% of this total, Killick evaluated them as not fully implementing the provisions of their agreements. Killick inferred that the partial draw down occurred because the loan recipient government had been 11 Note that the value of numbers of years under IMF programs maximizes at 22 rather than 23 because we exclude the first year a government enters into a program unless it is already under conditionality from a previous loan. 58 M.R. Abouharb, D.L. Cingranelli sanctioned by the IMF for failing to fulfil essential provisions of the program. This approach to evaluating the degree of program implementation has been criticized along a number of different dimensions (Bird and Willett 2004). In some cases the IMF may disburse its money even though implementation of conditions has been poor, because it feels that significant progress has been made, or even for political reasons (Bird and Willett 2004). Indeed, other work has described how politics permeates if and when loans are approved as well as the type and number of conditions associated with them (Stone 2004). In other cases, the whole of the available loan may not be used because of improvements in the economy. In still other cases governments negotiate programs as a precautionary tactic, without intensions to draw at all. More important, the use of a proxy measure of implementation of IMF programs that relies on the percentage of funds that were drawn down does not capture the “implementation-hardship-dissent-repression” linkage we have proposed. If any provisions of an IMF program were implemented that caused hardships for the poor, then discontent could have arisen that could have led to civil conflict and have evoked government repression. Imagine the hypothetical situation where a water system was privatized in one city pursuant to an IMF program but no other provisions of the program were implemented. Killick’s (1996) measure of the degree of implementation of IMF programs in countries around the world probably would indicate that this was a country where the program was only partially implemented. Despite the lack of full implementation, there may have been demonstrations, riots, and repression. We need a measure indicating whether any provision of a program was implemented that caused hardships for poor people in a particular country and in a particular year. No existing measure of IMF program implementation captures this idea. Finally, our study encompasses the 1981–2003 period. The units of analysis are country years. No data set on implementation covers this period, using country years as units of analysis for all less economically developed countries. Constructing a new measure of implementation was impractical, because it is difficult to obtain information about the kinds of conditions included in these programs for a large number of countries over a long period of time. Often documents concerning the degree of implementation of conditions in particular countries for particular years are proprietary in nature. The IMF’s own database on implementation of program conditions has been criticized for the non-random sample of loans included (Dreher 2006). Tables 1 and 2 explain the operationalization of the independent variables used in the first and second stages of the analysis. 5.2 The Dependent Variables: Human Rights Practices of Governments The human rights practices of governments are the dependent variables in the second stage. Four physical integrity rights from the CIRI human rights dataset (Cingranelli and Richards 2004) were used as dependent variables—extra-judicial killings, disappearances, political imprisonment, and torture. The annual US State Department Country Reports on Human Rights Practices and Amnesty International annual reports were used by trained coders to develop the scores in both data sets. Each of the four physical integrity variables was coded on a three-point ordinal scale where 0=frequent violations of the right (50 or more), 1=some violations (1–49), and 2=no violations. The correlations among the four physical integrity rights from 1981–2003 IMF programs and human rights, 1981–2003 59 Table 1 Operationalization of first-stage equation variables Dependent variable Indicator Source Years under IMF programs Running count of number of years under IMF Program. Each year of loan period coded “1” & 0 otherwise. Vreeland (2003) updated by authors using IMF Annual Report GDP per capita current U.S. $ (Purchasing Power Parity) Percentage change in GDP per capita current U.S. $ Purchasing Power Parity (PPP) Average government foreign reserves to reflect monthly imports Average annual official exchange rate local currency unit per US $ Trade as a percentage of GDP World Bank: World Development Indicators CD-Rom (WDI) World Bank: WDI Independent variables Economic GDP per capita Percentage change in GDP per capita Average foreign currency reserves Exchange rate value International trade Political Alliance with the United States Post cold war Correlates of war (COW) alliance measure Dichotomous, “0” before 1991; “1” if 1991 or later Level of respect for 0=Not protected by govt. workers rights 1= Somewhat protected by govt. 2=Protected by govt. Level of democracy Democracy-Autocracy measure Military regimes Type of regime: Civilian or military Japanese, French, UK, USA The decision rule of the most Dependent/Colonial recent possessor is used to Experience identify the relationships under examination. Number of countries under Number of countries under IMF IMF programs programs in each year World Bank: WDI World Bank: WDI World Bank: WDI COW alliance dataset Cingranelli and Richards CIRI) (2004) POLITY IV Dataset Banks (2002) updated using Freedom House Reports Issues COW Colonial History Dataset Hensel (2006) Vreeland (2003). Updated by authors using IMF annual reports. ranged from 0.35 between political imprisonment and extra-judicial killing to 0.56 between disappearances and extra-judicial killing in our sample. Over the period of this study, torture and political imprisonment were the most common forms of abuse of physical integrity rights. Frequent use of disappearance and extrajudicial killing by the state was much less common (Cingranelli and Richards 1999a, 1999b). As an additional test, the Political Terror Scale (PTS), an index including all four elements, was used as well (Gibney and Dalton 1996; Gibney et al. 2007). The CIRI scores measure government human rights practices. The PTS measures human rights conditions in each country. Human rights practices refer to what governments actually do to protect the physical integrity human rights of their citizens. Human rights conditions refer to the extent to which citizens actually can enjoy those rights. The distinction is important, because sometimes, as in the case of civil war, citizens cannot enjoy their human rights, but governments may not be responsible, or are only partially responsible for the human rights violations. Our theories concern government policies, so the CIRI scores are most relevant to the tests of the hypotheses. 60 M.R. Abouharb, D.L. Cingranelli Table 2 Operationalization of second-stage equation variables Dependent variable Indicator Source Disappearances 0=Frequent (50+) 1=Occasional (1–49 instances) 2=None 0=Frequent (50+) 1=Occasional (1–49 instances) 2=None 0=Frequent (50+) 1=Occasional (1–49 instances) 2=None 0=Frequent (50+) 1=Occasional (1–49 instances) 2=None Ordinal level of human rights conditions (1–5 measure). Original scale inverted for ease of comparison. 1=Worst Human Rights Conditions, 5=Best. Cingranelli and Richards CIRI (2004) CIRI (2004) Killings Torture Political imprisonment Political terror scale Independent variables Years under IMF programs CIRI (2004) CIRI (2004) (Gibney et al. 2007). Running count of number of years under IMF Programs. Each year of loan period coded “1” & 0 otherwise. Vreeland (2003) updated by authors using IMF Annual Report Control variables economic GDP per capita GDP per capita current U.S. $ (PPP) World Bank: World Development Indicators CD-Rom (WDI) Political Level of democracy Log of population Democracy Logged midyear country population POLITY IV Dataset U.S. census: International Data Base Issues COW Colonial History Dataset Hensel (2006) UK dependent/colonial experience Conflict proneness Level of interstate conflict Level of domestic conflict Temporal dependence Lagged dependent variable The decision rule of the most recent possessor is used to identify the relationships under examination. Ordinal level of international conflict (0–3 measure) Ordinal level of civil conflict (0–3 measure) Gleditsch et al. (2002) Disappearances, killings, torture, political imprisonment lagged 1 year Constructed Gleditsch et al. (2002) The PTS is one of the most widely used indices measuring levels of political violence and terror that a country experiences in a particular year based on a 5-level “terror scale” originally developed by Freedom House. The ordinal scale ranges from level 1, where the level of enjoyment of physical integrity rights is very high to level 5 where the level of enjoyment is very low. The human rights situation in levelone countries is described as follows: Countries are under a secure rule of law, people are not imprisoned for their views, and torture is rare or exceptional. Political murders are extremely rare. The human rights situation in level-five countries is described as follows: Terror has expanded to the whole population. The leaders of these societies place no limits on the means or thoroughness with which they pursue personal or ideological goals (Gibney and Dalton 1996). IMF programs and human rights, 1981–2003 61 5.3 Statistical method This research project incorporates a number of methodological refinements. As noted, the models we present provide a better identification of the selection and second stage equations. We argue that those countries that spend more time under IMF programs represent a non-random sample of all the possible countries often poor and in economic distress, which other work has found are more likely to be repressors of human rights. It is important to control for these effects when estimating the human rights impacts of IMF programs. In our earlier work (Abouharb and Cingranelli 2006, 2007), we corrected for the probability that a country would enter into an IMF program. However, in this work we correct for the probability that a specific country would spend more or less time under a program. Arguably, this is a better approach, since, at the second stage, we wish to estimate the human rights effects of the amount of time spent under an IMF program. We chose to use a three-stage least squares estimation procedure. Three-stage least squares is more efficient than two-stage least squares, but is more sensitive to model specification. As a test of robustness, the results also were estimated using two-stage least squares. Results concerning the human rights impacts of longer participation in IMF loan programs were the same.12 For ease of presentation we begin by discussing the model examining the determinants of time spent under IMF programs and then move to a discussion of the human rights effects of IMF programs. An OLS analysis was used in the first model to estimate the length of time countries were under IMF programs. We also employed a number of other specifications to assess the robustness of our first stage equation including time dummies for each year and an alternate negative binomial regression estimation procedure. We then ran our three-stage least squares models. We present just the second human rights stage equations. Table 3 presents the descriptive statistics. As shown in Table 3, the most respected physical integrity rights are the rights not to be disappeared and to be protected from extra-judicial killing. These are arguably the most extreme forms of abuse of physical integrity rights. Torture and political imprisonment are much more common as is indicated by their near-zero mean values. This ranking of government respect for physical integrity rights is consistent with the findings of earlier research (Cingranelli and Richards 1999a, 1999b). The average number of years under programs between 1981 and 2003 for the countries included in our study was about four and one-half years, but several countries were under programs for the entire period of the study—22 years. 6 Results 6.1 Numbers of Years Under IMF Programs The first-stage results in Table 4 display the factors that affect the numbers of years a country spent under an IMF program over the period between 1981 and 2003. We discuss the results that were robust across two or more of the three models presented 12 These are available from the authors upon request. 62 M.R. Abouharb, D.L. Cingranelli Table 3 Descriptive statistics Variable name Mean Dependent variables Disappearance 1.574 Extra judicial killing 1.165 Political imprisonment .89 Torture .643 Political terror scale 3.383 Key independent variables Years under IMF programs 4.342 (Cumulative Years under IMF (8.99) programs for 2003) Control variables Alliance with United States .712 Average foreign currency reserves 3.4 Level of democracy 3.193 Level of domestic conflict .553 Exchange rate value 2,417.984 French dependent/colonial experience .186 GDP per capita 4,041.62 International trade 75.3227 Level of interstate conflict .076 Japanese dependent/colonial experience .015 Military regime .123 Percentage change in GDP per capita 1.082 Log of population 15.46 Post cold war .54 Level of respect for workers rights .811 UK dependent/colonial experience .36 USA dependent/colonial experience .007 Standard deviation Minimum Maximum .693 .7936 .83 .689 1.15 0 0 0 0 1 2 2 2 2 5 5.095 (6.974) 0 (0) 22 (22) .453 2.989 3.756 1.035 47,229.83 .389 4,175.651 41.825 .429 .121 .329 6.131 2 .499 .75 .48 .086 0 1 −.092 27.084 0 10 0 3 9.33E-12 1,507,226 0 1 339.999 25,317.89 1.531 280.361 0 3 0 1 0 1 −50.486 89.828 10.326 20.979 0 1 0 2 0 1 0 1 in Table 4. The results indicate the importance of economic and political factors, both international and domestic, affecting the length of time countries spent under IMF programs. Countries spent more time under IMF programs if they had a lower GDP per capita or an over-valued exchange rate. Countries allied with the United States spent longer periods under IMF programs as did those who were under IMF programs after the end of the Cold War. We find that countries with higher levels of respect for workers’ rights and those that were more democratic spent longer periods under IMF programs. There is also some support that countries which have dependent relationships with key backers of the IMF were treated differently. Those that had a Japanese, French or U.S. dependent/colonial relationship were likely to spend longer periods under IMF programs. Finally as the competition for IMF programs increased, indicated by the number of countries under an IMF program, the length of time countries spent under IMF programs declined. 6.2 The Impact of IMF Programs on Government Respect for Physical Integrity Rights Four separate models were run—one for each of the four physical integrity rights. IMF programs had no significant effect on freedom from political imprisonment or IMF programs and human rights, 1981–2003 63 Table 4 Number of years under IMF programs, 1981–2003, all developing countries Number of years under IMF programs Economic variables GDP per capita Percentage change in GDP per capita Average foreign currency reserves Exchange rate value International trade International political variables Alliance with United States Post cold war Domestic political variables Level of respect: workers rights Level of democracy Military regime Japanese dependent/colonial experience French dependent/colonial experience UK dependent/colonial experience USA dependent/colonial experience Number of countries under IMF programs Constant N R-Squared Ordinary least squares (Robust St. Errors) OLS with year dummies1 (Robust St. Errors) Negative binomial regression model −.0004*** (.00008) .017 (.022) −.142 (.105) 2.01e-06** (6.65e-07) −.015 (.009) −.0004*** (.00008) −.004 (.022) −.202* (.093) 1.27e-07 (6.77e-07) −.016 (.009) −.0001*** (.00003) .009 (.006) −.045 (.03) 3.19e-07** (1.12e-07) −.004 (.002) 1.623* (.67) 6.534*** (.45) 1.471* (.639) 14.302*** (.892) .329* (.139) 1.183*** (.091) .522* (.27) .273** (.084) −.667 (.518) 3.473*** (.798) 2.359** (.847) .967 (.763) 4.343*** (.588) −.027* (.013) 3.267*** (1.181) 1,874 .46 .575* (.28) .243** (.081) −.54 (.518) 4.336*** (.601) 2.204** (.859) 1.078 (.762) 4.336*** (.601) −.249*** (.023) 12.754*** (1.538) 1,874 .58 .154 (.055) .068*** (.021) −.069 (.151) .938*** (.243) .358 (.197) .092 (.17) .464*** (.124) .003 (.003) .749** (.284) 1,874 Standard errors in parentheses; *p<0.05; **p<0.01; ***p<0.001; two tailed test. included, not displayed because of space constraints 1 Year Dummies disappearance, so these results are not shown.13 The results in Tables 5 and 6 indicate that greater exposure to programs had a significant negative impact on government practices respecting the citizens’ rights to freedom from torture and extra-judicial killing for both the entire 1981–2003, and the shorter 1981–1999 periods. Both human rights variables range from zero to two, with lower scores indicating more frequent violations of the right by the government. Thus statistically significant, negative relationships between the number of years under an IMF 13 These results are available as a web appendix on the Review of International Organization’s website. 64 M.R. Abouharb, D.L. Cingranelli Table 5 3-stage least squares model of years under IMF programs on freedom from torture, 1981–2003, all developing countries1 Respect for Human Rights Column I 1981–2003 Column II 1981–1998 Column III 1999–2003 Years under IMF programs −.015*** (.004) −.017** (.006) −.004 (.009) −2.81e-06 (3.81e-06) .013*** (.004) −.075*** (.009) .055 (.032) −.082*** (.014) .003 (.027) .444*** (.021) 1.639*** (.152) 1,794 7.34e-06 (5.03e-06) .016*** (.004) −.074*** (.01) .058 (.037) −.093*** (.016) .006 (.03) .442*** (.024) 1.64*** (.175) 1,398 .00001 (7.72e-06) .002 (.007) −.078*** (.018) .051 (.063) −.045 (.026) −.005 (.053) .393*** (.045) 1.533*** (.318) 396 Control variables GDP per capita Level of democracy Log of population Level of interstate conflict Level of domestic conflict UK dependent/colonial experience Lagged dependent variable Constant N Standard errors in parentheses; *p<0.05; **p<0.01; ***p<0.001; two tailed test. 1 Only Second stage equations are displayed. Table 6 3-stage least squares model of years under IMF programs on freedom from extra judicial killing, 1981–2003, all developing countries1 Respect for human rights Column I 1981–2003 Column II 1981–1998 Column III 1999–2003 Years under IMF programs −.01* (.004) −.02** (.006) −.003 (.01) 7.72e-06* (3.98e-06) −.005 (.004) −.074*** (.01) .039 (.034) −.149*** (.015) −.004 (.028) .513*** (.02) 1.893*** (.163) 1,795 6.99e-06 (5.11e-06) −.005 (.004) −.063*** (.011) .063 (.038) −.175*** (.017) .032 (.031) .49*** (.023) 1.781*** (.182) 1,399 .00001 (9.07e-06) −.006 (.009) −.104*** (.021) −.033 (.072) −.077** (.03) −.112 (.061) .533*** (.043) 2.279*** (.375) 396 Control variables GDP per capita Level of democracy Log of population Level of interstate conflict Level of domestic conflict UK dependent/colonial experience Lagged Dependent Variable Constant N Standard errors in parentheses; *p<0.05; **p<0.01; ***p<0.001; two tailed test. 1 Only Second stage equations are displayed. IMF programs and human rights, 1981–2003 65 program and these indicators of respect for physical integrity rights in columns I and II support our first hypothesis. We hypothesized that we would find a positive relationship in Column III, estimating the relationship in the post-reform period, but this coefficient is insignificant in both tables. Thus, these results support our first, but not our second hypothesis. As a test of robustness, we substituted the Political Terror Scale (PTS) in the second stage equation. The results from this specification are displayed in Table 7. The PTS is a five-point ordinal scale (Gibney and Dalton 1996) designed to measure the extensiveness of political terror in a society. The scale was inverted for this test, so the direction of the coefficient would be consistent with our two earlier tables. Low scores indicate more terror. As a reminder, the PTS measures human rights conditions in a country, whereas the CIRI scores measure government human rights practices. Again, the results provide strong support for our first hypothesis. Longer periods under IMF programs significantly worsened the personal security rights situation in developing countries across all three periods examined. The significant and negative relationship for the 1999–2003 period provides additional evidence that the changes instituted by the IMF in the late 1990s have not improved the personal security rights situation in developing countries. The control variables included in Tables 5, 6 and 7 that exerted statistically significant effects behaved as one would have expected given the results of previous research. Other things being equal, countries that were more democratic, had larger populations, and had lower levels of violent domestic conflict also had better human rights practices. Table 7 Test of robustness 3-stage least squares model of years under IMF programs on political terror scale, 1981–2003, all developing countries1 Respect for human rights Column I 1981–2003 Column II 1981–1998 Column III 1999–2003 Years under IMF programs −.014*** (.004) −.019*** (.006) −.022* (.01) 6.32e-06 (4.01e-06) .02*** (.004) −.073*** (.01) −.008 (.034) −.203*** (.016) .002 (.028) .617*** (.017) 2.534*** (.184) 1,835 5.5e-06 (5.19e-06) .021*** (.005) −.065*** (.011) −.023 (.039) −.226*** (.019) .024 (.032) .609*** (.019) 2.445*** (.208) 1,443 7.84e-07 (8.95e-06) .021* (.009) −.105*** (.021) .069 (.069) −.137*** (.031) −.091 (.061) .62*** (.039) 3.178*** (.411) 392 Control variables GDP per capita Level of democracy Log of population Level of interstate conflict Level of domestic conflict UK dependent/colonial experience Lagged dependent variable Constant N Standard errors in parentheses; *p<0.05; **p<0.01; ***p<0.001; two tailed test. 1 Only Second stage equations are displayed. PTS scale inverted. 66 M.R. Abouharb, D.L. Cingranelli 6.3 Model Predictions Figure 1 displays the relationship between differing levels of exposure to IMF programs over the entire 1981–2003 period and the frequency of torture. These predictions hold all the independent variables constant to their mean or modal value and provide the baseline to compare the effects of IMF programs on the frequency of torture. The intercept indicates that in developing countries the frequency of torture is close to a value of ‘1’ on our dependent variable indicating that peoples’ rights are sometimes violated (1–49 violations) even if a country had no exposure to IMF programs. This prediction accords with actual levels of violations in most developing countries. Figure 1 indicates that the longer a government had been under an IMF program the more frequent its use of torture. The level drops by almost .4 from countries that have been never under these programs to those that have been under for the entire period of our study. While these predictions are mean predictions they do indicate a steady worsening of the situation in developing countries under IMF programs. With longer exposure to IMF programs, on average, governments moved from occasionally torturing their citizens to becoming frequent violators of this right. Figure 2 displays the relationship of the level of exposure to IMF programs over the 1981–2003 period and its average impact on a government’s use of extra judicial killing. The intercept in this case indicates that developing countries have a slightly higher level of respect for the right not to be extra judicially killed with an average value of just above 1.2. Since this value is close to a value of ‘1’ on our dependent variable it suggests that developing countries with no exposure to IMF programs still occasionally extra-judicially kill their citizens. The impact of increased exposure to IMF programs also leads to a progressive, if smaller, worsening in the frequency of extra judicial killing from a value of just over 1.2 for those countries who have never gone under these programs to a value of about 1.0. While this drop is smaller it reflects the fact that at least a few countries with long exposure to IMF programs moved from occasionally to frequently extra-judicially killing their citizens. Fig. 1 The impact of years under IMF programs on the frequency of use of torture 1981–2003. Low scores indicate more extra judicial killing. Full scale ranges from zero to two IMF programs and human rights, 1981–2003 67 Fig. 2 The impact of years under IMF Programs on the frequence of extra judicial killing 1981–2003. Low Scores indicate more extra judicial killing. Full scale ranges from zero to two We also generated predictions about the human rights situation in developing countries using the PTS measure. Figure 3 displays the relationship of the level of exposure to IMF programs over the 1981–2003 period and its impact on the human rights situation in developing countries. The intercepts in this case indicates that developing countries have a value of just over 3.4 on our inverted PTS scale. While this mean score falls within two categories, the PTS descriptions give a good indication of the human rights situation in developing countries that have not undertaken IMF programs. A score of three on the PTS scale is described as follows “There is extensive political imprisonment, or a recent history of such imprisonment. Execution or other political murders and brutality may be common. Unlimited detention, with or without a trial, for political views is accepted” (Gibney et al. 2007). Our initial score suggests that the conditions are somewhat better than a value of three on the PTS scale. In comparison a score of 4 on our inverted scale would correspond to an original PTS score of two described as follows: “There is a limited Fig. 3 The impact of years under IMF programs on political terror scale 1981–2003. PTS scale inverted. Lower Scores indicate more terror. Full scale ranges from one to five 68 M.R. Abouharb, D.L. Cingranelli amount of imprisonment for nonviolent political activity. However, few persons are affected, torture and beatings are exceptional. Political murder is rare” (Gibney et al. 2007). Thus our baseline resides in between these two descriptions. Countries that spend the entire period of our study under IMF programs have scores which drop in value by about 0.4 to a mean of 3.1. This corresponds to almost a value of four on the original PTS scale which would be described as follows: “Civil and political rights violations have expanded to large numbers of the population. Murders, disappearances, and torture are a common part of life. In spite of its generality, on this level terror affects those who interest themselves in politics or ideas” (Gibney et al. 2007). This description corresponds with our earlier findings that the impacts of IMF programs may well shift countries from being occasional violators of physical integrity rights to becoming frequent violators of these rights. 7 Discussion The findings of this study showed that longer exposure to IMF programs was associated with more frequent use of torture and extra judicial killing by governments and worsened human rights conditions (as indicated by lower Political Terror Scale scores). The other important substantive finding was that recent reforms of IMF lending have not led to better human rights impacts. To the contrary, the Political Terror Scale findings indicated that longer exposure to IMF programs between 1999 and 2003 led to worsened human rights conditions in developing countries. This is the first study to compare the human rights effects of IMF programs before and after some reforms in IMF lending took effect in the late 1990s. These findings support criticisms of the IMF that are common in the case study literature. They are generally consistent with the findings of previous comparative and case study research on the human rights effects of IMF programs (Franklin 1997; Keith and Poe 2000; McClaren 1998; Pion-Berlin 1984). The variation in the effects of programs across the four human rights practices examined illustrates the usefulness of using disaggregated measures of human rights violations as advocated by McCormick and Mitchell (1997) rather than simply focusing on indices of respect for human rights. They are also consistent with our previous findings concerning the separate effects of World Bank programs (Abouharb and Cingranelli 2006) and joint effects of World Bank and IMF programs on human rights practices (Abouharb and Cingranelli 2007). The evidence from all studies has pointed to the negative effects of program lending by either or both the World Bank and IMF on government respect for physical integrity rights in developing countries. Why would governments be more likely to torture and extra judicially kill their citizens as a result of longer exposure to IMF programs, but not be more likely to make them disappear, or imprison them for their political beliefs? It would be a mistake to attach much significance to the type of human right that was protected less. To a large extent various types of physical integrity rights abuse are interchangeable or substitutable tools of repression available to the state. A government can choose to politically imprison and torture its citizens or it can IMF programs and human rights, 1981–2003 69 simply have its security forces kill them. Which tool it uses is probably determined most by what it can afford and what behavior is most beneficial to the government’s maintenance of political control. What is important is that there was a statistically significant increase in two types of physical integrity right abuse associated with longer exposure to IMF programs. Why IMF program lending has any negative human rights consequences is less clear and a more important subject for future research. We argued for some new theoretical linkages between longer exposure to IMF programs and worsened government respect for security rights. These theoretical arguments are as important as the empirical findings. In our earlier work, our theory linked implementation of the common provisions of IMF programs to hardships on the poor, discontent, rebellion, and then repression (Abouharb and Cingranelli 2007). Critics questioned the causal argument, because some previous studies have shown that many IMF programs were not fully implemented. In this work, we argued that, even if the liberalization and privatization provisions of programs were not fully implemented, partial implementation could have been sufficient to cause discontent. Even if no provisions of IMF agreements were implemented, negotiation of programs could have caused greater government repression by reducing the need for governments to raise revenue through taxes on their populations; angering citizens who prefer that their government not cede sovereignty to foreign powers; and/or causing citizen expectations to outpace improvements in economic conditions. These causal arguments are not mutually exclusive. All of them could contribute to our understanding of how and why the existence of IMF programs causes discontent and stimulates repression in developing countries. Our previous research provides evidence that longer exposure to conditional lending is associated with higher levels of political discontent and violent political conflict in developing countries (Abouharb and Cingranelli 2007). But more research will be required to determine which of the causal mechanisms or which combination of causal mechanisms, on average, best explains why. More generally, these findings provide additional evidence that transnational actors including international financial institutions affect the human rights practices of governments. Most previous studies of repression of human rights have focused mainly on state-level characteristics such as a country’s wealth or level of democracy. The results of this study suggest that greater attention should be given to transnational causal forces in developing theories that explain the human rights practices of governments. International organizations such as the IMF, the World Bank, the World Trade Organization, and the International Labor Organization are sources of policies that may affect the human rights practices of governments that choose to participate. Scholars have only begun to explore those impacts. The IMF should be concerned about the human rights consequences of program lending. The IMF is a specialized agency of the United Nations. The United Nations Charter states that the two main goals of that organization are to protect world peace and to ensure that governments’ protect the human rights of their citizens. No organization created by the United Nations should undertake activities that undermine either of the parent organization’s two most important missions. Moreover, previous research has shown that a relatively high level of respect for some human rights, including physical integrity rights, is a necessary precondition 70 M.R. Abouharb, D.L. Cingranelli for economic development (Kaufman 2004). Thus, if IMF programs undermine respect for human rights, they also undermine the effort to promote economic development around the world. Acknowledgements We would like to thank David Clark, Axel Dreher, David Sobek, Randall Stone and James Vreeland for their comments on earlier versions of this manuscript. This research was supported by a grant (No.SES-0318273) from the Political Science Division of the National Science Foundation. References Abouharb, M. R., & Cingranelli, D. (2006). The human rights effects of World Bank structural adjustment lending, 1981–2000. International Studies Quarterly, 50(2), 233–262. Abouharb, M. R., & Cingranelli, D. (2007). Human rights and structural adjustment: The impact of the IMF and World Bank. New York: Cambridge University Press. Achen, C. H. (1986). The statistical analysis of quasi experiments. Berkley: University of California Press. Alexander, N. (2006). The IMF and the World Bank Group: An overview of selected financial and accountability issues. Citizens’ Network on Essential Services. http://www.servicesforall.org. Banks, A. S. (2002). Cross-national time-series data archive [computer file]. Binghamton, NY: Computer Solutions Unlimited. Bird, G., & Willett, T. (2004). IMF conditionality, implementation and the new political economy of ownership. Comparative Economic Studies, 46(3), 423–450. Blackmon, P. (2008). Rethinking Poverty through the Eyes of the International Monetary Fund and the World Bank. International Studies Review, 10, 179–202. Capdevila, G. (2001). IMF not taking into account human rights issues. Global Policy Forum. http://www. globalpolicy.org. Accessed 13 August 2007. Callaghy, T. (1990). Lost between state and market: The politics of economic adjustment in Ghana, Zambia, and Nigeria. In: J. Nelson (Ed.), Economic crisis and policy choice: The politics of adjustment in the third world (pp. 257–319). Princeton: Princeton University Press. Cingranelli, D. L., & Richards, D. L. (1999a). Measuring the level, pattern, and sequence of government respect for physical integrity rights. International Studies Quarterly, 43(2), 407–418. Cingranelli, D. L., & Richards, D. L. (1999b). Respect for human rights after the end of the Cold War. Journal of Peace Research, 36(5), 511–534. Cingranelli, D. L., & Richards, D. L. (2004). The Cingranelli-Richards (CIRI) human rights data project. http://www.humanrightsdata.com. Commonwealth Secretariat (1989). Engendering adjustment for the 1990s: Report of a Commonwealth expert group on women and structural adjustment. London: Commonwealth Secretariat. Collier, D. (1991). The comparative method: Two decades of change. In: D. Rustow, & K. Erickson (Eds.), Comparative political dynamics: Global research perspectives (pp. 7–31). New York: Harper Collins. Collier, P. (2003). Natural Resources, Development and Conflict: Channels of causation and Policy Interventions, World Bank. Cranston, M. (1964). What are human rights?. New York: Basic Books. Davenport, C., & Armstrong II, D. A. (2004). Democracy and the violation of human rights: A statistical analysis from 1976–1996. American Journal of Political Science, 48(3), 538–554. Davies, J. C. (1969). The J-Curve of rising and declining satisfactions as cause of some great revolutions and a contained rebellion. In H. D. Graham, & T. R. Gurr (Eds.), Violence in America: Historical and comparative perspectives (pp. 547–576). Washington, DC, National Commission on the Causes and Prevention of Violence: U.S. Government Printing Office. Davidson, J. (2002). Overcoming resistance to change. Public Management, 84(11), 20–23. Dollar, D., & Svensson, J. (2000). What explains the success or failure of structural adjustment programmes? The Economic Journal, 110, 894–917. Donnelly, J. (2003). Universal human rights in theory and practice. 2nd ed. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press. IMF programs and human rights, 1981–2003 71 Dreher, A. (2006). IMF and economic growth: The effects of programs, loans, and compliance with conditionality. World Development, 34(5), 769–788. Dreher, A., & Jensen, N. M. (2007). Independent actor or agent? An empirical analysis of the impact of US interests on IMF conditions. The Journal of Law and Economics, 50(1), 105–124. Feierabend, I. K., & Feierabend, R. L. (1966). Aggressive behavior within polities 1948–1962: a crossnational study. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 10, 249–271. Franklin, J. (1997). IMF conditionality, threat perception, and political repression: A cross-national analysis. Comparative Political Studies, 30, 576–606. Friedman, M. (1962). Capitalism and Freedom. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Gibney, M., & Dalton, M. (1996). The political terror scale. In Human rights and developing countries (pp. 124–156). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Gibney, M., Cornett, L., & Wood, R. (2007). Political Terror Scale 1976–2006. Data Retrieved from the Political Terror Scale Web site: http://www.politicalterrorscale.org/. Gleditsch, N. P., Wallensteen, P., Eriksson, M., Sollenberg, M., & Håvard, S. (2002). Armed conflict 1946–2001: a new dataset. Journal of Peace Research, 39, (5), 615–637 http://www.prio.no/jpr/ datasets.asp. Gurr, T. R. (1968). Psychological Factors in Civil Violence. World Politics, 20(2), 245–278. Haggard, S. (1995). Democracy and economic growth. In Structural adjustment: Retrospect and prospect (pp. 199–219). Wesport, CT: Praeger Publishers. Hayek, F. A. (1984). Principles and expedience. In The essence of Hayek pp. 299–317. Stanford, CA: Hoover Institution Press. Heckman, J. J. (1988). The microeconomic evaluation of social programs and economic institutions. In Chung-Hua Series of Lectures by Invited Eminent Economists. No. 14. Taipei: The Institute of Economics Academia Sinica. Hensel, P. (2006). ICOW Colonial History Data Set. Version 0.4. Released 13 June 2006 http://garnet. acns.fsu.edu/~phensel/icowdata.html#colonies. Isham, J., Kaufman, D., & Pritchett, L. H. (1997). Civil liberties, democracy, and the performance of government projects. World Bank Economic Review, 11(2), 219–42. Jayarajah, C. A. B., Branson, W. H., Sen, B. (1996). The social dimensions of adjustment: International Monetary Fund experience, 1980–1993. An International Monetary Fund Operations Evaluation Study. Joyce, J. P. (1992). The economic characteristics of IMF program countries. Economics Letters, 38, 237–242. Kaufman, D. (2004). Human rights and governance: The empirical challenge. Paper presented at a conference co-sponsored by the Ethical Globalization Initiative and the NYU Center for Human Rights and Global Justice. New York City: New York University March 1. Keith, L. C., & Poe, S. C. (2000). The United States, the IMF, and human rights. In: D. F. Forsythe (Ed.), The United States and human rights (pp. 273–299). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press. Kienle, E. (2001). A Grand Delusion: Democracy and Economic Reform in Egypt. London: I. B. Tauris. Killick, T. (1996). IMF programmes in developing countries: Design and impact. London: Overseas Development Institute. Landman, T. (2005). Protecting human rights: A comparative study. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press. Leite, S. P. (2001). Human rights and the IMF. Finance and Development, 38(4). http://www.imf.org. McCormick, J., & Mitchell, N. (1997). Human rights violations, umbrella concepts, and empirical analysis. World Politics, 49(4), 510–525. McLaren, L. M. (1998). The effect of IMF austerity programs on human rights violations: An exploratory analysis of Peru, Argentina, and Brazil. Paper presented at the 1998 Meeting of the Midwest Political Science Association. Mitchell, N., & McCormack, J. (1988). Economic and political explanations of human rights violations. World Politics, 40(4), 176–198. Moore, M. (1998). Death without taxes: Democracy, state capacity, and aid dependence in the fourth world. In M. Robinson, & G. White (Eds.), The Democratic Developmental State: Politics and Institutional Design. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Moore, M. (2007). How Does Taxation Affect the Quality of Governance? Working Paper 280. Institute of Development Studies. University of Sussex: UK. Pettifor, A. (2001). Global economic justice: Human rights for debtor nations. Journal of Human Development, 2(1), 47–51. Pion-Berlin, D. (1983). Political repression and economic doctrines. Comparative Political Studies, 16, 37–66. 72 M.R. Abouharb, D.L. Cingranelli Pion-Berlin, D. (1984). The political economy of state repression in Argentina. In M. Stohl, & G. A. Lopez (Eds.), The state as terrorist: The dynamics of governmental violence and repression (pp. 99– 123). Westport, CT: Greenwood. Pion-Berlin, D. (1989). The ideology of state terror: Economic doctrine and political repression in Argentina and Peru. Boulder, CO: Lynne Rienner. Pion-Berlin, D. (1991). Between confrontation and accommodation: Military and government policy in democratic Argentina. Journal of Latin American Studies, 23, 543–566. Poe, S. C., & Tate, C. N. (1994). Repression of physical integrity in the 1980s: a global analysis. American Political Science Review, 88(4), 853–872. Poe, S. C., Tate, C. N., & Keith, L. C. (1999). Repression of the human right to physical integrity revisited: A global cross-national study covering the years 1976–1993. International Studies Quarterly, 43, 291–313. Policzer, P. (2004). How organizations shape human rights violations. In: S. C. Carey, & S. C. Poe (Eds.), Understanding human rights violations (pp. 221–238). Burlington, VT: Ashgate. Przeworski, A., & Vreeland, J. R. (2000). The effects of IMF programs on economic growth. The Journal of Development Economics, 62, 385–421. Sen, A. (1999). Development as freedom. New York: Anchor Books. Stiglitz, J. (2002). Globalization and its discontents. New York: WW Norton. Stone, R. (2004). The political economy of IMF lending in Africa. American Political Science Review, 98 (4), 577–592. Structural Adjustment Participatory Review International Network (SAPRIN) (2004). Structural adjustment: The SAPRI report. London: Zed Books. Van de Walle, N. (2001). African economies and the politics of permanent crisis, 1979–1999. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Vreeland, J. R. (2002). The effect of IMF programs on labor. World Development, 30(1), 21–139. Vreeland, J. R. (2003). The IMF and economic development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. World Bank (1998). Development and human rights: The role of the world bank. Washington, D.C.: World Bank. World Bank (2005). Human rights. FAQs. http://web.worldbank.org/