Infant learning of phonological alternations is biased by phonetic similarity Megha b Sundara , Yun Jung a. University College London Introduction b Kim & Adam b Chong b. UCLA Study 1 (White & Sundara, 2014) Question: • Infants are excellent at statistical learning: • Discriminating speech sounds (Anderson et al., Does 12-month-olds’ generalization of newly learned alternations depend on phonetic similarity? 2003; Maye et al., 2002) • Phonotactics (Chambers et al., 2003) • Word segmentation (Saffran et al., 1996) 2008) • Evidence mounting that learners are biased by phonetic similarity – they prefer alternations between phonetically similar sounds. • Typology (Steriade, 2001; Hayes & J. White, sub.) • Adult artificial language studies (Skoruppa et al., 2011; J. White, 2014) • Computational modeling (Peperkamp et al., 2006; Wilson, 2006; J. White, 2013) 40 monolingual English-learning 12month-olds at UCLA. Procedure: • Virtually no work with infant learners!! Research questions: 1. Are 12-month-olds biased by phonetic similarity when generalizing newly learned alternations in an artificial language? (Study 1) 2. Are 12-month-olds biased by similarity when learning alternations in their first language? (Study 2) b Visual Fixation Procedure rom poli na poli rom voli na voli rom timu rom zimu rom timu na zimu na timu na zimu rom timu na zimu Test pairs: buni/vuni, bagu/vagu, dilu/zilu, dari/zari CONTROL condition – [b ~ v] or [d ~ z] Labials Alternating Coronals Alternating rom boli rom boli na voli na voli rom boli na boli Exposure: Should not generalize ≠ v d v b Should not generalize z p Should not generalize v = z t No difference in looking times Results: BIAS condition - [p ~ v] or [t ~ z] Labials Alternating Coronals Alternating na voli na voli v Difference in looking times 2. Test: Novel pairs of CVCV words (e.g. buni…vuni…) rom poli rom poli p Should generalize 1. Familiarization: Phrases providing evidence for an alternation. • ‘Function’ element (na or rom) + CVCV ‘content’ word (e.g. rom poli…na timu…) • 16 phrases, repeated twice per trial • 3 trials (45s each, 135s total exposure) Control condition BIAS condition Exposure: Participants: • Tracking statistics likely plays a role in learning phonological alternations as well. • Computational work (Peperkamp et al., 2006) • Experimental work with infants (K. White et al., Predictions (if biased by similarity): rom voli na voli Alternating place Contrastive place 12 Mean looking time (s) James a White , * 10 8 6 4 2 0 BIAS condition CONTROL condition à 12-month-olds generalized the alternations to new pairs of sounds that were more similar, but not to ones that were less similar. à Similar results found with adult learners in a previous study. (J. White, 2014) rom dimu rom zimu rom dimu na zimu na dimu na zimu rom dimu na zimu Test pairs: puni/vuni, pagu/vagu, tilu/zilu, tari/zari Study 2 (Sundara, Kim, White, & Chong, submitted) Phonetic similarity: Do 12-month-olds depend solely on input statistics when learning phonological alternations in their first language, or is this learning biased by phonetic similarity? Tapping in American English: /t/ and /d/ (partially) neutralized to [ɾ] in certain contexts: /t/ /d/ /pæd/ [ˈpæɾɪŋ] –ting/–ding words (tapping contexts only) 146 598 Procedure: Headturn Preference Procedure (HPP) • Brent corpus (Brent & Siskind, 2001) • 9 infant-mother dyads chosen (infant ages 0;9–2;2). • All words ending in –ting/–ding extracted. 44 Based solely on input statistics: [t ~ ɾ] learned first. Bias based on phonetic similarity: [d ~ ɾ] learned first. 48 monolingual English-learning 12-month-olds (24 in each exp.) Corpus search of infant-directed speech: 15 Predictions: Participants: /pæt/ [ɾ] / V__V[–stress] • [d] and [ɾ] more similar than [t] and [ɾ] (Herd et al., 2010) –ting –ding 1. Familiarization: 2 passages (45s to each) • E.g. Cutting papers with scissors is a lot of fun… Mommy is really good at cutting tofu… • Target word appeared 6 times/passage. • 2 groups (cutting/meeting or patting/shooting, counterbalanced) 2. Test: 4 wordlists, 2 familiar and 2 novel (4 trials x 2 blocks). Types Tokens • Exp. 1: cut…cut…cut… (also: meet, pat, shoot) à Infants hear far more –ting words than –ding words in IDS. • Exp. 2: cud…cud…cud… (also, meed, pad, shood) General Discussion • Input statistics are not sufficient for explaining infants’ learning and generalization of phonological alternations. • Instead, this learning is biased by phonetic similarity. • Previous experimental work with adults (J. White, 2014) and computational modeling (J. White, 2013) suggests that this bias is a substantive bias favouring alternation between perceptually similar sounds (i.e. a P-map bias, Steriade 2001). • Future infant work will look more into the type of bias (e.g. perceptual vs. features). • Prediction: alternations between similar sounds learned first, all else being equal. Acknowledgements We would like to thank Bruce Hayes, Sharon Peperkamp, and Kie Zuraw for helpful discussion; to Chad Vicenik, Robyn Orfitelli, and Anya Mancillas for help in recruiting and testing infants; and to Jeffrey Lamontagne for help with the corpus analysis in study 2. This research was funded by a COR Faculty Research Grant and NSF BCS-0951639 to MS, and a University of Ottawa seed grant to JW. Results: 14 Mean looking time (s) Question: * 12 10 Familiar Novel 8 6 4 2 0 Exp. 1 [t ~ ɾ] Exp. 2 [d ~ ɾ] à 12-month-olds know [d ~ ɾ], but not [t ~ ɾ], despite greater evidence for [t ~ ɾ] in the input. à Exp. 3: Confirmed that 12-month-olds can discriminate [d] and [ɾ] perceptually. à Note: if [t ~ ɾ] learned first, this would be a saltatory alternation: t • • • d ɾ Particularly egregious P-map violation. Poses theoretical difficulties. (Hayes & J. White, sub.) Disfavoured by learners (J. White, 2014). References Anderson, J. L., Morgan, J. L., & White, K. S. (2003). A statistical basis for speech sound discrimination. Language and Speech, 46(2–3), 155–182. Brent, M. & Siskind, J. (2001). The role of exposure to isolated words in early vocabulary development. Cognition, 81(2), 31-44. Chambers, K. E., Onishi, K. H., & Fisher, C. (2003). Infants learn phonotactic regularities from brief auditory experience. Cognition, 87(2), 69–77. Hayes, B. & White, J. (submitted). Saltation and the P-map. Ms. Herd, W., Jongman, A. & Sereno, J. (2010). An acoustic and perceptual analysis of /t/ and /d/ flaps in American English. Journal of Phonetics, 38(4), 504-516. Maye, J., Werker, J F., & Gerken, L. (2002). Infant sensitivity to distributional information can affect phonetic discrimination. Cognition, 82(3), B101– B111. Peperkamp, S., Le Calvez, R., Nadal, J.-P., & Dupoux, E. (2006a). The acquisition of allophonic rules: Statistical learning with linguistic constraints. Cognition, 101, B31–B41. Saffran, J. R., Aslin, R. N., & Newport, E. L. (1996). Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science, 274, 1926–1928. Skoruppa, K., Lambrechts, A., & Peperkamp, S. (2011). The role of phonetic distance in the acquisition of phonological alternations. In S. Lima, K. Mullin, & B. Smith (Eds.), Proceedings of the 39th North Eastern Linguistics Conference (pp. 717–729). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press. Steriade, D. (2001). The phonology of perceptibility effects: the P-map and its consequences for constraint organization. Ms. Sundara, M., Kim, Y., White, J., & Chong, A. There is no pat in patting: Acquisition of phonological alternations by English-learning 12-month-olds. Ms. White, J. (2013). Bias in Phonological Learning: Evidence from Saltation. Ph.D. dissertation, UCLA. White, J. (2014). Evidence for a learning bias against saltatory phonological alternations. Cognition, 130(1), 96-115. White, J. & Sundara, M. (2014). Biased generalization of newly learned phonological alternations by 12-month-old infants. Cognition, 133(1), 85–90. White, K., Peperkamp, S., Kirk, C., & Morgan, J. (2008). Rapid acquisition of phonological alternations by infants. Cognition, 107, 238–265. Wilson, C. (2006). Learning phonology with substantive bias: An experimental and computational study of velar palatalization. Cognitive Science, 30, 945–982. Workshop on Learning Biases in Natural and Artificial Language Acquisition 2014 Annual Meeting of the LAGB, Oxford