Volume (9), Issue (4), October, 2015 ISSN 2218 - 6506 Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies JEPS Peer Reviewed Journal Sultan Qaboos University Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University 2015 Journal of Educational & Psychological Studies Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies (JEPS) is a refereed periodical issued by Sultan Qaboos University. It is concerned with publishing original research that has originality of thought, clarity of methodology, and accurateness of documentation, in curricula and teaching methods, educational psychology and educational administration. The journal also publishes reports about conferences and symposia, reviews of recently published educational and psychological books and synopses of university dissertations in different fields of education. JEPS is published in Arabic and/or in English. Academic Publication Board Prof. Amer Ali Al-Rawas, Chairman Prof. Maher M. Abu Hilal Prof. Boualem Belkacemi Dr. Michel R. Claereboudt Editor-in-Chief Prof. Maher M. Abu Hilal Prof. Mohammad S. Khan Prof. Lamk M. Al Lamki Mr. Ali Al-Hadhrami Prof. Mohamed Khaoua Prof. Khaled Day Editorial Board Editor Prof. Abdulqawi S. Al-Zubaidi AssociateEditors Prof. Salha A. Essan Prof. Ali M. Kadhim Dr. Ali S. Al Mosawi Dr. Ali M. Ibrahim Dr. Wajeha T. Al Ani Dr. Mohamed A. Osman Dr. Sulyman A. Al Shuaili Dr. Hussain A. Al Kharusi Mrs. Intisar Nasser (Editorial Assistant) Prof. Abdullah K. Ambusadidi Dr. Ahmad H. Al Rabaani Dr. Mohamed A. Al Ayasra International Advisory Board Prof. Dennis McInerney, Hong Kong Institute of Education, China Prof. Ahmad Audeh, Yarmouk University, Jordan Prof. Fahed Al Dulaim, King Saud University, Saudi Arabia Prof. Shaker A. Sulaiman, Egyptian Academy of Arts, Egypt Prof. Vivian Khamis, American University, Beirut Prof. SalhaSunqur, Damascus University, Syria Prof. Ahmad Bustan, Kuwait University, Kuwait Prof. AbduRahman Al-Tariri, King Saud University, Saudi Arabia Prof. Mohamed Ally, Athabasca University, Canada Volume 9, No. 4, October, 2015 Copyright © 2015by Sultan Qaboos University Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies. All rights reserved. The Journal is not responsible for opinions printed in its publications; they represent the views of the individuals to whom they are credited and are not binding to the Journal. Annual subscriptions are payable in advance in Omani Riyals or US Dollars. Oman R.O. 15/-. Outside Oman US $50. All correspondence should be addressed to the Journal office at Sultan Qaboos University, College of Education, P.O. Box. 32, Muscat 123, Sultanate of Oman- Tel.: (00968) 24143868 Fax: (00968) 24143817- Email: jesedu@squ.edu.om Printed at Sultan Qaboos University Press. Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University 2015 Journal of Educational & Psychological Studies, SQU INSTRUCTIONS TO AUTHOR REVIEWING: Submitted manuscripts are sent to specialized referees for review. A manuscript will be accepted after all revisions are made as requested by referees. Authors are responsible for the content of their manuscripts and ideas and opinions in published manuscripts are of the authors and not the editorial board or SQU. Each manuscript is initially reviewed by the editorial board to decide its credibility and adherence to the journal style. The editorial board reserves the right to reject a manuscript without providing a justification. Manuscripts that don’t adhere to the following instructions will be automatically returned to authors. 1. An electronic copy of the manuscript using Microsoft Word can be submitted via email to: jesedu@squ.edu.om Manuscripts in Arabic must use a (12) font size Simplified Arabic (SA) with (2) cm top, bottom and left margins and a (3) cm right margin with 1.5 line spacing. Manuscripts in English must use a (12) font size Times New Roman with (2) cm top, bottom and right margins and a (3) cm left margin. 2. The manuscript should not exceed 8000 words including the graphs, references, and tables. Number of tables in a manuscript should not exceed 7 tables. 3. A manuscript should not have been published or submitted to another journal; the authors attest that they don’t submit their manuscript to other journals before a decision is made for their manuscript. Authors should confirm that they have read the instructions and have adhered to them. 4. The manuscript should not be part of a published book. 5. Authors should not publish their manuscripts in any other journal once it is accepted by JEPS unless a written consent is issued by the editor-in –chief. 6. The journal retains the copyright. 7. All manuscripts must include Arabic and English abstracts containing a maximum of 200 words each along with keywords which will help readers to search through related databases. 8. All references must be documented throughout the research and in the bibliography using the APA (The American Psychological Association) Style. Arabic references come first followed by English references. Manuscripts that don't follow APA format will be returned to the authors directly. Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University 2015 9. JEPS reserves the right to ask the author(s) to omit, reformulate or re-word his/her manuscript or any part thereof in a manner that conforms to the publication policy. 10. Authors are to be informed about the results within three to six months. 11. Priorities and order of publication are subject to technical considerations. JEPS publishes: 1. Original empirical research, 2. Qualitative research, 3. Book reviews, 4. Critical review studies. References should be presented in accordance with the following APA examples: A- BOOK )ONE AUTHOR(: Ellis, R. (2003). Task-based language learning & teaching. New York: Oxford University Press. In-Text: Ellis (2003) or (Ellis, 2003) B- BOOK )TWO OR MORE AUTHORS( Strunk, W., Jr. & White, E.B. (1979). The elements of style (3rd Ed.). New York: Macmillan. In-Text: (two authors): Strunk and White (1979)or (Strunk & White, 1979) In-Text (three or more authors): Strunk, White, & Smith (1979)or (Strunk, White & Smith, 1979) In-Text (subsequent references): Strunk et al. (1979) or (Strunk et al., 1979) C- CHAPTERS IN AN EDITED VOLUME: Abu Hilal, M. (2008). Motivation, attributions of academic experiences & achievement among Arab students within a socio-cultural context. In T. O. Seng, D. M. McInerney, A. D. Liem, & T. Ai-Girl (Eds.) Research in Multicultural Education & International Perspectives (Vol. 7, pp. 217-244). Greenwich, CT: Information Age Publishing. In text: Abu-Hilal (2008) or (Abu-Hilal, 2008) D- CONFERENCE PROCEEDINGS: Pica, T., Washburn, G., Evans, B., & Jo, V. (1998). Negative feedback in content-based second language classroom interaction: How does it contribute to second language learning? Paper presented at the Annual Pacific Second Language Research Forum, Tokyo, Japan. E- UNPUBLISHED DISSERTATIONS: Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University 2015 Boyd-Kletzander, R. (2000). Student responsibility in a whole language classroom. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Pennsylvania. F- JOURNAL ARTICLE )ONE AUTHOR( Abu-Hilal, M. (2000) A structural model of attitudes toward school subjects, academic aspiration & achievement. Educational Psychology, 20, 75-84. In-Text: Abu-Hilal (2000) or (Abu-Hilal, 2000) G- JOURNAL ARTICLE )TWO AUTHORS( Atkinson, R.C., &Shiffrin, R.M. (1971). The control of short-term memory. Scientific American, 225, 82-90. In-Text (two authors): Atkinson and Shiffrin (1971) or (Atkinson &Shiffrin, 1971). In-Text (three to five authors): Smith, Zapplla, Rosen, Gustman& Rock (1194), or (Smith, Zappella, Rosen, Gustman, & Rock, 1994). In-Text (six or more authors): or In-Text (subsequent references): Smith et al. (1994) or (Smith et al., 1994). H- MAGAZINE ARTICLE )ONE AUTHOR( Kuel, E. R. (2000, November 10). Neuroscience: Breaking down scientific barriers to the study of brain & mind. Science, 290, 1113-1120. In-Text: Kuel(2000) or (Kuel, 2000). I- NEWSPAPER ARTICLE ) NO AUTHOR( Amazing Amazon region. (1989, January 12). New York Times, p. D11. In-Text: "Amazing Amazon Region," (1989). J- SPECIFIC INTERNET SITE Electronic reference formats recommended by the American Psychological Association. (2000, October 12). Retrieved January 3, 2001 from http://www.apa.org/journals/jwebref.html In-Text: American Psychological Association [APA](2000) In-Text (subsequent references): APA (2000) or (APA, 2000). K- ELECTRONIC ARTICLE )JOURNAL ARTICLE FOUND IN A DATABASE( Jacobson, J.W., Mulick, J.A., & Schwartz, A.A. (1995). A history of facilitated communication: Science, pseudoscience, &antiscience. American Psychologist, 50, 750-765. Retrieved January 12, 2001, from PsycARTICLES database. In text: Jacobson, Mulickand Schwartz (1995) or (Jacobson, Mulick, & Schwartz, 1995); Jacobson, et al. (1995), or (Jacobson et al., 1995) in subsequent references. Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University 2015 TABLE OF CONTENTS Title Omani Stakeholders’ Preferences for Educational Placement of Students with Disabilities Author Jalal H. Hussien Madison Metropolitan School District, Wisconsin, USA Page 628-644 Abdelhafez Q. Al-Shayeb Al al-Bayt University, Jordan Ibrahim Al-Qaryouti Sultan Qaboos University, Sultanate of Oman The Relation between Omani Students' Perceptions of the Writing Strategies and their Writing Performance Juma B. Busaidi & Dina A. Al-Jamal Spelling Errors of Omani EFL Students Sheikha A. Al-Bereiki Yarmouk University, Jordan Ministry of Education, Sultanate of Oman 645-659 660-676 Abdo M. Al-Mekhlafi Sultan Qaboos University, Sultanate of Oman Omani Students’ Application of the Second Standard for Technology Coaches in Internship Program Ahmed Y. Abdelraheem & Talal S. Amir Academic Delay of Gratification and its Relationship to Motivational Determinants, Academic Achievement, and Study Hours among Omani High School Students: A Path Analysis Sabry M. Abd-El-Fattah & Sahar El Shourbagi Arabic in Foreign Language Programmes: Difficulties and Challenges Fatma Y. Al-Busaidi Relationship between Social Anxiety and Parental Authority among College of Education Students’ at SQU Hilal Z. Al- Nabhani & Abdulhameed S. Hassan Mindfulness of Career Counselors within the Omanis Context Muna A. Al-Bahrani& Bakkar S. Bakkar Sultan Qaboos University, Sultanate of Oman 677-690 691-700 Sultan Qaboos University, Sultanate of Oman Sultan Qaboos University, Sultanate of Oman 701-717 718-729 Sultan Qaboos University, Sultanate of Oman Sultan Qaboos University, Sultanate of Oman 730-737 Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Pages 628-644) Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 Omani Stakeholders’ Preferences for Educational Placement of Students with Disabilities Jalal H. Hussien* Madison Metropolitan School District, Wisconsin, USA Abdelhafez Q. Al-Shayeb Ibrahim Al-Qaryouti Al al-Bayt University, Jordan Sultan Qaboos University, Sultanate of Oman ___________________________________________ Received: 31/3/2015 Revised: 5/5/2015 _____________________________________________ Accepted: 18/5/2015 Abstract: The purpose of the current study was to survey the stakeholders’ opinions of the best educational placement setting for students with disabilities in the Sultanate of Oman. Two thousand four hundred and thirty stakeholders participated in this study. The findings provided evidence that the majority of the Omani stakeholders prefer educating students with disabilities in regular schools over separate facilities. In addition, the stakeholders’ preference on the continuum of placement options varied significantly. The results also suggested that the stakeholders’ preference for educating students in the regular school setting versus a separate facility varied according to the type of disability. Moreover, a significant association between the stakeholders’ role and their preference of educational placement setting for students with disabilities was found. However, the findings revealed that there was no significant relationship between the stakeholders’ gender and their educational settings preference. Finally, educational services for children with disabilities in Oman were discussed and suggestions were provided to improve these services. Keywords: Educational placement, inclusive classrooms, students with disabilities, Sultanate of Oman. املكان الرتبوي املفضّل لتعليه الطلبة ذوي اإلعاقة من وجوة نظر أصخاب العالقة يف سلطنة عمان إبراهيه القريوتي عبد احلافظ قاسه الشايب *جالل حاج حسني سلطنة عنان،جامعة السلطان قابوس األردن،جامعة آل البيت ، والية وسكانسن،منطقة مادسن التعلينية الواليات املتخدة االمريكية _____________________________________________ ٍدفت الدزاسة احلالًة إىل استطالع آزاء ذوٍ العالقة حىل املهاٌ الرتبىٍ املفضّل لديَه لتعلًه الطلبة:مستخلص وقد. مصازنًا ومصازنة مً ذوٍ العالقة2430 حًث اشتنلت عًّية الدزاسة على،ٌذوٍ اإلعاقة يف سلطية عُنا ٌأشازت اليتائج إىل أٌ معظه املصازنني يف الدزاسة مً ذوٍ العالقة يعتكدوٌ أٌ مدازس التعلًه العاو ٌٍ املها وأسفست اليتائج عً وجىد فسوم جىٍسية بني آزاء. األفضل لتعلًه الطلبة ذوٍ اإلعاقة مكازىة يف البًئاتِ الرتبىيَّة وتبًًّ أيضاً أٌ تفضًل املصازنني ملدازس التعلًه العاو مكابل.املصازنني فًنا يتعلل ب البًئاتِ الرتبىيَّة اليت يفضلىىَا تبًًّ وجىد عالقة بني، باإلضافة إىل ذلو.البًئاتِ الرتبىيَّة التعلًه الطلبة ذوٍ اإلعاقة خيتلف باختالف ىىع اإلعاقة بًينا مل تسفس اليتائج عً وجىد،متغًّس دوز املصازنني وآزائَه يف املهاٌ الرتبىٍ املفضّل لتعلًه الطلبة ذوٍ اإلعاقة واىتَت الدزاسة.عالقة بني متغًّس جيس املصازنني وآزائَه يف املهاٌ الرتبىٍ املفضّل لتعلًه الطلبة ذوٍ اإلعاقة مبياقصة اخلدمات الرتبىية املكدّمة للطلبة ذوٍ اإلعاقة مع عدد مً التىصًات املتعلكة بتحسني ٍره اخلدمات يف سلطية .ٌعُنا .ٌ سلطية عُنا،ذوٍ اإلعاقة الطلبة، الصفىف الصاملة،ٍ املهاٌ الرتبى:الهلنات املفتاحًة 628 Omani Stakeholders’ Preferences for Educational Placement of Students with Disabilities Jalal Hussien & et al. Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 specifically the current study aimed to answer the following questions: Special education services are relatively new in the Sultanate of Oman. In 2008, the Sultanate signed the International Agreement on the Rights of Persons with Disabilities for Education in an Inclusive Educational Setting (Ministry of Education, 2008). Inclusive education refers to teaching students with disabilities in their neighborhood school within the regular classroom with their peers without disabilities (Rafferty, Boettcher, & Griffin, 2001). The Ministry of Education provides educational services for students with disabilities in special education classes in many public schools as well as in special education schools (Al-Balushi, Al-Badi, & Ali, 2011; Weber, 2012). Oman has been in the process of reforming its educational system and significant efforts have been made towards achieving this goal (Haj Hussien & Al-Qaryouti, 2014). The Ministry of Education in Oman aims to create inclusive schools (Ministry of Education, 2008). The previous literature consistently showed that the attitudes of the principals (Balboni & Pedrabissi, 2000; Dyson, Howes, & Roberts, 2004; Kugelmass & Ainscow,2004; Semmel, 1986; Villa, Thousand, Meyers, & Navin, 1996), teachers (Ahsan, Sharma & Deppeler, 2012; Avramidis, Balyliss, & Burden, 2000; Avramidis & Norwich, 2002; Emam & Hassan, 2011; Haj Hussien & Al-Qaryouti, 2014; Sari, Celikoz, & Secer, 2009), parents (Gilmore, Campbell, & Cuskelly, 2003; Grove & Fisher, 1999; Tafa & Manolitsis, 2003), and students (Gannon & McGilloway, 2009; Georgiadi, Kalyva, Kourkoutas, & Tsakiris, 2012; Haj Hussien & Al-Qaryouti, 2015; Miller, Garriott, & Mershon, 2005; Morin, Crocker, Beaulieu-Bergeron, & Caron, 2013; Panagiotou et al., 2008; Papaioannou, Evaggelinou, & Block, 2014; Patel & Rose, 2014) play a major role in developing and implementing inclusive education successfully. 1. Is the stakeholders’ gender associated with their preference of educational placement setting for students with disabilities? 2. Is the stakeholders’ role associated with their preference of educational placement setting for students with disabilities? 3. What is the pattern of the stakeholders’ preference of educational placement settings for students with disabilities? 4. Do the differences between percentages of each category of stakeholders’ responses on each pair of educational placement for students with disabilities differ significantly? 5. What is the stakeholders’ educational placement preference (separate facilities vs. regular school) for students with disabilities? 6. What is the stakeholders’ educational placement preference (separate facilities vs. regular school) for each type of disability? 7. Does the stakeholders’ educational placement preference for students with disabilities differ significantly according to the student’s type of disability? Context of the study The Ministry of Education in Oman aims to create inclusive schools (Ministry of Education, 2008). Currently a total of 5,325 individuals with disabilities are receiving services in Oman. These services are provided by the Ministry of Education, and the Ministry of Social Development. The Ministry of Education provides educational services in special education classes in 155 public schools for a total of 1,565 students; 1,262 students with intellectual disabilities and 303 students with hearing impairments (Ministry of Education, 2015). The number of students with disabilities enrolled in special education classes in public school has increased steadily over the last ten school years. Teachers, parents, students, and administrators are the critical stakeholders and play a vital role in creating inclusive schools; considering their opinions is critical to the success of systematic school reform (Hunt & McDonnell, 2007; Wang, 2009). The purpose of the current study was to survey the Omani stakeholders’ opinions of the best educational placement for students with disabilities. More 629 2015 Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Vol. 9 Issue 4 Oct.) Variables students in the three special education schools; 192 students enrolled in the Hope School for Students with Hearing Disability, 213 students enrolled in the School of Intellectual Disability, and 137 students enrolled in Omar Ben Alkatab for Students with Visual Impairments (Ministry of Education, 2015). Stakeholders: This variable involved regular teachers, special education teachers, social workers, principals, parents of students with disabilities, parents of students without disabilities, and students. Gender: This variable involved males and females. The Ministry of Social Development provides services for a total of 3,218 individuals with disabilities; among them 2,180 individuals are enrolled in 25 governmental centers, 605 individuals with disabilities are enrolled in 13 private centers, and 433 individuals with disabilities are enrolled in 10 nonprofit associations’ centers (Ministry of Social Development, 2015). Educational placement: This variable involved separate facilities, self-contained class, resource room, and general education classroom. Types of disabilities: This variable involved intellectual disability, autism, emotional and behavioral disability, hearing impairment, other health impairment, specific learning disability, vision impairment, speech and language disability, & physical disability. Method Participants A total of 3,000 questionnaires were sent to the administrators of the public schools in Sultanate Oman in various provinces; 2,430 stakeholders (1,211 males & 1,219 females), representing all grade levels, volunteered to complete the questionnaires. The sample involved 703 regular teachers (359 males & 344 females), 225 special education teachers (36 males & 189 females), 160 social workers (64 males & 96 females), 318 principals (169 males & 149 females), 234 parents of students with disabilities (143 males & 91 females), 386 parents of students without disabilities (244 males & 142 females), and 404 students (196 males & 208 females). Table 1 shows the distribution of the participants according to province by stakeholders. Province Muscat Al Batinah Janoob Al Batinah Shamal Al Dakhiliyah Ash Sharqiyah Janoob Ash Sharqiyah Shamal Al Burimi Al Dhirah Dhofar Al Wusta Musandam Total Stakeholder’s Opinion of the Best Placement for Students with Disabilities Questionnaire: The authors developed this questionnaire to identify the stakeholders’ opinions of the best educational placement for students with disabilities. This questionnaire developed by the authors was based on the special education services continuum from the most segregated environment to the most inclusive. A continuum of placement options available for students with disabilities is necessary to meet the needs of all special education students (Kauffman, Bantz, & McCullough, 2002). The placements from the most segregated to the most inclusive are the following: a) separate facilities, b) self-contained class, c) resource room, and d) general education. Table 1 Participants’ distribution according to province by stakeholders Stakeholders Regular Special Social Principal Parents of Parents of stuteacher education worker students dents without teacher with disadisabilities bilities 404 179 33 48 168 76 56 12 7 30 15 36 78 8 8 48 18 78 21 4 3 15 17 31 5 9 1 10 5 6 Student Total 93 46 85 31 5 1001 202 323 122 41 26 4 7 28 2 28 25 120 16 12 59 11 15 703 1 3 4 1 0 225 8 7 61 10 15 160 16 12 83 8 20 318 3 4 0 2 0 234 14 23 60 13 21 386 12 15 60 14 18 404 70 76 327 59 89 2430 630 Omani Stakeholders’ Preferences for Educational Placement of Students with Disabilities Jalal Hussien & et al. The questionnaire included the following ten questions: Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 Procedure: The purpose of the study was explained to the participants with the emphasis on the importance of them expressing their personal opinion while considering that there are no right or wrong responses. Finally, the participants were asked to select the best educational placement among the four placements options by marking their selection with a tick (). 1. In your opinion what is the best educational placement for students with intellectual disabilities? 2. In your opinion what is the best educational placement for students with autism? 3. In your opinion what is the best educational placement for students with emotional and behavioral disabilities? Results 6. In your opinion what is the best educational placement for students with specific learning disabilities? The purpose of the current study was to survey the stakeholders’ opinions of the best educational placement setting for students with disabilities in the Sultanate of Oman. Two thousand four hundred and thirty stakeholders participated in the study. Frequencies and percentages, a chi-square test of independence and a chi-square test of goodness of fit were performed to answer the questions of the study. Each research question and its findings are presented below. 7. In your opinion what is the best educational placement for students with a visual impairment? Question 1: What is the pattern of the stakeholders’ preference of educational placement settings for students with disabilities? 8. In your opinion what is the best educational placement for students with speech and language disabilities? The frequencies and percentages of stakeholders’ responses on educational placement preference were calculated and are presented in Table 3. In addition, a chi-square test of goodness of fit was performed to determine whether the four educational settings (separate facilities, self-contained class, resource room, and general education) for students with disabilities were equally selected by the stakeholders. The findings showed that the stakeholders’ selection of the four educational placement settings for students with disabilities were not equally distributed in the population, χ2 (3, 2240) = 541.95, p < 0.000. The results shown in Table 2 indicate that 43.3%, 29%, 12%, and 15.8% of the stakeholders selected separate setting, selfcontained, resource room, and general education, respectively, as the best educational settings for educating students with disabilities. 4. In your opinion what is the best educational placement for students with a hearing impairment? 5. In your opinion what is the best educational placement for students with other health impairments? 9. In your opinion what is the best educational placement for students with physical disabilities? 10. In your opinion what is the best educational placement for students with disabilities? Each question was followed by the four educational placements: ( ) Full-time outside the regular school ( ) Full-time in special education classrooms in the regular school ( ) Part-time in the regular classroom with part-time in a resource room ( ) Full-time in the regular classroom with other support services provided there The questionnaire was examined by 4 experts, comprising of two experts in measurement and psychometric theory and two experts in special education. The four experts agreed that the questionnaire was written in clear and precise language as well as measuring what it intended to measure. 631 Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Vol. 9 Issue 4 Oct.) Gender Male Female Total Total Table 2 Educational placement preference by the role of the stakeholder and gender Educational Setting Preference Stakeholder Descriptive measure Separate SelfResource facilities contained room class Regular Teacher Count 149 108 34 % within Source 46.4% 33.6% 10.6% Special EducaCount 18 7 9 tion Teacher % within Source 50.0% 19.4% 25.0% Social Worker Count 17 22 3 % within Source 29.8% 38.6% 5.3% Principal Count 63 40 24 % within Source 42.0% 26.7% 16.0% Parents of stuCount 45 28 19 dents with dis% within Source 34.9% 21.7% 14.7% abilities Parents of stuCount 98 53 40 dents without % within Source 42.4% 22.9% 17.3% disabilities Student Count 99 41 17 % within Source 51.0% 21.1% 8.8% Total Count 489 299 146 % within Source 43.7% 26.7% 13.1% Regular Teacher Count 154 102 28 % within Source 49.8% 33.0% 9.1% Special EducaCount 76 45 25 tion Teacher % within Source 40.4% 23.9% 13.3% Social Worker Count 30 52 5 % within Source 33.3% 57.8% 5.6% Principal Count 50 54 12 % within Source 35.5% 38.3% 8.5% Parents of stuCount 32 25 7 dents with dis% within Source 40.5% 31.6% 8.9% abilities Parents of stuCount 56 31 15 dents without % within Source 43.1% 23.8% 11.5% disabilities Student Count 82 42 30 % within Source 44.3% 22.7% 16.2% Total Count 480 351 122 % within Source 42.8% 31.3% 10.9% Regular Teacher Count 303 210 62 % within Source 48.1% 33.3% 9.8% Special EducaCount 94 52 34 tion Teacher % within Source 42.0% 23.2% 15.2% Social Worker Count 47 74 8 % within Source 32.0% 50.3% 5.4% Principal Count 113 94 36 % within Source 38.8% 32.3% 12.4% Parents of stuCount 77 53 26 dents with dis% within Source 37.0% 25.5% 12.5% abilities Parents of stuCount 154 84 55 dents without % within Source 42.7% 23.3% 15.2% disabilities Student Count 181 83 47 % within Source 47.8% 21.9% 12.4% Count 969 650 268 % within Source 43.3% 29.0% 12.0% 632 2015 General education Total 30 9.3% 2 5.6% 15 26.3% 23 15.3% 37 28.7% 321 100.0% 36 100.0% 57 100.0% 150 100.0% 129 100.0% 40 17.3% 231 100.0% 37 19.1% 184 16.5% 25 8.1% 42 22.3% 3 3.3% 25 17.7% 15 19.0% 194 100.0% 1118 100.0% 309 100.0% 188 100.0% 90 100.0% 141 100.0% 79 100.0% 28 21.5% 130 100.0% 31 16.8% 169 15.1% 55 8.7% 44 19.6% 18 12.2% 48 16.5% 52 25.0% 185 100.0% 1122 100.0% 630 100.0% 224 100.0% 147 100.0% 291 100.0% 208 100.0% 68 18.8% 361 100.0% 68 17.9% 353 15.8% 379 100.0% 2240 100.0% Omani Stakeholders’ Preferences for Educational Placement of Students with Disabilities Jalal Hussien & et al. Question 2: Is the stakeholders’ gender associated with their preference of educational placement setting for students with disabilities? Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 and their preference of educational placement setting (separate facilities, self-contained class, resource room, and general education) for students with disabilities. A significant association between stakeholders’ role and their preference of educational placement setting for students with disabilities was revealed, χ2 (18, 2240) = 105.85, p < .0001. The chi-square test of independence was performed to examine the association between the stakeholders’ gender and their preference of educational placement setting (separate facilities, self-contained class, resource room, and general education) for students with disabilities. The results revealed no association between the stakeholders’ gender (male & female) and their preference of educational placement setting for students with disabilities, χ2(3, 2240) = 3.02, p > .05. Based on the results shown in Table 2 and Figure 1, it appears that the stakeholders preferred the separate facilities setting followed by the self-contained class setting, with the exception of the social workers, who preferred the self-contained class setting firstly, followed by the separate facilities setting. Moreover, it seems that the resource room setting was selected by stakeholders as the least preferred setting, with the exception of the regular teachers who selected the general education setting as the least preferred option. Question 3: Is the stakeholders’ role associated with their preference of educational placement setting for students with disabilities? The chi-square test of independence was performed to examine the association between the stakeholders’ role (regular teachers, special education teachers, social workers, principals, parents of students with disabilities, parents of students without disabilities, and students) Question 4: Do the differences between percentages of each category of stakeholders’ responses on each pair of educational placement for students with disabilities differ significantly? Figure 1 Educational placement preference by the role of the stakeholder 633 Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Vol. 9 Issue 4 Oct.) The chi-square test of goodness of fit was performed to examine the differences between percentages for each category of stakeholders’ responses on each pair of educational placement separately. These results are presented below according to each category of stakeholders. 2015 tion of those teachers who selected the general education classroom setting. Special education teachers: The results in Table 4 indicate that a significantly higher proportion (p < .001) of special education teachers selected separate facilities as the best educational placement setting for students with disabilities compared with any other educational placement setting. In addition, a significantly higher proportion (p < .05) of special education teachers selected self-contained class as a better educational placement setting for students with disabilities compared with the resource room setting. However, there was no significant difference (p = .05) between the proportion of special education teachers who selected self-contained class setting and the proportion of those teachers who selected general education classroom setting. Similarly, there was no significant difference (p = .05) between the proportion of special education teachers who selected the resource room setting and the proportion of those teachers who selected the general education classroom. Regular classroom teachers: The results in Table 3 indicate that a significantly higher proportion (p < .001) of regular classroom teachers selected separate facilities as the best educational placement setting for students with disabilities compared with any other educational placement setting. In addition, a significantly higher proportion (p < .001) of regular classroom teachers selected self-contained class as the best educational placement setting for students with disabilities compared with resource room and general education classroom settings. However, there was no significant difference (p = .05) between the proportion of regular classroom teachers who selected the resource room setting and the propor- Table 3 Results of the chi-square on the difference between percentages of regular classroom teachers’ responses for each pair of educational placement Setting Observed N Expected N Residual Chi-Square Df Separate facilities 303 256.5 46.5 16.860 1 Self-contained class 210 256.5 -46.5 Total 513 Separate facilities 303 182.5 120.5 159.126 1 Resource room 62 182.5 -120.5 Total 365 Separate facilities 303 179.0 124.0 171.799 1 General education 55 179.0 -124.0 Total 358 Self-contained class 210 136.0 74.0 80.529 1 Resource room 62 136.0 -74.0 Total 272 Self-contained class 210 132.5 77.5 90.660 1 General education 55 132.5 -77.5 Total 265 Resource room 62 58.5 3.5 0.419 1 General education 55 58.5 -3.5 Total 117 Asymp.sig. .000 .000 .000 .000 .000 .518 Table 4 Results of the Chi-Square on the difference between percentages of special education teachers’ responses for each pair of educational placement Setting Observed N Expected N Residual Chi-Square Df Asymp.sig. Separate facilities 94 73.0 21.0 12.082 1 .001 Self-contained class 52 73.0 -21.0 Total 146 Separate facilities 94 64.0 30.0 28.125 1 .000 Resource room 34 64.0 -30.0 Total 128 Separate facilities 94 69.0 25.0 18.116 1 .000 General education 44 69.0 -25.0 Total 138 634 Omani Stakeholders’ Preferences for Educational Placement of Students with Disabilities Jalal Hussien & et al. Table 4 Results of the Chi-Square on the difference between percentages of special education teachers’ responses for each pair of educational placement Setting Observed N Expected N Residual Chi-Square Df Self-contained class 52 43.0 9.0 3.767 1 Resource room 34 43.0 -9.0 Total 86 Self-contained class 52 48.0 4.0 0.667 1 General education 44 48.0 -4.0 Total 96 Resource room 34 39.0 -5.0 1.282 1 General education 44 39.0 5.0 Total 78 Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 Asymp.sig. .05 .414 .258 principals selected separate facilities as the best educational placement setting for students with disabilities compared with the resource room, and general education classroom settings. On the other hand, there was no significant difference (p = .05) between the proportion of principals who selected separate facilities and the proportion of those principals who selected self-contained class setting for students with disabilities. The results also indicate that a significantly higher proportion (p < .001) of principals selected self-contained class as the best educational placement setting for students with disabilities compared with the resource room and general education classroom settings. It has also been revealed that there was no significant difference (p = .05) between the proportion of principals who selected resource room and general education classroom settings. Social workers: The results in Table 5 indicate that a significantly higher proportion (p < .01) of social workers selected self-contained class as the best educational placement setting for students with disabilities compared with any other educational placement setting. In addition, a significantly higher proportion (p < .001) of social workers preferred separate facilities as a better educational placement setting for students with disabilities compared with the resource room and general education classroom settings. The results also revealed that a significantly higher proportion (p < .05) of social workers preferred the general education classroom setting as a better educational placement setting for students with disabilities compared with the resource room setting. Principals: The results in Table 6 indicate that a significantly higher proportion (p < .001) of Table 5 Results of the Chi-Square on the difference between percentages of social workers’ responses for each pair of educational placement Setting Observed N Expected N Residual Chi-Square Df Separate facilities 47 60.5 -13.5 6.025 1 Self-contained class 74 60.5 13.5 Total 121 Separate facilities 47 27.5 19.5 27.655 1 Resource room 8 27.5 -19.5 Total 55 Separate facilities 47 32.5 14.5 12.938 1 General education 18 32.5 -14.5 Total 65 Self-contained class 74 41.0 33.0 53.122 1 Resource room 8 41.0 -33.0 Total 82 Self-contained class 74 46.0 28.0 34.087 1 General education 18 46.0 -28.0 Total 92 Resource room 8 13.0 -5.0 3.846 1 General education 18 13.0 5.0 Total 26 Table 6 Results of the Chi-square on the difference between percentages of principals’ responses for each pair of educational placement Setting Observe N Expected N Residual Chi-Square df Separate facilities 113 103.5 9.5 1.744 1 Self-contained class 94 103.5 -9.5 Total 207 Separate facilities 113 74.5 38.5 39.792 1 635 Asymp. sig. .014 .000 .000 .000 .000 .050 Asymp. sig. .187 .000 2015 Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Vol. 9 Issue 4 Oct.) Table 6 Results of the Chi-square on the difference between percentages of principals’ responses for each pair of educational placement Setting Observe N Expected N Residual Chi-Square df Resource room 36 74.5 -38.5 Total 149 Separate facilities 113 80.5 32.5 26.242 1 General education 48 80.5 -32.5 Total 161 Self-contained class 94 65.0 29.0 25.877 1 Resource room 36 65.0 -29.0 Total 130 Self-contained class 94 71.0 23.0 14.901 1 General education 48 71.0 -23.0 Total 142 Resource room 36 42.0 -6.0 1.714 1 General education 48 42.0 6.0 Total 84 Asymp. sig. .000 .000 .000 .190 facilities as the best educational placement setting for students with disabilities compared with any other educational placement setting. In addition, a significantly higher proportion (p < .01) of parents of students without disabilities selected self-contained class as a better educational placement setting for students with disabilities compared with resource room setting. Moreover, it has been revealed that there was no significant difference (p = .05) between the proportions of parents of students without disabilities who selected selfcontained class setting and those parents who selected general education classroom setting for students with disabilities. Similarly, there was no significant difference (p = .05) between the proportions of parents of students without disabilities who selected resource room setting and those parents who selected general education classroom setting for students with disabilities. Parents of students with disabilities: The results in Table 7 indicate that a significantly higher proportion (p < .05) of parents of students with disabilities selected separate facilities as the best educational placement setting for students with disabilities compared with any other educational placement setting. In addition, a significantly higher proportion (p < .01) of parents of students with disabilities selected self-contained class as the best educational placement setting for students with disabilities compared with the resource room setting, but not the general education classroom setting. The results also indicate that a significantly higher proportion (p < .01) of parents of students with disabilities selected general education classroom as a better educational placement setting for students with disabilities compared with resource room setting. Parents of students without disabilities: The results in Table 8 indicate that a significantly higher proportion (p < .001) of parents of students without disabilities selected separate Table 7 Results of the Chi-Square on the difference between percentages of parents of students with disabilities responses for each pair of educational placement Setting Observed N Expected N Residual Chi-Square df Separate facilities 77 65.0 12.0 4.431 1 Self-contained class 53 65.0 -12.0 Total 130 Separate facilities 113 74.5 38.5 25.252 1 Resource room 36 74.5 -38.5 Total 149 Separate facilities 77 64.5 12.5 4.845 1 General education 52 64.5 -12.5 Total 129 Self-contained class 53 39.5 13.5 9.228 1 Resource room 26 39.5 -13.5 Total 79 Self-contained class 53 52.5 .5 0.010 1 General education 52 52.5 -.5 Total 105 636 Asymp. sig. .035 .000 .028 .002 .922 Omani Stakeholders’ Preferences for Educational Placement of Students with Disabilities Jalal Hussien & et al. Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 Table 7 Results of the Chi-Square on the difference between percentages of parents of students with disabilities responses for each pair of educational placement Setting Observed N Expected N Residual Chi-Square df Resource room 26 39.0 -13.0 8.667 1 General education 52 39.0 13.0 Total 78 Table 8 Results of the Chi-Square on the difference between percentages of parents of students without disabilities responses for each pair of educational placement Setting Observed N Expected N Residual Chi-Square Df Separate facilities 154 119.0 35.0 20.588 1 Self-contained class 84 119.0 -35.0 Total 238 Separate facilities 154 104.5 49.5 46.895 1 Resource room 55 104.5 -49.5 Total 209 Separate facilities 154 111.0 43.0 33.315 1 General education 68 111.0 -43.0 Total 222 Self-contained class 84 69.5 14.5 6.050 1 Resource room 55 69.5 -14.5 Total 139 Self-contained class 84 76.0 8.0 1.684 1 General education 68 76.0 -8.0 Total 152 Resource room 55 61.5 -6.5 1.374 1 General education 68 61.5 6.5 Total 123 Asymp. sig. .003 Asymp.sig. .000 .000 .000 .014 .194 .241 Question 5: What is the stakeholders’ educational placement preference (separate facilities vs. regular school) for students with disabilities? Students: The results in Table 9 indicate that a significantly higher proportion (p < .001) of students selected separate facilities as the best educational placement setting for students with disabilities compared with any other educational placement setting. Moreover, a significantly higher proportion (p < .01) of students preferred the self-contained class setting over the resource room setting for students with disabilities. On the other hand, the findings revealed no significant difference (p = .05) between the proportion of students who selected self-contained class setting and those students who selected general education classroom setting. Finally, the findings indicate a significantly higher proportion (p < .001) of students preferred general education classroom setting to the resource room setting for students with disabilities. The binomial test was performed for each category of stakeholders’ responses to examine the significance of the difference between the proportion of stakeholders who selected the regular school setting and the proportion of those who selected the separate facilities setting as the best educational placement setting for students with disabilities. The results of the binomial test are presented in Table 10. These results showed a significantly higher proportion (p < 0.01) of stakeholders who preferred the regular school over the separate facilities setting for students with disabilities for all stakeholders’ Table 9 Results of the Chi-square on the difference between percentages of students’ responses for each pair of educational placement Setting Observed N Expected N Residual Chi-Square Separate facilities 181 132.0 49.0 36.379 Self-contained class 83 132.0 -49.0 Total 264 Separate facilities 181 114.0 67.0 78.754 Resource room 47 114.0 -67.0 Total 228 Separate facilities 181 124.5 56.5 51.281 General education 68 124.5 -56.5 Total 249 637 Df 1 1 1 Asymp.sig. .000 .000 .000 Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Vol. 9 Issue 4 Oct.) Table 9 Results of the Chi-square on the difference between percentages of students’ responses for each pair of educational placement Setting Observed N Expected N Residual Chi-Square Self-contained class 83 65.0 18.0 9.969 Resource room 47 65.0 -18.0 Total 130 Self-contained class 83 75.5 7.5 1.490 General education 68 75.5 -7.5 Total 151 Resource room 47 57.5 -10.5 3.835 General education 68 57.5 10.5 Total 115 Df 1 Asymp.sig. .002 1 .222 1 .050 2015 with separate facilities by 78% of stakeholders for students with other health impairments, 77% for students with specific learning disabilities, 69% for students with speech and language disabilities, 61% for students with autism, 61% for students with hearing impairments, 59% for students with emotional and behavioral disabilities, 51% for students with physical disabilities, 51% students with visual impairments, and 50% for students with intellectual disabilities. The results also revealed that a significantly higher proportion (p < .000) of stakeholders selected regular school as a better placement for each type of disability, with the exception of physical disabilities, visual impairments, and intellectual disabilities, which showed non-significant differences (p = .05) between the proportion of stakeholders who selected the regular school and those who selected the separate facilities. categories, with the exception of two categories, i.e., regular education teachers and students showed no significant differences in their preference. Question 6: What is the stakeholders’ educational placement preference (separate facilities vs. regular school) for each type of disability? The binomial test was performed to examine the differences between the proportions of the stakeholders who selected the regular school option compared with those who selected the separate facilities as the best educational placement setting for each separate type of disability. The results of the binomial test are presented in Table 11. The findings indicate that the regular school was selected as a better educational placement setting in comparison Table 10 Results of the binomial test for stakeholders’ responses on educational placement (separate facilities & regular school) for students with disabilities Type of disability Response category N Observed Test Exact sig. prop. prop. (2-tailed) Regular Teacher Separate Facilities 303 .48 .50 .359 Regular School 327 .52 Total 630 1.00 Special Education Teacher Separate Facilities 94 .42 .50 .019 Regular School 130 .58 Total 224 1.00 Social Worker Separate Facilities 47 .32 .50 .000 Regular School 100 .68 Total 147 1.00 Principal Separate Facilities 113 .39 .50 .000 Regular School 178 .61 Total 291 1.00 Parents of students with disabilities Separate Facilities 77 .37 .50 .000 Regular School 131 .63 Total 208 1.00 Parents of students without disabilities Separate Facilities 154 .43 .50 .006 Regular School 207 .57 Total 361 1.00 Student Separate Facilities 181 .48 .50 .411 Regular School 198 .52 Total 379 1.00 638 Omani Stakeholders’ Preferences for Educational Placement of Students with Disabilities Jalal Hussien & et al. Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 Table 11 Results of the binomial test of stakeholders’ responses on educational placement setting (separate facilities & regular school) for each type of disability Type of disability Response category N Observed Test Exact prop. prop. sig. (2tailed) Intellectual disability Separate facilities 1114 .50 .50 .784 Regular school 1128 .50 Total 2242 1.00 Autism Separate facilities 877 .39 .50 .000 Regular school 1364 .61 Total 2241 1.00 Emotional & Behavioral disability Separate facilities 909 .41 .50 .000 Regular school 1331 .59 Total 2240 1.00 Hearing Impairment Separate facilities 866 .39 .50 .000 Regular school 1375 .61 Total 2241 1.00 Other Health Impairment Separate facilities 495 .22 .50 .000 Regular school 1747 .78 Total 2242 1.00 Specific Learning Disabilities Separate facilities 523 .23 .50 .000 Regular school 1718 .77 Total 2241 1.00 Visual Impairment Separate facilities 1093 .49 .50 .237 Regular school 1150 .51 Total 2243 1.00 Speech and Language Disability Separate facilities 703 .31 .50 .000 Regular school 1538 .69 Total 2241 1.00 Physical Disability Separate facilities 1097 .49 .50 .342 Regular school 1143 .51 Total 2240 1.00 Question 7: Does the stakeholders’ educational placement preference for students with disabilities differ significantly according to the student’s type of disability? The results suggest that the stakeholders’ educational placement preference for students with disabilities differs significantly (p < .000) according to the students’ disabilities, with the exception of the following disabilities: other health impairment vs. specific learning disabilities, autism vs. emotional and behavioral disabilities, autism vs. hearing impairment, hearing impairment vs. emotional and behavioral disabilities, intellectual disability vs. visual impairment, intellectual disability vs. physical disability, and physical disability vs. visual impairment. The stakeholders’ educational placement preference did not change significantly (p > .05). McNemar’s test was performed to examine the differences between the proportion of the stakeholders who selected the regular school option compared with those who selected the separate facilities for each separate pair of various disabilities (health disabilities, specific learning disabilities, speech and language disabilities, autism, hearing impairments, emotional and behavioral disabilities, physical disabilities, visual impairments, and intellectual disabilities). The results of McNemar’s test is summarized in Table 12. Table 12 Results of McNemar’s test of stakeholders’ responses on educational placement setting (separate facilities & regular school) for students with disabilities Type of disability* Pair differences % N Chi-Square** ID – AUT -11 2241 77.248 ID – EBD -9 2239 51.585 ID – HI -11 2240 78.418 ID – OHI -28 2241 377.023 ID – SLD -27 2240 361.475 ID – VI -1 2242 .492 ID – S-L -19 2240 192.832 ID – PHD -1 2239 .319 AUT – EBD 2 2239 1.538 639 Asymp. sig. .000 .000 .000 .000 .000 .483 .000 .572 .215 Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Vol. 9 Issue 4 Oct.) 2015 Table 12 Results of McNemar’s test of stakeholders’ responses on educational placement setting (separate facilities & regular school) for students with disabilities Type of disability* Pair differences % N Chi-Square** Asymp. sig. AUT – HI 0 2240 .136 .712 AUT – OHI -17 2240 181.451 .000 AUT – SLD -16 2239 156.247 .000 AUT – VI 11 2241 55.030 .000 AUT – S-L -8 2239 39.066 .000 AUT – PHD 10 2238 60.386 .000 EBD – HI -2 2240 2.413 .120 EBD – OHI -19 2240 210.060 .000 EBD – SLD -18 2239 180.485 .000 EBD – VI 8 2240 40.543 .000 EBD – S-L -10 2238 53.878 .000 EBD – AUT – PHD 8 2237 43.129 .000 HI – OHI -17 2241 189.875 .000 HI – SLD -16 2240 158.854 .000 HI – VI 10 2241 77.979 .000 HI – S-L -8 2239 40.885 .000 HI – PHD 10 2238 72.169 .000 OHI – SLD 1 2241 1.736 .188 OHI – VI 27 2242 395.132 .000 OHI – S-L 9 2240 68.449 .000 OHI – PHD 27 2239 400.445 .000 SLD – VI 26 2241 364.596 .000 SLD – S-L 8 2239 53.580 .000 SLD – PHD 2238 345.495 .000 VI – S-L -18 2241 198.869 .000 VI – PHD 0 2240 .023 .880 S-L – PHD 18 2240 211.574 .000 *ID (Intellectual Disability), AUT (Autism), EBD (Emotional and Behavioral Disability), HI (Hearing Impairment), OHI (Other Health Impairment), SLD (Specific Learning Disabilities), VI (Vision Impairment), L-D (Speech and language disability), PHD (Physical Disability) **Continuity Corrected The data also indicates that no special education services have been provided for children with disabilities in the general education classrooms so far. In comparison, 87% of children with disabilities in the United States are educated in the general education classroom setting (U.S. Department of Education, 2014). Discussion The information provided by the Ministry of Education and the Ministry of Social Development (2015) revealed that 5,325 children with disabilities received educational services in Oman. Twenty nine percent of these children are enrolled in special education classes in public school and 71% are enrolled in special education schools or centers. In comparison, 95% of children with disabilities in the United States receive their education in regular schools and the remaining in separate settings (Giangreco, Smith, & Pinckney 2006; Hocutt, 1996; U.S. Department of Education, 2014). Moreover, the data showed a separate and segregated special education system in Oman. Unfortunately, history shows that a separate and segregated education system was not successful in achieving inclusive schools in the U.S.A. (Lamport, Graves, & Ward, 2012) and more likely will not in Oman. The results of the current study also showed a significantly higher proportion of special education teachers, social workers, principals, parents of students with disabilities, and parents of students without disabilities preferred the regular school as a better setting than the proportion of those who preferred the separate facility settings for educating students with disabilities. However, there were no significant differences between the proportion of the regular education teachers and the propor- The data suggests that a very small number of Omani children with disabilities are receiving educational services; considering that WHO (2011) estimated about 15% of any population have a disability. This finding is consistent with the estimation of the percentage of children with disabilities who attend school in developing countries, which ranges from less than 1% to 10% (Peters, 2004; UNESCO, 2009). 640 Omani Stakeholders’ Preferences for Educational Placement of Students with Disabilities Jalal Hussien & et al. Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 tion of the students who preferred regular school or the proportion of those who preferred the separate facilities. However, Byrnes, Sigafoos, Rickards, and Brown (2002) reported that 60.3% of students with hearing impairments from Australia preferred to be educated in their local school, not in a separate setting. significant differences between the proportion of stakeholders who selected the regular school as a better placement and the proportion of stakeholders who selected separate facilities for educating students with the following disabilities: physical, visual impairment, and intellectual. Further investigation into the stakeholders’ preference of the educational settings (separate facilities, self-contained class, resource room, and general education) indicate the following: a) a significantly higher proportion of each category of stakeholder selected separate facilities as the best educational placement setting for students with disabilities compared with any other educational setting, with exception of the social workers, who preferred the self-contained class setting over the separate facilities setting, b) a significantly higher proportion of each category of stakeholder selected self-contained class as the best educational setting for students with disabilities compared with resource room and general education classroom settings, with the exception being the proportion of special education teachers, parents of students with disabilities, parents of students without disabilities and students who preferred the self-contained class, who did not differ significantly from the proportion of those who selected the general education classroom setting, and c) there were no significant differences between the proportion of each category of stakeholder who selected the resource room setting and the proportion of those stakeholders who selected the general education classroom setting, with the exception of a significantly higher proportion of social workers, parents of students with disabilities, and students, who preferred the general education classroom setting over the resource room setting. Similarly, Livingston, Reed, and Good (2001) found that principals from the state of Georgia, U.S.A, preferred the self-contained classroom in regular school as the best placement option for children with disabilities. Moreover, the findings suggest that the stakeholders’ preference for educating students in the regular school setting versus a separate facility varied according to the type of disability. The order, from more likely to least likely, of the stakeholders’ preference for educating students in a regular school setting versus a separate facility according to the type of disability is the following: a) other health impairment or specific learning disability, b) speech and language disability, c) autism, emotional and behavioral disability, or hearing impairment, and d) physical disability, visual impairment, or intellectual disability respectively. Finally, there were no significant differences in stakeholders’ preference for educating students in the regular school setting versus a separate facility between the following disabilities: other health impairment vs. specific learning disabilities, autism vs. emotional and behavioral disabilities, autism vs. hearing impairment, hearing impairment vs. emotional and behavioral disabilities, intellectual disability vs. visual impairment, intellectual disability vs. physical disability, and physical disability vs. visual impairment. Similarly, TASH (2009) and UNESCO (2010) documented that the likelihood of educating students with disabilities in the general education setting varied with the type of disability. In addition, the results revealed that a significantly higher proportion of stakeholders selected the regular school as a better placement over separate facilities for educating students with the following disabilities: specific learning, other health impairment, speech and language, autism, hearing impairment, and emotional and behavioral. However, there were no Overall, the stakeholders in Oman support the education of children with disabilities in regular school. In spite of this, the implementation of inclusive education is a complicated process. Successful inclusive education requires restructuring the education system, resources, accessible schools, and competent staff with positive attitudes. It is fundamental to develop Finally, a significant association between stakeholders’ role and their preference of educational placement setting for students with disabilities was found. However, the findings revealed that there was no significant relationship between stakeholders’ gender and educational settings preference. Recommendations 641 Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Vol. 9 Issue 4 Oct.) national policies and procedures that regulate and operationally define the following: special education services, the specific process of providing special education services, the rights of students with disabilities and their parents, definitions of each type of disability, and their eligibility criteria for special education. 2015 tion authority. Educational Psychology, 20(2), 191-211. Avramidis, E., & Norwich, B. (2002). Teachers’ attitudes towards integration/inclusion: a review of the literature. Eur. J. of Special Education, 17(2), 129-147. Balboni, G. & Pedrabissi, L. (2000). Attitudes of Italian teachers and parents toward In order to provide effective and efficient special education services in inclusive classrooms, the Ministry of Education must assure the availability of a sufficient number of special education teachers and support staff (psychologists, occupational therapists, physical therapists, and speech therapists). The availability also of qualified staff (administrators, supervisors, regular classroom teachers) in teaching students with disabilities is essential. school inclusion of students with mental retardation: the role of experience. Education and Training in Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities, 35(2), 148-159. Byrnes, L., Sigafoos, J., Rickards, F., & Brown, P. (2002). Inclusion of students who are deaf or hard hearing in government schools in New South Wales, Australia: Development and implementation of a policy. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 7(3), 244-257. In addition, the availability of a continuum of placement options is necessary to meet the needs of all special education students. Finally, the availability of valid and reliable assessment instruments that measure academic, social, and emotional development, speech and language, adaptive behaviors, gross motor skills, fine motor skills are crucial for screening, determining eligibility, planning, progress monitoring, and evaluation. Dyson, A., Howes, A., & Roberts, B. (2004). What do we really know about inclusive schools? A systematic review of the research evidence. In D. Mitchell (Ed.) Special Educational Needs and Inclusive Education: Major themes in education. London: Routlege Falmer. Acknowledgment Emam, M., & Hassan, H. (2011). Preschool and primary school teachers’ attitudes towards inclusive education in Egypt: the role of experience and self-efficacy. Procedia-Social and Behavioral Sciences, 29, 976985. The investigators would like to express their sincere appreciation to the Research Affairs at Sultan Qaboos University and the United Arab Emirates University for the financial support of this project under Grant # SQUUAEU/09/02. Gannon, S., & McGilloway, S. (2009). Children’s attitudes toward their peers with Down syndrome in schools in rural Ireland: an exploratory study. European Journal of Special Needs Education, 24 (4), 455-463. References Ahsan, T., Sharma, U., & Deppeler, M., (2012). Exploring pre-service teachers’ teaching-efficacy, attitudes and concerns about inclusive education in Bangladesh. International Journal of Whole Schooling, 8(2), 2-20. Georgiadi, M., Kalyva, E., Kourkoutas, E., & Tsakiris,V. (2012). Young children’s attitudes toward peers with intellectual disabilities: Effect of the type of school. Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 25, 531-541. Al-Balushi, T., Al-Badi, A., & Ali, S. (2011). Prevalence of disability in Oman: statistics and challenges. Canadian Journal of Applied Sciences, 1(3), 81-96. Giangreco, F., Smith, C., & Pinckney, E. (2006). Addressing the paraprofessional dilemma in an inclusive school: A program description. Research and Practice for Person with Severe Disabilities, 31(3), 215229. Avramidis, E., Balyliss, P., & Burden, R. (2000). A survey into mainstream teachers’ attitudes towards the inclusion of children with special educational needs in the ordinary school in one local educa- 642 Omani Stakeholders’ Preferences for Educational Placement of Students with Disabilities Jalal Hussien & et al. Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 an Journal of Business and Social Sciences, 1(5), 54-69. Gilmore, L., Campbell, J. & Cuskelly, M. (2003). Developmental expectations, personality Livingston, M., Reed, T., & Good, J. (2001). Attitudes of rural school principals toward inclusive practice and placements for students with severe disabilities. The Journal of Research for Educational Leaders, 1(1), 49-62. http://www2.education.uiowa.edu/arch ives/jrel/fall01/Livingston_0102.pdf stereotypes, and attitudes towards inclusive education: community and teacher views of Down Syndrome. International Journal of Disability, Development and Education, 50(1), 65-76. Grove, K., & Fisher, D. (1999). Entrepreneurs of meaning: Parents and the process of inclusive education. Remedial and Special Education, 20(4), 208-215 Miller, M., Garriott, P., & Mershon, D. (2005). Special education students’ placement preferences as shown in special education journals, Electronic Journal for Inclusive Education, 1 (9), 1-19. Haj Hussien, J., & Al-Qaryouti., I. (2014). Regular education teachers’ attitudes towards inclusion in Oman. Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies (JEPS), 8(4), 617-626. Ministry of Education. (2008). Inclusive education in the state of Oman: national report of the Sultanateof Oman: Muscat. Haj Hussien, J., & Al-Qaryouti, I. (2015a). Students’ attitudes toward inclusive education in Oman, International Journal for research in Education, 37, 1-11. http://www.ibe.unesco.org/National_Repor ts/ICE_2008/oman_NR08.pdf Ministry of Education, General Administration of Learning, Special Education Administration. (2015). Inclusive education Statistics. Sultanate of Oman: Muscat. Haj Hussien, J., & Al-Qaryouti, I. (2015b). General Education Teachers’ Perceived Self-Efficacy in Teaching Students with Disabilities in Oman AJIE Asian Journal of Inclusive Education, 3(1), 3-23. Ministry of Social Development, General Administration of Persons with Disabilities Affairs, Rehabilitation Administration (2015). Sultanate of Oman: Muscat. Hocutt. A. (1996). Effectiveness of special education: Is placement the critical factor? The Future of Children Special Education for Students with Disabilities, 6(1), 77-102. Morin, D., Crocker, A., Beaulieu-Bergeron, R., & Caron, J. (2013). Validation of attitudes toward intellectual disability – ATTID questionnaire. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 57(3), 268-278. Hunt, P., & McDonnell, J. (2007). Inclusive education. In Odom, S., Horner, R., Snell, M., & Blacher, J. (Eds.) Handbook of developmental Disabilities (269-291). New York, NY: The Guilford Press. Panagiotou, A., Evaggelinou, C., Doulkeridou, A., Mouratidou, K., & Koidou, E. (2008). Kauffman, J., Bantz, J., & McCullough, J. (2002). Separate and Better: A special public school class for students with emotional and behavioral disorders. Exceptionality, 10(3), 149-170. Attitudes of 5th and 6th grade Greek students toward the inclusion of children with disabilities in physical education classes after a paralympic education program. European Journal of Adapted Physical Activity, 1(2), 31-43. Kugelmass, J., & Ainscow, M. (2004). Leadership for inclusion: a comparison of international practices. Journal of Research in Special Educational Needs, 4, 3, 133-141. Papaioannou, C., Evaggelinou, C., & Block, M. (2014). The effect of a disability campprogram on attitudes towards the inclusion of children in a summer sport and leisure activity camp. International Journal of Special Education, 29(1), 1-9. Lamport, M., Graves, L., & Ward, A. (2012). Special students in inclusive classrooms: the impact of social interaction on educational outcomes for learners with emotional and behavioral disabilities. Europe- 643 Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Vol. 9 Issue 4 Oct.) Patel, M., & Rose, J. (2014). Students’ attitudes towards individuals with an intellectual disability. Journal of Intellectual Disabilities, 18(1), 90-103. 2015 of education 2014. http://nces.ed.gov/pubs2014/2014083.p df Peters, S. (2004). Inclusive education: an EFA strategy for all children. World Bank. www.worldbank.org. Villa, B., Thousand, J., Meyers, H. & Navin, A. (1996). Teacher and administrative perceptions of heterogeneous education. Exceptional Children, 63, 29-45. Rafferty, Y., Boettcher, C., & Griffin, K. (2001). Benefits and risks of reverse inclusion for preschoolers with and without disabilities: Parents’ perspectives. Journal of Early Intervention 24, 266–86. Wang, H. (2009). Should all students with special educational needs (SEN) be included in mainstream education provision? – A critical analysis. International Education Studies, 2(4), 154-161. Sari, H., Celikoz, N., & Secer, Z. (2009). An analysis of pre-school teachers’ and student teachers’ attitudes to inclusion and their self-efficacy. International Journal of Special Education, 24(3), 29-43. Weber, A.S. (2012). Inclusive education in the gulf cooperation council. Journal of Educational and Instructional Studies in the World, 2(2), 85-97. WHO. (2011). World disability report. Geneva: WHO. Semmel, M. (1986). Special education in the year2000 and beyond: A proposed actionagenda for addressing selected ideas. Reston, VA: The Council for Exceptional Children. (ERIC Document Reproduction Services No. LD276245). Tafa, E., & Manolitsis, G. (2003). Attitudes of Greek parents of typically developingkindergarten children towards inclusive education. European Journal of Special Needs Education, 18(2), 155-171. TASH Congressional Briefing on Inclusive Education (2009). Inclusive Education and Implications for Policy: The State of the Art and the Promise. http://www.edlawcenter.org/assets/file s/pdfs/issues-special education/Inclusive%20Education%20and%20I mplications%20for%20Policy.pdf UNESCO. (2009). Towards inclusive education for children with disabilities: A guideline. Bangkok, Thailand. http://www.uis.unesco.org/Library/Do cuments/disabchild09-en.pdf UNESCO. (2010) EFA Global Monitoring Report. Reaching the marginalized.http://unesdoc.unesco.org/images /0018/001866/186606E.pdf U.S. Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics, Institute of Education Sciences. (2014). The condition 644 Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Pages 645-659) Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 The Relation between Omani Students' Perceptions of the Writing Strategies and their Writing Performance Juma B. Busaidi *& Dina A. Al-Jamal Yarmouk University, Jordan ___________________________________________ Received: 3/2/2015 Revised: 18/5/2015 Accepted: 20/5/2015 _____________________________________________ Abstract: The present study aimed at exploring the relation between Omani students' perceptions of writing strategies and their own writing performance. Three types of key universal strategies (metacognitive, cognitive, social-affective), in master’s degree research, were assumed as effective in promoting students' successful writing processes. A strategy questionnaire in order to map Omani EFL students' perception of strategy use as well as a writing test to identify students' actual writing performance was constructed. The present study reported that the participants perceived the metacognitive strategy of selective attention as prevailing in their writing practice. They also demonstrated interest in the social affective strategy of cooperation. Cognitive strategy use, however, seemed to be complex and challenging, and was perceived as the least apparent. Furthermore, the study reported inefficiency of strategy use, as obtained by the questionnaire, which corresponded with students’ very poor performance in the writing test. The test scores indicated that 84.40% of the participants failed the writing test, which means that they are far away from being successful language writers. Keywords: Writing strategies, EFL, tenth grade students, Oman. العالقة بني إدراك الطمبة العمانيني واستخدام اسرتاتيجيات الكتابة و أدائهم الكتابي مجعة بو بطي البوسعيدي* و ديها اجلمن األردى،جامعة الريموك _____________________________________________ ٍدفت الدزاسُ احلالًُ إىل كشف العالقُ بني تصىزات الطلبُ العناىًني حنى استدداو اسرتاتًحًات:مشتدلص ،ًُ حًث افرتضت ٍره الدزاسُ أٌ االسرتاتًحًات العاملًُ السًٓشُ الثالث (ما وزاْ املعسف.ٌالكتابُ وأدآَه الكتاب ُ يف حبث أجسٍ للحصىل علِ دزجُ ماجشتري ذات فعالًُ يف إجناح عنلًُ الكتاب،)ًُ والىجداىًُ االجتناع،ًُواملعسف ومت بياْ استبًاٌ مً أجل معسفُ تصىزات الطلبُ الريً يدزسىٌ اللغُ اإلجنلًزيُ كلغُ أجيبًُ حنى.ُلدّ الطلب ٌ وخلصت الدزاسُ إىل أ.ُ كنا ومت بياْ اختباز لتحديد األداْ الكتابٌ الفعلٌ للطلب،ُاستدداو اسرتاتًحًات الكتاب ِالطلبُ كاىىا يتصىزوٌ أٌ اسرتاتًحًُ ما وزاْ املعسفًُ لالىتباه االىتقآٌ ٌٍ الشآدَ يف أدآَه الكتابٌ وعالوَ عل ُ كنا أظَسوا تصىزات تعكص صعىب.ٌذلك أبدو مً ال حنى استدداو االسرتاتًحًُ الىجداىًُ االجتناعًُ للتعاو ٍرا وقد أشازت الدزاسُ إىل عدو تىافق تصىزات الطلبُ الستدداو اسرتاتًحًات.االسرتاتًحًُ املعسفًُ وتعقًدٍا حًث.الكتابُ اليت مت احلصىل علًَا مً االستبًاٌ مع أداْ الطالب الفعلٌ يف اختباز الكتابُ الرٍ كاٌ ضعًفا وٍرا قد يعين أٌ مشتىّ كتابتَه،ُ مً املشازكني قد فشلىا يف اختباز الكتاب٪84.40 ٌأشازت ىتآخ االختباز أ .كاٌ بعًدا كل البعد عً وصفَه بكاتيب لغُ ىاجحني .ٌ عنا، طلبُ الصف العاشس،ًُ اللغُ اإلجنلًزيُ كلغُ أجيب،ُ اسرتاتًحًات الكتاب:ًُالكلنات املفتاح *deena.j@yu.edu.jo 645 Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Vol. 9 Issue 4 Oct.) A noteworthy drift in the teaching of the writing skill in the last decade has been a prototype shift from stressing the process of writing rather than the product of writing. As such, such a drift motivated the researchers of the current study to explore the teaching of the writing skill in Oman, which will, in turn, affect the teaching of the writing skill. 2015 the findings of a study which revealed that more experienced Japanese academic writers differ from their junior counterparts when using “language-oriented” writing strategies. Language learning strategies have three types: cognitive, metacognitive and social-affective strategies (Oxford, 1989, 1990; Chamot, 2005). Cognitive strategies comprise unconscious interactions with the material to be learned, for instance differencing, resourcing, and notetaking. They refer to steps used in problemsolving that require analysis, transformation and synthesis of learning materials. Metacognitive strategies, conversely, include conscious management and control over the learning process, for example planning, paying attention, and monitoring. That is, metacognitive strategies refer to the knowledge about the cognitive process as well as to the regulation of cognition through processes of planning, monitoring and evaluating. Social-affective strategies involve interacting with one another or using affective control to assist learning, such as questioning, working with peers, and lowering anxiety. They refer to the activities learners engage in to practice this knowledge (Rubin, 1987; Oxford, 1990; O'Malley & Chamot, 2005). Researchers such as Tsui and Ng (2000) and Liu and Hansen (2005) highlighted this move from studying writing itself to reviewing what writers do as they write. Experts dedicated their teaching to a sequence of strategies the writer engages in so as to produce a piece of writing. Successful language writers often make use of appropriate learning strategies to facilitate their writing process (Nyikos, 1987). With the development of the research on second language acquisition, more and more attention has been paid to the research on language learning strategies as they have the potential to enhance the development of the writing skill (Fazeli, 2012; Kang & Pyun, 2013). As early as Oxford (1990[a or b]), learning strategies have been theorized to have the principal influence on the rate and level of second language acquisition. It was felt by the researchers that teachers have begun to be aware of the need for providing support for students with the writing process. Inquiry into language learning strategies explored the possibility of assisting students to become more effective language learners by teaching them some of the writing strategies that empirical studies have identified as characteristics of the successful language writer (e.g. Rubin, 1987; Shih-Chieh, 2012; Fazeli, 2012). Learning strategies can be defined as steps, deliberate actions, techniques and behaviors that learners take in order to facilitate the learning process (Rubin, 1987; Schmeck, 1988, O’Malley & Chamot, 1990). Writing strategies are valuable in many learning settings. The potential will be achieved when the person acquires ability in strategy employment and formality with strategy application. This process approach to instruction views writing in progress as a dynamic entity, which can be substantively improved by multiple drafts and revisions. This is not to say that learning strategies will substitute specific knowledge of content domains; strategies are rather simply necessary conditions for more effective learning (Harold & O'Neil, 1978; Huwari & Aziz, 2011). Shapira and Lazarowitz (2005). Such performances demonstrate four clusters of procedures; namely, metacognitive, cognitive, social, and affective. Since writing proficiency affects one's achievement, writing was perceived as an integral part of second/foreign language learning (Anson, 2006). Kobayashi and Rinnert (2008) indicated that students use metacognitive strategies in their L1 and L2 essays as a result of interaction between the two languages. Previously, however, Sasaki (2000) stated that skillful writers use strategies while less skilled ones do not. In his study, Defining writing strategies Second language research on writing strategies refers to the comparison between experienced writers’ writing behavior and that of inexperienced writers. The difference in writing behaviors in first and second language writing, the use of first language in second language writing, and also the writers’ perception about writing tasks (Petric & Czarl, 2003; Okamura, 2006; Erkan & Saban, 2011) have been investigated. Okamura (2006), for example, supported this argument when showing 646 The Relation between Omani Students' Perceptions of the Writing Strategies Juma Busaidi & Dina Al-Jamal Sasaki compared the use of planning and revision strategies by two groups of writers. The study reported that skillful writers use strategies while less skillful ones do not use writing strategies. Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 study of successful and unsuccessful learners of English in Chinese universities. The data were collected through interviews, diaries, and follow-up email correspondence with nine successful and nine unsuccessful second-year EFL students at two Chinese mainland universities. The findings revealed that the unsuccessful students relied on rote memorization, while the successful students relied on a systematic plan and supplemented rote-learning with strategies for supporting what they had learnt. Metacognitive strategies echo students' consciousness of using strategies in order to manage learning. Metacognitive strategies were defined by Wiles (1997: 17) as “selfmanagement and the capability to plan, monitor and revise, or control learning”. Cognitive strategies, in contrast, echo students' processing and transforming information, which entail using language dynamically. Examples of cognitive strategies are: organizing, reading out loud, analyzing, summarizing and reasoning (Oxford, 1990; Weinstein & Mayer, 1986). Social strategies, however, echo students' interaction with their teacher or colleagues in class such as asking questions, cooperating with others, and peer work (Shapira & Lazarowitz, 2005). The social context was demonstrated as vital in writing strategies research. In this regard, Kang & Pyun (2013) examined the writing strategies used by L2 writers while stressing the mediated activities included in their writing processes. The sample number was as small as two participants, where there were interviews, a think-aloud technique, and motivated recall. A number of themes and trends were built in light of data analysis. Each theme was interpreted in relation to each individual's sociocultural context. The results revealed that a learner's socially situated setting is strongly related to the types of writing strategies and mediating instruments that the learner employs or favors. Language proficiency and the writing skill Writers who are not skillful in English cannot express multi-faceted ideas due to vocabulary, grammar, background, culture, style, and L1 transfer deficiency issues (Kobayashi & Rinnert, 2008; Petric & Czarl, 2003). In such cases, the growth of L2 writing promotes proficiency which has been one of the core research themes in EFL/ESL writing settings Evans and Green (2007) reported significant differences between proficient and non-proficient language users in terms of the writing process as skillful and less-skilled writers employ different prewriting, planning, editing, and revision activities. Writing and cognition Cognition was further examined in the context of EFL writing strategies. Specifically, cognitive and compensatory learning strategies were adopted study in order to develop writing skills. Twenty one females and 3 males in their second year undergraduate writers participated in this study in Costa Rica. All participants were EFL students joining a writing course. Brand and Jimenez employed a questionnaire which particularized Oxford's cognitive and compensatory learning strategies taxonomies and a pair of checklists for writers to fill in so as to overcome their writing difficulties. The study reported that most writers did not make the most of compensatory learning strategies in order to write well, and besides they had flaws in different writing areas, for instance, grammar, process, and vocabulary. Brand and Jimenez associated such findings with the fact that these writers did not know about the existence of cognitive and compensatory learning before the study commenced. An in-depth investigation of the process was carried out by Mu and Carrington (2007), who examined the writing strategies of three Chinese post-graduate learners in an Australian higher education institution. Data were collected by a semi-structured interview, questionnaire, and reflective post-writing discussion, and written drafts of papers were analyzed. The study reported that the participants used rhetorical strategies, metacognitive strategies, cognitive strategies and social-affective strategies in their writing. Strategy use features successful/unsuccessful language learners. Gan, Humphreys, and Hamp-Lyons (2004) conducted a comparative Cognitive approaches to manage writing tasks were also introduced by Shih-Chieh (2012) 647 Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Vol. 9 Issue 4 Oct.) who examined students' use of writing strategies as correlated to Taiwanese students’ English writing achievements. The cognitive approach was used so as to investigate the process of writing. The sample of the study involved 40 student writers (consisting of 20 low and 20 high achievers). The study employed a simultaneous think-aloud technique and instant effective interviews with students. The study reported that high-achieving students were more proficient than low-achieving students. Such student writers formulated their thesis statement, produced texts, and edited their texts through making the text as meaningful as possible. 2015 group L2 writers consistently in light of their writing capabilities. The study trained four Malay engineering undergraduates in Malaysia who had completed their foundation program. The results indicated that cognitive strategies were used most by the engineering students in composing; in contrast, the study concluded that social strategies were the least used strategies. Personality and writing Personality traits were studied carefully when it came to the writing strategies research paradigm. Such a relation was explored by Fazeli (2012). Four research instruments were employed; namely: Oxford's strategy inventory for learning language (SILL), a background questionnaire, a personality inventory, and a test of English as a foreign language. Two hundred and thirteen Iranian female EFL university undergraduates in Iran participated in this study. The results of the study demonstrated positive and negative noteworthy correlations that accounted for frequency of English language strategies and personality traits. Multiple intelligences were evidenced as relevant to writing strategies. Such a relationship was identified by Moheb and Bagheri (2013). The sample of the study consisted of 120 male and female Iranian EFL learners studying at a language institute. Two questionnaires were used. The first questionnaire was a multiple intelligences inventory reporting nine types of intelligences based on Gardner’s theory. The second questionnaire, however, was a writing skills and strategies inventory. A relationship between certain kinds of intelligences among females and certain writing strategies was reported. The male group, in contrast, did not display such a relationship. Advanced level students presented more noteworthy correlations than high level students. Conversely, it was revealed that none of the intelligences could foretell writing strategies individually. A further correlation between strategy use and concept development was established by AlJabali (2012). Al-Jabali led a longitudinal study of language strategy use and concept improvement. Forty-five Jordanian EFL undergraduates majoring in English participated in the study, where study-semester and gender variables were explored as well. The SILL inventory was adopted as a tool for responding to the questions of the study. The findings established that Jordanian undergraduates majoring in English had great strategy employment for most strategies. The study reported a hierarchy of strategies as ranking from social, compensation, affective, cognitive, to memory strategies. Gender differences were not significant in strategy use; yet, the study-semester variable showed significant differences in favor of third and fourth semesters’ responses. The relationship between strategy use and proficiency was highlighted. A study by Magno (2010) examined the English proficiency of Korean students, through adopting the SILL inventory together with the number of months spent in studying English. The sample consisted of 302 Korean learners who were asked to complete the SILL in addition to an English proficiency test. The study showed the compensation strategy as very effective in promoting students' English proficiency. A new result was that the number of months learning formal English increased as the English proficiency of Korean students also increased. The prominence of writers' motivation featured many studies, among which is the one carried out by Soo-Eun (2011) in order to survey Korean college students’ L2 writing improvement, motivation, and strategies. The study used interviews and self-report methods for students enrolled in writing classes. Verdicts of the study pointed out that L2 [L1?] and L2 writing background knowledge were considerably linked to L2 writing motivation, performance, and strategy use. Interview data Arranging writers in terms of their writing proficiency is a priority of writing strategy research. Abdullah (2011) accounted for the differences in the use of writing strategies between skillful and less skilled writers. Abdullah used multiple assessment criteria so as to 648 The Relation between Omani Students' Perceptions of the Writing Strategies Juma Busaidi & Dina Al-Jamal supported the self-report method by establishing the students' level of L2 writing selfefficacy, motivation, and strategy uses. Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 Al-Barwani, Mekhlafi, & Nagaratnam (2013: 3) studied the reality of Omani students in early pre-university grades and noted weakness in mastering the English language as a result of lack of students’ knowledge about means and effective strategies that can help them to learn the English language as required. Very few researches on learning strategies of the English language have been conducted in Oman (Abu Radwan, 2011; Al-Barwani et al, 2013). Omani students need to be fully competent in learning English as a consequence of the spread of globalization and technological development. As a result, the need arises for studying the effect of using learning strategies of the English language on the students’ achievement in the English language writing skill. Students' perceptions of the writing learning strategies may play a role in drawing conclusions regarding their writing skill development. Moreover, surveying Omani EFL strategies is an exciting, still-unfolding area of L2 writing and curriculum design. It is hoped that the present study will guide the main issues and considerations in EFL education particularly in Oman. Beliefs towards strategy use were perceived as crucial in the writing strategy research. In this regard, Sioson (2011) examined the relationship between students' beliefs and their strategy use among 300 undergraduates in the Philippines. The SILL questionnaire was managed in order to gather information on language learners’ beliefs and their learning strategies. The study found that language learning strategies mostly were negatively associated with language learning beliefs. Furthermore, only the motivation subscale of beliefs was the important predictor of speaking performance. Gender and proficiency were investigated as variables that may affect the use of strategies. As such, Abu Radwan (2011) examined the use of language learning strategies by 128 students majoring in English at Sultan Qaboos University, Oman by means of using Oxford's (1990a) SILL. The study relied on three-way criteria: students' grade point average (GPA) in English courses, study duration in the English Department, and students’ perceived selfrating. Results of the study indicated that students employed metacognitive strategies meaningfully more than any other category of strategies, with memory strategies ranking as lowest on students' preference scale, and male students used more social strategies than female students. Moreover, the results revealed that more proficient students used more cognitive, metacognitive and affective strategies than less proficient students. The primary research concern addresses the congruence between students' perceptions of writing strategies and their own writing performance by examining their perceptions as well as their real writing performance. Step by step questions that reflect such concern are as follows: The Omani setting In Oman, English teaching has assumed extraordinary attention since as early as the 1970s when His Majesty Sultan Qaboos Bin Said rose to the throne and pursued substantial educational reforms (Al-Issa, 2011). English language command is seen as a crucial element in the development of Oman and its real incorporation into the modern world. At school level, from 1998-1999, English was taught as early as the first grade onwards, giving pupils twelve years of instruction in the language before entering higher education. However, the majority of Omani students find it very difficult to listen, speak, read or write in English (Al-Mahrooqi & Asante, 2010). 649 1. What types of writing learning strategies are most frequently perceived by EFL tenth grade students at Khrayes Al Hobos School? 2. What is their actual writing performance? 3. What is the relationship between students' perceptions of the writing strategies and their writing performance? Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Vol. 9 Issue 4 Oct.) Method 2015 The strategy survey questionnaires as well as the writing test were given to a jury of eight university professors and six senior teachers and educational supervisors in order to elicit their views and to make sure that it suited the level of the students. The kind of feedback on[?] the moderation process by the jury was steered to elicit their views as to the accuracy, clarity, and appropriateness of the instruments. The questionnaire was then reviewed and modified in light of the jury's comments. In order to ensure the reliability of the questionnaire, the internal consistency calculated the coefficient on a pilot study of twenty students who were excluded from the sample. The reliability coefficient of the test was calculated by using Pearson which reached (0.87) and considered acceptable for conducting the questionnaire. The following table displays the estimation of each strategy. The present study adopted a descriptive research design. Khrayes Al Hobos School was selected as a case study. The participants included all tenth grade students at the school, comprising 186 students as distributed over 6 sections in Muscat City in Oman during the second semester of the academic year 20132014. The rationale behind having the tenth grade students as the sample is relevant to the fact that it is the last grade in the basic stage, where students can draw conclusions on their strategy use. All participants, thus, responded to a strategy survey questionnaire in addition to the writing test. To successfully conduct the study, the following instruments were used: o A language strategy questionnaire. A questionnaire was used in order to identify the performances employed by learners when they rewrite in English. The language learning strategy questionnaire was derived from SILL as developed by Oxford (1990b), as shown in Appendix 1. This self-report instrument used a fivepoint Likert scale, ranging from very strongly disagrees to strongly agrees, to assess the frequency that the students used different techniques for Englishlanguage writing. The questionnaire comprised 24 items distributed across three strategy types, where the metacognitive strategy included items 1-10, the cognitive strategy included items 11-22, and the social-affective strategy encompassed items 23 and 24. The form of the language learning strategy questionnaire was as follows. Table 2 Coefficient of the internal consistency measures Dimension coefficient of the internal consistency Metacognitive strategy 0.81 Cognitive strategy 0.86 Social-affective strategy 0.77 Writing strategies as a whole 0.88 Students' perceived strategies The following table shows the rank order of perceived writing strategy types used by Omani tenth grade students. Table 3 presents mean scores and standard deviations of the types of writing learning strategies. It demonstrates that the rank order of writing strategy types used by Omani tenth grade students were metacognitive strategy as the most frequent type with a mean score of 3.43, and then social-affective strategies with a mean score of 3.34, and finally cognitive strategies, which were uncommon with a mean score of 3.25. The results of the standard deviation scores, ranging, between 0.60 and 0.83, refer to a rapprochement (i.e. the standard deviation is a measure of discrepancy among the values) in the study participants' answers. The following table shows the meta-cognitive strategies used, ordered in a descending manner by Omani tenth grade students. Table 1 Strategy types and their categorizations Strategy type Strategy name Number of items 1-Selective attention 1-10 Metacognitive -Directed-attention 2-Cognitive -Translation 11-22 -Elaboration -Inferencing -Summarizing 3-Social-Cooperation 23-24 affective o A writing test was designed so as to evaluate students' use of learning strategies. This test was parallel to tenth grade materials, as shown in Appendix 2. It consisted of two questions which assessed students’ writing skill. The first question was graded out of 5 marks and the second question was graded out of 10 marks. 650 The Relation between Omani Students' Perceptions of the Writing Strategies Juma Busaidi & Dina Al-Jamal Rank 1 2 3 Item 10 4 5 9 2 1 6 3 8 7 Number of strategy 1 3 2 Table 3 Writing strategy types Std. deviation 0.60 0.83 0.70 Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 Mean 3.43 3.34 3.25 Strategy type Metacognitive Social-affective Cognitive Table 4 Metacognitive strategies used, ordered in a descending manner Item Mean Std.deviation I revise whatever I write and edit it. 3.71 1.15 I determine answers with explanations before starting writing the 3.58 1.05 outline I think about how the teacher may understand my writing. 3.56 1.28 I keep checking grammar when I write. 3.54 1.15 I identify the topic and collect information about it, from model writ3.52 1.00 ten work written by native speakers, if the topic is new to me. I think in Arabic whenever I write in English. I can write the outline precisely. I identify points supporting the thesis by listing down paragraphs, sentences and words relevant to the writing topic. I do mind mapping to create relevant ideas to the topic in question. I can write the main idea and supporting ideas precisely. Rank 1 2 Degree High Medium 3 4 5 Medium Medium Medium 3.44 3.38 3.35 1.13 1.04 1.03 6 7 8 Medium Medium Medium 3.17 3.08 1.14 1.03 9 10 Medium Medium *(2.33 or less=Low; 2.34-3.67=Medium; 2.68 or more= High) Table 4 illustrates the order of metacognitive strategies which came top when strategies were compared earlier. Mean sores of metacognitive strategies ranged between 3.08 and 3.71. Item 10 and its text "I revise whatever I write and edit it" came first with a mean score of 3.71. This indicates that most Omani tenth grade students usually revise and edit their writing. One explanation is that they tend to be anxious as a result of the lack of writing experiences and practices in writing the English language. Thus, revision and editing are established in order to ensure their efficiency during the writing process. Item 4 (“I determine answers with explanations before starting writing the outline") came second with a mean score of 3.58. Item 5, then, and its text “I think about how the teacher may understand my writing” came third with a mean score of 3.56. Item 7 (“I can write the main idea and supporting ideas precisely”), however, came last with a mean score of 3.08. The results of the standard deviations, which ranged between 1.00 and 1.28 bring up a rapprochement in the participants' answers. This refers to the fact that Omani tenth grade students tend to feel that they have problems when it comes to making an indicator of their knowledge base in general. Cognitive strategies came third when strategies were compared earlier in Table 3. Mean sores of cognitive strategies ranged from 3.61 to 2.94. Table 5 demonstrates how the participants felt towards each cognitive strategy. The following table shows the cognitive strategies used, ordered in a descending manner by Omani tenth grade students. Table 5 Cognitive strategies used, ordered in a descending manner Item Item Mean Std. deviation 16 I can compare and contrast similarities and differences between differ3.61 1.05 ent things. 19 I make sure that I write all the needed elements/components of a 3.48 1.04 certain argument. 17 I can provide sufficient examples to make my idea clear. 3.44 1.00 18 I can give evidence on my argument in order to make myself clear. 3.42 1.13 11 I can write without using the English-Arabic dictionary. 3.23 1.20 20 I can summarize key topics/ ideas precisely. 3.23 1.20 14 I can describe a place/an object/a friend in detail. 3.21 1.14 15 I can use different words/images every time I refer to the same thing. 3.18 1.18 21 I write redundant and trivial details. 3.16 1.30 22 I write everything and word for word when I summarize. 3.12 1.19 12 I can write without asking my teacher /friends about word meanings. 3.06 1.16 13 I write sentences in Arabic first then I translate into English. 2.94 1.36 *(2.33 or less=Low; 2.34-3.67=Medium; 2.68 or more= High) 651 Rank 1 Degree Medium 2 Medium 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 Medium Medium Medium Medium Medium Medium Medium Medium Medium Medium Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Vol. 9 Issue 4 Oct.) In Table 5, item 16 and its text "I can compare and contrast similarities and differences between different things" came first with a mean score of 3.61. Such a finding can be explained in terms of the commonest of 'compare and contrast' theme in education which is typically introduced first. Item 19 (“I make sure that I write all the needed elements/components of a certain argument”) came second with of a mean score of 3.48. Then, item 17 and its text “I can provide sufficient examples to make my idea clear” came third through a mean score of 3.44. Item 13 (“I write sentences in Arabic first then I translate into English”), however, came last with a mean score of 2.94. This can be interpreted as Omani students feeling that their teachers do not direct them to use such cognitive strategies where English writing classes are taught in a way similar to Arabic writing classes. Standard deviation scores of 1.00-1.36 denote a rapprochement in the participants' answers. 2015 text “I can write without negotiating meaning with teacher/friends” came second through a mean score of 3.04. The results of the standard deviation were 1.24-1.27, which refers to a rapprochement in the participants' answers. The results of this table suggest that some students have the motivation to study and get high degrees, which pushes them to consult and ask for assistance from their colleagues or teachers, while others avoid that, perhaps as a result of social reasons related to shame or a sense of inferiority in their opinion when relying on others, or as a result of indifference towards academic achievement in general. Students' writing performance To recap, the second question involved information on the following question: What is the actual writing performance of the participants? Mean scores and standard deviations were calculated, in addition to number of students and their marks in the test to answer this question. Table 7 displays mean scores and standard deviations of Omani tenth grade students on the writing test. Table 8 outlines such details. Social-affective strategies came second when strategies were compared previously in Table 3. Mean sores of social affective strategies ranged between 3.04 and 3.66. Table 6 proves how the participants felt towards each socialaffective strategy. The following table shows results of students on the writing test by Omani tenth grade students. The following table shows social-affective strategies use, ordered in a descending manner by Omani tenth grade students. Table 6 Social-affective strategies use as ordered in a descending manner No. item Mean Std. Rank Degree of deviation Item 24 I need to dis3.66 1.27 1 Medium cuss the topic with my teacher first before commencing writing the outline. 23 I can write 3.04 1.24 2 Medium without negotiating meaning with teacher/friends. Social affective 3.34 *(2.33 or less=Low; 2.34-3.67=Medium; 2.68 or more= High) Table 6 illustrates that item 24 and its text "I need to discuss the topic with my teacher first before commencing writing the outline" came first with a mean score of 3.66. This reflects students' lack of self-confidence as a result of teacher-centered approaches. Item 23 and its 652 Table 7 Results of students on the writing test Questions of Mean Std. deRank test viation Question 1 1.52 1.54 1 Question 2 1.27 2.52 2 Total Marks 2.78 3.73 The table indicates that the mean score of the overall marks obtained by the test is as low as 2.78. The mean scores of both questions were also low ranging from 1.27 for the second question and 1.52 for the first question.Understanding the students' actual performances further entailed classifying them in terms of successful or unsuccessful language writers on the grounds of their real answers in the test. The following Table, hence, points out such classification. The following table shows the participants' classification in terms of successful/unsuccessful language writers by Omani tenth grade students. The Relation between Omani Students' Perceptions of the Writing Strategies Juma Busaidi & Dina Al-Jamal Table 8 Successful/unsuccessful language writers' classification Number of Number of Number of Students' successful stuunsuccessful students marks dents students 71 0 29 157 86 1-6 27 7-14 2 15 Table 8 shows that the number of unsuccessful students according to the test is as high as 157 students out of 186. In contrast, the number of successful students according to the test is only 29 students. Surprisingly, 71 students received the score of zero. Eighty-six students had marks that ranged between 1 and 6. Twenty-seven students had marks that ranged from 7 to 14. Only two students got full marks. Strategy use is always correlated with proficiency. Accordingly, these very low figures obtained by the test correspond precisely with the results obtained by the strategy survey questionnaire, where low and very moderate results were obtained. Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 writing performance and increases in students’ perceptions of using writing strategies, both independently and together. Students' perceptions, as investigated in the present study, were not a much stronger predictor of students’ performance in using writing strategies. Summary of results The present study reported the following results: o Omani tenth grade students feel that they use metacognitive strategies more than cognitive or social-affective strategies when they write in English. They feel that they use revising and editing metacognitive strategies most while believing that they cannot manipulate using writing main/supporting ideas met cognitive strategies as least. It is worth pointing out that 'revising' is a selective attention strategy and 'editing' is a directed attention one. Perception versus performance o The third research question entailed an analysis of the relationship between students' perceived strategies and their actual writing performance. The present study embarked on metacognition, cognition, and social-affective writing strategies in light of the growing interest in such strategies. This has been connected to developing students' writing skills. This study searched for the relationship between students' self-reports of using writing strategies and their real use of these writing strategies. Using a language strategy questionnaire survey data for 186 tenth grade students as well as a writing test, we found a decline in writing performance and increased statistical indications of students’ perceptions of using writing strategies. Omani students believe that they use the cognitive strategy of 'compare and contrast', which is an inference cognitive strategy, more while not being certain of their ability to write without asking the teacher. The participating students regarded[?] using social-affective strategies, such as discussing the topic with the teacher, as paramount. o Omani tenth grade students' beliefs, perceptions and feelings towards the use of strategies did not correspond with their actual writing performance. Provided that[?], results obtained by the writing test demonstrated that 157 students out 186 did not pass the writing test. Discussion This study reports many empirical based studies on the writing skill achievement. Its aim was to derive a set of pedagogical implications to help Omani students improve their writing performance. It is found that there is a gap between students' perceptions of the use of writing strategies and their actual writing performance. In other words, the study showed no relationship between students' perception and their actual writing output. Such variance in students' perceptions and practices reflected evidence that there were drops in students’ The present study investigated the English language writing strategies among tenth grade Omani students. The results of the study demonstrated that Omani students perceived themselves as using metacognitive strategies foremost; namely, selective attention and directed attention strategies. This finding is consistent with the findings obtained by Abu Radwan (2011), Nikoopour, Farsani, and Neishabouri (2011) and Al-Jabali (2012), which established that EFL writers, generally, tend to 653 Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Vol. 9 Issue 4 Oct.) adopt metacognitive strategies, since metacognitive strategies do not entail complex thinking processes; thus, they suit several students’ levels. Accordingly, the present study showed that the learners favored using metacognitive strategies as they were the most frequently used English language writing strategies. 2015 the whole, unsuccessful language proficiency prevents students from communicating appropriately. Students with low English proficiency always find it difficult to communicate comfortably in English. In Omani tenth grade English language classes, as indicated by the present study, students have a weak proficiency in English writing skills where they cannot write efficiently. Therefore, as reported previously, they find it hard to express ideas in their writing due to a lack of their use of Arabic first before translating into English. In this regard, Kobayashi and Rinnert (2008) demonstrated that the L1 writing ability of L2 students is the basic determinant of their L2 writing performance. Such views are based on the supposition that writers transfer their writing skills from their L1 to the target L2 writing (Kobayashi & Rinnert, 2008). In such a case, L2 writing difficulties are the result of and influenced by L1 transfer of culturally preferred linguistic patterns from the L1. The present study, furthermore, demonstrated Omani students' sensible inclination towards using social-affective strategies of cooperation. Such moderate level of using social-affective strategies can be attributed to teacher-centered approaches to teaching. In such classes, students are not self-confident and keep asking for assistance from their colleagues or teachers. This result is consistent with the study of Al-Jabali (2012). On the other hand, Omani tenth grade students perceived cognitive strategy employment in their writing as the lowest. This result is also consistent with the findings of Al-Jabali (2012) and Abdullah (2011). Cognitive strategies entail the strategies of translation, elaboration, differencing, and summarizing; that is, cognitive strategies are complex and not easily used. Not all learners can manipulate cognitive strategies; unsuccessful language learners fail to use this type of strategy. Overall, the results of L2 writing process research displayed noteworthy differences between successful and unsuccessful language writers (Brand and Jimenez, 2013; Evans & Green, 2007) . Successful and unsuccessful language writers differ in pre-writing activities. Unsuccessful writers devote only a short time to planning before starting to write. However, successful writers seem to devote more time to planning and revision. The current study noted that Omani tenth grade students' ineffective writing categorized them as unsuccessful language writers with low L2 proficiency as they made grammatical and lexical errors when they created texts. Results obtained by the strategy survey questionnaire concluded a shortage of use of writing strategies by Omani tenth grade students. The results of the writing test likewise corresponded to the questionnaire results. The test results exhibited a low level of students’ writing skill in the English language, where their marks in the test were not promising. Additionally, the percentage of failure among students was as high as 84.40%. That is, 157 students out of a total of 186 failed the test. It was concluded that the majority of students' writings demonstrated obvious and overt weaknesses and flaws when expressing their ideas in the English language. In addition, a large number of errors like spelling and grammar were prevailing and dominant all through the students' writings. Pedagogical implications The present study stressed the importance of using strategies in learning the skill of writing. Still further examination is crucial to confirm the results of this study. Obviously, EFL teachers are intensely recommended to foster their students' strategic use. Teachers, likewise, are really steered to teach students how to write expressively through strategy use rather than evaluating their final product of writing. EFL teachers are strongly recommended to increase learners’ engagement with pre-task activities by enabling them to plan their writing because this would enhance the quality of the language used during the task and reduce the overall mental burden during In conclusion, strategy use was correlated with unsuccessful language writing. This result is consistent with the findings of many researchers, for example, Evans & Green (2007), Kuiken and Vedder (2008) [not in references list], and Moheb and Bagheri (2013), who associated writing proficiency with strategy use. On 654 The Relation between Omani Students' Perceptions of the Writing Strategies Juma Busaidi & Dina Al-Jamal writing. The study, hence, proposes designing applicable writing activities to promote the use of strategies, and explicitly teaching writing strategies and monitoring their application in writing classes. Teachers should be aware of the role of metacognitive, cognitive and socialaffective strategies in regulating students’ thoughts and emotions. Therefore, teachers are invited to clarify explicitly to their students how such strategies can help them cope with their writing problems and as a result make the writing process more creative and enjoyable. Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 er Education in Oman: International Journal of Instruction, 6(2), 109-128. Al-Issa, A. (2011). Advancing English Language Teaching Research in Gulf Cooperation Council States universities. MJAL, 3(2), 60-77. Al-Jabali, M. (2012). Language Learning Strategy Use and Concept Development among Jordanian Undergraduate English Language Majors, International Journal of Education, Amman, 4(1), 161-180. Al-Mahrooqi, R., & Asante, C. (2010). Promoting Autonomy by Fostering a Reading Culture. In R. Al-Mahrooqi & V. Tuzlukova (Eds.). The Omani ELT Symphony: Maintaining Linguistic and Socio-cultural Equilibrium (pp. 477-494). Muscat: Sultan Qaboos University Academic Publication Board. Understanding the effectiveness of strategy use is important in designing EFL materials. Thus, the present study recommends the need for developing principled instruction of all language skills in light of language learning strategies exemplified through this study. Moreover, this study proposes directing research-based strategic instruction across different subjects. The study, then, displays gratitude to promising strategic instruction techniques that convey practical and comprehensive materials. Anson, C. M. (2006). Assessing Writing in Cross-Curricular Programs: Determining the Locus of Activity. System, 11(3), 100112. Bagheri, M., & Fazel, I. (2011). EFL Learners’ Beliefs about Translation and its Use as a Strategy in Writing. Journal of the Reading Matrix, 11 (3), p 292-301.[not mentioned in text] Although the emphasis of this study was not on the syntactic features of the text, it was found, however, that grammatical and spelling problems prevented understanding. Therefore, tutors and language instructors are recommended to teach students these aspects of the text, not in isolation but in communicative classes, and they should monitor the application of the rules in the students’ writing. The current study, accordingly, might be of educational assistance to textbook designers, academics, instructors and communicative competence advocates. Chamot, A.U. (2005). Language Learning Strategy Instruction: Current Issues and Research. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 25: 112-30. Erkan, D. Y., & Saban, A. I. (2011). Writing Performance Relative to Writing Apprehension, Self-Efficacy in Writing, and Attitudes towards Writing: A Correlational Study in Turkish Tertiary-Level EFL. Asian EFL Journal, 5(4), 164-192. References Abdullah, M. (2011). Writing Strategies of Malaysian ESL Undergraduate Engineering Learners. International Journal of Engineering & Technology, 11(2), 27-33. Evans, S., & Green, C. (2007). Why EAP is Necessary: A Survey of Hong Kong Tertiary Students. Journal of English for Academic Purposes, 6(1), 3-17. Abu Radwan, A. (2011). Effects of L2 Proficiency and Gender on Choice of Language Learning Strategies by University Students Majoring in English .Asian EFL Journal, 13(4), 114-153. Fazeli, S.H. (2012).The Prediction Use of English Language Learning Strategies Based on Personality Traits among the Female University Level Learners .Indian Journal of Science and Technology, 5(8), 3211-3217. Al-Barwani, T; Mekhlafi, A. & Nagaratnam, R. (2013).Service- Learning Might be the Key: Learning From the Challenges and Implementation Strategies in EFL Teach- Gan, Z., Humphreys, G., & Hamp-Lyons, L. (2004). Understanding Successful and Unsuccessful EFL Students in Chinese 655 Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Vol. 9 Issue 4 Oct.) Universities. The Modern Language Journal, 88, 229-44. 2015 Okamura, A. (2006). Two types of Strategies Used by Japanese Scientists, When Writing Research Articles in English. System, 34, 68–79. Harold, F., & O’Neil, J. (1978). Learning Strategies. Academic Press: New York. Oxford, R.L. (1989). Use of Language Learning Strategies: A Synthesis of Studies with Implications for Strategy Training. System, 17, 235-247. Huwari, I. F., & Aziz, N. H. (2011). Writing Apprehension in English Among Jordanians Postgraduate Students at University Utara Malaysia (UUM). Academic Research International, 1(2), 190-198. Oxford, R.L. (1990a). Language Learning Strategies: What Every Teacher Should Know. New York: Newbury House. Kang, Y.-S., & Pyun, D.O. (2013). Mediation Strategies in L2 Writing Processes: A Case Study of Two Korean Language Learners. Language, Culture and Curriculum, 26(1), 52-67. Oxford, R.L. (1990b). What Every Teacher Should Know. Newburg House/Harper &Row, New York. Now Boston: Heinle & Heinle. Kaouther, F. (2010). Writing strategies of Tunisian First Year University Students Learning English as a Foreign Language. M.A. Thesis, University of Manouba, Tunis. [not mentioned in text] Petric, B., & Czarl, B. (2003). Validating a Writing Strategies Questionnaire. System, 31, 187-215. Rubin, J. (1981). Study of Cognitive Processes in Second Language Learning. Applied Linguistics, 2, 17-31. 265[?].[And not mentioned in text] Kobayashi, H., & Rinnert, C. (2008).Task Response and Text Construction across L1 and L2 Writing. Journal of Second Language Writing, 17(1), 7-29. Rubin, J. (1987). Learner strategies: Theoretical Assumptions, Research History and Typology. New York: Prentice Hall International. Liu, J., & Hansen, J. (2005). Peer Response in Second Language Writing Classrooms. Michigan series on teaching multilingual writers. Sasaki, M. (2000). Toward an Empirical Model of EFL Writing Processes: An Exploratory Study. Journal of Second Language Writing, 9, 91-115. Magno, C. (2010). Korean Students' Language Learning Strategies and Years of Studying English as Predictors of Proficiency in English. Teaching English to Speakers of Languages Journal, 2, 39-61. Schmeck, R. (1988). An Introduction to Strategies and Styles of Learning. New York: Plenum Press . Mu, C., & Carrington, S. (2007). An Investigation of Three Chinese Students’ English Writing Strategies. Teaching English as a Second of Foreign Language – EJ, 11(1), 123. Shapira, A., & Lazarowitz, R.H. (2005). Opening Windows on Arab and Jewish Children’s Strategies as Writers. Language Culture and Curriculum, 18(1), 72-90. Nikoopour, J., Farsani, F., & Neishabouri, J. (2011). Language Learning Strategy Preferences of Iranian EFL Students. International Conference on Social Science and Humanity. Singapore, IPEDR, 5(2), 356-360. Shih-Chieh, C. (2012). Students' Use of Writing Strategies and Their English Writing Achievements in Taiwan. Asia Pacific Journal of Education, 32(1), 93-112. Sioson, C. (2011). Language Learning Strategies, Beliefs, and Anxiety in Academic Speaking Task. Philippine ESL Journal, 7, 3-27. Nyikos, M. (1987). The effect of color and imagery as mnemonic strategies on learning and retention of lexical items in German. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Purdue University, IN: West Lafayette. Soo-Eun, C. (2011). Contributions of Prior Knowledge, Motivation, and Strategies to Korean College Students' L2 writing development. PhD dissertation, University of Maryland. O’Malley, J.M., & Chamot, A. (1990). Learning Strategies in Second Language Acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 656 The Relation between Omani Students' Perceptions of the Writing Strategies Juma Busaidi & Dina Al-Jamal Stem, H. (1983). Fundamental Concepts of Language Teaching. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [not mentioned in text] Tsui, A., & Ng, M. (2000). Do secondary L2 writers benefit from peer comments? Journal of Second Language Writers, 9, 147170. Weinstein, C.E., & Mayer, R.E. (1986). The Teaching of Learning Strategies. New York: Macmillan, 315-327. Wiles, W. (1997). Meta-cognitive Strategy Programming for Adult Upgrading Students. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Alberta, Edmonton, Alberta, Canada. Writing Strategies. Journal of Language Teaching and Research, 4(4), 777784. 657 Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Vol. 9 Issue 4 Oct.) 2015 Appendix 1 Strategy questionnaire This questionnaire is designed for investigating the writing strategies among EFL tenth grade students in Oman. I would be grateful if you could answer the following questions .The information provided will be of great help in my study and will be treated anonymously. مت تصنيه ٍذا االستبياٌ للحصْل على معلْمات حْل التحقق مً اسرتاتيجيات الكتابة لدى طالب الصف العاشر يف ً ّسْف تستخدو املعلْمات اليت سيته احلصْل عليَا م. أشكر تفضلكه باإلجابة عً األسئلة التالية.ٌسلطية عنا .ٍذِ االستباىُ ألغراض البحث العلني دٌّ أٌ تعطى إىل طرف ثالث No. 12345678910 1112131415161718192021222324- Item Meta-cognitive strategy I think in Arabic whenever I write in English. أفكر تانهغح انعرتٍح عُذيا أكتة تانهغح االَجهٍسٌح I can identify the topic and collect information about it, from model written work. استطٍع أٌ أحذد انًٕضٕع ٔأجًع انًعهٕياخ عُّ يٍ ًَٕرج انعًم انًكتٕب I identify points supporting the thesis by listing down paragraphs, sentences and words relevant to the writing topic. أحذد َقاط دعى االطرٔحح يٍ خالل سرد انفقراخ ٔانجًم ٔانكهًاخ راخ انعالقح تًٕضٕع انكتاتح I determine answers with explanations before starting writing the outline. أحذد األجٕتح ٔانتفسٍراخ قثم انثذء تكتاتح انخطٕط انرئٍسٍح I think about how the teacher may understand my writing. ًأفكر كٍف سٍفٓى يعهًً كتاتت I can write the outline precisely. أستطٍع أٌ أكتة انخطٕط انرئٍسٍح تذقح I can write the main idea and supporting ideas precisely. أستطٍع أٌ أكتة انفكرج انرئٍسٍح ٔاألفكار انذاعًح تذقح I can do mind mapping to create relevant ideas to the topic in question. أستطٍع أٌ أقٕو ترسى خرائط رٍُْح نخهق أفكار راخ عالقح تًٕضٕع انسؤال I keep checking grammar when I write. أتحقق يٍ ساليح قٕاعذ انهغح عُذيا أكتة I revise whatever I write and edit it. ِأٌقٕو تًراجعح كم يا أكتثّ ٔأحرر Cognitive strategy I can write without using the English-Arabic dictionary إَجهٍسي-ًأستطٍع انكتاتح تذٌٔ استخذاو قايٕش عرت I can write without asking my teacher /friends about word meanings. أستطٍع انكتاتح تذٌٔ سؤال يذرسً أٔ أصذقائً حٕل يعاًَ انكهًاخ I write sentences in Arabic first then I translate into English. أكتة انجًم تانهغح انعرتٍح أٔالً ثى أحٕنٓا إنى انهغح اإلَجهٍسٌح I can describe a place/an object/a friend in detail. ًأستطٍع أٌ أصف يكاَاً أٔ كائُاً أٔ صذٌقاً تانتفص I can use different words/images every time I refer to the same thing. ّأستطٍع أٌ استخذو كهًاخ ٔصٕر يختهفح فً كم يرج أٔد أٌ أشٍر فٍّ نهشًء َفس I can compare and contrast similarities and differences between different things. أستطٍع أٌ أقارٌ ٔأيٍس أٔجّ انشثّ ٔاالختالف تٍٍ األشٍاء انًختهفح I can provide sufficient examples to make my idea clear. أستطٍع تقذٌى أيثهح ٔافٍح نجعم فكرتً ٔاضحح I can give evidence on my argument in order to make myself clear. ًأستطٍع أٌ أقذو دنٍالً عهى حجتً يٍ أجم جعم َفسً أكثر ٔضٕحا I make sure that I write all the needed elements/components of a certain argument. ٍٍأتأكذ أًَُ أكتة كم انًكَٕاخ ٔانعُاصر انًطهٕتح نًٕضٕع يع I can summarize key topics/ideas precisely. أستطٍع أٌ أنخص انًٕاضٍع ٔاألفكار انرئٍسٍح تذقح I write redundant and trivial details. أكتة تفاصٍم غٍر يًٓح ٔزائذج عٍ انحاجح I write everything and word for word when I summarize. أكتة كم شً كهًح كهًح عُذيا أنخص Social affective strategy I can write without negotiating meaning with teacher/friends. أستطٍع أٌ أكتة تذٌٔ أٌ أستفسر حٕل انًعاًَ يٍ انًعهى أٔ األصذقاء I need to discuss the topic with my teacher first before commencing writing the outline. أحتاج نًُاقشح انًٕضٕع يع يعهًً أٔالً قثم انثذء تكتاتح انخطٕط انرئٍسٍح نهًٕضٕع 658 The Relation between Omani Students' Perceptions of the Writing Strategies Juma Busaidi & Dina Al-Jamal Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 Appendix 2 Writing Proficiency Test Name: Class: WRITING 1 [5 marks] Write a PARAGRAPH about a film called ‘Home Alone’. Use all the information in the box. Your paragraph should be correct and well-organized. Home Alone Star/Macaulay Culkin Child/protect house/thieves family/gone/Paris Produce/1990 write/John Hughes Suitable/12 and above direct/Chris Columbus Earn/$ 480 million comedy ……………………………………………………………………………………………………………………… ……………………………………………………………………………………………………………………… ……………….…………………………………………………………………………………………………… Marker A Name: Marker B Average Class: WRITING 2 [10 marks] Complete the following task. Write at least 75 words. Situation: Imagine that you have just received this e-mail from a friend. Write a reply. Hi, As you know, I have just moved to a new school in a different area. I’m worried about finding new friends. What should I do? Can you advise me? Your writing should be clear. ……………………………………………………………………………………………………………………… ……………………………………………………………………………………………………………………… ……………………………………………………………………………………………………………………… ……………………………………………………………………………………………………………………… ………………………………………………………………… Marker A SCORE 659 Marker B Average Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Pages 660-676) Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 Spelling Errors of Omani EFL Students Sheikha A. Al-Bereiki* & Abdo M. Al-Mekhlafi Ministry of Education, Sultanate of Oman Sultan Qaboos University, Sultanate of Oman Received: 30/3/2015 ___________________________________________ Revised: 5/5/2015 _____________________________________________ Accepted: 9/6/2015 Abstract: The purpose of this study was to explore the types of spelling errors students of grade ten make and to find out whether there were any significant differences between males and females with respect to the types of spelling errors made. The sample of the study included 90 grade ten students from four different schools in North Batinah. The researchers manipulated the use of a test that consisted of two questions: an oral dictation test of 70 words with a contextualizing sentence and a free writing task. The misspellings were classified into nine different types. The findings revealed that the most common spelling errors among Omani grade ten students were vowel substitution, then came vowel omission in the second place and consonant substitution in the third place. Male students omitted more vowels than female students while females made more true word errors than their male counterparts. In light of the findings, the study presents some recommendations and suggestions for further studies. Keywords: Spelling errors, errors, ESL/EFL, error analysis. األخطاء اإلمالئًة يف تعلم اللغة اإلجنلًزية عند الطالب العمانًني و عبده حممد املخاليف *شًخة بنت علٌ الربيكًة سلطنة عمان،جامعة الشلطان قابىس سلطنة عمان،وزارة الرتبًة والتعلًم _____________________________________________ ُ هدفت هره الدزاسُ إىل التعسف علِ أكجس األخطاء اإلمالئًُ شًىعا عند طالب الصف العاشس يف سلطن:مشتخلص ُ أما عًنُ الدزاس,ُ تلىن جمتنع الدزاسُ من مجًع طالب وطالبات الصف العاشس يف حمافظُ مشال الباطن.عنان ًُ وقد مت مجع البًانات عرب إدساء اختباز للطالب ملعسفُ أكجس األخطاء اإلمالئ,ُ طالب49 طالبا و41 فكد تلىنت من وتىصلت.َ كلنُ والجانٌ كتابُ حس70 األول اختباز إمالء شفىٍ ملىن من:شًىعا وذلم باإلدابُ علِ سؤالني :ًُالدزاسُ إىل النتائج التال أكجس األخطاء اإلمالئًُ شًىعا بني طالب الصف العاشس هى استبدال حسف أو حسوف العلُ و الجانٌ أكجس.1 .ُشًىعا هى حرف حسف أو حسوف العلُ والجالح أكجس شًىعا هى استبدال حسف أو حسوف ساكن تىدد فسوق ذات داللُ إحصائًُ بني الطالب الركىز واإلناخ يف نىعني من األخطاء اإلمالئًُ وهٌ حرف.2 حًح كان متىسط حرف حسف,ُحسف أو حسوف العلُ وكتابُ إمالء صحًح وللن لًص الللنُ املشتودف ُأو حسوف العلُ أكرب عند الطالب الركىز يف حني كان متىسط كتابُ إمالء صحًح وللن لًص بالللن .املشتودفُ أكرب عند الطالبات . حتلًل األخطاء،ًُ اللغُ اإلجنلًزيُ كلغُ أدنبًُ أو لغُ ثان، األخطاء،ًُ األخطاء اإلمالئ:ًُالللنات املفتاح *rayan3@gmail.com 660 Spelling Errors of Omani EFL Students Sheikha Al-Bereiki & Abdo Al-Mekhlafi Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 Literature review The process of learning a second or foreign language is challenging. Thus, ESL/EFL learners face difficulties with various aspects of the language including spelling. Spelling is considered as the starting point of the written language as indicated by Mahmoud (2013). Also, writing compositions demands a high level of spelling proficiency in order to produce a good piece of writing. Allred (1990) pointed out that spelling ability can influence the writer's effectiveness and creativity. Spelling and its related concepts Spelling is often defined as the process of recognizing and reproducing sounds of language into a sequence of letters in a written form or in an oral form (Santoro, Coyne & Simmons, 2006). It is also defined as “the grammar of letter sequences that generates permissible combinations without regard to sound” (Peters, 1985, p. 11-12). Here, the definition suggests that spelling is not a task of translating sounds into their corresponding letters in a direct sound to letter relationship. It is more complex than this. There are rules that govern letter combinations. Spelling is a challenging skill to master because it is made up of different layers of knowledge. The ability of encoding a sound or phoneme into the appropriate corresponding grapheme or spelling is complex to many learners of English. Likewise, Arab learners of English face various difficulties when learning English spelling. A large body of research analyzes students' errors as a way of gaining insight into how students learn and how to address their learning needs. For example, Brown (2000) encouraged systematic analysis of learners' errors in order to address their needs via designing appropriate curricula. A considerable amount of research has been devoted to analyze the patterns of spelling errors (AlHarrasi, 2012). These studies surveyed the errors in spelling and then classified them into distinct categories. Studies done in the Omani context have surveyed spelling difficulties at either early grade levels like grade five and six or at a tertiary level. To the researchers‟ best knowledge, no study about learners' spelling difficulties has been conducted on high school students. Therefore, this study aims to find out the most common spelling errors grade ten EFL students in Oman make. Spelling, as Moats (1984) indicated, is a multifaceted skill that depends upon several layers of knowledge; phonological awareness, morphological awareness, semantic knowledge and orthographic knowledge (cited in Santoro, Coyne & Simmons, 2006). Phonological awareness is the ability to identify sounds in spoken words as explained by Bourassa and Treiman (2009). It is the explicit knowledge of the sounds‟ system in the language (Santoro, Coyne & Simmons, 2006). Morphological awareness is defined by Bourassa and Treiman (2009) as the knowledge of relations among word forms and how they influence spelling. Semantic knowledge is “the knowledge of the effect of spelling on word meanings and vice versa” (Apel, 2008, p.15). Wasowicz (2007) explained morphological knowledge and semantics relationships as the knowledge that allows the individual to spell inflected words (that contain suffixes and provide information about time or quantity without changing the meaning, e.g. walk-walked, cat-cats) and derived forms (that contain suffixes or prefixes and can change the meaning and sometimes the class of words, e.g. friend-friendly). Bourassa and Treiman (2009) explained orthographic knowledge as the knowledge of legal and illegal letter sequences. It can also refer to, as indicated by Apel (2008), the use of spelling rules and patterns. It is also, according to Apel (2008), the knowledge that governs the alphabetical principles, for example, a /k/ sound can be k, c, cc, ch, ck, qu or x, and a (gh) can be pronounced as a /f/ or a /g/. It also covers knowledge of rules for combining letters, for Purpose of the study The purpose of this study is to analyze the spelling errors of grade ten EFL students in Oman. It also aims to find out whether there are any significant differences between male and female students with respect to the types of the spelling errors made. Study questions 1. What are the most common types of spelling errors made by Omani grade ten EFL students? 2. Is there a statistically significant difference between male and female students regarding the types of the spelling errors made? 661 Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Vol. 9 Issue 4 Oct.) instance, there is no letter string like (kr) in English. In spelling, the concern is in letter sequences not word sequences. 2015 spelling. Consequently, written production may fail to convey its intended message. Additionally, there is the question of courtesy which is connected to spelling as it is connected to speaking and writing. Educated people have to acquire the habit of being precise (Peters, 1985). Being precise requires being able to write what exactly conveys the meaning, not avoiding certain words for being difficult. What is more is that good spelling contributes effectively to a learner's self-concept. It gives any person the status and knowledge to communicate adequately and acceptably in writing (Peters, 1985). Significance of correct spelling The emphasis of spelling accuracy is closely related to its role in successful writing, effective communication, its link to courtesy and self-concept, its role in success at school and other educational pursuits, and finally its relation to success in society. Treiman and Kessler (2013) pointed out that learning to spell is important “because human attention is limited. Children who must devote a good deal of attention to spelling have fewer mental resources available for other aspects of writing” (p. 317). Thus, being a good speller reserves mental effort for other aspects of writing. McMurray (2006) explained that a speller with automatic spelling skills can produce a high standard piece of writing since s/he has more resources in working memory for other composition skills. Indicated by Kreiner, Schnakenberg, Green, Costello and McClin (2002), poor spelling denotes lower writing ability since spelling is a component of successful writing. Also, writing is seen as a permanent activity. What we write remains there for anybody to read, criticize, or notice any errors. One's writing reflects his/her education, reliability and even intelligence (Jennings, 1998; Stirling, 2011). Accurate spelling is of high importance and teachers have the responsibility for convincing learners that accurate spelling is mandatory for several parts in their exams. It is also required for job applications and businesses. Conversely, poor spelling reflects negatively on the individuals and companies that employ it (Warda, 2005). The last point has to do with international proficiency tests (e.g. TOEFL and IELTS). Test takers are penalized for spelling errors in these major scale tests. Students and job seekers are required to display a certain level of English proficiency reflected in TOEFL or IELTS. Thus, spell checkers are not available in circumstances where students and test takers have to demonstrate their knowledge of English (Cook, 1999). There are also reasons that concern communication. Spelling words correctly is crucial because spelling conveys meaning, grammar and intent; thus misspellings may interfere with comprehension and eventually may hinder communication between the writer and the reader (Apel, 2008; Jennings, 1998; Peters, 1985). A similar notion expressed by the Department of Education and Children‟s Services in Australia (DECS) (2011) illustrated that the purpose of spelling is for the writer to communicate the ideas clearly and properly and for the reader to understand the written message. Thus, poor spelling affects written communication negatively. Additionally, several researchers (Kohnen, Nickels & Castles, 2009; Peters, 1985; Stirling, 2011; Warda, 2005) pointed out that poor spelling capabilities affect the individual‟s choice of words in writing, forcing learners to avoid using certain words because they are not certain of their The definition of error Any learning experience is most likely to involve attempts of learners as trial and error which might end up with making errors. It is essential to make a distinction between mistakes and errors. According to Corder (1981, cited in Al-Jayousi, 2011), mistakes are incorrect instances that are a result of performance. They are said to be slips that learners can correct by themselves. However, errors are the incorrect outputs that are due to problems in the underlying knowledge of the learner. Here, the learner lacks the competence to rectify the inaccurate responses. Error analysis and its significance Error analysis has gained a considerable amount of attention by scholars. It is defined as “the process of determining the incidence, nature, causes and consequences of unsuccessful language” (James, 1998, p.1). 662 Spelling Errors of Omani EFL Students Sheikha Al-Bereiki & Abdo Al-Mekhlafi Learners' errors are significant for various instructional reasons. For example, studying learners' errors can throw some light on how learners process language and what kind of assistance they need. Wasowicz (2007) emphasized the notion that spelling errors can inform educators about the kind of linguistic knowledge individual learners possess. Also, studying learners‟ misspellings can inform educators about which stage of spelling development the learner is stuck at. This assessment, as indicated by Kohnen, Nickels and Castles (2009), can draw the training framework and the types of strategies needed for the learner in order to move to the next stage. It is emphasized by DECS (2011) that the teacher can best assist learners by analyzing their writing, looking for recurring patterns and discussing with them ways of successful learning. Kohnen, Nickels and Castles (2009) suggested identifying learners‟ spelling difficulties as early as possible in order to enhance spelling abilities and to facilitate text writing. Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 es. He classified misspellings according to their types into six categories; substitution, omission, addition, disordering, segmentation and unrecognizable. Misspellings were classified into three categories according to their likely causes: 1. irregularity of English (errors mostly caused by the non-phonetic nature of the English orthographic system), 2. mother tongue interference (which included errors that involve misuse of sounds like /p/,/v/ and /tʃ/, consonant clusters, confusion in using mirror letters: b/d, p/q and deletion of short vowels: writing svn for seven, and 3. lack of knowledge of spelling rules and their exceptions. Very few studies focused on other aspects of spelling errors such as gender differences. Allred (1990) investigated gender differences in spelling achievement of male and female students from grade one through grade six in the United States. He compared the spelling achievement of boys and girls on both a spelling test and a proofreading standardized test. The researchers found out that girls were better spellers than boys in all grade levels. Allred discussed possible reasons for the gender differences in spelling achievement; two of which were maturation and cultural expectations. Maturation favors girls over boys since girls mature earlier than boys. Identifying the types of spelling problems is of central importance in the process of spelling error analysis. Some research work has been devoted to analyze the patterns of spelling errors (Al-Harrasi, 2012) and findings of spelling errors' studies vary according to the aims of each study (Emery, 2005). Consequently, different studies used different systems for classifying the spelling errors. For example, Al-Jarf (2010) suggested classifying spelling problems into phonological and orthographic problems. Cook (1997) classified spelling errors into four types: the first type was omission and addition, the second was substitution, the third was transposition and the fourth was called sound-based errors. Other researchers such as Al-Harrasi (2012), Al-Jabri (2006), Al-Zuoud and Kabilan (2013) and Khan and Itoo (2012) followed the classification of spelling errors suggested by Cook (1997). Mahmoud (2013) classified spelling errors in his corpus into inter-lingual and intra-lingual. He justified his two-way classification for the purpose of finding implications for classroom instruction. Classifying spelling errors into different categories helps detect the sources of difficulties and would consequently facilitate the choice of the appropriate classroom instruction. Cook (1999) carried out a study with the purpose of looking at students' spelling errors with specific L1s. Cook's study looked at 1,498 errors made by eleven different groups of preuniversity students including Arab learners. The analysis revealed that ESL Arab learners substituted vowels (e.g. obundant for abundant), added 'epenthetic' vowels (e.g. punishement for punishment) and made phonological errors (e.g. manshed for mentioned). The researcher collected the errors from their writings. However, students' writings do not always reveal all types of errors students may make since topic familiarity is under question (Cramer, 1998). Students may avoid writing words if they are not certain of their spelling (Schonell, 1985). Arabs speak different dialects which are themselves quite distinct from Standard Arabic Language (Fender, 2008), yet Al-Jayousi (2011) classified spelling errors according to their types and to their likely caus- 663 Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Vol. 9 Issue 4 Oct.) Cook did not indicate which background those Arab students came from. 2015 In some cases, the percentage of passing in the vocabulary question was as low as 8%. A look at the analysis of writing questions showed that students scored low marks too. In some schools, the percentage of failure was higher than the percentage of passing. All in all, the anecdotal evidence of the analysis of 2012/2013 summative English language examination results uncovered the fact that students of grade ten face difficulties in spelling (Afra School 5-10; Hunain School 5-10; Jumanah School 5-10; Khadeejah School 5-10, personal communication, November 14th, 2013). But, what are the types of their spelling errors? A number of studies have analyzed learners' spelling errors conducted in the Omani context. A study done by Al-Harrasi (2012) on grade six female students utilized the spelling list suggested by Cook (1997). However, the words in Cook's spelling list are not normally covered in grade six syllabus. Kibel and Miles (1994) stated that words selected for spelling tests should be ones that students are familiar with. Al-Jabri (2006) conducted a study on grade five students who were taught Our World Through English (OWTE) syllabus in their second year of learning a foreign language and it revealed similar results to AlHarrasi's study. A third study was conducted with tertiary students in Dhofar University by Vaddapalli (2012). The researcher tested students' difficulties in spelling three-letter words containing one vowel for the purpose of highlighting spelling and auditory discrimination difficulties of students in Oman. He found that students misused most of the vowels and some of the consonants. For example, 95% of the students misspelled the word „rub‟ and instead they wrote rup, rap, rab, raue, rib. Studies done in the Omani context have surveyed spelling difficulties at either early grade levels such as grade four (Al-Yahyai, 2009), grade five (Al-Hassan, 2006; Al-Jabri, 2006) and grade six (Al-Harrasi,2012 ) or at a different educational system such as grade nine of general education (Al-Mezeini, 2009) or at a tertiary level (Emery, 2005; Mahmoud, 2013 Vaddapalli, 2012). To the researchers' best knowledge, no study about learners' spelling difficulties has been conducted on high school students in Oman. Expressed by Emery (2008), good spelling is necessary for secondary students simply because their “courses are geared towards passing written exams” (p.17). Therefore, studying grade ten students‟ spelling errors is critical for a number of reasons. Firstly, students are at a different age level from students being studied in previous research studies, and they have been learning English for almost ten years. Secondly, anecdotal evidence shows that tenth graders suffer from difficulties in their spellings. Thirdly, the tenth graders are assessed differently from all previous grade levels (25 marks out of the total 100 are allocated for writing and 5 marks are given for accurate spelling in a vocabulary question). In addition, grade ten marks the end of cycle two of basic education. So, students are expected to compose more complex texts displaying proficiency in different language skills including spelling (Ministry of Education, 1999). Thus, studying learners‟ spelling difficulties can help improve their writings (Al-Jabri, 2006) and their language proficiency. In light of the above discussion, there is a need to investigate spelling errors made by grade ten students. A study done by Al-Mezeini (2009) on grade nine learners of general education in Oman revealed that explicit teaching of spelling rules did not improve students' performance in spelling tests but it helped to improve their writing performance. The researcher manipulated the use of an oral dictation test and a free writing task in order to collect his data. In his study, sound-based spelling errors constituted the largest category for a percentage of 55. Cook (1999) found out that sound-based spelling errors came in third place with 6%. This supports the fact that ESL learners who have different L1s face different spelling difficulties due to the interference of their mother tongue. An obvious limitation to Al-Mezeini‟s study would be the kind of intervention he employed in his teaching of spelling rules. A preliminary field survey conducted by the researchers of the present study on the analysis of the academic year's (2012/2013) summative English language examination results of grade ten Basic Education has revealed some important findings. This survey study included four cycle two (5-10) schools. Students in grade ten scored very low marks in vocabulary questions that required accurate spelling. 664 Spelling Errors of Omani EFL Students Sheikha Al-Bereiki & Abdo Al-Mekhlafi Methodology words which they probably won't use in their writings. Thus topic-related vocabulary at the back of grade ten text books was not included in the dictation question. Population and sample The study population comprised all students in grade ten Basic Education in North Batinah Governorate. There were 280 male classes, 259 female classes and 5 coeducation classes in North Batinah Governorate. So, two male classes and two female classes were the random sample in this study. Table 1 displays the distribution of male and female students who took the test and their test papers were valid for analysis. Using words from frequency lists to assess students' progress in spelling has several advantages. Schlagal (2002) pointed out that words in these lists are sequenced according to their frequency of use. The words students learn are the ones that they will need in their writings. Secondly, the word frequency lists are ordered by level of difficulty. Easier words come first. A very interesting idea highlighted by Schlagal is that “the 4000 most frequently used words constitute nearly 98% of the vocabulary used by children and adults in and out of school” (2002, p.46). Thus, the underlying principle of the vocabulary selection for the oral spelling test should be frequency of use as well as familiarity with the words. Table 1 Distribution of male and female students who participated in the study Gender No. of students Male students 41 Female students 49 Total 90 Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 Therefore, in order to ensure familiarity of the selected vocabulary, the words were selected from previous grades (grade 5 to grade 9). Then, these words were checked against the Word Zone for 4,000 simple word families (“Text Project,” 2012) to ensure their frequency. Informal interviews with grade ten teachers were made to further ensure the familiarity and frequency of the selected words. The chosen words represented various aspects of the spelling system; regularly spelled words; e.g. got and hand, irregularly spelled words; e.g. daughter, homophones; e.g. their and wear, and morphologically derived words; e.g. education and entertainment. (See appendix A for the spelling words). Instruments An English test was given to grade ten students. It was designed in order to find out the most common types of spelling errors students of grade ten in North Batinah Governorate in Oman make. It was also intended to find out whether there were any significant differences between Omani male and female EFL students with respect to the types of spelling errors made. The English test had two questions: a dictation question and a free writing task. There were seventy words in the dictation test. There were also contextualizing sentences, a sentence for each spelling word. The words were selected on the basis of familiarity and frequency. According to a curriculum specialist in the Ministry of Education, vocabulary selection in grade ten of basic education and other grade levels is topic-related. Word lists at the back of the skills books (grades 8-10) and at the back of the class books (grades 5-7) include new vocabulary related to the topics of each unit and words that are commonly used (Al-Jardani, personal communication, January.8th.2014). Schlagal (2002) argued that content vocabulary lists are problematic in different ways. They are challenging, they contain lower frequency words and they do not reflect any orthographic patterning since they have been selected for the purpose of advancing ideas and not to teach orthography. Therefore, students will be learning the spellings of The second question was a free writing task. The students were asked to write about a given situation. Again, the researchers discussed the possible topics with some grade ten teachers and topic familiarity was taken into consideration. The primary version of the English test was validated by a panel of experts. The validation panel included two faculty members in the College of Arts at SQU, one faculty member in the College of Education at SQU, one curriculum officer in the Ministry of Education, two English language supervisors from North Batinah Governorate, one senior English teacher and an English teacher from North Batinah Governorate. 665 Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Vol. 9 Issue 4 Oct.) It included seventy words for the oral spelling question and three topics in the writing question for students to choose one and write about. The validators had to give their comments on the level of difficulty of the selected spelling words, the number of words, the clarity of the contextualizing sentences and the level of difficulty for the topics selected to choose from in writing. Feedback from the validity check panel included changing some of the contextualizing sentences for the sake of more clarity and limiting students to one topic in the writing question in order to save students' time and to keep them focused. Thus, the final English test was modified accordingly. 2015 2. For question one, the oral test, each instance was marked as either correctly, incorrectly spelled or no attempt. If the spelling of the word was incorrect, then the type of the error was identified. The spelling errors were classified under nine distinct categories. These categories were consonant omission, vowel omission, consonant insertion, vowel insertion, consonant substitution, vowel substitution, transposition, complex word errors and true word errors. It is noteworthy that substitution, omission, insertion and transposition were the most common classifications used in literature (AlJayousi, 2011). Also, the first seven categories were straightforward adaptations from Brooks, Gorman and Kendall (1993) for L1 children and Bebout (1985) for L2 adults (cited in Cook, 1999). The last two categories were adapted from Emery (2005). Below is a detailed description of the categories used to classify the spelling errors. In order to ensure the reliability level of the English test, the validated version of the test was piloted on two grade ten classes: 25 male students and 27 female students. These two classes were not included in the actual implementation of the test later on. The first question of the test was marked in terms of accuracy of spelling. Then, the internal consistency of the test was measured using Cronbach's alpha test in the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS). The result showed that the oral spelling test had a high level of consistency as Cronbach's alpha coefficient was r= 0.97. In order to ascertain the reliability of the scoring of the writing question, inter-rater reliability was calculated using Pearson's correlation coefficient between the ratings given by the researchers and those given by a senior English teacher. The correlation coefficient of the writing question was r= 0.93, which was a high level of inter-rater consistency. The following procedure was followed in administering and marking the test. A. Consonant omission refers to the dele- tion of one consonant letter or more from the target word. B. Vowel omission refers to the deletion of one vowel or more from the target word. C. Consonant insertion refers to the addition of one consonant letter or more in the target word. D. Vowel insertion refers to the addition of one vowel letter or more in the target word. 1. Students were firstly oriented with the test, its purpose and its questions. Then, an example was given in order to clarify the instructions for the first question. Then, the test administrator, the researchers of the present study, read the word once. Then, the word was repeated in the context of a sentence. Finally, the word was read twice. This procedure has been followed by Al-Yahyai (2009) among other researchers. Once the first question was completed, the test administrator discussed the writing topic with the students in order to generate ideas to write about. E. Consonant substitution refers to the replacement of a consonant letter or more for others. This also involves substituting a consonant digraph or tri-graph. F. Vowel substitution refers to the replacement of a vowel letter or more for others. This also involves substituting a vowel diagraph or a tri-graph. G. Transposition refers to reversing the order of a letter or some letters in a word. Transposition is treated as one category with disregard to the type of letter being mis-ordered. This includes mis-ordering of: 666 Spelling Errors of Omani EFL Students Sheikha Al-Bereiki & Abdo Al-Mekhlafi 1. one letter as in reversing the order of two correctly adjacent letters such as writing caemra for camera, 2. one letter as in reversing the order of two adjacent letters, one of them is not a correct letter in the word as in writing akmre for camera, 3. one letter as in reversing the order of two letters which are not adjacent to each other as in writing deegre for degree and bussines for business; this counts as one transposition, 4. two letters in the place of each other as in misplacing the e and the i as writing cimestry for chemistry; this counts as two transpositions, and 5. two letters in the place of two other letters as in bussnies for business; this counts as two transpositions. classification. The second rater was a senior English teacher. It was decided that one English test paper would be done by the second rater for the first test question since there were seventy words. Four test papers would be done for the free writing question since students wrote passages with different lengths. The level of agreement between the researchers and the second rater was .88 for the oral spelling question and .90 for the free writing question. Both correlation coefficients showed a high level of agreement indicating a high level of consistency in the classification of errors. M. A second review by the researchers was carried out to further ensure the consistency and accuracy of classification of errors. A few changes were made accordingly. N. A third table was created to include H. True word spelling error: Here, the the errors of both questions. target word is spelled as another word, e.g. writing live instead of leave. I. J. K. L. Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 O. The data in the three table were en- tered in the SPSS to run the appropriate statistical procedures in order to answer research questions one and two. Complex spelling error refers to the misspellings that have more than two types of spelling errors or the spelling is not readable or has no explanation, e.g. apaksi for education, or durt for their. Findings and Discussions The first question: What are the most common types of spelling errors made by Omani grade ten EFL students? The different types of misspellings were counted manually for every student and recorded in a table. Table 2 shows the sums and percentages of the different types of spelling errors for grade ten students in both test questions one and two. It is noteworthy that correct spellings and no attempts to spell the words were also recorded, yet they were not included in the tables of the types of spelling errors because they were not errors. For question two, the written text for each student was read twice. Misspellings were underlined and then classified. After that, different types of spelling errors were counted manually for every student and recorded in a table. The writing question differed from the oral spelling question in terms of the absence of two categories; correct spelling and no attempt, and that was due to the nature of the two questions. Table 2 Sums and percentages of different types of spelling errors for all students in test questions one and two Types of spelling errors Sum *Percentage Vowel substitution 2,470 24.58% Vowel omission 2,131 21.29% Consonant substitution 1,698 16.90% Complex spelling error 1,246 12.40% Consonant omission 1,039 10.34% True word error 535 5.32% Vowel insertion 524 5.21% Transposition 271 2.70% Consonant Insertion 136 1.35% Total 10,050 A second rater classified the spelling errors of a random sample from the test papers. This step was important because it ensured consistency of 667 Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Vol. 9 Issue 4 Oct.) 2015 Table 4 displays the sums and percentages of the different types of spelling errors made by grade ten students in the second question of the English test, the free writing task. There is a very similar order of the different types of spelling errors as being mostly made by the students to the order in question one and when the misspellings were combined in the two test questions. In table 4, vowel substitution and vowel omission came in the first and second places respectively, similar to tables 2 and 3. The least common type of spelling error was consonant insertion. However, true word spelling errors were in third place and consonant substitution came in fourth place. *out of the total number of errors Students made a large number of spelling errors. Over ten thousand spelling errors were made by ninety students and that shows how big the spelling problem is among this grade. Table 2 presents the types of spelling error committed by the students. Vowel substitution was the most common type of spelling error made by students. Vowel omission was the second most common spelling error. The third in the list of spelling problems was consonant substitution while complex spelling errors came in fourth place. Finally, consonant insertion was the least common typeof spelling error. For specific details of the types of spelling errors made by students, table 3 and table 4 displays the sums and percentages of the different types of spelling errors for all students in questions one and two of the test respectively. Noticeably, the order of the types of spelling errors as the most common and the least common is the same for question one of the test, question two of the test and when combining both questions one and two of the test. Similarities are apparent in the order of the types of the misspellings for the first and second and the least common. Differences occurred in the orders of the other types. A simple explanation for this is that the largest number of spelling errors were made in question one since students were asked to write certain words while in question two students had to use their own knowledge in order to generate their written compositions. Students did not write much in the second part of the English test, the free writing task, and thus the number of spelling errors found in students‟ text writing was not high compared to the spelling errors made in the oral spelling question. Table 3 Sums and percentages of the different types of spelling errors for all students in question one of the test Types of the spelling Percentage errors Sum Vowel Substitution 2,243 24.54% Vowel Omission 1,959 21.43% Consonant Substitution 1,595 17.45% Complex Spelling Error 1,214 13.28% Consonant Omission 941 10.29% Vowel Insertion 446 4.88% True Word Error 440 4.81% Transposition 186 2.03% Consonant Insertion 117 1.28% Total 9,141 Table 3 shows that the most common spelling error in question one of the test vowel substitution. Vowel omission types of errors were in second place. Consonant substitution spelling errors and complex spelling errors represented the third and fourth types of errors respectively. The least common spelling errors were consonant insertion. The findings in tables 2, 3 and 4 correspond with the findings of some previous studies. Discussion in this section uses table 2, where all the spelling errors from both test questions are included. Corresponding with the present study‟s findings, Emery (2005) found that vowel substitution was the most common type of spelling errors among all other types of errors that involved vowel letters. Also, Al-Jarf (2008) found out that substituting the vowel by another faulty vowel was the most common specific strategy used by university students in her study. Table 4 Sums and percentages of the different types of spelling errors for all students in question two of the test Types of spelling errors Sum Percentage Vowel Substitution 255 27.36% Vowel Omission 171 18.35% True Word Error 99 10.62% Consonant Substitution 96 10.30% Consonant Omission 86 9.23% Vowel Insertion 78 8.37% Complex Spelling Error 66 7.08% Transposition 62 6.65% Consonant Insertion 19 2.04% Total 932 Previous studies (Al-Harrasi, 2012; Al-Zuoud & Kabilan, 2013) showed that substituting the correct letter with another letter was the most common spelling erroramong their students involved in the studies; however, the research- 668 Spelling Errors of Omani EFL Students Sheikha Al-Bereiki & Abdo Al-Mekhlafi ers did not specify the types of letters that were most problematic; were they substituting the vowels or substituting the consonants? Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 fifth. Vowel insertion came in seventh place whereas consonant insertion was the least common type of misspellings. This suggests that vowels constitute particular difficulties for Arab learners of English. The literature supports this finding. Emery (2005) found out that vowel-related spelling errors constituted 83% among all the spelling errors in the corpus collected by the researcher compared to 17% consonant-only misspellings. Additionally, Cook (1999), in his analysis of the spelling errors made by L2 users of English from different L1s, indicated that Arab learners substituted vowels and added more vowels than other L2 users and the researcher attributed this to the linguistic differences between English and Arabic. A question to be raised: were there certain vowel that were more problematic than others? This is a question that may constitute a possible area of future research studies. Additionally, Al-Hassan (2006) found that consonant substitution was of particular difficulty to Omani grade five male students in his study. The researcher indicated that his learners had difficulty with mirror-shaped letters, e.g. b and d, but he did not include any percentages or means of all the error types in his analysis of misspellings. Al-Jabri (2006) found that substitution and omission spelling errors were the most common among his students. Yet, the researcher did not specify the types of letters. Also, the findings of Al-Jayousi‟s (2011) study revealed that spelling errors of Arab learners of English in the UAE were highly concentrated in substitution errors with as high as 50% while omission came in second place with 33%. On the other hand, the findings of the present research study were not in consistency with Al-Mezeini‟s (2009) study which he conducted with Omani grade nine male students. The researcher found that sound-based spelling errors were the most common spelling errors (55%) while substitution and omission were categorized in second place (35%). Obviously, Al-Mezeini followed a different categorization from the one followed in the present study since sound-based misspellings were not specified in the present study. Substituting the correct vowel with another vowel was found to be the most common type of misspelling among grade ten students. This finding might raise the concern about phonemic awareness in the vowel sounds students possess. Vaddapalli (2012), in his study about spelling and auditory discrimination difficulties students in Oman face, concluded that “lack of phonemic awareness of English sounds is one of the main reasons of spelling problems” (p.273 ). The second question: Is there a statistically significant difference between male and female students regarding the types of spelling errors made? This question was answered by comparing the types of spelling errors of male and female students. Table 5 displays t-test results for differences between males and females in mean spelling errors in question one and question two. The results presented in table 5 revealed that there were significant differences between male and female students with respect to two types of misspellings; vowel omission and true word errors. Male students omitted more vowels when spelling as opposed to female students. Similarly, more female students made true word errors compared to their male counterparts. Vowel related spelling errors seemed to be more problematic for Arab EFL learners of English than consonant related misspellings. According to table 2, vowel substitution came in first place while consonant substitution came in third place. Similarly, vowel omission was the second most common type of misspellings while consonant omission was the 669 Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Vol. 9 Issue 4 Oct.) 2015 Table 5 T-test results for differences between males and females in mean spelling errors in questions one and two of the test Types of spelling errors Gender Mean* SD. T Sig.(2-tailed) Consonant Omission Male 12.60 4.91 1.77 0.08 Female 10.88 4.33 Vowel Omission Male 27.27 10.65 3.23 0.00** Female 20.67 8.71 Consonant Insertion Male 2.18 1.39 0.11 0.92 Female 2.14 1.29 Vowel Insertion Male 5.29 3.53 1.49 0.14 Female 6.53 4.20 Consonant Substitution Male 20.80 9.70 1.91 0.06 Female 17.24 8.00 Vowel Substitution Male 27.22 10.57 0.18 0.86 Female 27.63 11.58 Transposition Male 3.61 2.64 0.46 0.65 Female 4.05 4.54 True Word Error Male 5.17 2.60 2.21 0.03** Female 6.59 3.36 Complex Spelling Error Male 16.33 10.73 1.59 0.12 Female 12.89 9.31 *The average of misspellings for each student ** The mean difference is significant at 0.05 level Vowel omission is a characteristic of a speller at the phonetic stage of spelling development. There were more male students at the phonetic stage than female students. This is quite alarming since phonetic spelling is an attribute of young learners‟ writing. Making true word errors shows that the students have good knowledge of permissible strings of letters and the constituents that are more likely to make correct words in English language. Students who make more true word errors can be said to know how a word looks right or have good orthographic knowledge; however, their semantic knowledge is poor. More exposure to the language through directed reading or extensive reading can help students develop good semantic knowledge. For specific de- tails, tables 6 and 7 represent the t-test results for the differences between males and females in mean spelling errors for question one only and question two only respectively. With reference to table 6, there were significant differences between males and females with respect to types of spelling errors in three categories; vowel omission, consonant substitution and true word errors. Table 6 reveals that male students made more vowel omission misspellings than their female counterparts. Also, they used the incorrect consonant in their spellings more frequently than female students. Similar to the findings in table 5, female students made more true word errors than male students. Table 6 T-test results for differences between males and females in mean spelling errors in question one only Types of spelling errors Gender Mean* SD. T Sig.(2-tailed) Consonant Omission Male 11.34 4.68 1.33 0.19 female 10.13 3.85 Vowel Omission Male 25.27 10.46 3.31 0.00** female 18.84 7.96 Consonant Insertion Male 1.79 1.32 -0.750.46 female 2.03 1.24 Vowel Insertion Male 4.65 3.15 -1.240.22 female 5.53 3.44 Consonant Substitution Male 20.15 9.68 2.43 0.02** female 15.69 7.23 Vowel Substitution Male 24.85 9.93 -0.060.95 female 24.98 9.52 Transposition Male 3.19 1.92 0.85 0.40 female 2.78 1.87 True Word Error Male 4.24 1.91 2.71 0.01** female 5.66 2.94 Complex Spelling Error Male 15.83 10.93 1.42 0.16 female 12.63 9.90 *The average number of misspellings for each student ** The mean difference is significant at 0.05 level 670 Spelling Errors of Omani EFL Students Sheikha Al-Bereiki & Abdo Al-Mekhlafi Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 Table 7 T-test results for differences between males and females in mean spelling errors in question two only Types of spelling errors Gender Mean SD. T Sig (2-tailed) Consonant Omission Male 1.74 1.05 1.23 0.20 Female 2.19 1.25 Vowel Omission Male 3.00 1.88 1.47 0.15 Female 2.37 1.57 Consonant Insertion Male 1.22 0.67 1.00 0.35 Female 1.00 0.00 Vowel Insertion Male 2.067 1.22 0.17 0.87 Female 2.14 1.28 Consonant Substitution Male 1.86 1.03 1.17 0.25 Female 2.33 1.35 Vowel Substitution Male 3.13 2.49 0.68 0.50 Female 3.51 2.38 Transposition Male 2.00 1.05 0.76 0.45 Female 1.75 0.79 True Word Error Male 2.33 2.11 1.42 0.17 Female 1.58 1.02 Complex Spelling Error Male 1.64 0.67 1.12 0.27 Female 2.09 1.24 their linguistic performance would most probably be better than boys of the same age. The difference between the different types of spelling errors with respect to gender is an area that needs further investigation. Table 7 shows that there were no significant differences between male students and female students with respect to the types of spelling errors found in students‟ text writing. Although there were mean differences between males and females in vowel omission spelling problem and true word errors, this difference was not significant at 0.05 level. A possible explanation to this finding is the nature of the two types of questions. In the writing question, students needed to bring in more language knowledge in order to compose their writing. The length of what students wrote varied from no writing to two pages. Many students could not write anything even though they were encouraged and assisted by the researchers of the present paper. Conclusion Spelling is a complex language skill and the findings from this research study uncovered the different difficulties students face with spelling. Vowel substitution, vowel omission and consonant substitution were the most common types of spelling difficulties Omani students in the present study face. Thus, mastering the spelling of the most frequent English words is something that needs time and a lot of effort. Spelling instruction in Arab EFL in general and in Omani classrooms in particular should be reviewed in order to provide the appropriate spelling instruction that corresponds with students‟ actual spelling needs. A change to alleviate the spelling problems is necessary. This change needs to combine the efforts of all change agents in education namely curriculum designers, supervisors, trainers and teachers. Previous spelling studies based on corpus analysis did not compare the types of misspellings made by male and female students in composition writing. Gender difference between the types of spelling errors was not the focus of previous studies. However, the comparison of spelling achievement between male and female learners was the focus of some spelling studies. An Omani study conducted by Al-Yahyai (2009) on grade four male and female students found out that girls were better spellers than their male counterparts in both the pretest and posttest. Allred (1991) studied the achievements of boys and girls from grade one through grade six in the USA and found out that girls were better spellers than boys. The researcher attributed the difference between the two genders to maturity and cultural expectations. According to Allred, girls mature earlier than boys, thus Recommendations In light of the findings of the present study, the following recommendations are made: 1. 671 Evaluating students‟ spelling proficiency particularly at the beginning of grade ten in order to measure students‟ actual achievements in spelling through the nine years of learning English. Subsequently, stakeholders at the curriculum department in the Ministry of Education Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Vol. 9 Issue 4 Oct.) are well informed about students‟ actual needs and thus can design the most appropriate intervention programs. 2. English language syllabus can include more effective spelling learning strategies for students to employ when learning spelling. 4. Further studies on spelling problems might consider investigating the misspellings of different grade levels. 5. Comparisons between the spelling performance of different grade levels, e.g. between grade five and grade ten, are recommended in order to examine whether students make different types of spelling errors as they progress through the years of learning English or not. 6. eam/123456789/7716/1/Listeningspelling%20strategies%20in%20EFL%20Ar ab%20college%20students.pdf Teachers need to be well trained to deal with students‟ spelling difficulties. Therefore, curriculum developers in coordination with training and supervision departments can survey teachers‟ training needs with respect to teaching and learning spelling. Then, in-service training needs that can correspond with students‟ actual needs can be designed and implemented. 3. 2015 Al-Jarf, R. (2010). Spelling error corpora in EFL. Sino-US English Teaching, 7(1), 6-15. Al-Jayousi, M.T. (2011). Spelling errors of Arab students: Types, causes and teachers' responses. Master Thesis. American University of Sharjah. Retrieved from https://dspace.aus.edu/xmlui/bitstream /handle/11073/2716/29.2322011.09%20Mohannad%20Al%20Jayousi.p df?sequence=1 Allred, R. A. (1990). Gender differences in spelling achievement in grades 1 through 6. Journal of Educational Research, 83(4), 187193. Al-Mezeini, H. (2009). Does teaching spelling rules make a difference?. In S. Borg (Ed.), Understanding English Language Teaching and Learning in Oman (pp. 124-131). Sultanate of Oman: Ministry of Education. Al-Yahyai, S. (2009). Spelling Difficulties in Grade 4. In S. Borg (Ed.),Understanding English Language Teaching and Learning in Oman (pp.132-139 ) Sultanate of Oman: Ministry of Education. Al-Zuoud, K. & Kabilan, M. (2013). Investigating Jordanian EFL students‟ spelling errors at tertiary level. International Journal of Linguistics, 5(3), 164-176. Testing the effectiveness of a teaching approach or a teaching/learning strategy for spelling is recommended in order to measure their effect on improving students‟ spelling proficiency. Anonymous, (2012). Text project. Retrieved from http://textproject.org/archive/resources /wordzones-for-4000-simple-wordfamilies/ References Al-Harrasi, K. (2012). The most common spelling errors among Omani learners. Arab World English Journal, 3(2), 96 –116. Apel, K. (2008). Using spelling instruction to improve literacy outcomes. [PowerPoint slides]. Retrieved from http://www.education.canterbury.ac.nz/ seminars/apel_handout09.pdf Al-Hassan, S. (2006). An alternative approach to teaching spelling in grade 5. In S. Borg (Ed.), Classroom Research in ELT in Oman (pp.81-87). Sultanate of Oman: Ministry of Education. Bourassa, D., & Treiman, R. (2009). Linguistic foundations of spelling development. In D. Wyse, R. Andrews, & J. Hoffman (Eds.), Routledge international handbook of English, language and literacy teaching (pp. 182–192). London: Routledge. Al-Jabri, F. (2006). Common English spelling difficulties of Omani learners. In S. Borg (Ed.), Classroom Research in ELT in Oman (pp.88-93). Sultanate of Oman: Ministry of Education. Brown, H. D. (2000). Principles of language learning and teaching (4th edition). White Plains, NY: Pearson Education. Al-Jarf, R. (2008). Listening Spelling Strategies in EFL Arab College Students. Retrieved from http://repository.ksu.edu.sa/jspui/bitstr 672 Spelling Errors of Omani EFL Students Sheikha Al-Bereiki & Abdo Al-Mekhlafi Cook, V. (1997). L2 users and English spelling, Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 18(6), 474-488. Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 113-150. doi: 10.1080/19404150902783450 Kreiner, D., Schnakenberg, S., Green, A., Costello, M. & McClin, A. (2002). Effects of spelling errors on the perception of writers. The Journal of General Psychology, 129 (1), 5-17. Cook, V. (1999). Teaching spelling. Retrieved from http://homepage.ntlworld.com/vivian.c/ Writings/Papers/TeachingSpelling.htm Mahmoud, A. (2013). Spelling errors of Arab learners of EFL: A two-way analysis. TESOL Arabia Perspectives, 20(1), 6-12. Cramer, R. L. (1998). The spelling connection: Integrating reading, writing and spelling instruction. New York: The Guilford Press. McMurray, S. (2006). Learning to spell: Raising standards in spelling and independent writing. Support for learning, 21, 100-107. Department of Education and Children‟s Services. (2011). Spelling: From beginnings to proficiency. The State of South Australia. Ministry of Education. (1999). English language: Curriculum framework. Sultanate of Oman: Ministry of Education. Emery, H. (2005). An investigation into the nature and causes of reading and spelling errors made by Arab ESL learners. (PhD thesis). The University of Wales, Cardiff. Nyamasyo, E. A. (1994). An analysis of the spelling errors in written English of Kenyan pre-university students. Language, Culture and Curriculum [Abstract]. 7(1), 7992. Retrieved from http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/1 0.1080/07908319409525167 Emery, H. (2008). Eyes and ears. English Teaching Professional, 55, 17-19. Fashola, O., Drum, P. & Mayer, R. (1996). A cognitive theory of orthographic transitioning: Predictable errors in how Spanishspeaking children spell English words. American Educational Research Journal, 33(4), 825-843. Peters, M. L. (1985). Spelling caught or taught: A new Look. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul plc. Fender, M. (2008). Spelling knowledge and reading development: Insights from Arab ESL learners. Reading in a Foreign Language, 20(1), 19-42. Santoro, L., Coyne, M. & Simmons, D. (2006). The reading-spelling connection: Developing and evaluating a beginning spelling intervention for children at risk of reading disability. Learning Disabilities Practice, 21(2), 122-133. James, C. (1998). Errors in Language Learning and Use: Exploring Error Analysis. London: Longman. Schlagal, B. (2002) Classroom spelling instruction: History, research, and practice. Reading Research and Instruction, 42(1), 4457. doi: Jennings, M. (1998). Individualize your spelling instruction! Preventing School Failure: Alternative Education for Children and Youth, 42(1), 44-45. doi: 10.1080/10459889809603168 Schonell, F. J. (1985). Essentials in teaching and testing spelling. London: Macmillan. Kharma, N. & Hajjaj, A. (1997). Errors in English among Arabic speakers: Analysis and remedy. Beirut: Librairie du Liban. Stirling, J. (2011). Teaching spelling to English language learners. Raleigh, USA: LULU. Kibel, M. & Miles, T. (1994). Phonological errors in the spelling of taught dyslexic children. Inc.Hulme. & M. Snoling (Eds.), Reading Development and Dyslexia, London: Whurr Publications. Tavosanis, M. (2007). A causal classification of orthography errors in web texts. Proceedings from AND 2007, 99–106. Retrieved from http://research.ihost.com/and2007/cd/P roceedings_files/p99.pdf Kohnen, S., Nickles, L. & Castles, A. (2009). Assessing spelling skills and strategies: A critique of available resources, Australian Journal of Learning Difficulties, 14(1), Treiman, R. & Kessler, B. (2013). Learning to use an alphabetic writing system. Language Learning and Development, 9, 317– 330. doi: 10.1080/15475441.2013.812016 673 Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Vol. 9 Issue 4 Oct.) Vaddapalli, M. (2012). Spelling and auditory discrimination difficulties of students in Oman: An analysis. Language in India, 12(1), 261-276. Warda, R. (2005). Research based tutoring of English spelling: white paper. eSpindle Learning Inc. Retrieved from http://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED49213 7.pdf Wasowicz, J. (2007). What do spelling errors tell us about language knowledge? Retrieved from: http://www.learningbydesign.com/uplo ads/ What_Do_Spelling_Errors_Tell_ Us_ Language_Knowledge.pdf 674 2015 Spelling Errors of Omani EFL Students Sheikha Al-Bereiki & Abdo Al-Mekhlafi Appendix A The Spelling Words 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. 21. 22. 23. 24. 25. 26. 27. 28. 29. 30. 31. 32. 33. 34. 35. 36. 37. 38. 39. 40. 41. 42. 43. 44. 45. 46. 47. 48. 49. 50. The word Angry Beach camera Degree Energy Forest got hand inside junk Keep Leave market Nine Orange paint Queen reduce salary team under very Water fox Yet zoo country view receive elephant because mountain serious Creature people daughter Should Climb Knife wrong Building Chocolate chemistry Machine laugh Language Would Knew wear right Sentence Don't drive when you are angry. I like walking on the beach. Take a photo with your camera. My brother has a master‟s degree. We can make solar energy from the sun. Many animals live in the forest. I've got a nice gift on my birthday. Eat with your right hand. Put the book inside the bag. You shouldn't eat junk food. People keep goats in their farms. You should leave the building if there is fire. I bought my new dress from a big market in Matrah. My sister is nine years old. An orange is a fruit. Paint the picture with different colors. A queen lives in a palace. We should reduce the amount of plastic we use. In some jobs, you get a high salary. Our football team won the match. There is a bag under the table. I like this book very much. Drink at least two litres of water every day. I saw a fox in the zoo. I haven't finished writing yet. There are many animals in the zoo. Oman is a beautiful country. Write your view about the topic. Did you receive my message? The elephant is a big animal. She won the race because she was the fastest. Al-Jabel Al-Akhdher is a famous mountain in Oman. If you have a serious problem, talk to your parents about it. A creature is an animal. More people live in cities than in villages. I have one daughter and two sons. You should see a doctor if you are sick. A monkey can climb a tree. You need to use a sharp knife to cut that fish. You don‟t get marks for wrong answers. Our school is a large building. Children like eating chocolate. He studies chemistry at the college. We need a big machine to dig the well. I always laugh at the jokes he makes. Arabic is a language of many countries. Would you get me a cup of coffee please? I knew she could do it. Muslims wear new clothes during Eid. Turn right at the roundabout and then go straight. 675 Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Vol. 9 Issue 4 Oct.) Appendix A The Spelling Words 51. 52. 53. 54. 55. 56. 57. 58. 59. 60. 61. 62. 63. 64. 65. 66. 67. 68. 69. 70. The word Buy their business education beautiful entertainment walked finished wanted Personality interesting Carefully helpless slowly centimeter unhappy using goats boxes wives Sentence Did you buy milk from the supermarket? Their son works in a big company. Some people have their own business. Education in Oman is improving. This is a beautiful flower. Nowadays, there are many activities one can do for entertainment. Yesterday, I walked for 5 kilometers. He finished the report last week. When I was a child I wanted to become a superman. Some people have a lovely personality. It is an interesting movie. You should drive carefully. You shouldn't be helpless when you face a problem. Turtles move slowly. This line is one centimeter long. I was unhappy about my marks. I like using the computer. Farmers usually keep goats and cows in their farms. There are three boxes of mangoes on the shelf. Wives are happy with the new law. 676 2015 Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Pages 677-690) Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 Omani Students’ Application of the Second Standard for Technology Coaches in Internship Program Ahmed Y. Abdelraheem* & Talal S. Amir Sultan Qaboos University, Sultanate of Oman ___________________________________________ Received: 5/6/2015 Revised: 5/9/2015 Accepted: 10/9/2015 _____________________________________________ Abstract: The aim of this study was to investigate the extent to which Omani students apply International Society for Technology in Education (ISTE) second standard for technology coaches and its relation with gender, training institution type, student’s specialization, GPA and cohort. A questionnaire of twenty three items to measure the components of the second standard was derived from these components and used. A sample of 171 students was used to collect data. Onsite supervisors were used to evaluate students’ application of the second standard. The results indicate that the overall performance of the students reflects a reasonable and acceptable performance but does not reach the target and desirable performance. Statistical analysis shows no significant differences due to gender, and cohort but there were significant differences due to students’ specialization, institution type and students’ GPA. The study concludes with some recommendations to improve the weak components and revise the procedures and processes of the internship program. Keywords: ISTE standards, internship, technology coaches, Omani students. تطبيق الطلبة العنانيني للنعيار الثاني ملدربي التكنولوجيا يف برنامج اإلحلاق املهين أمحد يوسف عبد الرحيه* و طالل شعبان عامر سلطنة عنان،جامعة السلطان قابوس _____________________________________________ ٍدفت ٍرِ الدزاسة إىل تكصٕ مدى تطبٔل الطلبة العناىٔني للنعٔاز الثاىٕ للجنعٔة العاملٔة للتهيْلْحٔا يف الرتبٔة:مستخلص مت.ملدزبٕ التهيْلْجٔا ّعالقة ذلو بيْع الطلبة ّىْع مؤسسة تدزٓبَه ّختصصَه ّمعدالتَه الرتاننٔة ّسية ختسجَه . طالبا ّطالبة171 ً ننا مت استخداو عٔية مهْىة م.ٕ فكسة أشتكت مً مهْىات املعٔاز الثاى23 ًاستخداو استباىة مهْىة م أشازت اليتائج إىل أٌ األداء العاو للطلبة ناٌ معكْال.ّنرلو مت استخداو املشسفني املكٔنني لتكٔٔه تطبٔل الطلبة هلرا املعٔاز ّقد أظَس التخلٔل اإلحصائٕ عدو ّجْد فسّم يف املتْسطات ىاجتة عً ىْع الطلبة.ّمكبْ ال ّلهيُ مل حيكل اهلدف املطلْب ّسية التخسج بٔينا أظَس فسّق ًا ىاجتة عً ختصص الطالب ّىْع مؤسسة التدزٓب ّاملعدل الرتاننٕ للطلبة ّاختتنت الدزاسة .بتْصٔات لتخسني مْاضع الضعف ّمساجعة اإلجساءات ّالطسم اخلاصة بربىامج اإلحلام املَين الطلبة، مدزبٕ التهيْلْجٔا، االحلام املَين، معآري اجلنعٔة العاملٔة للتهيْلْجٔا يف الرتبٔة:الهلنات املفتاحٔة .العناىٔني *ahmedyar@squ.edu.om 677 Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Vol. 9 Issue 4 Oct.) The history of student internships originated in the U.S. in the early twentieth century (Driscoll, 2006). According to Thiel and Hartley (1997) the university of Cincinnati's cooperative education program developed the first college‐endorsed internship program in the USA in 1906. The program was developed based on the premise that college students pursuing a professional program of studies needed to find a way to finance their education. The first framework of managing the practical experience element was adapted in the field of education where a major requirement of the teaching certification is practice. Internships are career-oriented curricular endeavors of practical application. Internships also address the issue of “lack of practical application” by giving students an active learning experience in the workplace. Students are able to develop the various applied workplace skills they will need to enable them to make a smooth transition from the classroom to the world of business. Feedback from the aggregated evaluations can be used to revise the curriculum in order to improve student performance and meet employers’ needs and expectations (Divine, Miller & Wilson, 2015). Internships are work-based learning experiences that relate to future professions. Students are placed as interns with a wide variety of sponsoring organizations based on their individual fields of interest. These organizations can serve internships in the arts, education, health, communications, business and industry, technology, and many other areas. Students are released from school for part of the school day or school year and work a variable number of required hours on a part-time basis for a designated period. Student interns receive on-the-job, one-on-one training in a work setting from skilled professionals, who provide the knowledge and expertise of their field. Students learn by doing in actual situations through direct, hands-on experiences. They are evaluated and assessed by both their school internship coordinator and their on-site professional supervisor or mentor using an authentic, competency- and performancebased model, portfolios, and exhibitions. Among the many positive educational outcomes of internships are practical experiences, new skills, and improved attitudes and behaviors (Merritt, 2008). 2015 service experience in which an individual has intentional learning goals and reflects actively on what she or he is learning throughout the experience or duration of attachment. Hendrikse (2013) indicated that the benefits of completing an internship include gaining valuable work experience, having an edge in the job market, an opportunity to decide if this is the right career choice and it is a valuable way to build confidence and gain experience. Amir & Ismail (2014) indicate that the internship program plays a significant role in developing the interns’ skills and makes them better prepared for a future career. They added that clear and achievable objectives, experienced site-supervisors, orientation, continuous evaluation and feedback throughout the internship period are major factors for a successful internship program. Most definitions of the concept of internships have been consistent, making it simple to explain the term. According to Furco (1996), internships are defined as programs that engage students in service activities primarily to provide them with hands-on experience that enhances their learning or understanding of issues relevant to a particular area of study. Meanwhile, McMahon and Quinn (1995) noted that internships are supervised work experiences whereby students leave their institutions and become engaged in work-related programs, during which period they are closely supervised by experienced job incumbents. Internships are therefore any carefully monitored piece of work or service experience in which an individual has intentional learning goals and reflects actively on what she or he is learning throughout the experience or duration of attachment. The internship program described in this study is designed for undergraduate students to gain work experience, that is, experience gained through the workplace as opposed to experiences students gain in lectures and classrooms. Thus, the term Work-Based Learning (WBL) will be broadly used to encompass these experiences, and the literature on WBL is also included to explore the importance of internship programs for undergraduates. Internships can be used as a pedagogical tool. Student interns are employed and receive on-thejob, one-on-one, practical training in hands-on learning experiences. They work with and learn from skilled professionals in a work setting, which provides them opportunities to Bukaliya (2012) pointed out that internships are any carefully monitored piece of work or 678 Omani Students’ Application of the Second Standard for Technology Coaches in Internship Program Ahmed Abdelraheem & Talal Amir associate with the people and the resources that can make work real (Littke, 2004). The job-site professionals provide interns with assignments and responsibilities that will allow them to serve as assistants. Students are exposed to workplace environments, norms of the workplace, work expectations, and obligations (Wynn, 2003). Students participate in meetings and get a feel for what work days are like in their field of interest. Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 will result in better coaching of teaching and learning. Swan & Dixon (2006) investigated the influence of a mentor-supported model of technology training on mathematics teachers’ attitudes and use of technology in the classroom. The treatment included six coaching sessions, informal focus groups, and mentorprovided support. The results indicated that mathematics teachers male and female participating in mentor-supported, professional development increased the amount and level of technology use in their practice. Teachers had a desire to learn about technology and understood it was important. Although the majority of higher education institutions offer internship programs for their students, the exact nature of each program may differ based on the aims and objectives set by the respective institutions. However, all internship programs are mainly formed to provide undergraduates with the opportunity to experience and gain practical knowledge in authentic, professional environments before they graduate. In general, internship programs attempt to merge students’ learning gained in a campus-based environment with a real-work environment. Thus, terminologies used to describe this relationship between learning and work become important. Terms such as workrelated learning, workplace learning, and work-based learning (WBL) have been used to discuss and describe internship programs. However, the similarities and differences of these terms are not entirely clear (Streumer & Kho, 2006). In a study by Holahan, Jurkat, and Friedman (2000), 34 teachers from 33 New Jersey schools were trained not only to use new technology but also to serve as coaches of other teachers at their home schools. The results showed that a mentor-based teaching model permitted greater efficiency as compared to traditional training approaches. The program emphasized mutual sharing, learning, and collaboration versus superior-subordinate relationships between the mentors and those who attended training. Felicen, Rasa, Sumanga & Buted (2014) utilized the descriptive qualitative design using 50 percent of the interns of second semester 2012-2013 as participants in the area of food and beverage and travel agencies. Results of the study revealed that interns have a satisfactory level of academic performance. In training performance they were rated as very good in terms of knowledge, skills, attitude and personality. Their study also revealed that there is no significant relationship between academic performance and training performance. Martinez, et al (2014) found that Mass Communication students obtained an excellent performance rating in their On-the-Job Training with a high academic performance rating in media marketing and average in advertising principle. Students with high academic performance in Advertising Principle also obtained high training performance except in adherence. Yeswa, Okaka, Mutsotso, Odera & Mumbo (2012) conducted a study in Kenya to assess whether the contextual factors influence the relationship between internship programs and performance of public healthcare institutions. They found a significant positive relationship between contextual factors and internship programs in public healthcare institutions (r=0.501; P <0.05). Barron, Dawson, & Yendol-Hoppey (2009) surveyed a Microsoft program workshop in Florida. The Survey results revealed that many coaches did not perceive that computers changed the role of the classroom teacher. This view suggests a lack of deep understanding about technology integration. They suggested that the program could be substantially improved by helping the coaches think more deeply about technology integration. In addition, most of the coaches and facilitators who attended the workshops were enthusiastic about the peer coaching concept and had positive attitudes about the integration of technology. However, the perpetual issues of adequate time and resources for the implementation of peer coaching and the integration of technology in K-12 classrooms were recurring themes. Bradshaw (2002) concludes his study with recommendations for strengthening technology initiatives to increase the likelihood that they 679 Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Vol. 9 Issue 4 Oct.) Walo (2001) assessed students’ perceptions of their level of management competence, before and after the internship component of their degree program. A self assessment instrument utilizing the management competencies within the competing values was used for the empirical stages of the study. The research implies that the internship program has proved effective in contributing towards the development of management competencies for this cohort of students. The study provides valuable insight into the relationship between internship and the development of students’ management competencies and highlights the need for further research in this area. 2015 number of empirical studies that investigated that influence on the perceived value of internship appear to be inconsistent. Therefore, there is a need for more studies that empirically investigate the influence of the individual characteristics on the perceived value of internships in order to enhance our understanding of such influence and help design effective internship programs. The above studies outline the value and variety of the benefits enjoyed by those students participating in internships, including a better understanding and knowledge of the tasks and practices performed by industry professionals, improved self‐confidence, enhanced employment and professional growth opportunities, the ability to network within the industry by creating personal contacts, exposure to management activities, and the development of skills relevant to their particular career choice. Furthermore, internships provide an opportunity for students to apply classroom theories to practical issues in the actual business setting, and most importantly to evaluate whether their career choice is compatible with their interests and personality. These studies did not touch directly the topic investigated by this study, namely the International Society for Technology in Education (ISTE) standards application. Regarding students’ academic performance and success on internship, Callanan and Benzing (2004) posit that there is evidence that students with higher GPAs are more likely to pursue an internship, although it is unclear whether or not they benefit to a greater extent than do students with lower GPAs. Kim, Kim, and Bzullak (2012) found 2.5 or higher to be the most popular GPA prerequisite for internship in accredited schools. Hergert (2011) found a strong correlation between the perceived value of the internship and the student's age and GPA, as older students with higher GPAs had higher perceived values of business internship than did younger students with lower GPAs at a large, public, USA university. On the other hand, Gault et al. (2000) found that major area of study, GPA, and gender were not correlated with the extrinsic measures of career success for the undergraduate business alumni. Hayes (1981) found no relationship between a student's GPA and the completion of required work experience. Also, Casado (1991) suggested that GPA is not a predictor of success or achievement in a hospitality curriculum. In terms of program preparedness Beard and Morton (1999) found academic preparedness to correlate with successful outcomes of a mass communication internship program. Cannon and Arrnold (1998), however, found that students with lower GPAs believed more strongly that an internship in marketing should lead to a full-time job. Recently, Sultan Qaboos University (SQU), the national university in the Sultanate of Oman, invested a considerable amount of money to purchase educational technology software and hardware (WebCT, blackboard learning systems, computer labs, and computers at every teaching room with different projectors) that will be used in teaching and learning. This development is accompanied by comprehensive revisions of the curriculum of the College of Education to obtain the accreditation of the National Council for Accrediting Teachers' Education (NCATE), which is changed to Council for Accrediting Educators’ Programs (CAEP). All these efforts are expected to increase the productivity of the instructional process and the overall educational outputs of the university. Students are supposed to use these benefits and technologies in the learning process. Simply having the technology resources in the school does not necessarily mean that the staff will use them in their teaching. Educational planners wish to increase the use of technology by students when they leave school and enter the work life. Although the literature is rich with studies that investigated and assessed the value of internships in different settings, few studies empirically investigated the influence that individual characteristics could have on such a value. Furthermore, the findings of the limited 680 Omani Students’ Application of the Second Standard for Technology Coaches in Internship Program Ahmed Abdelraheem & Talal Amir The Instructional and Learning Technologies Department (ILTD) in the College of Education, SQU, was established in 2005. The department strives to achieve excellence in its provision of teaching, research, and societal services in the field of instructional and learning technologies. Currently, the department is offering B.Ed in Instructional and Learning Technologies to meet the potential need for information technology teachers and learning resources centers at both basic education and general education levels in the Sultanate of Oman. The department program is undergoing an accreditation process by CAEP by the use of the ISTE standards for coaches. Importance of the study The importance of this study stems from the nature of the topic under study. It is the first study in this area to the best of the researcher’s knowledge and there are no previous studies for the variables considered for this study. Coaching support for teachers is a powerful means of both modeling and harnessing the potential of technology to improve teaching and learning. Teachers who receive coaching in the use of technology tools to improve student learning, and who learn from and collaborate with peers via professional learning communities, will develop confidence and effectiveness in designing and supporting technology-rich environments that maximize student learning. Therefore, the importance of this study could be viewed as follows: ISTE is a premier, nonprofit organization that serves educators and education leaders committed to empowering connected learners in a connected world. ISTE has five sets of standards for each individual who wants to work with technology. These standards are as follows: ISTE Standards for Students ISTE Standards for Teachers ISTE Standards for Administrators ISTE Standards for Coaches ISTE Standards for Computer Science Educators Our emphasis in this study is on ISTE standards for coaches. These standards have eight dimensions, and specifically our focus is on the second standard. This second standard focuses on teaching, learning, and assessing technology. In this standard, technology coaches assist teachers in using technology effectively for assessing students’ learning, differentiating instructions, and providing rigorous, relevant, and engaging learning experiences for all learners. Technology coaches help bridge the gap from where we are to where we need to be. The ISTE Standards for coaches describe the skills and knowledge they need to support their peers in becoming digital age educators. Internship at the ILTD of the College of Education, SQU, started in the summer of 2008 with 18 students distributed among 6 government and private sector institutions. By the end of the summer 2014, the total number of students who were offered the internship opportunity increased to 211. The internship institutions reached 38 institutions. The main objective of this program is to provide students hands-on experience that relates to their future positions. The nature of the internship of ILTD is slightly different from the normal internship program. In this internship program, the students play two roles. They become trainees at the beginning of the program, and they become trainers at the second part of the internship period, which is two months. They play double roles. Students’ evaluation was based on on-site professional supervisors’ report, students’ daily report, and the university faculty member who administers the program. The work of the intern is an integral part of the student’s course of study. No grades are provided for this internship, but it is a required course for student graduation. Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 681 It will cast light on the internship program by showing how technology coaches practice their roles at the internship institutions. The findings of this study will be useful for accreditation process by providing useful data on the application of the second standard for technology coaches. ILTD will benefit from this study in determining the weaknesses and strengths of the preparation program and take action where necessary. The findings of this study will help in showing if there are any differences in internship institutions, students’ cohort, Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Vol. 9 Issue 4 Oct.) 2015 cohort is a very old-fashioned word, little used these days. I’m not sure what you mean by this word. Maybe group? Intake? and gender and how these differences could be treated. Statement of the Problem Internships have been hailed for integrating classroom education with practical experience in enabling graduates to develop their professional knowledge and professional skills (Beard, 1998). However, unlike the conventional system and owing to a diversity of factors in an educational setting, the concept has encountered challenges. The reason for evaluating internship is to determine its effectiveness. When the evaluation is done, we can hope that the results are positive and gratifying, both for those responsible for the program and for upper-level managers who will make decisions based on their evaluation of the program. Therefore, much thought and planning need to be given to the program itself to make sure that it is effective. The internship program at ILTD has never been evaluated in terms of ISTE standard since its establishment in 2008. This is the first time for this program to be evaluated according to the ISTE second standards for technology coaches (teaching, learning and assessment). As mentioned before, ILTD program is currently undergoing an accreditation process using ISTE standard as SPA (ISTE Standards for Coaches). Therefore, there is a need for this study especially at this time. The study might cast light on the current practice of the internship program in the second standard. This standard is central to the program since the graduates of this program will work in the future as information technology teachers or learning resources specialists. In both positions they are required to coach teachers at school in technology utilization. Therefore the study focuses on uncovering the extent to which the students apply the ISTE second standard for coaches (teaching, learning, and assessments of technology). Having these concerns in mind, this study seeks to answer the following research questions: Instrument The ISTE second standard for technology coaches (Appendix 1 ) consists of eight elements (components), which have been broken down into twenty-three Liker-type items of five points (strongly agree to strongly disagree) to make assessments easier. They constitute the questionnaire (contact the authors for the questionnaire). The questionnaire was given to a panel of faculty members who specialized in educational technology to assess its face validity. They suggested minor revisions, and the researchers fixed them. The reliability coefficient of the instrument was found to be 0.89 based on Cronbach’s alpha. The instrument also contains demographic information (gender, specialty, year of internship, and type of institution) about the students and their supervisors who participated in the study. Thereafter, the questionnaire was distributed to the population of the study to respond to its items. The researcher contacted the population through e-mail and telephone calls to urge them to respond to the questionnaire. Collecting the data from the population took two months. Population The population of this study consists of 171 student interns (50 male and 121 female), which is the total number for the last four years (2011 to 2014), and 53 on-site supervisors (35 male and 18 female). A total of 140 students obtained their internship at training institutions supervised by 39 supervisors, and 31 obtained their training at educational institutions supervised by 14 supervisors. Results and Discussion The means and standard deviations of the first question, namely, “To what extent do student interns apply ISTE second standard for technology coaches in internship institutions as viewed by their supervisor?” were calculated, as shown in Table 1: 1. To what extent do student interns apply the ISTE second standard for technology coaches in internship institutions as viewed by their supervisor? 2. Do student interns’ applications of the second standard vary according to student interns’ gender, type of internship institution, academic specialization, GPA, and internship year (cohort)? NB: Table 1 shows that the first element, element (a), has the highest mean (4.13), which means that the supervisors believe that the student interns did well in this element. This result could be attributed to the number of courses 682 Omani Students’ Application of the Second Standard for Technology Coaches in Internship Program Ahmed Abdelraheem & Talal Amir (four courses) that deal with content and student technology standards in their degree plan. Element (h), which deals with research skills, came at second rank. This result could be explained by the fact that these students have been exposed to many courses that developed their research skills inside and outside the department. The elements with relatively low means are (d) and (f). Element (f) deals with instructional design skills, and this element has only two courses in the degree plan. Element (d) deals with creativity, higherorder thinking skills and processes, and mental habits. This element has no specific courses and is distributed over the whole program. Table 2 shows that the overall performance of the students in the second ISTE standard for technology coaches has a mean value of 3.88, which reflects a reasonable and accepted performance but not the target and desirable performance which is 5 points as indicated in the questionnaire. The ILTD program also requires more revisions to fix the weak areas Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 and reach the target performance. More work should be done in Coaching teachers in and modeling design and implementation of technology-enhanced learning experiences emphasizing creativity, higher-order thinking skills and processes, and mental habits (e.g., critical thinking, metacognition, and self-regulation.) Also an emphasis should be given to coach teachers in and model incorporation of research-based best practices in instructional design when planning technology-enhanced learning experiences. To answer the second question which states: “Do student interns’ applications of the second standard vary according to student interns’ gender, type of internship institution, academic specialization, GPA, and internship year?”, t-test was performed for the three variables of the second question, namely, gender, type of internship institution, and academic specialization. The results are shown in Table 2. Table 1 Means and standard deviations of students’ responses Elements of the second ISTE standard for technology for coaches a. Coach teachers in and model design and implementation of technology-enhanced learning experiences addressing content and student technology standards. h. Coach teachers in and model effective use of technology tools and resources to systematically collect and analyze student achievement data, interpret results, and communicate findings to improve instructional practice and maximize students’ learning. c. Coach teachers in and model engagement of students in local and global interdisciplinary units in which technology helps students assume professional roles, research real-world problems, collaborate with others, and produce products that are meaningful and useful to a wide audience. e. Coach teachers in and model design and implementation of technology-enhanced learning experiences using differentiation, including adjusting content, process, product, and learning environment based upon student readiness levels, learning styles, interests, and personal goals. g. Coach teachers in and model effective use of technology tools and resources to continuously assess student learning and technology literacy by applying a rich variety of formative and summative assessments aligned with content and student technology standards. b. Coach teachers in and model design and implementation of technology-enhanced learning experiences using a variety of research-based, learner-centered instructional strategies and assessment tools to address the diverse needs and interests of all students. d. Coach teachers in and model design and implementation of technology-enhanced learning experiences emphasizing creativity, higher-order thinking skills and processes, and mental habits (e.g., critical thinking, metacognition, and self-regulation). f. Coach teachers in and model incorporation of research-based best practices in instructional design when planning technology-enhanced learning experiences. Total 683 N 171 Mean 4.13 Std. .65 171 4.01 .87 171 3.96 .79 171 3.87 .69 171 3.83 .77 171 3.80 .80 171 3.77 .68 171 3.67 .73 3.88 .46 Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Vol. 9 Issue 4 Oct.) Table 2 T-test for differences in means caused by gender, type of institutions and academic specialization Variables N Mean SD. T df Male 50 4.0137 .45825 .756 169 Female 121 3.9537 .47643 Educational 31 4.1795 .52219 2,776 169 Training 140 3.9252 .44747 IT teachers 145 3.9400 47480 -2.069 169 LC specialists 26 4.1455 .41283 2015 Sig. .450 .006 .040 means that contextual factors influence internship program in public training institutions and facilitate the use of internship program to achieve the intended objectives. This result could be justified by the fact that in educational institutions, students had the chance to show and apply their skills in the application of the ISTE standard more than their fellows in other institutions. This result could also be justified by the fact that the nature of the educational programs at educational institutions focuses on instructional topics, such as designing learning environments, instructional design, electronic management systems, and digital photography. These topics are fully covered in their study plan, whereas at noneducational institutions, the concentration is on topics such as special software serving their needs, networking, human resource development, and other systems for managing companies and institutions. Table 3 shows that no significant differences exist in the means between the male and the female (t169=.756, p >0.05) (mean = 4.01 for male and mean = 3.95 for female). This result means that female and male students apply the ISTE standards for teaching, learning, and assessing technology in a similar way without differences. This result could be justified by the fact that both of them received the same training in the Department of Instructional and Learning Technologies. This result agrees with Gault, Redingtion, & Schlager (2000). They found that gender was not correlated with the extrinsic measures of career success for the undergraduate business alumni. This result is inconsistent with that of Ju, Emenheiser, Clayton, & Reynolds (1998), who found that gender was an important factor that influenced students’ perception of their internship experiences. Males were more satisfied with their internship experiences than females. Males tended to have a stronger resolve to work in the hospitality industry than females. They added that in Korea, it may be very difficult for female college graduates to be offered management positions by hospitality and other companies. Many female hospitality management graduates are employed as entrylevel employees even when they exhibit greater potential than male graduates. Concerning the comparison between students’ track of study, IT teacher against learning center specialists in applying ISTE standards for teaching, learning, and assessing technology in internship program. Table 3 shows a significant difference in the application of ISTE standards ( t169=-2.06, p<0.05) between learning center specialists (mean = 4.1455) and instructional technology teachers (mean = 3.9400). This result disagrees with Parsa, Aghazadeh, Nejatisafa, Amini, Mohammadi, Mostafazadeh, & Moghaddam, (2010). They found that there was no significant difference between pediatrics, gynecology, psychiatry, and general practice among the two groups in medical internship program. Also, it disagrees with Gault et al. (2000) who found that major area of study, was not correlated with the extrinsic measures of career success for the undergraduate business alumni. This result could be justified by the fact that learning center specialists have more in technology courses, such as networking, multimedia, and more hands-on laboratory work. They usually take their internship in educational institutions that meet their interests. Concerning the comparison between the training institution, Table 3 shows significant differences in means between institution type ( t169=2.77, p<0.05) (mean = 4.17 for educational institution and mean = 3.92 for training institution), which means that students who were offered the opportunity to undertake their internship at educational institutions showed greater application of the ISTE standard for teaching, learning, and assessing technology than their fellows who obtained their training at non-educational institutions. This result agrees with the findings of Yeswa, Okaka, Mutsotso, Odera, & Mumbo (2012). They found a significant positive relationship between contextual factors and internship program in public healthcare institutions. This 684 Omani Students’ Application of the Second Standard for Technology Coaches in Internship Program Ahmed Abdelraheem & Talal Amir To determine the differences in means between students’ GPA and differences in students’ cohort, the ANOVA test was used. Table 3 shows the results of the differences in means caused by the GPA variable. Table 3 ANOVA test for students’ GPA variable Source SS df MS F Between 5.466 2 2.733 14.263 Groups Within 32.189 168 .192 Groups Total 37.655 170 Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 age- and low-GPA students). The results also show that the applications of the ISTE standard for teaching, learning, and assessing technology of students with average GPA were found to be significantly greater than those of low GPA students. These results could be justified by the fact that during the program of study, high and average GPA students usually exerted more efforts on their studies, which were reflected in the internship program performance. Those students usually reflect on their performance and apply ISTE standards the way they should be applied. This implies that those students with high final grades in courses have also high performance on job training. Sig. .000 Grade point average in particular is important to examine for students participating in internships, due to both its link to retention and the link between retention and gainful employment. While the rationale for internships is focused on career-related outcomes, GPA plays an essential role in the link between internship participation and the job market. Table 3 shows that a significant difference exists in means between the students’ application of ISTE standard for teaching, learning, and assessing technology caused by their GPA ( F2,168 =14.26 p < 0.05). The result agrees with Martinez et al, (2014) who found that students with high academic performance in advertising principle also obtained high training performance except in adherence to company policy. In addition, it agrees with Hergert (2011) and Callanan and Benzing (2004). But it disagrees with the findngs of Felicen, Rasa, Sumanga & Buted (2014), Buted, Felicen, & Manzano (2014), Casado (1991) and Hayes (1981) who found that there is no significant relationship between academic performance and training performance. Also, this study disagrees with Coutinho (2007) who found no relationship between performance goals and GPA. GPA might be one measure of success, but does not necessarily embody determination or career success as found by Beard and Morton (1999). Although Beard and Morton found that GPA was a less important predictor of internship success, Bacon (2006) found that GPA was a valuable indicator of job success. Table 4 Scheffe’s multiple comparisons (I) GPA (J) GPA Std. Sig. mean mean error 1.00 2.00 3.00 2.00 3.00 1.00 3.00 1.00 2.00 .13680 .13138 -.13680 .18272 -.13138 -.18272 .000 .010 .000 .002 .010 .002 In table 4, 1.00 stands for students with low GPA, 2.0 for average GPA, and 3.00 for high GPA. Comparisons between cohorts help examine trends and provide historical context. To determine the differences caused by internship year (cohort), ANOVA was used. Table 5 shows that no significant difference existed in the cohort variable ( F2,50 =2.267 p >0.05). Table 5 ANOVA test for the cohort variable Source SS df MS F Sig. Between 1.391 2 .696 2.607 .084 Groups Within 13.341 50 .267 Groups Total 14.732 52 This result indicated that students’ performance in the internship program was stable and consistent. This result could be attributed to the fact that in the last three years, students went to the same institutions that offer the same internship programs. The improvement of students’ performance in these programs was minor. This result shows a need to revise the internship program because the natural thing for this program is to improve over the years, not to stay stable. This finding disagrees with Frenette’s (2015) findings in which he In order to know which group of students perform better than others, Scheffe’s multiple comparisons were used. Table 4 shows that the applications of the standard for teaching, learning, and assessing technology of students with high GPA were found to be significantly greater than those of their counterparts (aver- 685 Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Vol. 9 Issue 4 Oct.) mentioned that a considerable rise in the prevalence and improvement of internships over years among arts graduates; in particular, that paid and unpaid internships were increasing at an equally common rate until a decade ago, when unpaid internships began growing more substantially. Among the many potential reasons for this shift is a greater demand for internships by students attempting to launch their careers during a recession. The rise in demand is also likely linked to demographic changes, especially the larger number of college-age youth. Also, the result of this study disagrees with Khalil (2015) who found that the year of the internship influenced the interns' satisfaction. Since technology is moving faster and faster, the ILTD needs to with the advancement in technology and incorporates that in the program curriculum in order to increase its effectiveness over time. Improvement in the internship program requires careful monitoring and selection of training institutions that guide students effectively. Also, students who enter the program should be selected carefully after sufficient screening and that might invite a good cohort who makes a difference. As with any worthwhile experience, developing a successful internship experience requires thought and planning. It could be beneficial if we help interns to be an integral part of the training institution from the very beginning. Providing professional networking opportunities to help a new cohort gain additional insights might increase their future performance in the internship. 2015 financial considerations. Internships offer students a chance to gain a deeper understanding of one or more specialties within their chosen profession through actual work experience. As students move through their academics, classes expose them to new knowledge which stimulates their interest. They take more upper-division classes and build a skill set and some expertise within their major. This interest then manifests itself as a career option. In order to gain a better understanding of what this career option is like in reality, undergraduates participate in internship programs through their college or university. This study attempted to answer the following questions: (1) To what extent do student interns apply the second standard of ISTE for technology coaches in internship institutions as viewed by their supervisor? And (2) Do student interns’ application of the second standard vary according to student interns’ gender, type of internship institution, academic specialization, GPA, and internship year. The findings of this study show that the overall performance of the students in the second ISTE standard for technology coaches has a mean value of 3.88 out of 5.00, which reflects a reasonable and acceptable performance but not the target and desirable performance. No significant difference was observed in the application of the ISTE standard caused by gender and cohort. This study also reveals the following significant differences: type of internship institution in favor of educational institution, student GPA in favor of high GPA and average GPA in favor low GPA, and students’ academic specialization in favor of learning center specialists. Based on these findings, a number of salient implications can be obtained: Conclusion and Implications Internship practices will continue to play a significant role in educational degree programs. Educational institutions throughout the world must strive to develop a unique, triangular partnership between students, the industry and educational institutions, forming a relationship based on intrinsic motivators and the common interests of the industry. The aim should be for an innovative, educational experience that would best fit the personality traits of individuals and, as a result, committing them to the values governing the modern technology era. The challenge for all stakeholders is to further commit to the practice and embrace new and innovative approaches that can greatly improve the development of the next digital generation; a generation that will depend more on human relations and technological skills rather than impersonal, To reach the target performance ( 5/5) in the application of the second standard of ISTE for technology coaches, ILTD should place more courses on two elements: (f), which deal with instructional design skills and have only two courses in the degree plan, and (d), which deal with creativity, higher-order thinking skills and processes, and mental habits and which have no specific courses and are distributed over the whole program. Students’ performance in the internship during three years shows no improvement, and this result means there is a 686 Omani Students’ Application of the Second Standard for Technology Coaches in Internship Program Ahmed Abdelraheem & Talal Amir need to revise the internship procedures and processes for the purposes of improvement. Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 perience. Journalism and Mass Communication Educator, 53(4,) 42-53. Bukaliya, R. (2012). The potential benefits and challenges internship programs in an ODL institution: a case for the Zimbabwe Open University. International Journal on New Trends in Education and Their Implications, 3(1), 118- 133. Retrieved July 22, 2014 from www.ijonte.org. Information technology teachers require more courses on technology, such as school networking, hardware and multimedia. Students perform well in the use of technology tools and resources to systematically collect and analyze student achievement data, interpret results, and communicate findings to improve instructional practice and maximize students’ learning. This performance should be reinforced and maintained. Buted, D. R., Felicen, S. S. Manzano, A.I. (2014). A Correlation Study between Student Performance in Food and Beverage Services Course and Internship in F&B Department of Hospitality Business, International Journal of Academic Research in Business and Social Sciences, 4(6), 54-66 Technology integration into the classroom teaching, learning and assessment needs careful consideration when planning for effective use. Internship programs should support the innovation of technology as a tool that makes teaching more efficient and not as another layer in the curriculum. Bradshaw, L. (2002). Technology for teaching and learning: Strategies for Staff Development and Follow-Up Support. Journal of technology and teacher education, 10(1), 131150. Callanan, G., & Benzing, C. (2004). Assessing the role of internships in the careeroriented employment of graduating college students. Education and Training, 46, 82-89. More research is needed in examining other standards for technology coaches. Further investigation could be carried on overall students’ experiences on internship program. Cannon, J. A., & Arrnold, M. J. (1998). Student expectations regarding collegiate internship programs in marketing: a 10-year update. Journal of Education for Business, 73(4), 202-205. References Amir, T. & Ismail, O. (2014). An evaluation of the internship program at the college of education, Sultan Qaboos University, Muscat. International Journal of Humanities, Social Sciences and Education, 1(10), 5-10. Casado, M. (1991). Perceptions of corporate recruiters, alumni, and educators toward critical factors of hotel/restaurant management programs: A comparative study. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan. Bacon, D. R. (2006). GPA in research studies: An invaluable but neglected opportunity. Journal of Marketing Education, 28(1), 35-42. Coutinho, S. (2007). The relationship between goals, metacognition, and academic success. Educate, 7 (10), 39-47. Barron, A., Dawson, K. & Yendol-Hoppey, D. (2009). Peer coaching and technology integration: an evaluation of the Microsoft peer coaching program. Mentoring & Tutoring: Partnership in Learning, 17(1), 83– 102. Divine, R., Miller, R. & Wilson, H. (2015). Analysis of Student Performance in an Internship Program in a U.S. University. International Journal of Quality and Productivity Management, 6 (1), 1- 14. Beard, D. F. (1998). The status of internships’ cooperative education experiences in accounting education. Journal of Accounting Education, 15(2), 496 –505. Driscoll, J. (2006). A century of internships: A quick history of internships and co-ops in the business world. Journal of Accounting Education, 16(3), 507–516. Retrieved March 16, 2015, from Beard, F., & Morton, L. (1999). Effects of internship predictors on successful field ex- 687 Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Vol. 9 Issue 4 Oct.) http://news.pghtech.org/teq/teqstory.cf m?id=1573education. 2015 Khalil, O. (2015). Students' experiences with the business internship program at Kuwait University. The International Journal of Management Education 13, 202-217 Felicen, S., Rasa, L., Sumanga, J. ,& Buted, D. (2014). Internship performance of tourism and hospitality students: Inputs to improve internship program. International Journal of Academic Research in Business and Social Sciences, 4(6), 42-53. Kim, E. B., Kim, K., & Bzullak, M. (2012). A survey of internship programs for management undergraduates in AACSBaccredited institutions. The InternationalJournal of Educational Management, 26(7), 696-709. Frenette, A. (2015). The Internship Divide: The Promise and Challenges of Internships in the Arts. SNAAP special report 2015. Retrieved September 8, 2015 from: http://snaap.indiana.edu/pdf/SNAAP15 /SNAAP_Special_Report_2015.pdf Littke, D. (2004). The big picture: Education is everyone’s business. Alexandria, VA: Association for Supervision and Curriculum Development. Furco, A. (1996). Service learning and schoolto-work. Journal of Cooperative Education, 53(1), 7–14. Cynthia Martinez ,C., Lontoc , J. Villena , A. & Laguador, J. (2014). Correlation of OnThe-Job training performance on print media of AB mass communication students and academic performance in selected professional courses for school Year 2012-2013. Journal of Education and Literature,2 (3), 2 80-88 Gault, J., Redingtion, J., & Schlager, T. (2000). Undergraduate business internships and career success: are they related? Journal of Marketing Education, 22, 45-53. Hayes, D. (1981). The relationship of practicum to subsequent academic performance in a hospitality curriculum. Unpublished Doctoral dissertation, Purdue University. McMahon, U., & Quinn, U. (1995). Maximizing the hospitality management student work placement experience: A case study. Education and Training, 37, 13–17. Hendrikse, J. (2013). Teacher education by means of internship: A case study. Unpublished master of education, University of South Africa. Merritt, R. (2008). Student internships. EBSCO research starters. Retrieved September 30, 2015 from https://www.ebscohost.com/uploads/im ported/thisTopic-dbTopic-1072.pdf Hergert, M. (2011). Student perceptions of the value of internships in business education. American Journal of Business Education, 2(8), 9e14. Parsa, S., Aghazadeh , A., Nejatisafa, A., Amini, H., Mohammadi, M., Mostafazadeh , B., Moghaddam, Y. (2010). Freshmen versus Interns’ Specialty Interests. Archives of Iranian Medicine, 13(6), 509-514. Holahan, P., Jurkat, M., & Friedman, E. (2000, Spring). Evaluation of a mentor teacher model for enhancing mathematics instruction through the use of computers. Journal of Research on Computing in Education, 32(3), 336-350. Streumer, J. N., and Kho, M. (2006). The world of work-related learning. In Streumer, J. N. (ed.), Work-Related Learning, pp 211230, Springer. International Society for Technology in Education. (2012). ISTE standards for coaches. Retrieved March 1, 2015, from http://www.iste.org/standards/ISTEstandards/standards-for-coaches. Swan, B. & Dixon, J (2006). The effects of mentor-supported technology professional development on middle school mathematics teachers’ attitudes and practice. Contemporary Issues in Technology and Teacher Education [Online serial], 6(1). Retrieved September 8, 2015 from http://www.citejournal.org/vol6/is s1/mathematics/article1.cfm. Ju, J., Emenheiser, D., Clayton, H., & Reynolds, J. (1998). Korean students’ perceptions of the effectiveness of their internship experiences in the hospitality industry in Korea. Asia Pacific Journal of Tourism Research, 3(1), 37–44. Thiel, G. & Hartley, N. (1997). Cooperative education: A natural synergy between 688 Omani Students’ Application of the Second Standard for Technology Coaches in Internship Program Ahmed Abdelraheem & Talal Amir business and academia. SAM Advanced Management Journal, 62, 19-24. Walo, M. (2001).Assessing the contribution of internship in developing Australian tourism and hospitality students’ management competencies. Asia Pacific Journal of Cooperative Education, 2 (2), 12-28. retrieved September 9, 2015 from http://www.apjce.org/files/APJCE_02_2 _12_28.pdf , Asia Pac Wynn, J. R. (2003), High school after school: Creating pathways to the future for adolescents. New Directions for Youth Development, 97, 59–74. doi: 10.1002/yd.35 Retrieved February 16, 2015, from http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.10 02/yd.35/epdf. Yeswa, E., Okaka, O., Mutsotso, S., Odera, O. & Mumbo, H. (2012). Impact of Internship Programme on the Performance of Public Health Care Institution. Global Journal of human social science arts & Humanities, 12 (13) 1, 39-44. 689 Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Vol. 9 Issue 4 Oct.) 2015 Appendix 1 ISTE Second standard for technology coaches Teaching, Learning, and Assessments of Technology Technology coaches assist teachers in using technology effectively for assessing student learning, differentiating instructions, and providing rigorous, relevant, and engaging learning experiences for all students. a. Coach teachers in and model design and implementation of technology-enhanced learning experiences addressing content and student technology standards. b. Coach teachers in and model design and implementation of technology-enhanced learning experiences using a variety of research-based, learner-centered instructional strategies and assessment tools to address the diverse needs and interests of all students. c. Coach teachers in and model engagement of students in local and global interdisciplinary units in which technology helps students assume professional roles, research realworld problems, collaborate with others, and produce products that are meaningful and useful to a wide audience. d. Coach teachers in and model design and implementation of technology-enhanced learning experiences emphasizing creativity, higher-order thinking skills and processes, and mental habits (e.g., critical thinking, metacognition, and self-regulation). e. Coach teachers in and model design and implementation of technology-enhanced learning experiences using differentiation, including adjusting content, process, product, and learning environment based upon student readiness levels, learning styles, interests, and personal goals. f. Coach teachers in and model incorporation of research-based best practices in instructional design when planning technology-enhanced learning experiences. g. Coach teachers in and model effective use of technology tools and resources to continuously assess student learning and technology literacy by applying a rich variety of formative and summative assessments aligned with content and student technology standards. h. Coach teachers in and model effective use of technology tools and resources to systematically collect and analyze student achievement data, interpret results, and communicate findings to improve instructional practice and maximize students’ learning. 690 Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Pages 691-700) Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 Academic Delay of Gratification and its Relationship to Motivational Determinants, Academic Achievement, and Study Hours among Omani High School Students: A Path Analysis Sabry M. Abd-El-Fattah* & Sahar El Shourbagi Sultan Qaboos University, Sultanate of Oman ___________________________________________ Received: 21/4/2015 Revised: 20/8/2015 Accepted: 20/9/2015 _____________________________________________ Abstract: This study was aimed at investigating the relationships of academic delay of gratification to motivational determinants, academic achievement, and study hours. The sample of the study included 200 Omani high school students. A path analysis showed that motivational determinants were positively related to academic delay of gratification which in turn was positively related to academic achievement and study hours. A mediational analysis showed that academic delay of gratification mediated the relationships among motivational determinants and academic achievement and study hours. There were significant gender differences in academic delay of gratification which favored females. Keywords: Motivational determinants, academic delay of gratification, academic achievement, study hours, high school students تأجيل اإلشباع األكادميي وعالقته باحملددات الدافعية و التحصيل األكادميي وساعات االستذكار لدي طالب حتليل املسار:املزحلة الثانوية يف سلطنة عمان صربي عبد الفتاح* وسحز الصورجبي سلطنة عمان،جامعة السلطان قابوس _____________________________________________ ُ هدفت الدراسة احلالَة إىل فحص تأجَن اإلشباع األكادميُ ًعالقتى باحملددات الدافعَة ًالتحصَن الدراس:مستخمص ًقد أظوز حتمَن املسار أن احملددات. طالب ًطالبة باملزحمة الثانٌٍة يف سمطنة عمان200 ًساعات االستذكار لدى عَنة من ًأظوز.الدافعَة تزتبط إجيابَا بتأجَن اإلشباع األكادميُ الذِ بدًري ٍزتبط إجيابَا بالتحصَن الدراسُ ًساعات االستذكار .حتمَن التٌسط أن تأجَن اإلشباع األكادميُ ٍتٌسط العالقات بني احملددات الدافعَة ًالتحصَن الدراسُ ًساعات االستذكار .كذلك كانت هناك فزًقا دالة إحصائَا يف تأجَن اإلشباع األكادميُ لصاحل اإلناث طالب املدرسة، ساعات االستذكار،ُ التحصَن الدراس، احملددات الدافعَة،ُ تأجَن اإلشباع األكادمي:الكممات املفتاحَة الثانٌٍة *sabryamin@squ.edu.om 691 Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Vol. 9 Issue 4 Oct.) A path analysis 2015 male counterparts. Likewise, Bembenutty (2007) found in a sample of college students that minority females had higher levels of academic delay of gratification than Caucasian males. Bembenutty (2009) reported that gender was a significant predictor of academic delay of gratification of college students with female students reporting higher levels of academic delay of gratification than their male counterparts. In addition, Villarroel (2008), using a sample of Spanish undergraduates, found that females reported higher levels of academic delay of gratification than their male counterparts. This is confusing as first Bembenutty states gender is a significant predictor then in this sentence he does not. Perhaps make it clear that this was an earlier study. Beginning in the late 1960s and continuing into the early 1970s, Walter Mischel and his colleagues (Mischel, 1961; Mischel, Ebbesen, & Zeiss, 1972) conducted a series of studies, known as the Stanford marshmallow experiment, to measure children's willpower to defer gratification. In these experiments, a preschooler would be given two marshmallows if she waited until the researcher returned to the room. The length of time individual children waited until ringing the bell was taken as a measure of their ability to delay gratification. According to Mischel and his colleagues (Mischel, 1981; Mischel & Metzner, 1962; Mischel, Shoda, & Peake, 1988), delay of gratification represents people's attempts to delay an attractive, immediately obtainable goal (e.g., get one marshmallow immediately) with that of pursuing long-range objectives (e.g., wait for few minutes and get two marshmallows). Delay of gratification has been conceived as an ability or competence (Mischel et al., 1988) that children develop over time and as a relatively stable generalized disposition (Funder, Block, & Block, 1983). Academic delay of gratification and motivational determinants One important framework that proves to be helpful in explaining learners‘ preferences for diverse alternatives of action in an academic delay of gratification situation is the expectancy-value theory (Eccles, 2005, 2007; Wigfield, & Eccles, 2000). In this theory, achievement outcomes, such as task performance and future aspirations, are primarily influenced by internalized perceptions of outcome expectancies and the value of specific tasks or domains. The expectancy component corresponds to beliefs about one‘s own competence and selfefficacy. The value component refers to the reasons for engaging in a specific task and includes four principal components: attainment value, intrinsic value, utility value, and cost. Attainment value is defined as the personal importance of doing well on a task, whereas intrinsic value refers to the enjoyment an individual gets from performing an activity, or to the subjective interest an individual has in a subject or activity. Utility value is determined by how well a task or domain relates to current and future goals, such as career goals and academic aspirations. Finally, cost is conceptualized in terms of the negative aspects of engaging in a task, such as performance anxiety and fear of both failure and success, as well as the amount of effort needed to succeed and the lost opportunities that result from making one choice rather than another (Eccles, 2005; Eccles & Wigfield, 2002). From an academic perspective, many students strive to remain goal oriented and committed to tasks while facing distractions that are typical features of learning contexts. These distractions may include turning to more enjoyable activities such as going out to a party with friends, shopping, and going to the cinema or theater. Because remaining goal oriented and committed to tasks often involves foregoing an attractive, immediately obtainable goal (e.g., going to a party) in order to pursue long-range academic objectives (e.g., obtain a high score on a test), this process can be linked to a delay of gratification (Mischel 1961, 1981). Bembenutty and Karabenick (1998, p. 329) defined academic delay of gratification as students postponement of immediately available opportunities to satisfy impulses in favor of pursuing chosen important academic rewards or goals that are temporally remote but ostensibly more valuable." Gender differences in academic delay of gratification With respect to gender differences and their impact on academic delay of gratification, Bembenutty and Karabenick (1998) reported that female college students had higher levels of academic delay of gratification than their Mischel (1996) has assessed motivational determinants of delay of gratification, such as relevance, value, and expectancy for an im- 692 Academic Delay of Gratification and its Relationship to Motivational Determinants Sabry Abd-El-Fattah & Sahar El Shourbagi mediate reward versus delayed reward option. His research has suggested that students‘ willingness to delay gratification depends upon the relative value placed on the competing alternatives. In addition, students‘ choice to delay gratification depends upon their expected likelihood of successful performance, given that they devote their time to this academic goal instead of a more immediate reward. Bembenutty and his colleagues (Bembenutty, 1999, 2008, 2009; Bembenutty & Karabenick, 1998) have demonstrated a relationship between academic delay of gratification and several motivational determinants. For example, Bembenutty (2008) found that college students were more likely to engage in academic delay of gratification when they liked the delay alternative, considered the delay alternative more important than the nondelay alternatives, and had higher expectations that the delay alternative would provide better outcomes than the non-delay alternatives. Further, after controlling for gender, importance of the delay alternative versus immediate alternative was a significant predictor of academic delay of gratification. Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 students between academic delay of gratification, time management and study environment. However, there is insufficient evidence of a link between academic delay of gratification and self-reported intentions and behavior that is indicative of academic delay of gratification, such as the actual time high school students devote to their study. In a recent study, Zhang, Karabenick, Maruno, and Lauermann (2011) assessed Chinese elementary school children‘s willingness to delay gratification, and the time they devoted to non-school study and playtime during an extended interval prior to taking a high stakes final exam. They found that children who exhibited a higher willingness to delay gratification were more likely to spend time studying and less time playing several weeks prior to the exam in contrast to those children with a lower willingness to delay gratification. Aim and rational of the present study Several studies have sought to investigate the relationships of academic delay of gratification to motivational determinants, academic achievement, and management of study time in college student samples using correlational and regression analyses techniques despite the fact that these statistical procedures do not provide a complete picture of any intercausal connections among the variables (Pedhazur, 1997). Academic delay of gratification and academic achievement There is compelling research evidence that individual differences in children‘s delay preferences are associated with subsequent higher academic achievement, intelligence, and the need for achievement (Mischel, 1961; Mischel et al., 1988). Bembenutty and Karabenick (1998) reported that academic delay of gratification correlated positively with expected and obtained final course grades in a sample of college students. Bembenutty (2007) found a positive relationship between academic delay of gratification and final course grade for Caucasian male and female college students. Bembenutty (2009) demonstrated that the positive relationship between academic delay of gratification and academic achievement held even after controlling for the effects of students‘ ratings of the course, expected grades, and the degree of interest, importance, and utility of the academic task. Furthermore, although early writings on academic delay of gratification are rooted in cross-cultural psychology (Gallimore, Weiss, & Finney, 1974), the vast majority of academic delay of gratification research has been conducted in Western cultures. Thus, there is a need for more research to be conducted on how participants from different cultures construe academic delay of gratification, and how academic delay of gratification is related to other psychological constructs within nonWestern cultures. Given these insights/perceptions, the present study extends the existing literature on the relationships of academic delay of gratification to motivational determinants, academic achievement, and management of study time in several ways. Firstly, the present study explores the relationships among these variables in a non-Western, Middle Eastern Arab cultural context and as such, it provides evidence of the applicability of motivational constructs Academic delay of gratification and study time allocation Bembenutty and his colleagues (Bembenutty, 2007, 2009; Bembenutty & Karabenick, 1998) reported a positive association among college 693 Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Vol. 9 Issue 4 Oct.) based on the theories largely developed in the West. The cultural variation in construing human behavior important for the study of academic delay of gratification because some cultures may be more conducive to academic delayed gratification than others. This may be true because cultures differ in educational opportunities, available attractive alternative activities that are typical features of learning contexts, and the values associated with academic tasks. For example, in societies with stronger collectivistic values, students have greater filial allegiances that result in stronger incentives for higher academic performance and academic delay of gratification (Ratner & Hui, 2003). 2015 stage that contributes decisively towards students‘ academic and career future. To summarize, the aim of the present study is to test a path analysis model that can articulate the relationship of academic delay of gratification to motivational determinants, academic achievement, and study time allocation in a sample of Omani high school students. It was predicted that: (a) motivational determinants will be positively related to academic delay of gratification, (b) academic delay of gratification will be positively related to academic achievement, (c) academic delay of gratification will be positively related to study time allocation, (d) academic delay of gratification will mediate the relationship between motivational determinants and academic achievement, and (e) academic delay of gratification will mediate the relationship between motivational determinants and study time allocation. Given that motivational determinants are expected to predict academic delay of gratification, and academic delay of gratification, in turn, is expected to predict academic achievement and study time allocation, it is possible that academic delay of gratification mediates the relationship between the antecedents and the consequences. Secondly, the present study provides a test of a theoretical model combining variables derived from the expectancy-value theory and the theory of self-regulated learning using a path analysis technique. One of the strengths of the path analysis is that it estimates a system of equations that specifies all the possible causal linkages among a set of variables. In addition, path analysis enables researchers to break down or deconstruct correlations among variables into causal (i.e., direct and indirect) and noncausal (e.g., superious) components. Thus, path analysis helps researchers disentangle the complex interrelationships among variables and identify the most significant pathways involved in predicting an outcome. Furthermore, researchers using nonexperimental, quantitative, or correlational data can test whether their hypotheses regarding the relationships among variables are plausible and supported by the data and represent underlying (causal) processes (Pedhazur, 1997). Participants A total of 200 Omani students (110 males and 90 females) from 5 public secondary schools in four governorates in Oman (Musandam, Muscat, Ad Dakhiliyah, and Dhofar) participated in this study. All participant students were at Year 11 and were chosen using a multistage stratified sampling strategy. All schools were located in metropolitan areas and had singlegender populations (three male schools and two female schools). The mean sample ages were 16.68 (SD = .76) and 16.23 (SD = .44) for boys and girls, respectively. Only students with complete data were retained for the present study. The percentage of missing data was 2% which represents those students who left several items blank on the Academic Delay of Gratification Scale (ADGS) and the Motivational Determinants Scale (MDS). The analysis of demographic data showed that participant students were from the same ethnic background and that 97% of them were from the working and lower social class strata. Arabic was the native language of all participant students. Thirdly, although the findings of several studies have shown that college students higher in academic delay of gratification are more likely to manage their time and study environment, there is insufficient evidence for the link between academic delay of gratification and selfreported intentions and behavior that is indicative of academic delay of gratification, such as the actual time students devote to their study. One of the most important issues, especially for high school students, would be whether students higher in academic delay of gratification devote adequate out-of-school time to academic tasks. This may be true given that high school represents a highly competitive and academically demanding educational 694 Academic Delay of Gratification and its Relationship to Motivational Determinants Sabry Abd-El-Fattah & Sahar El Shourbagi Measurements Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 minants (Eccles, Wigfield, Harold, & Blumenfeld, 1993). Academic delay of gratification Academic achievement Bembenutty and Karabenick (1998) developed the 10-item ADGS to assess college students' tendencies to delay gratification within specific academic situations. For each situation, the students rated their preference for an option that offered immediate gratification, such as "Miss several classes to accept an invitation for a very interesting trip" or a delay gratification option such as "Delay going on the trip until the course is over." Students responded to each item on a 4-point scale: Definitely choose A; Probably choose A; Probably choose B, or Definitely Choose B. Abd-El-Fattah and Al-Nabhani (2012) translated the ADGS from English into Arabic using a sample of 195 Year 11 students in Oman. They reported that an exploratory factor analysis with principal components of responses retained a 10-item single factor (Cronbach‘s alpha = .87) Students' academic achievement scores were obtained from their school records at the end of the academic year. These were the courses aggregated total scores, that is, the sum of oncourse assignments and midterm and final examination scores and were expressed as a percentage. Study hours Students were requested to respond to one question concerning their study time allocation ―On average, how many hours a day do you spend studying?‖. Procedure Approval was obtained to conduct the research investigation at the schools prior to data collection. Students were recruited to participate in the present study during their normal classes at their schools. All students agreed by signing a consent form prior to their participation in the present study that stated that they were willing to respond to the ADGS, the Motivational Determinants Scale, and one question concerning their study time allocation. The consent form also indicated that participant students agreed that their end-of academic year achievement scores could be obtained from their school records. Students first responded to the ADGS, then the Motivational Determinants Scale, and finally to the question concerning study time allocation. The measures were administered by trained according to standardized instructions. Students were informed that participation was voluntary and that confidentiality of their answers would prevail at all times. Only certain classes in each school participated in the present study depending on students‘ classroom schedules on the day and time of the administration of the measures. Students completed the three measures in 15 to 20 minutes. Motivational determinants Students were asked to report how strongly they agreed or disagreed with statements that described motivation-related features of the delayed and immediately-available alternatives. These motivational-related features were as follows: Liking (e.g., ‗‗This is something that I would like to do‘‘); Importance (e.g., ‗‗This is something that is important to me‘‘); Expectancy (e.g., ‗‗This is something that would help me to achieve my academic goals‘‘); Utility (e.g., ‗‗This is something that can be useful to me‘‘), Negative Consequences (e.g., ‗‗This is something that can have negative consequences to me‘‘), and Time/Effort (e.g., ‗‗This is something that can be costly in time or effort to me). Students responded to all items of the Motivational Determinants Scale on a 4-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (Strongly Disagree) to 4 (Strongly agree). Differences in scores between motivation for the delay and immediate preferences were obtained by subtracting responses to the immediate alternative from the delay alternative for the four items (e.g., liking of the delay alternative minus liking for the immediate alternative = difference in liking). Higher scores were thus indicative of greater liking, importance, expectancy, utility, and time/effort for the delay versus non-delay alternatives. Scores on these motivational features were summed up to form a single index of motivational deter- Path analysis Given that the data appear normally distributed at univariate and multivariate levels, the full information maximum likelihood estimation was used to analyze the variancecovariance matrices and estimate the path analysis model parameters and obtain fit indexes using the path analysis technique (Byr- 695 Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Vol. 9 Issue 4 Oct.) ne, 2010; Kline, 1998). The AMOS 7.0 program (Arbuckle, 2006) was used to run all analyses. In this path analysis model, depicted in Figure 1, motivational determinants were set as a positive predictor of academic delay of gratification, and academic delay of gratification was set as a positive predictor of academic achievement and study hours. Several absolute and relative goodness-of-fit indexes were used to evaluate the path model‘s goodnessof-fit to the data. Absolute fit indices included Chi-square (χ2), Standardized Root MeanSquare Residual (SRMR), and Root-MeanSquare Error of Approximation (RMSEA). Relative fit indices included Comparative Fit Index (CFI) and Nonnormed Fit Index (NNFI). When modeling normally distributed data, SRMR values of approximately .08 or below, RMSEA values of approximately .06 or below, CFI values of approximately .95 or above, and NNFI of approximately .95 or higher suggest 2015 adequate model-data fit (Byrne, 2010; Hu & Bentler, 1998). Because the χ2 is sensitive to sample size, Hoelter (1983) recommended reporting the χ2/df ratio and suggested that ratios below 2.0 indicate a reasonable fit. The analysis showed that the path model fitted the data adequately (χ2 = 5.31, df = 3; χ2/df = 1.77, RMSEA = .04 (CI. .02 - .07), CFI = .98, SRMR = .06, NNFI = .97). The full set of significant paths is presented in Figure 2 along with the associated variance explained (R2) for each criterion variable. In line with our hypotheses, the analysis showed that motivational determinants positively predicted academic delay of gratification (β = .37). Academic delay of gratification positively predicted academic achievement (β = .32) and study hours (β = .28,). Motivational determinants positively predicted academic achievement (β = .29) and study hours (β = .33). Table 1 Descriptive statistics, Pearson's correlation, and Cronbach's alpha for motivational determinants, academic delay of gratification, academic achievement, and study hours Variable 1 2 3 4 M SD SkewKurtoCronbach‘s ness sis alpha 1. Motivational determi3.1 .85 1.33 .95 .84 nants 2. Academic delay of .35 3.3 .73 1.12 .83 .89 gratification 3. Study hours .30 .32 4.4 .80 .63 .22 NA 4. Academic achievement .39 .36 .34 86.6 1.7 1.51 1.20 NA Note. NA = Not applicable + + + + + + Figure 1 A hypothesized path analysis model of the relationships among motivational determinants, academic delay of gratification, academic achievement, and study hours (positive ‘+’ indicates a positive effect of an independent variable on a dependent variable when all other independent variables in the model are held constant) 696 Academic Delay of Gratification and its Relationship to Motivational Determinants Sabry Abd-El-Fattah & Sahar El Shourbagi Mediation analysis Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 ing is in line with the findings reported by Bembenutty and his colleagues (Bembenutty, 2007, 2009; Bembenutty & Karabenick, 1998) using samples of college students. This finding is noteworthy since it suggests that males and females can show differences in the way they manage academic situations by remaining goal oriented and committed to tasks while facing distractions that are typical features of learning contexts. However, this finding should not be interpreted as suggesting that males and females are inherently different in ways that lead them to engage in academic endeavors in a particular way. The socialization process and classroom contexts, including academic tasks, reward structures, instructional methods, and instructors‘ behaviors, may be associated with the patterns of the academic behavior reported by the students in the present study. Given the findings of the path analysis model, we proceeded to test whether academic delay of gratification mediates the relationship between motivational determinants and academic achievement and study hours. In this analysis, we simultaneously regressed academic achievement and study hours on academic delay of gratification and on motivational determinants. Gender was set as a covariate. We ran the mediation analysis using the SPSS script that accompanies the paper by Preacher and Hayes (2008) on the usage of the bootstrapping method to test mediation models. The bootstrapping method involves repeated random repeated random sampling observations with replacement from the dataset to compute the desired statistic in each resample (Chernick, 1999). In the present study, we set the estimation convergent index to 1000 bootstrap samples to allow for the convergence of the indirect effect estimates. The bootstrapped estimates of the indirect effects, along with the bias-corrected accelerated (BCA) 95% confidence intervals (CI), were calculated. The path analysis showed that motivational determinants were positively related to academic delay of gratification. This finding firmly embeds academic delay of gratification within the framework of the expectancy–value theory (Eccles & Wigfield, 2002; Eccles et al., 1993) and the motivational view of delay of gratification (Mischel, 1996). Within hindsight, this appears to be a readily understood relationship: students‘ willingness to delay gratification in order to pursue long-term academic goals is associated with their motivationrelated judgments of delay vs. non-delay alternatives as articulated by incentive value, such as the benefits, or rewards associated with the academic alternatives and the tempting alternatives. Thus, an expectancy-value mechanism seems to underlie the subjective calculation and ultimately the decision of whether the value and feasibility of attaining a delayed reward, relative to the value of the immediately available one, is high enough to warrant a choice between waiting or working to attain it. This finding is consistent with the findings of Bembenutty and his colleagues (Bembenutty, 1999, 2008, 2009; Bembenutty & Karabenick, 1998). For example, Bembenutty (2009) reported that value based incentives were positively associated with how important, useful, and interesting college students perceived the delay alternative to be, but were inversely related to students‘ consideration of negative consequences associated with The analysis showed a significant indirect effect of motivational determinants on academic achievement through academic delay of gratification (indirect effect = .13; BCA 95% CI lower bound = 0.10, BCA 95% CI upper bound = 0.17). This finding indicated that academic delay of gratification mediates the relationship between motivational determinants and academic achievement. The analysis also showed a significant indirect effect of motivational determinants on study hours through academic delay of gratification (indirect effect = .11; BCA 95% CI lower bound = 0.08, BCA 95% CI upper bound = 0.15). This finding indicates that academic delay of gratification mediates the relationship between motivational determinants and study hours. Discussion The aim of the present study was to investigate the relationships among motivational determinants, academic delay of gratification, academic achievement, and study hours and whether academic delay of gratification mediates the relationship between motivational determinants and academic achievement and study hours. Descriptive analyses showed that females reported higher tendencies of academic delay of gratification than males. This find- 697 Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Vol. 9 Issue 4 Oct.) possible selection of the immediate alternatives versus delay alternatives. 2015 sult, definitive conclusions about the relationship among motivational determinants, academic delay of gratification, academic achievement, and study hours could not be drawn. A different method for understanding the developmental precursors and consequences of academic delay of gratification would be to examine them over time rather than at a single point in time. The second limitation was the use of self-reported measures of motivational determinants, academic delay of gratification, and study hours. Although selfratings of these constructs remain the standard used by most studies, future studies should assess behaviors associated with these constructs either as observed by others or by researchers‘ direct observations. Until such studies are conducted, these constructs will remain largely defined as cognitive self-construal processes rather than observable traits. The path analysis also showed that academic delay of gratification is positively related to academic achievement and study hours. This relationship can be explained within the framework of the self-regulated learning theory because academic delay of gratification has commonly been conceptualized as involving successful self-regulated learning (Bembenutty & Karabenick, 1998; Zimmerman, 1998). According to Bembenutty (2007, 2009), successful self-regulated learners engage in academic delay of gratification by deferring attractive activities in order to achieve long-term goals. Those students also orchestrate their study environment to serve an adaptive purpose that facilitates academic achievement and selfimposed constraints of their own actions in order to devote more time to their study. In contrast, less-skilled self-regulated learners engage in immediate gratification that could preclude them from academic success. Mischel (1996) conceptualized the ability to delay gratification as part of the self-regulatory system necessary to guide behavior without external controlling stimuli. He suggested that the ability to delay gratification is a process of a self-regulatory system of willpower that orchestrates repetitive—used this word before maintenance of motivation and engagement in goals. Zimmerman (1998) argued that lessskilled self-regulated learners "must generate extraordinary personal motivation to delay of gratification until distal goals are achieved." (p. 6). In line with this finding, Bembenutty and his colleagues reported a positive relationship between academic delay of gratification and the control of time and study environment (Bembenutty, 2007; Bembenutty & Karabenick, 1998) and that of academic achievement (Bembenutty, 2007, 2009; Bembenutty & Karabenick, 1998). To summarize, the current findings provide further insight into the dynamics which underpin students‘ academic delay of gratification. It could be argued that motivational determinants are associated indirectly and positively with academic achievement and study hours because they encourage positive mediating factors (i.e., academic delay of gratification) that facilitate important educational outcomes. References Abd-El-Fattah, S. M., & Al-Nabhani, H. Z. (2012). From self-theories of intelligence to academic delay of gratification: The mediating role of achievement goal. Australian Journal of Educational and Developmental Psychology, 12, 93 - 107. Arbuckle, J. L. (2006). AMOS (Version 7.0) [Computer Program]. Chicago, IL: SPSS. Bembenutty, H. (1999). Sustaining motivation and academic goals: The role of academic delay of gratification. Learning and Individual Differences, 11, 233-257. The mediation analysis also showed that academic delay of gratification mediated the relationship between motivational determinants and academic achievement and study hours. Specifically, motivational determinants were positively associated with academic delay of gratification which in turn was positively associated with academic achievement and study hours. Bembenutty, H. (2007). Self-regulation of learning and academic delay of gratification: Gender and ethnic differences among college students. Journal of Advanced Academics, 18, 586-616. Bembenutty, H. (2008). Academic delay of gratification and expectancy-value. Personality and Individual Differences, 44, 193202. The major limitation of the present study was the cross sectional nature of the data. As a re- 698 Academic Delay of Gratification and its Relationship to Motivational Determinants Sabry Abd-El-Fattah & Sahar El Shourbagi Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 30, 72-80. Bembenutty, H. (2009). Academic delay of gratification, self-regulation of learning, gender differences, and expectancy value. Personality and Individual Differences, 46, 347-352. Hair, J. F. Jr., Anderson, R. E., Tatham, R. L. & Black, W. C. (1995). Multivariate Data Analysis (3rd ed.). New York, NY: Macmillan. Bembenutty, H., & Karabenick, S. A. (1998). Academic Delay of Gratification. Learning and Individual Differences, 10, 329-346. Hoelter, J. W. (1983). The analysis of covariance structures: Goodness-of-fit indices. Sociological Methods and Research, 11, 325344. Chernick, M. R. (1999). Bootstrap methods: A practitioner’s guide (Wiley series in probability and statistics). New York, NY: Wiley. Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. M. (1998). Fit indices in covariance structure modeling: Sensitivity to underparameterized model misspecification. Psychological Methods, 3, 424-453. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. DeCarlo, L. (1997). On the meaning and use of kurtosis. Psychological Methods, 2, 292-307. Kline, R. (1998). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. New York, NY: Guilford Press. Eccles, J. S. (2005). Subjective task value and the Eccles et al. model of achievement related choices. In A. J. Elliot & C. S. Dweck (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation (pp. 105-121). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Mischel, W. (1961). Preference for delayed reinforcement and social responsibility. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 62, 1-7. Mischel, W. (1981). Metacognition and the rules of delay. In J. H. Flavell & L. Ross (Eds.), Social cognitive development: Frontiers and possible futures (pp. 240- 271). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Eccles, J. S. (2007). Motivational perspective on school achievement: Taking responsibility for learning and teaching. In R. J. Sternberg & R. F. Subotnik (Eds.), Optimizing student success in schools with the new three Rs (pp. 199-202). Charlotte, NC: Information Age. Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (2002). Motivational beliefs, values, and goals. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 109-132. Mischel, W. (1996). From good intentions to willpower. In P. M. Gollwitzer & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The psychology of action: Linking cognitions and motivation to behavior (pp. 197-218). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Eccles, J. S., Wigfield, A., Harold, R., & Blumenfeld, P. B. (1993). Age and gender differences in children‘s self- and task perceptions during elementary school. Child Development, 64, 830-847. Mischel, W., Ebbesen, E. B., & Zeiss, A. R. (1972). Cognitive and attentional mechanisms in delay of gratification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 21, 204218. Finney, S. J., & DiStefano, C. (2006). Nonnormal and categorical data in structural equation models. In G. R. Hancock & R. O. Mueller (Eds.), A second course in structural equation modeling (pp. 269-314). Greenwich, CT: Information Age. Mischel, W., & Metzner, R. (1962). Preference for delayed reward as a function of age, intelligence, and length of delay interval. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 64, 425-431. Funder, D. C., Block, J. H., & Block, J. (1983). Delay of gratification: Some longitudinal personality correlates. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 1198-1213. Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., & Peake, P. K. (1988). The nature of adolescent competencies predicted by preschool delay of gratification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 687-696. Gallimore, R., Weiss, L. B., & Finney, R. (1974). Cultural differences in delay of gratification: A problem of behavior classification. Pedhazur, E. J. (1997). Multiple regression in behavioral research (3rd ed.). Fort Worth, TX: Harcourt Brace. 699 Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Vol. 9 Issue 4 Oct.) Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models Behavior Research Methods, 40, 879-891. Ratner, C., & Hui, L. (2003). Theoretical and methodological problems in cross-cultural psychology. Journal for the Theory of Social Behavior, 67-94, 33. Villarroel, J. R. (2008). An examination of the psychometric properties of the Spanish version of the academic delay of gratification scale. Advances in Applied Developmental Psychology, 2, 156-167. Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. S. (2000). Expectancyvalue theory of achievement motivation. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25, 68–81. Zhang, L., Karabenick, S. A., Maruno, S., & Lauermann, F. (2011). Academic delay of gratification and children‘s study time allocation as a function of proximity to consequential academic goals. Learning and Instruction, 21, 77-94. Zimmerman, B. J. (1998). Developing selffulfilling cycles of academic regulation: An analysis of exemplary instructional models. In D. H. Schunk & B. J. Zimmerman (Eds.), Self-regulated learning: From teaching to self-reflective practice (pp. 1-19). New York, NY: Guilford. 700 2015 Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Pages 701-717) Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 Arabic in Foreign Language Programmes: Difficulties and Challenges Fatma Y. Al-Busaidi* Sultan Qaboos University, Sultanate of Oman ___________________________________________ Received: 20/8/2015 Revised: 14/9/2015 Accepted: 22/9/2015 _____________________________________________ Abstract: The purpose of this study was to review the reported literature regarding Arabic language programmes. It gives an overview of the historical background of Teaching Arabic as a Foreign Language (TAFL) programmes. It also provided a brief description of the Arabic language and its characteristics, and how they might cause some difficulties. Specifically, the diglossic phenomenon in Arabic programmes and how Arabic programmes deal with diglossia was discussed. Pedagogical factors, such as the lack of clearly articulated objectives in TAFL, the lack of coordination between Arabic programmes, the lack of experienced and qualified teachers, the shortage of materials and resources and insufficient presentation of Arab culture in (TAFL) programmes were also discussed. Keywords: Teaching Arabic as a foreign language, MSA, NSA, linguistic factors, pedagogical factors, diglossia. الصعوبات والتحديات:برامج اللغة العربية لغري الناطقني بها *فاطمة بنت يوسف البوسعيدية سلطنة عمان،جامعة السلطان قابوس _____________________________________________ وحتلًل أيم التحديات اليت تىاجًًا، صعت يذه الدراصة إىل مزاجعة األدبًات اخلاصة بربامج تعلًم اللغة العزبًة:مضتخلص ووصف ًا مىجشًا عو طبًعة اللغة العزبًة وخصائصًا اليت، قدمت الدراصة نبذة تارخيًة عو بزامج اللغة العزبًة يف العامل.ومهاقشتًا ميكو أى تشكل صعىبات وحتديات ملتعلمٌ اللغة العزبًة مو الهاطكني بغرييا واليت يهبغٌ مزاعاة تذلًلًا يف الربامج املكدمة كما ناقشت يذه الدراصة قضًة االسدواجًة اللغىية يف اللغة العزبًة وأصالًب التعامل معًا يف بزامج تعلًم اللغة العزبًة.إلًًم وأخريا تهاقش الىرقة نكاط الضعف يف بعض بزامج اللغة العزبًة واملتمثلة يف عدم وجىد أيداف.املكدمة هلذه الفئة مو الطلبة وضعف االيتمام بالطزائل والىصائل، وقلة املعلمني املؤيلني لتعلًمًا هلذه الفئة،واضحة لتعلًم اللغة العزبًة للهاطكني بغرييا . وقلة االيتمام باجلانب الثكايف يف بزامج اللغة العزبًة،واألصالًب احلديثة يف التعلًم ، العىامل الرتبىية، العىامل اللغىية، اللًجة، اللغة العزبًة الفصحى، تدريط اللغة العزبًة كلغة أجهبًة:الكلمات املفتاحًة .اإلسدواجًة اللغىية fbusaidi@squ.edu.om* 701 Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Vol. 9 Issue 4 Oct.) The developments in the 1940s in the field of foreign language education had a strong influence on teaching Arabic. This influence began to be evident in the 1950s, under the support of the Ministry of Education of Egypt. For example, Egyptian professors began to conduct a number of programmes and activities such as conferences, seminars and discussions regarding the teaching of Arabic to non-native speakers. All these activities led to the establishment of a number of institutions for teaching Arabic as a foreign language in both Arab and non-Arab countries. The most important institutions founded in the period of 1958 1979 in Arab countries were as follows: 2015 United States as early as the 17th and 18th centuries. McCarus (1992) described the history of Arabic teaching in the United States in the following way: “Arabic was being taught in the United States over a century before the signing of the Declaration of Independence, introduced to complement the study of Hebrew and the Old Testament” (p. 207). In fact, efforts to improve teaching Arabic as a foreign language (TAFL) in America began in 1958 when the Social Science Research Council sponsored a conference of 20 teachers of Arabic to make recommendations in the field, specifically relating to textbooks and the evaluation system (McCarus, 1987). Based on these suggestions, the American Association of Teachers of Arabic (AATA) was established in 1963 under the direction of the Modern Language Association (MLA). Soon after its establishment, AATA began publishing a newsletter, An-Nashrā, „which later became AlArabiyya (Al-Batal, 1995a). Centre for Arabic studies, Lebanon, 1958. The Arabic Unit of the Language Centre, Kuwait University, 1965. The Institute of Teaching Arabic, Islamic University of Medina, 1965. Habib Bourguiba Institute, Tunis, 1968. The AATA made a great effort to encourage study, criticism, and research in the field of TAFL in both linguistics and Arabic literature. Additionally, in order to improve the quality of Arabic programmes across America, the AATA planned to assemble a textbook evaluation package, designed to distribute information on textbooks and other materials to teachers of Arabic. The idea for this package came from a survey conducted with financial support from the Ford Foundation channelled to the National Council of Less Commonly Taught Languages. This study focused on the institutional setting of Arabic language teaching in the United States (Belnap, 1995). Centre of Arabic Studies at the American University in Cairo, 1974. Khartoum International Institute in Sudan under the support of the Islamic Educational, Scientific and Cultural Organisation (ISESCO), 1974. Institute of Arabic Language, King Saud University in Riyadh, Saudi Arabia, 1975. Institute of Arabic Language, University of Umm Al Qura, Maakkah Al- Mukarramah, Saudi Arabia, 1979. Following these initiatives, a number of institutions in various Arab countries established private Arabic schools. Thus, currently there are a number of private Arabic programmes for non-native speakers in Morocco, Tunisia, Egypt, Lebanon, Palestine, Jordan, Syria, Saudi Arabia, Kuwait, Qatar, United Arab Emirates, Yemen and Oman. The AATA also developed the Arabic language learning framework. Thus, they conducted studies to better understand the nature and variety of existing Arabic curricula. In addition, while American universities and institutions were unlikely to settle on a particular Arabic curriculum for non-native speakers, they worked to produce a set of universal curriculum strategies meant to serve as a resource for teachers and learners (McCarus, 1987). The growth of awareness in teaching and learning Arabic is not limited to Arab countries. For example, teaching Arabic has a long history in Britain and Ireland, and the language is currently being taught in many of its universities and institutions. Other European countries, the United States of America, and many Islamic countries have witnessed an increase in institutions that teach Arabic for Iislamic purposes (Al-Batal, 1995a). For example, Arabic teaching started in universities in the Furthermore, in response to and as a consequence of the increasing interest in the Arabic language, a number of universities and colleges in America and Europe, and many Islamic from countries such as Malaysia, Indonesia, and India expanded and added new courses to their Arabic programmes. Moreover, in order 702 Arabic in Foreign Language Programmes: Difficulties and Challenges Fatma Al-Busaidi to provide English-speaking learners of Arabic with the opportunity to study the language in its real cultural setting, several universities conducted intensive summer language programmes in the Arab world to foster Arabic study abroad (Al-Batal, 1995a). Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 among non-native speakers. Al-Batal and Belnap (2006), for instance, found that an extremely small number of foreigners engage in Arabic language learning in U.S. universities in contrast to other languages, and very few of them achieve a high level of proficiency. Major concerns for the field of TAFL The United States Foreign Service Institution (FSI) has classified languages into four levels or degrees of difficulty based on the amount of time required to reach a certain level of proficiency (Liskin-Gasparro, 1982). According to the FSI rankings, Arabic is grouped with those relatively difficult languages such as Chinese, Korean and Japanese (Stevens, 2006). It has been argued that Arabic cannot be fully learned as a second or foreign language to a level where a learner would be able to be at the educated native speaker level. As a result, there has been a serious argument over whether or not non-native learners can ever achieve the highest level, level 5, in Arabic (in terms of FSI proficiency ratings) (Ryding, 1994). Likewise, it has been found that some Arabic teachers consider this level of achievement simply not possible (Ryding, 1995). Despite all of the efforts to produce Arabic language programmes worldwide, the field of TAFL still has major concerns. For example, although in 1974 the United Nations adopted Arabic as one of its six official languages, the National Foreign Language Centre considered Arabic to be one of the less commonly taught languages in America, the United Kingdom and even in the Arab world. Ryding (1994) highlights this issue, stating that: “When considering some of the facts about Arabic, it may be surprising that it is one of the less commonly taught languages in the West in general and in the United States in particular.” (p.23). Similarly, Allen (2007) confirms the lack of attention given to Arabic in the United States: “The events of 9/11/2001 found American „preparedness‟ in terms of Arabic-competent citizens at a very low level.” (p. 258). Belnap (1987) conducted a study to investigate the number of students who learned Arabic in the United States and to find out what motivated them to learn this language. The results showed that the number was on the rise; however, the great majority did not continue their study of the language beyond the second year. Another study conducted by Belnap (2006) to understand students‟ beliefs and attitudes towards the Arabic language indicated that more than half of students felt that Arabic was a difficult language to learn and achieving a high level of proficiency was difficult. Moreover, a study by Abdelhadi, Ibrahim and Eviatar (2011) found that learning the written form of Arabic took a longer compared to other languages. However, this changed after the terrorist attacks of September 11 2001. Allen (2007) explains this as follows: Since that day, the status of Arabic in the national consciousness has been transformed almost overnight to become the number-one desideratum of the American government and its various agencies. Huge amounts of money are being spent and will be spent in an attempt to produce an increased number of Americans who are competent in the Arabic language at levels considerably higher than those of the majority of previous learners of the language. (p. 258). In order to understand more, many researchers (e.g. Al-Batal & Belnap, 2006; Belnap, 2006; Stevens, 2006; Wahba, 2006; Palmer, 2008; Fragman & Russak, 2010; Abdelhadi, Ibrahim & Eviatar, 2011; Myhill, 2014) began to investigate the factors influencing this issue. The majority of these studies reported that there are general problematic factors that lead to Arabic being considered a difficult language to acquire. Their findings can be grouped into Although the Arabic language has attracted interest in the United States and huge sums of money are being spent to improve its teaching in Arab and non-Arab countries, challenges facing the field of teaching Arabic for non-native speakers seem to be an issue for many researchers. For example, one of these issues is the significant lack of Arabic language competence 703 Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Vol. 9 Issue 4 Oct.) two main categories: firstly, linguistic difficulties which might well be understood by reviewing the literature relating to the characteristics of the Arabic language; and secondly, difficulties related to pedagogical factors, such as the qualities and the characteristics of Arabic programmes, the quality of teachers and the teaching materials used. All of which will be discussed in the following sections. 2015 they are nonetheless quite different. As a result, diglossia has often been described as being the „bilingualism of a monoglot‟. Some professionals in the field go beyond this and state that all Arabs are bilingual, as they believe that Arabs are native speakers of NSA and not MSA (Eisele, 2006; Palmer, 2008). Until a few decades ago, this was the type of Arabic most commonly taught in American and European universities (Versteegh, 2006). Although this level of Arabic is still used in some TAFL programmes, it is restricted to religious and highly formal contexts. Linguistic factors affecting teaching and learning Arabic as a foreign anguage Arabic is the official language of some twenty nations, stretching from the Atlantic coast of North Africa in the west to the Sultanate of Oman in the east, and from Syria in the north to Sudan in the south. Arabic belongs to the Semitic group of languages, and it is a synthetic rather than an analytic language. Therefore, there are significant differences between the structures of Arabic and Indo-European languages, such as English, Spanish, French, and German. Hence, Arabic has some characteristics that European languages do not, along with a very complex morphological system (Holes, 1995). The second form of Arabic is Modern Standard Arabic (MSA). It is a direct descendant of classical Arabic and, until recently, it was the language of important discourse, contemporary literature, and the media (newspaper, radio, television, and the Internet). It is also the language that is used in formal situations such as speeches and public lectures. MSA is a formal, mostly written language, which is not used for daily communication. There are no native speakers of MSA, but educated people in the Arab world learn this form during formal education (Versteegh, 2006). To understand the linguistic factors that affect the learning and teaching of Arabic and how it is different from other languages, a brief overview of its characteristics is necessary. Firstly, a discussion of the phenomenon of „diglossia‟ in Arabic is in order. However, this kind of language has produced a new form of comprehensible spoken Arabic called „Educated Spoken Arabic‟ (ESA). Educated Arabs of most nationalities use ESA as a way to communicate verbally for interdialectal conversation, social discussions and other occasions when dialects are considered too informal and literary Arabic is too formal. The pronunciation of ESA is very closely related to that of MSA and it has an exceedingly classical vocabulary. There are differences however, in some aspects of syntax and morphology (Harvey, 1979). Phenomenon of diglossia The Arabic language is widely characterized by diglossia. Ferguson (1959) has defined diglossia as: A relatively stable language situation in which, in addition to the primary dialects of the language (which may include a Standard or regional standards), there is a very divergent, highly codified (often grammatically more complex) superimposed variety, the vehicle of a large and respected body of written literature, either of an earlier period or in another speech community, which is learned largely by formal education and is used for most written and formal spoken purposes but is not used by any sector of the community for ordinary conversation. (p.334). The third form of Arabic is called Colloquial Arabic or Non-Standard Arabic (NSA). This refers to regional dialects used in everyday conversations and popular cultural media. There are in fact many different Arabic dialects, which vary not only from one country to another, but also from one region to another within one country (Cote, 2009). There are significant differences in structure and vocabulary between MSA and NSA at all linguistic levels. (Ferguson, 1959; Versteegh, 2006; Cote, 2009; Myhill, 2014). Although many other languages have formal and informal variations, the differences between formal Two forms of Arabic are in use, namely Modern Standard Arabic (MSA) and Non-Standard Arabic (NSA). Whilst being closely related, 704 Arabic in Foreign Language Programmes: Difficulties and Challenges Fatma Al-Busaidi and informal Arabic are substantially greater than those in European or any other language (Eisele, 2006; Myhill, 2014). the biggest challenges facing Arabic programme designers (Alosh, 1997; Ryding, 2006; Palmer, 2008; Myhill, 2014). As a result, a variety of approaches have emerged from different schools. These approaches will be highlighted in the next section. Even in cases where MSA and the NSA share some lexical items, the vowels may be different. For example, the word yktub (he writes) is pronounced yaktubu in MSA, but yktib in NSA. This means that learning a certain word in Colloquial Arabic does not necessarily mean that it will be the same in the written form. Van Mol (2006) argues that the major challenge for foreign learners of Arabic is that they have to learn at least two varieties of the language. This means that they have double the vocabulary to learn, as it is very common in Arabic to find different word meanings in different dialects, including Standard Arabic. It also causes the problem of needing to teach two different pronunciation systems to students at the same time, one for NSA and one for MSA (Abu-Hatab, 1992). Teaching modern standard Arabic This approach only emphasizes the teaching of Modern Standard Arabic. Many scholars consider MSA to be the only form of the language worth teaching. Maamouri (1998) stated that “standard was the „real language,‟ and that all other varieties of it were „degenerate‟ and „corrupt‟ versions” (p. 33). This philosophy is present today both inside and outside of the Arabic-speaking world. The proponents of this view state that MSA is the level of language that educated Arabs are able to understand in the different Arab countries. It is also the language that combines the needs of foreign learners who want to know the cultural, religious, and artistic inventions of the particular Arab country? Finally, it is the form that is most likely to remain with learners when they return to their home countries or when they travel to any Arab country, as it can be used for oral communication anywhere in the Arab world where communication is difficult in the local dialect (Ryding, 1995; Nahla, 2006). This can be better understood if we consider that the effect of diglossia is not only limited to non-native learners but also to native speakers themselves. For example, numerous studies have found that diglossia in Arabic has hindered the process of literacy acquisition among native Arabic speakers because of the linguistic distance between the spoken dialects and the standard written form. Confusing might re-word. “Young Arab users do not feel that they are free to use and innovate in [MSA]. Pupils entering school have to „unlearn‟ or even suppress most of their linguistic habits while they try to acquire a new set of „rigid‟ rules.” (p. 41). This is because, “[MSA] is nobody‟s mother tongue and is rarely or almost never used at home in the Arab world” (Maamouri, 1998, p. 33). Diglossic grammes phenomenon in Arabic Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 However, this approach has some limitations, and a negative impact on learners in many aspects. Firstly, it does not take into account the fact that Arabs themselves would never use this kind of Arabic in their everyday activities, such as informal conversation and shopping (Ryding, 1995; Younes, 1995; Alosh, 1997; Palmer, 2008). As a result, as argued by Wahba (2006), teaching this form has led to even the more advanced students experiencing a gap between their classroom achievement and the ability to integrate linguistically and culturally into Arab society. Similarly, Heath (1990) found that students who have only studied this form of Arabic do not feel integrated into society and often experience disappointment and embarrassment when trying to converse with Arabic speakers. He argues, “teaching students only MSA severely hampers their ability to communicate in the language they have striven so hard to learn. Given that Arabs will understand what such students are saying, the students themselves pro- Although some previous research has attributed poor student proficiency in Arabic to diglossia, there are other factors that affect the learning of this language which need to be taken into account. The central one is how Arabic programmes deal with diglossia. The literature in this field indicates that, as yet, there is no clear agreement on how to deal with the diglossic phenomenon. The questions of what kind of Arabic to teach and what kind of Arabic might be more practical to offer are 705 Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Vol. 9 Issue 4 Oct.) 2015 will not understand anything said to them outside the limited MSA linguistic register they have mastered.” (p. 43). seems to be applied differently by different schools. The differences are further examined as follows: Moreover, Al-Batal (1992) found that most students who learn this form of Arabic get easily discouraged and frustrated, and give up after one year because they do not have the ability to use the language in daily communication. Ignoring standard Arabic and teaching a selected dialect This approach ignores Standard Arabic and teaches a selected dialect in order to enable students to become involved in the activities of daily life. This approach has also led to another problematic issue regarding dialect to teach, as very often the chosen dialect might well not be understood by people living in other Arab countries (Alosh, 1997). Moreover, the negative aspect of this approach is that it does not address the needs of those who want to learn the written form of Arabic. Al-Batal (1995b) provides two strong arguments to support this view. Firstly, he refers to a survey conducted by Belnap (1987) indicating that some students of Arabic are interested in developing overall language proficiency. This means that there is a significant need for including MSA in any programme, at least for developing literacy abilities. Secondly, he concurs with Allen (1987), who maintains that no Colloquial Arabic programme is likely to take learners beyond the basic level. Another argument against this approach is that it ignores those students who want to learn Arabic for speaking purposes only. A survey from the National Middle East Language Resource Centre (NMELRC) found that over 650 students learning formal Arabic at U.S. institutions of higher education were mainly interested in learning spoken Arabic (Belnap, 1987). This finding was confirmed by a study conducted by Younes (1995), who found that the majority of students were learning Arabic to be able to communicate in the Arab world. Finally, the proponents of this approach do not include dialect courses in Arabic programmes because they believe that the classroom is not an effective environment in which to produce proficient speakers. It was generally accepted that students who wanted to acquire an Arabic dialect could do so naturally through interacting with local people (AlHamad, 1983). However, from another perspective, students should receive preparation for learning Arabic dialects in the classroom by a specialist teacher before they start to converse with people who might use different dialects and accents. The supporters of this view believe that Arabic teachers should play a vital role in helping learners function within a diglossic environment. Additionally, teachers should help students understand the difference between the two forms of Arabic in order to avoid any confusion that might result (Al-Juhany, 1990; Palmer, 2008). Although the teaching of Arabic dialects has become on approach in TAFL, very few programmes in the Arab world or in other countries for that matter offer this because of both logistical limitations and ideological reasons. Suleiman (2003), for example, clarified that any attempt to limit MSA in curricula and place emphasis on a spoken variety may be seen as a threat. Additionally, Palmer (2008) reported that there are numerous obstacles to introducing spoken Arabic. He suggests that issues of prestige, the preservation of the Quranic language, and integration are often cited as significant concerns that limit the idea of providing dialects in Arabic programmes. Therefore, at a philosophical level, some educators in the field believe that dialects are the poor relations of the standard written language and are, therefore, not useful in Arabic programmes. At a practical level, the programmes that see the value of offering classes in Arabic dialects may not have the required teachers and learning materials that lead to success (Alosh, 1997). Teaching colloquial Arabic Developments taking place in the field of teaching and learning foreign languages have led to some improvements in the Arabic teaching profession. One of the most significant improvements is the increased interest in the communicative competence approach, which is believed to lead to the oral proficiency of students (Al-Batal, 1992; Younes, 1995; Alosh, 1997; Wahba, 2006). However, this approach 706 Arabic in Foreign Language Programmes: Difficulties and Challenges Fatma Al-Busaidi Adoption of educated spoken Arabic Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 Additionally, Al-Batal (1992) proposed an alternative approach where both Colloquial Arabic and MSA are taught in the classroom. This reflects, he suggests, the linguistic reality in the Arab world today. As a result, some institutions such as the Foreign Service Institute (FSI) began to design a curriculum that develops communicative competence simultaneously with Modern Standard Arabic (Ryding, 2006). Also, the University of Cambridge adopted the communicative approach to help the students speak colloquial Arabic (Palestinian Arabic) from the very start of the course (Dickins and Watson, 2006). In addition, the communicative approach has been introduced in the Arabic programmes in Britain and Ireland, and most universities have moved to more communicatively oriented materials such as "Elementary Modern Standard Arabic" and "al-Kitab fi Ta’allum al-’Arabiyya". Furthermore, to enable learners of Arabic to understand and express themselves in the local dialects along with the MSA, the majority of Arabic programmes in the Arab world such as Yarmouk University programme in Jordanian, Sultan Qaboos College for Teaching Arabic to Non-Native Speakers programme in the Sultanate of Oman, and The American University programme in Cairo, have adopted this approach. There is another approach which calls for the adoption of „Educated Spoken Arabic‟. This form represents a viable option for the development of spoken proficiency because it is understood by most educated Arabs in Arab countries (Wahba, 2006). Although this has become the language of choice for some Arabic programmes, it is still not the form that suits those learners who prefer to achieve basic proficiency in the written form of Arabic. This has led many institutions to consider other approaches. Providing two successive courses Some programmes provide two courses. Students first study MSA and then, in a second course, they learn a chosen dialect. Proponents of this view believe that allowing students to become familiar with MSA is a good start for learning a dialect (Al-Hamad, 1983). However, others argue that it is best to start with the dialect because it theoretically reflects the order of acquisition of native speakers, who first learn the dialect, and later at school, learn Standard Arabic (Nicola, 1990; Ryding, 2006; Younes, 1995). Consequently, some researchers argue that the starting point should focus on building a dialectal foundation in listening and speaking, and then the MSA form should be gradually introduced. Although such an approach is consistent with the way Arabic is learnt and used by native speakers, the major problem is the time limitation. Students usually do not have time to master an Arabic dialect and then start MSA. Another criticism is that the Colloquial Arabic portion of the course focuses only on listening and speaking skills, which may result in students leaving the programme with no ability to read or write (Younes, 1995). Such an approach encompasses all language aspects, with each being afforded enough time. These aspects are related to the four language skills of listening, reading, writing and speaking. In these programmes, reading and writing are taught in Mmodern Standard Arabic and speaking and listening in a dialect (Holes, 1995). However, it has been argued that this approach has led to some problems and confusion for some learners of Arabic, especially for those at the beginning stages of learning (Al-Hamad, 1983). Some even think that although the MSA and dialects are, to some extent, related, they are still so different that learning them is similar to learning two different languages simultaneously (Stevens, 2006). This could be more easily understood if one keeps in mind that in learning languages, what one learns in conversation frequently supports and assists what one learns in reading. Teaching both standard Arabic and a dialect simultaneously Some educators believe that students should learn both forms at the same time. For example, Younes (1995) writes: “If the goal of an Arabic-as-a-foreign-language programme is to prepare students to function successfully in Arabic, then they should be introduced to both a Spoken Arabic dialect and “formal Arabic” from the beginning of an Arabic course” (p.233). However, Ryding (2006) found that programmes that provide both forms, formal and informal, do not inhibit the attainment of read- 707 Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Vol. 9 Issue 4 Oct.) ing and writing skills, as they very often reinforce each other. Nevertheless, he confirms that the achievement of advanced proficiency will take more time and need more focus. With the same perspective, Agius (1990) found that students who were exposed to both varieties of Arabic simultaneously seemed to be much more motivated to learn the language than those who had studied in the traditional way. 2015 cents in the eighth grade and found orthographic sources of difficulty. He reported that pupils made spelling errors, as they were confused by similar looking Arabic letters, which are at times differentiated only by dots. In addition, they were confused between letters with multiple shapes. In some cases, the letters looked similar to others. Vanderhoof (2011) also reported a lack of improvement in the overall spelling performance of 44 adult English as a Second Language (ESL) students. In sum, aAfter providing a brief definition of diglossia in the Arabic language and the teaching approaches developed, the following sections will provide a description of orthography, morphology and the Arabic phonetic system, and how they might createcause some challenges for students of Arabic. Likewise, features of Arabic orthography constitute a major difficulty for the foreign language learner since Arabic, in contrast to European languages, is read and written from left to right and uses completely different scripts and directionality. It has been found that this is cognitively complex and requires restructuring one‟s way of thinking (AlJuhany, 1990). Arabic orthography The Arabic writing system is an alphabetic logographic script, where every letter is assembled in order to generate meaning. The alphabet consists of twenty-eight letters without the inclusion of hamzah. Moreover, three letters are treated as long vowels (sounds): „alif’ ()أ, „waw‟, ( )وand „yaa‟, ( )يwhich represent the sounds /a: / /u: / and / i: / respectively in English. Furthermore, Arabic also has three short vowels which may be represented by diacritic marks above or below a letter with the same sound of alif, waw and yaa. The signs of these three vowels are, respectively: fathah )َ ) a small diagonal stroke above a consonant, dhammah ( َ ) a small waw above a consonant, and kasrah’ ( َ ) a small diagonal stroke under a consonant (Awde & Samano, 1986; Fragman & Russak, 2010). Arabic script is cursive, meaning that certain letters must be connected to others in writing. There are no capital letters in Arabic, but a letter may occur in more than one form depending on its position in the word and what letters surround it. Every letter has four different forms: isolated, initial, medial and final. However, the basic letter remains unchanged. Some Arabic letters are attachable only to the letters preceding them, and some are attachable to letters preceding and following them (Awde & Samano, 1986; Saiegh-Haddad & Henkin-Roitfarb, 2014). Al-Mutawa (1995) found that the majority of learners of Arabic faced difficulty in differentiating between short and long vowels which created pronunciation, reading and writing difficulties. In addition to the three vowel signs, there are another two significant signs called sukūn and shaddah. The former refers to a small circle written above the letter and indicates the absence of a vowel after a consonant; its sign is (َ). On the other hand, when a consonant occurs twice without a vowel in between, it is written only once with the sign ( َ ) shaddah. The letters which have shaddah are commonly called mūdhaaf. This means that the letter should be doubled in pronunciation. Attadheef „ ‟التضعيفhas a great impact on the meaning of the words in Arabic. For example, if you say Faāla, „ ’فعلit means „did‟ but with the attadheef „ ’فعلit means „activated‟. Therefore, some researchers (e.g. Al-Mutawa, 1995; Abdelhadi, Ibrahim & Eviatar 2011) stress the importance of helping Arabic students understand and perceive the idea of doubling consonants. The findings of some previous researchers, such as Al-Mutawa (1995), Al-Juhany (1990), Hansen (2010) and Abdelhadi, Ibrahim and Eviatar (2011) have revealed that dimensions connected with the letter architecture, and the fact that the majority of the letters have multiple shapes, are connected to adjacent letters, and are often distinguished from one another on the basis of the number and location of dots alone, contribute to the difficulty in learning the Arabic language. Fragman and Russak (2010), for example, examined the Arabic spelling accuracy of native Hebrew adoles- 708 Arabic in Foreign Language Programmes: Difficulties and Challenges Fatma Al-Busaidi The positions of Arabic letters and the complicated marks appearing above or under them can cause some challenges for Arabic learners. What makes the situation worse is that, unfortunately, in most modern written and printed Arabic, no signs (marks above or under letters), sukūn „ ’سكونor shaddah „ ’شدجare used to help while reading. This might be one reason for the delayed development of reading skills of Arabic learners (Al-Mutawa, 1995). Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 Although the majority of the words are spelt exactly as they are pronounced in Arabic, spelling has very often been reported as problematic. Many researchers (e.g. Burj, 1978; AlJuhany, 1990; Fragman & Russak, 2010) found that students struggle with Arabic spelling. Noticeably, spelling problems in Arabic do not occur, as is the case in the spelling of English or other European languages, out of irregularities in the sound-symbol relationship. Instead, they arise from learner inability to distinguish between some Arabic sounds whose pronunciations to some extent look/sound similar to non- native learners, for example (ط/ ṭ /) and (خ/ t). Another commonly known problem of written Arabic is hamzah ()همزج. Burj (1978) states that the rules governing hamzah are so complicated and vary according to its position within the word, that they cause problems not only for foreign learners, but also native speakers of Arabic. Moreover, Al-Juhany (1990) argues that the way hamzah is presented to students in Arabic programmes and the teaching methods used add to the complexity of this issue. He further states that this matter needs expert and well-trained teachers provided with educational resources. Arabic morphology Arabic, like all Semitic languages, is characterized by the use of certain morphological patterns. Thus, the majority of Arabic words are derived from, and can be analysed from their roots, which represent meaning. These roots usually consist of three consonants, which form the basis for the formation of numerous words not necessary. In other words, by using the three consonants of the root and by varying the vowel of the simple root, and adding prefixes, infixes, and suffixes, according to certain consonants, the actual words are produced. For example, the following derivations can be made from the root KTB, ‘( ’كتةwrite): Moreover, in Arabic, there are two different systems for definite and indefinite nouns. Firstly, a definite noun is indicated by the definite article, al-atareef corresponding to the word „the‟ in English and appearing with the noun as one word. Although the definite article always has the same written shape, its pronunciation differs depending on the following letter, which can lead to some confusion if not taught well (Al-Mutawa, 1995). Secondly, nouns and adjectives can also be indefinite when the vowel signs for one of three case endings, called in Arabic tanwīn „’التنويه, appear at the end of the word and make double short vowels. There are three tanwīn in Arabic: tanwīn adham, tanwīn alfatah and tanwīn alkasr. This means that they are to be pronounced with a final (n) but not written as an (n), for example, /kitabun/ كتاب, /kitaban / كتاتا, /kitabin / كتاب. كاتة/kātib/ „writer‟, مكتوب/maktub/ „something written‟, مكتثح/maktaba/ „library‟, مكتة/ maktab/ „office‟, كتاب/kitāb / „book‟. Some researchers found that the characteristics of Arabic morphology caused difficulties for students of Arabic, as some could not make a link between the new word they learnt and its official root (e.g. Burj, 1978; Al-Juhany, 1990). However, Stevens (2006, p.24) suggests that it is “learnable” and can even make acquiring vocabulary far easier than might be the case in many other languages where derivational patterns are haphazard. In Arabic, it is often not necessary to learn vocabulary as a separate activity because the general character of derivational morphology makes vocabulary learning more straightforward. Nevertheless, this necessitates using teaching methodologies that help familiarise students with these forms. In this case, understanding the root system will make learning and remembering vocabulary easier (Burj, 1978). The issue of using the nunation (or tanwīn „ ) ’التنويهin Arabic leads to some challenges in reading, writing and pronunciation if the text is not marked, especially for beginner learners who are not familiar with Arabic grammar. Therefore, learners must know which tanwīn they have to use to produce the correct meaning (Al-Tueriqy, 1988; Ibrahim, Khateb & Taha, 2013). 709 Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Vol. 9 Issue 4 Oct.) Arabic phonology 2015 students to learn how to pronounce them. He further argues that although adult learners are less likely to adopt the right Arabic pronunciation, they might have other skills which children do not seem to have which make them motivated to acquire these sounds. He reiterates that these sounds can be produced by Arabic learners if account is taken of the importance of phonetic training programmes and the application of new sound technology. As stated earlier, Arabic differs from other languages in many ways. One of the most crucial differences is the way in which some Arabic sounds are pronounced. It has been widely reported by non-native learners that at least one of the nine sounds presented below caused a problem in their speaking, understanding and even emotional attitude in learning Arabic (Taaima & Al-Naqa, 2006; Madkoor, 2007). Theses sounds are (ح/ ħ/), (خ /ḫ/), (ص/ ṣ /), (ض/ḍ /), (ط/ ṭ /), (ظ/ ẓā/), (ع/ ʕ /), ( غ/ ġ /), (ق/q/). One of the arguments regarding pronunciation is whether Arabic sounds should be presented using the transliteration method (the practice of using Latin script instead of Arabic script for rendering the language) or by using the Arabic script from the early stage of learning (Al-Juhany, 1990). Supporters of the transliteration method believe that it might provide beginner learners who know no Arabic with some idea of the sound intended. This helps them to avoid the difficulty of writing Arabic until they are familiar with Arabic script. However, as many Arabic sounds are totally unlike those of English or other European languages, the transliteration method can be a phonetically vague approximation. Consequently, students relying on any form of transliteration will be hindered and delay their ultimate grasp of authentic script and the acquisition of sound-script correspondences (Beeston, 1968). This difficulty mainly results from the fact that there is no equivalent to some of these sounds in English and most other languages. For example, the sound (خ/ḫ/) has no comparable sound in English. It is something like (kh), pronounced as far back in the throat as possible. (ح/ħ/) is a peculiarly Arabic consonant. It is a (h) but must be sharply distinguished from the throat. The same thing could apply to the (ص/ṣ /),(ض/ḍ /), (ق/q/), (ع/ʕ/) and (غ/ġ /) which have an equal in other spoken languages (Al-Mutawa, 1995). In order to produce these sounds, the majority of learners tend to convert them into other sounds that are easier to create or are closer to their native language. Al-Juhany (1990) and Burj (1978) found that this subsequently leads to confusion in student linguistic expression or the loss of the intended expression. Additionally, Al-Mutawa‟s (1995) findings showed that some learners might be able to produce some of these sounds correctly, but this involves an unnatural exaggeration in the movement of their lips and their articulation in general, which distorts their normal communication. It has also been found that the transliteration method confuses Arabic language learners in writing, speaking and dictionary usage. For example, both the word sāra ‘( ’سارhe went) and ṣāra „( ’صارto be) are written in the same way in Latin script ‘Saara’ despite their different meanings. Moreover, using an Arabic dictionary might be more problematic if the learners are not sure of the sound they heard; they might search for another word and get the incorrect meaning (Ibrahim, 2001). What makes the issue more complicated is that some learners may memorise the word with the wrong meaning and when they come to use it, either in speaking or writing, they do so incorrectly. This confusion has been found to lead to some emotional difficulties, as many learners report that very often they are surprised when they discover that the word they learnt had another meaning. Their embarrassment even increases when they fail to convey a clear linguistic message while communicating with others (Taaima & Al-Naqa, 2006). However, Al-Juhany (1990) proposes that these sounds can be produced by learners if a tremendous effort is made by Arabic teachers to help them recognize stress points. He also argues that the description of sounds cannot be understandable without the use of the technical phonetic methodology used by special Arabic native teachers who have themselves received training in Arabic phonetics. Additionally, Al-Juhany (1990) suggests that the beliefs of teachers might be another reason for the difficulties in Arabic pronunciation. He states that many teachers think that these consonants are too difficult for foreigners to produce and, therefore, they do not encourage 710 Arabic in Foreign Language Programmes: Difficulties and Challenges Fatma Al-Busaidi Despite the recurrence of this problem with a great number of Arabic learners, as reported by Madkoor (2007), the majority of student books that are designed to teach Arabic, especially for beginners, are still written in Latin script. This might help beginners to some extent, but it does not provide learners with the intensive strategies necessary to help them overcome the confusion that might appear from mixing up sounds. In addition, it will delay student acquisition of Arabic sounds and scripts (Al-Juhany, 1990). Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 for students who cannot be transferred from one programme to another, and those who begin to study Arabic in one Arab country and cannot carry on their learning in another. The present dilemma in Arabic teaching is complicated further by the varied quality of language training available in overseas Arabic programmes. Although, as stated by Al-Batal (2007), the Arabic study-abroad experiences of American students tend to be positive in terms of cultural exposure, such programmes frequently lack curricular articulation with their corresponding U.S. institutions, and teachers experience poor faculty training in language pedagogy to assist the needs of American learners. Batal (2007) refers also to the financial issues linked to this. Study-abroad programmes have no funding to bring about curricular changes or provide in-service teacher training and professionalization. Pedagogical factors affecting teaching and learning Arabic as a foreign language Despite the efforts that have been made by some professionals to develop programmes for TAFL, there are common pitfalls in some of these programmes. These can be summarized as follows: Programmes lacking clear objectives Lack of experienced or qualified teachers Abboud (1995) states the following: “It is sad to note that after all these years there are still Arabic programmes, and they seem to be the rule rather than exception, that do not have clearly articulated objectives for their sequential language courses” (p.26). In the same vein, Abboud (1995) highlights that Arabic programmes in the Arab world, in general, need to be improved to address this issue. This cannot be achieved unless the goal of learning the language is carefully addressed to cover the need of each group of learners. Lack of coordination between Arabic programmes There is almost complete agreement between professionals that a good teacher is the fundamental element of the TAFL programme. A good teacher can give life to the curriculum and language resources, attract student awareness, raise their curiosity and enthusiasm, and make learning an enjoyable process (Belnap, 1995; Nahla, 2006). However, qualified teachers are still in short supply in the field of Arabic instruction, as pointed out by several researchers (e.g. Nahla, 2006; Nash, 2010). They all agree that many teachers are not equipped with an Arabic teaching methodology that leads to language proficiency and some are not even qualified to teach Arabic to native speakers. Nash (2010) contends that being a native speaker of Arabic is not enough to be able to teach it. Similarly, Belnap (1995), in his evaluative study of Arabic teachers in some Arabic institutions, indicated that none of the teachers at these small institutions had formal training in Arabic teaching. He says that this might be a major reason for the limited number of students achieving high levels of language acquisition in the Arabic language. Lambert (1992) considers the lack of coordination between Arabic programmes as one of the greatest weaknesses of Arabic language education programmes. He argues that these programmes normally work essentially in isolation from each other. This leads to problems Unfortunately, even as late as 2006, the field of Arabic was still lacking in trained professionals. Ryding (2006) states that there are only a small number of people that can be considered professional teachers of Arabic as a foreign language and that “the active membership of Abboud (1995) refers to another significant challenge resulting from not having clear objectives in Arabic teaching and learning programmes. He found that students of Arabic courses complain that when they move from one level to another, or even from one semester to another in these programmes, they are often passed along to teachers who are unaware of what they did in previous courses. Moreover, they can often be presented with materials and methods that have nothing to do with those previously used. 711 Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Vol. 9 Issue 4 Oct.) 2015 Shortage of materials and resources the American Association of Teachers of Arabic [AATA] currently numbers about 130” (p. 13). Therefore, this has led many universities in the US to not be able to accommodate the increasing numbers wishing to enrol in Arabic programmes. Although some Arabic programmes in the West and in some Arab countries such as Egypt have introduced materials and resources to support the learning of Arabic, the lack of appropriate materials is a major concern for many professionals in the field. For example, while there are various textbooks, supplemental materials and E-learning programmes available to teach other languages such as English, all these resources are almost completely absent from Arabic programmes (Alosh, 1997; Stevens, 2006; Taaima & AlNaqa, 2006). This challenging issue seems to be associated with the lack of programmes that prepare teachers of Arabic as a foreign language. According to Al-Batal (2007), only one program at the University of Michigan offers a graduate degree in TAFL and, even there, the programme employs only one Arabic specialist. In Arab countries the situation is similar. Nahle (2006, p.76), for example, states the following: “There is no university in Egypt that has a programme for preparing teachers to teach Arabic to non-native speakers except the American University in Cairo.” This is also applicable to other Arab countries (except the Khartoum International Institute in Sudan which began some work on this in 1974). Madkoor (2007) evaluated the few valid programmes in the Arab world, such as those in Egypt and Sudan, and states that they struggle to provide teachers with the skills needed for Arabic teaching. One of the major recommendations was the inclusion of creative modern resources and varied materials in Arabic teacher education. This has led Arabic teachers in many Arab countries to be more dependent on materials available to teach Arabic as a first language. This issue was reported to be one of the major worries of many Arab educators. For example, Al-Batal (2007) found that Arabic teachers have to choose very difficult texts, which are only suitable for native speakers with a high level of Arabic. His findings showed that this matter has made learning the language a challenge and has led learners to suffer psychologically. They go through difficult experiences of disappointment, anxiety and, even worse, they form a negative attitude towards learning Arabic. This problem was reiterated by some researchers (e.g. Allen & Allouche, 1986; AlBatal, 1992) who found that it is difficult to find appropriate teaching materials in the Western context. This forces many AFL teachers to develop their own curriculum and materials to suit the particular needs of American students of Arabic Al-Mutawa (1995) argues that both native and non-native speakers find challenges in the field of teaching Arabic. She found that although non-native speaking teachers have good education and training, they have problems with the pronunciation of many Arabic sounds. Secondly, some native teachers were found to use their dialects in the classroom and some did not or were not able to pronounce some sounds, either because they do not have those sounds that sound in their dialects or they used different pronunciations. Versteegh (2006) added that there is a need for a reliable grammar reference. He found that most students had to make use of the oldfashioned grammar of classical Arabic. He also stated: “lexicographical tools are still a problem; the dictionaries that are published in the Middle East, especially in Lebanon, tend to focus on the classical language and are less practical for beginner students” (pp.9-10). Therefore, the only option available for students is an Arabic /English dictionary, which does not help those who do not speak English. It has also been found that the majority of teachers of Arabic use traditional teaching methodologies such as memorization, and grammar-translation. Additionally, most of the Arabic courses, including the communicative Arabic courses, are not designed to serve communicative purposes. The focus has always been to understand Arabic grammar and comprehension (Siti Ikbal, 2006). Additionally, the limitation of educational resources for developing is another major concern. The dialects of Arabic are not written down, and as they are different from one 712 Arabic in Foreign Language Programmes: Difficulties and Challenges Fatma Al-Busaidi country to another, it is very difficult to produce dialect resources for all these different dialects (Al-Batal, 1992, 1995b; Wahba, 2006). Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 standing in their Arabic course reported social difficulties in dealing with unfamiliar social customs. This was one reason cited for some of them being unable to use social interactions to improve their Arabic. To deal with this issue in Egypt, another approach was established which was to give the students orientation lectures to introduce them to the characteristics of Egyptian society and social customs as soon as they started the course (Nahla, 2006). Insufficient presentation of Arab culture It has been widely agreed that culture and language are entangled. Many researchers in the TAFL field (e.g. Al-Batal, 1988; Suleiman, 1993; Elgibali & Taha, 1995; Taha, 2006) have emphasised that cultural competence is one of the important factors of language competence. Therefore, they highlighted the significance of building a curriculum that helps students to understand religion, history, politics, and other issues related to Arabic culture. They found that students with knowledge of these important aspects of Arabic culture were more able to get involved with people and more confident in using the environment surrounding them. Conclusion This paper has presented a review of the literature on Arabic language programmes beginning with a brief review of the historical background of TAFL and its challenges, and followed by a description of the Arabic language and its characteristics. Additionally, factors that affect the learning of Arabic, such as linguistic and pedagogical factors have been presented. Belnap‟s (1995) findings confirmed that cultural proficiency has regained prominence as a primary objective of language learning, as the majority of students in his research placed cultural understanding among their primary reasons for studying the language. Similarly, Nahla (2006) found that many students believe that learning Arabic in Arab countries is more helpful than learning it in non-Arabic speaking countries. The learners believe that learning Arabic in Arab countries gives them the opportunity to learn valuable information with regard to Arab society in terms of how people live, how they deal with each other, what interests them, and what makes them happy, sad or angry. In addition, this presents a good opportunity for learning local stories, anecdotes, and the views of Arabic-speaking people towards the world around them. The conclusion to draw from this paper is that the Arabic language is clearly attracting attention worldwide; however, enormous problems in teaching Arabic as a foreign language need to be considered, and scholars in the field need to make a concerted effort to improve the profession. It seems that some of the difficulties and challenges are likely to be due to specific features of the Arabic language itself. This is possibly due to the radical difference between spoken Arabic dialects and the official written language taught in school and used for academic materials. Additionally, having different Arabic programmes in different universities shows that there is no agreement as to what kind of Arabic should be taught in Arabic programmes. Even in the case of providing the two forms of Arabic, Modern Standard Arabic and a chosen dialect, there is no agreement about which form to start with and which dialects should be taught. This fundamental question still needs to be answered by applying empirical research that will help our understanding of this issue. Although some of the studies that have sought to understand students‟ opinions regarding which form they would like to learn indicate that they prefer learning spoken Arabic (Schmidt, Inbar & Shohamy, 2004; Palmer, 2007, 2008), it is vital to select the approach that will best fit students‟ needs. It is highly Anghelescu‟s (2006) work indicates that students who studied Arabic in a programme in which cultural understanding was ignored faced challenges, even at advanced levels. She states that the issues of cultural and linguistic discontinuity hinder potential learners from the outset. She also cites Killean (1997), who argues that the difficulties of learning Arabic in the West pertain not only to the language system, but also to the cultural foreignness felt by the learners. In the same vein, Nahla (2006) found that foreign learners of Arabic studying in Egypt who were not provided with any cultural under- 713 Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Vol. 9 Issue 4 Oct.) 2015 recommended that the field strategically evaluate these programmes to understand more about the strengths and weaknesses of each approach and to determine which ones (programmes) work better for students. Finding the best approaches that fit students‟ needs will require constant monitoring and evaluation to ensure programme effectiveness. Finally, it is suggested that the field of Arabic language education cannot be improved unless policy makers in Arab countries are aware of the importance of improving the quality of Arabic as a foreign language programmes. The first step might be by establishing a clear policy or vision for teaching and learning Arabic as a foreign language. It is also vital for students interested in learning both forms of Arabic, (standard and a dialect), to balance and bridge the difficult gap between written and spoken skills by providing the two forms of Arabic concurrently. Although this approach poses inherent difficulties in some programmes, thoughtfully designed and carefully implemented combinations of the two forms can provide a more comprehensive and accurate understanding of the Arabic language. References Abboud, P. (1995). The teaching of Arabic in the United States: Whence & whither? In M. Al-Batal (Ed.), The teaching of Arabic as a foreign language: Issues and directions (pp.13–33). Provo, UT: American Association of Teachers of Arabic. Abdelhadi, S., Ibrahim, R., & Eviatar, Z. (2011). Perceptual load in the reading of Arabic: Effects of orthographic visual complexity on detection. Writing Systems Research, 3(2), 117–127. doi: 10.1186/17449081-8-3. The challenges that face Arabic programmes can also be attributed to the lack of a systematic approach to the integration of Arabic culture as an important factor for language competence. Developing such an integrated approach remains one of the most pressing challenges that the field faces at this time. Therefore, it is important to consider that teaching Arabic should not just be limited to classes, but should also involve organizing seminars and lectures for students to clarify aspects of Arab culture and civilization and to shed light on the cultural differences between their own societies and Arab society. Abu-Hatab, W. (1992). Error analysis of compositions written by Community College students. Unpublished Master dissertation. University of Jordan, Amman, Jordan. Agius, D. A. (Ed.), (1990). Diglossic tension: Teaching Arabic for communication. Leeds, England.: Folia Scholastic. Al-Batal, M. (1988). Towards cultural proficiency in Arabic. Foreign language Annals, 21(5), 443-448. Although the previous research has provided evidence enabling one to understand the factors behind Arabic learning difficulties, the majority of these findings have been based on studies involving native speakers of English. Thus, this research is not necessarily valid for speakers of other languages. It must be kept in mind that the difficulty in learning Arabic might be largely related to other factors such as the extent of similarity between Arabic and one‟s own native language or other languages one has previously studied. Furthermore, difficulties in learning Arabic could be related to what students believe about Arabic culture, the learning context, and the quality of the programmes and students‟ prior knowledge. Therefore, more research is needed in this field. In particular, there is an urgent need for research that seeks to understand how Arabic could be learnt despite these challenges and what could be done to facilitate such learning. Al-Batal, M. (1992). Diglossia proficiency: The need for an alternative approach to teaching. In A. Rouchdy, (Ed.), The Arabic language in America (pp.284-304). Detroit, MI: Wayne State University Press. AL-Batal, M. (1995a). The Arabic teaching profession: Current realities and future challenges. In M. Al-Batal, (Ed.), The teaching of Arabic as a foreign language: Issues and directions (pp.1-11). Provo, UT: American Association of Teachers of Arabic. Al-Batal, M. (1995b). Issues in the teaching of the productive skills in Arabic. In M. AlBatal, (Ed.), The teaching of Arabic as a foreign language: Issues and directions (pp.115133). Provo, UT: American Association of Teachers of Arabic. 714 Arabic in Foreign Language Programmes: Difficulties and Challenges Fatma Al-Busaidi Al-Batal, M. (2007). Arabic and national language education policy. Perspectives. The Modern Language Journal. 91(2), 268-271. Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 Al-Tueriqy, M. (1988). A field study of the major problems facing non-native speakers of Arabic. Unpublished Master dissertation, Islamic University in Medina,city?Kingdom of Saudi Arabia. Al-Batal, M., & Belnap, R. (2006). The teaching and learning of Arabic in the United States: Realities, needs, and future directions. In K. Wahba, Z. Taha, & L. England, (Eds.), Handbook for Arabic language teaching professionals in the 21st century (pp.389-399). Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Anghelescu, N. (2006). The Arabic language at the University of Bucharest. In K. Wahba, Z. Taha, & L. England, (Eds.), Handbook for Arabic language teaching professionals in the 21st century (pp.115-122). Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Al-Hamad, F. (1983). Problems in teaching Arabic to foreigners. In O. Jonathan, & A. Issam, (Eds.), Proceedings of the second Annual linguistics conference (pp.81-100). Yarmouk University, Irbid, Jordan. Awde, N., & Samano, P. (1986). The Arabic alphabet: How to read and write it. London, England: Saqi Books. Beeston, A. (1968). Written Arabic: An approach to the basic structures. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Al-Juhany, A. (1990). Problems and solutions of teaching Arabic to speakers of other languages. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Islamic University of Medina, city? Kingdom of Saudi Arabia. Belnap, R. (1987). Who's taking Arabic and what on earth for? A survey of students in Arabic language programmes. AlArabiyya, 20, 29-42. Al-Khatib, M. (2000). The Arab world: Language and cultural issues. Language, Culture and Curriculum, 13(2), 121-125. Belnap, R. (1995). The institutional setting of Arabic language teaching: A survey of programme coordinators and teachers of Arabic in U.S. institutions of higher learning. In M. Al-Batal, (Ed.), The teaching of Arabic as a foreign language: Issues and directions (pp.34-78). Provo, UT: American Association of Teachers of Arabic. Allen, R. (1987). The Arabic guidelines: Here now? In S. Charles, & H. Chip, (Eds.), ACTFL proficiency guidelines for the less commonly taught languages. Washington, DC: The Center for Applied Llinguistics & ACTFL. American Council on the Tteaching of Fforeign Llanguage (ACTFL).1989. Foreign Language Annals, 22(4), 373-392. Belnap, R. (2006). A profile of students of Arabic in U.S. universities. In K. Wahba, Z. Taha, & L. England, (Eds.), Handbook for Arabic language teaching professionals in the 21st century (pp.389-399). Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Allen, R., & Allouche, A. (1986). Let’s learn Arabic: A proficiency-based syllabus for Modern Standard Arabic. Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania Press. Allen, R. (2007). Arabic-flavor of the moment: Whence, why and how? The Modern Language Journal. 91(2), 258-261. Burj, T. (1978). The difficulties facing learners of Arabic as a foreign language: Scientific and practical solutions. Proceedings of the first international symposium on teaching Arabic to non-Arabic speakers. 2, (pp.131151), King Saud University, Al-Riyadh, The Kingdom of Saudi Arabia or Saudi Arabia. Al-Mutawa, N. (1995). Challenges facing learners of Arabic as a foreign language at Al-Kuwait University. The College of Education Journal, 2, 12-20. Alosh, M. (1997). Learner, text and context in foreign language acquisition: An Arabic perspective, pathways to advanced skills series. The Ohio State University National Foreign Language Resource Centre. If a book you need city and state. Cote, R. (2009). Choosing one dialect for the Arabic speaking world: A status planning dilemma. Arizona Working Papers in SLA & Teaching, 16, 75- 97. Retrieved October 10, 2009 from http://w3.coh.arizona.edu/awp. 715 Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Vol. 9 Issue 4 Oct.) Dickins, J, and Watson, J. (2006). Arabic Teaching in Britain and Ireland. In Kassem M, Wahba, Zeinab A. Taha and Liz England (eds) Handbook for Arabic Language Teaching Professionals in the 21st Century. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 107-114. 2015 Ibrahim, R., Khateb, A., & Taha, H. (2013). How does type of orthography affect reading in Arabic and Hebrew as first and second languages? Scientific Research, 3(1), 40-46. doi: 10.4236/ojml.2013.31005. Killean, C. (1997). Learning Arabic: A lifetime commitment. In A. Afsaruddin, & M. Zahniswe, (Eds.), Humanism, culture, and language in the Near East: Studies in Honor pf Georg Krotkoff (pp.145-154). Winona Lake, State?: Eisenbrauns. Eisele, J. (2006). Developing frames of reference for assessment and curricular design in a diglossic L2: From skills to tasks (and back again). In K. Wahba, Z. Taha, & L. England, (Eds.), Handbook for Arabic language teaching professionals in the 21st century (pp.197-220). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Lambert, R. (1992). Foreign language planning in the United States. NFLC Occasional Papers. Washington, DC: The National Foreign Language Center. Elgibali, A., & Taha, Z. (1995). Teaching Arabic as a foreign language: Challenges of the nineties. In M. Al-Batal, (Ed.), The teaching of Arabic as a foreign language: Issues and directions (pp.79-102). Provo, UT: American Association of Teachers of Arabic. Liskin-Gasparro, J. (1982). ETS Oral proficiency testing manual. Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service. Maamouri, M. (1998). Arabic diglossia and its impact on the quality of education in the Arab region. A paper presented at The World Bank Mediterranean Development Forum, Marrakech, Morocco (September 3-6) Ferguson, C. A. (1959). Diglossia. Word, 15, 325-340. Fragman, A., & Russak, S. (2010). A qualitative analysis of spelling errors in Arabic as a foreign language among native Hebrew speaking students. Teaching and learning the Arabic language, pp. 103– 114. Madkoor, A. (2007). Teaching Arabic as a foreign language. Cairo, Egypt: Dar Alfikr AlArabi. McCarus, E. N. (1987). The Study of Arabic in the United States: A history of its Development. Al-Arabiyya, 20, 13-27. Haeri, N. (2000). Form and ideology: Arabic sociolinguistics and beyond. Annual Review of Anthropology, 29, 61-87. McCarus, E. (1992). History of Arabic study in the United States. In Aleya Rouchdy (Ed.), The Arabic language in America (pp. 207-221). Detroit, MI: Wayne State University Press. Hansen, G. F. (2010). Word recognition in Arabic as a foreign language. The Modern Language Journal, 94(4), 567–579. doi:10.1111/j.1540-4781.2010.01094.x. Myhill, J. (2014) The Effect of Diglossia on Literacy in Arabic and Other Languages in E. Saiegh-Haddad, R. M. Joshi (eds.), Handbook of Arabic Literacy, Literacy Studies ( 197-223 ) Springer Science & Business, Media Dordrecht 2014. Harvey, D. (1979) Spoken Arabic. CS: Hodder & Stoughton. Heath, P. (1990). Proficiency in Arabic language learning: some reflections on basic goals. Al-Arabiyya, 23, 31-48. Holes, C. (1995). Modern Arabic: Structures, functions and varieties. London, England: Longman. Nash, A. (2010) Critical issues of Arabic learning and teaching, an interview with Michael Cooperson. Issues in Applied Linguistics, 18(1), 125-139. Ibrahim, M. H. (2001). Communicating in Arabic: Problems and prospects. In C. Florian, (Ed.), Language Adaptation (pp.39-59). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Nahla, M.A. (2006). Teaching Arabic to nonnative speakers at Alexandria University. In K. Wahba, Z. Taha, & L. England, (Eds.), Handbook for Arabic language teaching professionals in the 21st century (pp.67- 716 Arabic in Foreign Language Programmes: Difficulties and Challenges Fatma Al-Busaidi 79). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 difficulty in learning Arabic as a second language. In K. Wahba, Z. Taha, & L. England, (Eds.), Handbook for Arabic language teaching professionals in the 21st century (pp.34-63). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Nicola, M. (1990). Starting Arabic with dialect. In D. Aguis, (Ed.), Diglossic tension: Teaching Arabic for communication (pp.4245). Beaconsfield Papers. Leeds, England: Folia Scholastica. Suleiman, Y. (1993). TAFL and the teaching/learning of culture: Theoretical perspectives and an experimental module. Al-Arabiyya, 26, 61-111. Palmer, J. L. (2007). Arabic diglossia: Teaching only the standard variety is a disservice to students. Arizona Working Papers in Second Language Acquisition and Teaching, 14. Online at http://w3.coh.arizona.edu/awp/Volum es.htm. Suleiman, Y. (2003). The Arabic language and national identity. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press. Taaima. R., & Al-Naqa, K. (2006). Teaching Arabic communicatively: Instructions and strategies. AL- Rabat, Morocco: Islamic Educational Scientific Cultural Organization (ISESCO). Palmer, J. L. (2008). Arabic diglossia: Student perceptions of spoken Arabic after living in the Arabic-speaking world. Arizona Working Papers in Second Language Acquisition and Teaching, 15, 81-95. Online at http://w3.coh.arizona.edu/awp/ . don‟t switch to and from italics. Ryding, K. (1994). Fostering a learning community for Arabic. Theory into Practice, 33(1), 23-28. Taha, Z. (2006). Toward pragmatic competency in Arabic. In K. Wahba, Z. Taha, & L. England, (Eds.), Handbook for Arabic language teaching professionals in the 21st century (pp. 343-361). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Ryding, K. (1995). Discourse competence in TAFL: Skill levels and choice of language variety in the Arabic classroom. In M. AlBatal, (Ed.), The teaching of Arabic as a foreign language: Issues and directions (pp.115133). Provo, UT: American Association of Teachers of Arabic. Van Mol, M. (2006). Arabic receptive language teaching: A new CALL approach. In K. Wahba, Z. Taha, & L. England, (Eds.), Handbook for Arabic language teaching professionals in the 21st century (pp. 305-314). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Ryding, K. (2006). Teaching Arabic in the United States. In K. Wahba, Z. Taha, & L. England, (Eds.), Handbook for Arabic language teaching professionals in the 21st century (pp.13-20). Mahwah NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Versteegh, K. (2006). History of Arabic language teaching. In K. Wahba, Z. Taha, & L. England, (Eds.), Handbook for Arabic Language teaching professionals in the 21st century (pp.3-12). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Saiegh-H., initial? & Henkin- R. (2014). The Structure of Arabic Language and Orthography. In E. Saiegh-Haddad, R. M. Joshi (eds.), Handbook of Arabic Literacy, Literacy Studies (3-28 ). Wahba. K.M. (2006). Arabic language use and the? educated language user. In K. Wahba, Z. Taha, & L. England, (Eds.), Handbook for Arabic language teaching professionals in the 21st century (pp.139-155). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Siti Ikbal S. S. (2006). The problem of Arabic speaking skill among the students of religious secondary schools in Selangor. In M. Rosdi Ismail, & M. Taib Pa (Eds.), The teaching and learning of Arabic Language in Malaysia.Kuala Lumpur: city, country: University Malaya publications. Younes, M. (1995). An integrated curriculum for elementary Arabic. In M. Al-Batal, (Ed.), The teaching of Arabic as a foreign language: Issues and directions (pp.233255). Provo, UT: American Association of Teachers of Arabic. Stevens, P. B. (2006). Is Spanish really so easy? Is Arabic really so hard? Perceived 717 Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Pages 718-729) Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 Relationship between Social Anxiety and Parental Authority among College of Education Students’ at SQU Hilal Z. Al- Nabhani* & Abdulhameed S. Hassan Sultan Qaboos University, Sultanate of Oman ___________________________________________ Received: 30/8/2015 Revised: 20/9/2015 Accepted: 28/9/2015 _____________________________________________ Abstract: The purpose of this study was to explore the relationship between parental authority and social anxiety. The study attempted to answer the following question: Are there significant differences in social anxiety due to parental authority factors: (support, control, psychological control, emotion and accomplishment of missions)? Data was collected from 172 students of College of Education at Sultan Qaboos University. The Parental Authority Questionnaire (PAQ), and Social Anxiety Scale (SAS) were used to assess variables. To answer the study question, regression analysis was used. Findings indicated that there was significant relationship between parental authority and social anxiety in several domains. The findings also revealed that there were significant differences in social anxiety due to mother authority and student's gender. Keywords: University students, parental authority, gender, social aniety. عالقة القلق االجتناعي بسلطة الوالدين لدى طلبة كلية الرتبية يف جامعة السلطان قابوس هالل بن زاهر النبواني* وعبد احلنيد سعيد حسن سلطنة عنان،جامعة السلطان قابوس _____________________________________________ ًكنا تسعى إىل اإلجابة ع. ٌ سعت ٍذه الدراسة إىل اختبار العالقة بني سلطة الىالديً والقلق االجتناع:مستخلص والضبط، والضبط، ٍل تىجد فزوق يف القلق االجتناعٌ تعىد إىل عىامل سلطة الىالديً (الدعه:ٌالسؤال اآلت ٌ طالباً مً كلًة الرتبًة يف جامعة السلطا172 واإلجناس)؟ مجعت بًاىات الدراسة مً عًية بلغت، واالىفعال،ٌاليفس كنا مت استخداو حتلًل، ومت استخداو استبًاٌ السلطة الىالدية ومقًاس القلق االجتناعٌ جلنع البًاىات.قابىس أشارت اليتائج إىل وجىد عالقة بني سلطة الىالديً والقلق االجتناعٌ؛ كنا.االحندار لإلجابة عً سؤال الدراسة .ٌكصفت اليتائج عً أثز لسلطة األو واليىع االجتناعٌ يف عدد مً أبعاد القلق االجتناع .ٌ القلق االجتناع، اجليس،ً سلطة الىالدي، طلبة اجلامعة:الللنات املفتاحًة *munabh@squ.edu.om 718 Relationship between Social Anxiety and Parental Authority Hilal Al- Nabhani & Abdulhameed Hassan Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 Mcloed, Sigman, Hwang & Chu, 2003) emphasized that there was a relationship between the parental stereotype of excessive protection and high control and social anxiety. However, these findings were not harmonic with the findings of a study conducted by Ballash, Pernble, Usui, Buckley and WoodruffBorden (2oo6) which indicated an existing relationship between a family environment characterized by empathy, behavioural control, excessive protection and social anxiety symptoms. Anxiety disorders are among the most common mental health problems experienced by children and young people (Costello, Egger, & Angold, 2005).Social anxiety is the second most common anxiety disorder in adults (Kessler et al.,2005). According to the diagnostic criteria; social anxiety disorder (SAD), which is also referred to as Social Phobia is one of the six major anxiety disorders listed in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorder (DSM-V; APA, 2013). The social anxiety disorder is considered as marked and persistent fear of social or performance situation (Sparrevohn and Rapee, 2009). Research revealed that the most common mental health disorders are mood and anxiety disorders, affecting 6% of the adolescent population in the United States (Byrne, 2000). In Canada, anxiety disorders are the most common of all mental health problems, affecting one in 10 people, children and adults (Canadian Mental Health Association- CMHA, 2010). Research in related literature signified that environmental conditions play an important role in developing social anxiety particularly in children because of early childhood experiences perceived as threatening situations, these painful situations cause negative feelings in children which are considered as symptoms of social anxiety (Beauchaine & Hinshaw, 2013). The findings of several studies suggested that parenting style followed by negative methods and treatments may lead to increased social anxiety in their children (e.g., Akinsola & Udoka, 2013, Ballash, Pemble, Buckly & Woodruff – Borden, 2006; kulaksizoglu,1998). SAD may be caused by many factors, such as: negative and aversive experiences like: shyness, painful or vocational events, harsh treatment and lack of social skills. In addition, parental rejection, negligence, aggression, lack of warmth and intimacy, and inadequate adaptation are also other sources that may elicit social anxiety (Leahy & Holland, 2000; Zimmer-Gembeck, Nesdale, 2013; Rudoloph, 2013). Social anxiety is a general term that might be a result of social conditions. When social anxiety causes problems, it is called social phobia which also includes some symptoms existing in other disorders as shyness and fears caused by criticism, embarrassment, or rejection of others. SAD may lead to high risks of social danger causing disntegration or disturbance in social relationship ( Hanlon, 2011). SAD was examined by several studies. In literature, it was defined as social apprehension which leads to avoiding social situations accompanied by excessive obsession, rejection or embarrassment. Heimberg and Becker (2002) indicated that social anxiety is excessive fear of some social situations, so students with social anxiety demonstrated weak acedemic performance in different classroom activities specially in formal speech and classroom presentation. Patterns of parenting received a great attention in research. (Chan and Chan, 2009; Coln, Jordan and Mercer, 2011; Darling and Steinberg, 2013) asserted the importance of parental support and warmth responses in order to achieve positive results in their children. This is considered as an assessment of the parents' behavior ranging from a continuous amount of independence to complete control of children's behavior especially during adolescence (Driscoll, Russell and Crockett, 2008). SAD is considered a psychological state resulting from fear of negative assessment (Clark & Wells, 1995). Social fears are common among normal people and may range from simple to high (Ruscio, et al 2008). Parents are considered one of the important factors in the protection of their children from social anxiety. Several studies (e.g., Barnett,et al., 2004, Grcco & Morris, 2002; Lieb, et al, 2000; Mcloed, Wood & Weisz, 2007 and Wood, The results of this systematic review provided fairly consistent preliminary evidence for an association between anxiety and perceived parental control and anxious parenting in adolescence. (Waitea, Whittingtonb & Creswella, 2014). 719 Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Vol. 9 Issue 4 Oct.) In addition, parental practices played an important role in creating SAD. This importance was shown in some research, such as: (Andrews, et al.,2003; Erkan, 2002; Cagdas & Secer, 2004; Yavuzer, 2005 and Baldwin, Mcintyre & Hardway ,2007); which stressed that the authoritative parenting style may make the child's beliefs irrational and lead to lack in his/her ability to control threatening events. As a result of these events, the children are motivated to avoid diverse social situations. 2015 tional problems and difficulties that are strongly related to the quality of the relationship between parents and children. (Lerner, et al., 1996). Consequently, Bandura (1997) considered that it is necessary to clarify the factors affecting the parental stereotypes. The nature and quality of the interactions between parents and children continue to affect them in childhood, adolescence and adulthood. Among the factors affecting social anxiety is the type of the relation between adolescents and their parents. In this regard, Darling, Cumsille, Caldwell & Dowdy (2008); Darling, Cumsille & Martinez (2008) believed that adolescents viewed their parents’ authority as illegal in their developments. In this regard, Erozkan(2012); Marnnuzza, et al, ( 2002) and Rudolf ( 2013) maintained that the rejection style provided by parents might lead to social and emotional disorders . The findings pointed out that passive parental practices provoked anxiety and led to symptoms of depression and social anxiety (Kuhar-Meaka , 2010). Statement of problem As mentioned above, it is clear that SAD is a social problem affecting individuals in different degrees, and there are several factors causing it such as: painful early childhood experiences, child's personality, continuous frustrations, stresses, parenting styles, failure experiences specially in academic domain, and learning environment. These factors may contribute to shape social anxiety in university students.The rationale beyond conducting this study is that related literature in Middle East societies mainly depended on investigating the parental styles toghether in the same tool because our belief and cultural system see that the parental authority is only governed or determined by fathers' authority, and mothers are subordinate for this authority, but this study was to measure fathers' style and mothers' styles seperately. This method gives power for exploring differences among fathers' styles and mothers' styles in parenting and the role of these styles in shaping personality and causing various problems and disorders , such as social anxiety. Assadi, Zokaei, Kaviani, Mohammadi and Ghaeli (2007) noticed that In Middle East cultures not only provide information regarding parenting in other societies but also broaden our crosscultural database so as to better understand the role of cultural factors in parenting. The current study mainly focused on the invistigation of parenting styles as one of factors that cause SAD and social anxiety in the students of Sultan Qaboos University. Howerver, the study attempted to answer the following question: Are there significant The findings of a study conducted by (Daerling and Steinberg (1993); Esther (2013); Turner, Chadler & Heffer (2009); Chang,Oleson,Sameroff & Sexton (2009);Drisecoll, Russell & Crocket (2008) indicated that the authoritative parenting style was an important factor in causing social anxiety in children. McLeod, Wood, & Weisz (2007) studied parental rejection which involves withdrawal from or hostility towards the child and a lack of warmth, involvement, emotional support or reciprocity with the child. They hypothesized that this rejection undermines the child’s emotion regulation, thus increasing his/her sensitivity to anxiety. From the meta-analysis of 47 studies examining the association between childhood anxiety and parenting - a medium sized association between parental control and child anxiety, but a small association between parental rejection and child anxiety. Wood, et al. (2007) stressed that parental control of their children may be frightening and insecure, so this negative level of control may get the child inassertive and cause a sense of anxiety. Taylor, Jang, Stewart and Stein (2008) emphasized that hereditary factors, learning experiences and personal factors are all crucial determinants of high social anxiety. There are many change sex pertinence by individuals throughout their lives. These changes may occur during the adolescence stage which is characterized by a variety of emo- 720 Relationship between Social Anxiety and Parental Authority Hilal Al- Nabhani & Abdulhameed Hassan differences in social anxiety due to parental authority factors: (support, control, psychological control, emotion and accomplishment of missions)? were constructed based on two related scales (Kuhar, 2012; Reitman, Rhode, Hupp and Ahobello,2002). The scale consisted of 35 items distributed into five subscales: support, control, psychological control, emotion and accomplishment of missions. The PAQ items were responded according to a 5- point Likert scale :( strongly agree =5, agree=4, neutral=3, disagree=2, strongly disagree1=). The face validity scale was assessed by a panel of referees whose specializations are measurement and educational psychology. They were asked to assess the items based on the extent to which each is correlated to the concept of parental authority. The cut-off point of 80% was used to assess this correlation. The construct validity was also assessed by exploratory factor analysis. The loading rate of 0.30 was used to assess items' loadings. Factor analysis accounted for %48,066 and 48.066 of the total variance in fathers and mothers successively. Analysis extracted five factors (subscales), and the item loadings ranged from (0.328 – 0.709). Consequently, the following items: (3, 5, 6, 10, 11 in fathers), and (3, 4, 8, 11 in mothers) were deleted. Some extracted factors for the fathers were different from those of the mothers, and the others are similar. The extracted factors for the fathers were: (support, psychological control, open discussion of Ideas, control, emotion), and those for the mothers were: (support, control, psychological control, emotion, accomplishment). The results of factor analysis are illustrated in table 1. Method Participants A pilot sample of 328 students in all levels randomly chosen from the total number of students enrolled in the college of education (N= 1644) with percentage of (20%) was used to assess construct validity of the Parental Authority questionnaire (PAQ). A convenience sample of 172 students from the second level at College of Education at Sultan Qaboos University was used for the study. This sample consisted of 82 Male and 90 female represented all college majors. Study tools were sent to the total number of 371 students at this level, and they were asked to respond to the items of these scales. 172 students responded to both scales and returned back them to the researchers. The participants represented 46% of the total number of the targeted students. Instruments The Parental Authority questionnaire( PAQ) Parental authority was assessed based on parental seperation. This separation was found to be important in parenting ;because of the differences between fathers and mothers in parenting methods in dealing with adolescents (Meunier et al.,2011). The items of the Parental Authority questionnaire (PAQ) Fath. 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. 16. Moth .425 Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 Table1 Factor loadings on authority father and authority mother Fath. Moth Fath. Moth Fath. Moth. Fath. .459 .453 .516 .621 .510 .352 .495 .731 .615 .541 .415 .478 .428 Moth. .450 .495 .628 .328 .562 .387 .567 .346 .500 .594 .388 .490 .446 .519 .516 .446 721 .562 Fath Moth Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Vol. 9 Issue 4 Oct.) Fath. 17. 18. 19. 20. 21. 22. 23. 24. 25. 26. 27. 28. 29. 30. 31. 32. 33. 34. 35. %of Variance Eigen values .643 Moth .643 .709 Table1 Factor loadings on authority father and authority mother Fath. Moth Fath. Moth Fath. Moth. Fath. .455 .648 .621 Moth .554 .696 .519 .696 Fath .639 .498 .478 .709 Moth. 2015 .707 .398 .588 .591 .788 .586 .350 .586 .604 .770 .500 .646. .644 .355 .377 .399 .605 .635 .646 .655 .637 .592 %14. 3 .727 .656 %14. 3 %11. 4 %9.9 0 %7.1 1 %9.20 %9.95 %5.99 %6.81 %5.4 0 5.58 5.63 4.84 4.23 3.03 4.09 4.08 2.79 3.19 2.19 conditions of multiple regression analysis such as: homogeneity and degree of correla Reliability was estimated by internal consistency – Cronbach Alpha. The values of Cronbach Alpha were 0.83; 0.81; 0.86; 0.82. And 0.81 for the five subscales for fathers and 0.85, 0.83, 0.80, 0.81 and 0.80 for the five subscales for mothers. tions was achieved. The significance of correlations between parental authority and social anxiety was achieved. After that we found the Means, standard deviations, and results of multiple regression which are shown in tables 2 and 3. Second: The social anxiety scale The Social Anxiety Scale was constructed based on the literature of Olivares et al. (2005) and Vigil (2009). The scale consisted of 40 items assessing the different situations causing social fears especially with those in social environment and social interactions. The SAS items are answered according to a 3- point Likert scale: (always = 3, sometimes = 2, never = 1). The face validity scale was assessed by a panel of experts whose specializations were measurement and educational psychology. They were asked to assess the items based on the extent to which each is correlated to the concept of parental authority. The cut-off point of 80% was used to assess this correlation. Reliability was estimated by internal consistency – Cronbach Alpha. The value of Cronbach Alpha was 0.85 which means that the scale is accepted psychometrically. Table 2 shows that means of subscales of the PAQ for fathers and mothers were above average 3, but for social anxiety; Means were below the average 2. To meet the conditions of multiple regression analysis, correlations among all independent study variables were calculated, and found to be statistically significant. The values were not too big, and ranged between 0.03 and 0.492, but did not reach 0.70. This is consistent with the second condition of multiple regression analysis that the sample should be selected randomly. This (Randomness) is shown in diagram 'scatter plot' as illustrated in the appendex. Results and discussion To test the study hypotheses; means and standard deviations were calculated. The 722 Relationship between Social Anxiety and Parental Authority Hilal Al- Nabhani & Abdulhameed Hassan Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 Table 2 Means and standard deviation of the dependent and independent variables Variable M a- a- Independent : Parental authority (father’s role): Support Psychological control Open discussion of ideas. Control emotion Parental authority (mother's role): Support control psychological control emotion Accomplishment of missions Accumulation rate: depended : Social anxiety SD 3,4371 3,1920 3,5106 2,9980 3,3706 .41707 .38903 .60998 .46189 .66116 3.9380 3.4625 2.9088 2.7662 3,3088 2.7744 1.7440 .57500 .59504 .63181 .73739 .80599 .43767 28404. 0.192) and student gender (R2 = 0.109). These results are shown table 3. After verifying these conditions ;the regression analysis showed that three of the independent variables had an effect on the dependent variable (social anxiety) of the sample, and these variables were: the mother's support (R2 = 0.374), mother's psychological control (R2 = 0.192) and student gender (R2 = 0.109). These results are shown table 3. To determine the significant effect of the two styles (Support and psychological control) of mothers on students' social anxiety according to gender, regression analysis was used. Its results are shown in table 4. Table 4 shows the effect of the mother’s styles of support and psychological control on social anxiety was significant on daughter comparing to sons in terms of social anxiety level. The value of the effect of social support (t= -3.08, p< 0.003). As for the psychological control style (t =- 4.3, p < 0.001). The effect of both styles on son’s social anxiety was positive, but not significant. To determine the significant effect of the two styles (Support and psychological control) of mothers on students' social anxiety according to gender, regression analysis was used. Its results are shown in table 4. Table 4 shows the effect of the mother’s styles of support and psychological control on social anxiety was significant on daughter comparing to sons in terms of social anxiety These findings may be due to the mother's psychological, emotional, mental maturity, and interaction with the social environment that helps the mother to be initiative to control and direct the children's behavior. The social environment plays an important role in raising and taking care of her children. In this way, the mother creates a climate of trust, and assertiveness to encourage them to assume responsibility and to get ready for the future. level. The value of the effect of social support (t= -3.08, p< 0.003). As for the psychological control style (t=- 4.3, p < 0.001). The effect of both styles on son’s social anxiety was positive, but not significant. These findings may be due to the mother's psychological, emotional, mental maturity, and interaction with the social environment that helps the mother to be initiative to control and direct the children's behavior. The social environment plays an important role in raising and taking care of her children. In this way, the mother creates a climate of trust, and After verifying these conditions; the regression analysis showed that three of the independent variables had an effect on the dependent variable (social anxiety) of the sample, and these variables were: the mother's support (R2 = 0.374), mother's psychological control (R2 = The study results were in agreement with a study conducted by Tiffany (2008) who mainly focused on identifying the impact of maternal deprivation and its negative impact on children's behavior. In her study, Tiffany concluded that the mother's psychological maturity contributed in the children's psychological well-being and the deprivation of the mother whose children may face many problems such as tension, depression, anxiety and instability. This finding is consistent with research showing that parental support is correlated to many 723 Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Vol. 9 Issue 4 Oct.) children’s positive outcomes such as children’s school grades, internal control, lower anxiety, positive self- control (e, g., Assadi et al, 2001, Beshart et al, 20, Mcloed, Wood & Weisz, 2007, Smith & college, 2007). 2015 Additional findings revealed some ongoing changes in the Middle –Eastern parent rules (Assadie, et al., 2007; Besharart, et al., 2011). Concerning the gender differences found in the present study, they were in line with most of the studies that dealt with this issue (i.e., Rudolph & Zimmer-Gembeck, 2014; Sueda, 2009; Edelmann, 2005). These gender differences may be due to the influence of the culture and society. For example, the Arab woman heavily adheres to society values because she likes to appear socially nice and agreeable in front of others. If that does not take place, she feels ashamed, confused and embarrassed, particularly among university young women. This case is the most important symptom of social anxiety. These positive effects related to mothers’ parenting authority may result in some cultural changes in the Omani society because of the increasing numbers of the education opportunities available nowadays for women. This change in the education trend in society may influence people’s psychological health and social anxiety had its own effect on adolescents’ development. This direct effect of mothers on adolescents’ well-being was one of the findings reported by Chang, Oison, Sameroff, and Sexton (2011) who found that maternal use of positive discipline did not predict a child’s externalizing behaviors. This study is consistent with (Alkholif, 2002; Aldhafri, 2011) in the same target culture (Oman). This may indicate that the mother’s concerns of support and psychology control are more apparent in the Omani society than the father’s support and psychology control. The current study is consistent with the cumulative researchers (e.g., Frank et al., 2010; Hannum & Dovork, 2004) showing that it is important to examine each parental authority style separately to identify the degree of differences existing between the father’s effect and the mother’s on adolescents’ outcomes. Conclusion It was concluded that means of parenting authority factors for students were above the average (3) except (psychological control, emotion, accumulation rate), and these factors had low means in mother’s authority. In addition, there were factors affecting social anxiety – according to MRA- such as the mother's support, mother's psychological control and gender. The effect of the mother’s styles of support and psychological control on social anxiety was more significant on daughters than on sons in terms of decreasing social anxiety. In my opinion such above conclusion attributed Table 3 Findings of the regression analysis of the independent variables at the social – anxiety level (dependent variable) Independent variable R2 R2 Adj. Beta t Sig. Support ( father) -0.026 0.061 - 0.38 -0.427 0.670 Psychological control (father) 0.124 0.072 0.169 1.724 0.087 Open discussion (father) -0.053 0.036 -0.114 -1.451 0.149 Control ( father) Emotion (father) Support ( mother) Control ( mother) Psychological control (mother) Emotion ( mother) Accomplishment of Missions ( mother) Gender 0.028 0.011 0.374 -0.028 0.192 0.051 0.039 0.198 0.046 0.102 0.045 0.026 0.252 -0.059 0.228 0.554 0.292 2.852 -0.611 2.627 0.587 0.771 0.005 0.542 0.009 0.015 0.033 0.109 0.029 0.040 0.094 0.173 0.540 1.101 2.275 0.590 0.030 0.048 Table 4 Standardized beta values for mother’s support and psychological control on social anxiety Sex Parental authority B Beta t (Constant) 1.798 5.587 male Support -.009 -.205 -1.382 Psychological control -.011 -.249 -1.679 (Constant) 1.789 9.142 female BB1 -.011 -.252 -3.085 BB3 -.020 -.337 -4.130 Dependent variable: Social anxiety 724 0.273 0.024 Sig. .000 .174 .101 .000 .003 .000 Relationship between Social Anxiety and Parental Authority Hilal Al- Nabhani & Abdulhameed Hassan to cultural changes for mothers in Sultanate of Oman. Thus, these changes play an important role on psychological control and social support which results in eliminating the level of social anxiety in students. Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 lege student’s optimism. College Student Journal, 41(3), 550 – 557. Ballash, G., Pemble, K., Usui, M., Buckley, A. F., & Woodruff- Borden, J. (2006). Family functioning, perceived control, and anxiety: a meditational model. Journal of Anxiety Disorder, 20, 486-497. Recommendations and suggestions First: Studying the parental practices that predict social anxiety in all dimensions. Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: W.H.Freeman. Second: Fathers should assume responsibility in taking care of their boys and girls to eliminate social anxiety in them in future. Besharat, M., Azizi, K. & Poursharifi, I. (2011). The relationship between parenting styles and children’s academic achievement in a sample of Iranian families, Procardia Social and Behavioral Sciences,15, 1280-1283. Third: Focusing on the negative effects of parental practices that lead to social anxietygenerated disorders with regard to university students. Barnett, D., Kramer, M. L., Casat, D., Conner, M. & Davidson, J.T. (2004). Efficacy of Olanzapine in social anxiety disorder: a plot study. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 16(4), 365-368. doi: 10.1177/026988110201600412. Fourth: Conducting future studies in other samples or target groups to determine the effect of parental treatment on social anxiety. References Byrne, B. (2000). Relationships between anxiety, fear, self-esteem and coping strategies in adolescence. Adolescence, 35, 201215. Retrieved November 10th from Akinsola, E. & Udoka, P. (2013). Parental Influence on Social Anxiety in Children and Adolescences: Its Assessment and Management Using Psychodrama. Psychology, 4, 246-253. Doi: 10. 4236. 2013. 43A037. http://www.eric.ed.gov/ERICWebPortal/se arch/detailmini. Aldhafri, S. (2011). Self-efficacy and physical self – concept as mediators of parenting influence on adolescents school and health wellbeing, Journal of Psychology in Africa, 21(4), 511-520. Beauchaine, T. P., Hinshaw, Stephen P. (2013). Child and Adolescent Psychopathology (2nd Edition). NJ: Gohn Wiley & Sons, Inc. Alkholifi, S. (2002). The role of fathers in child rearing as perceived by a sample of mothers in Qatar and UAE societies. Journal of Education Research Center, 11(22), 59-91. Çağdaş, A. ve Seçer, Z. (2004). Anne baba eğitimi. Konya: Eği- tim Kitabevi. H. Canadian Public Health Association (2007). Health and Learning. Retrieved May 2nd, 2012 from www.cpha.ca/en/schoolhealth.aspx. Andrews, G., Creamer, M., Crino, R., Hunt, C., Lampe, L., & Page, A. (2003). The treatment of anxiety disorders: Clinician guides and patient manuals (2nd Ed.). Melbourne: Cambridge University. Clark, d. M., & Wells, A. (1995). A cognitive model of Social phobia in R. G. Heimberg, M. R. liaebwit22, D. A. Hope & F. R. Schneier (Eds), Social Phobia: diagnosis, assessment and treatment. New York, London: The Guilford press. Assadi, S., Zokaei, N., Kaviani, H., Mohammadi, M.,Ghaeli,P.,Gohari, M. & Van de Vijver, F.(2007). Effect of sociocultural context and parenting style on scholastic achievement among Iranian adolescents. Social Development, 16(1), 169-180. Chan, K. & Chan, S. (2009). Emotional autonomy and perceived parenting style: Relational analysis in the Hong Kong culture context, Asia Pacific Educational Review, 10,433 – 443. Baldwin, M., Mcintyre, A. & Hardway, E. (2007). Perceived parenting styles on Col- 725 Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Vol. 9 Issue 4 Oct.) Chang H., Olson S., Sameroff A. J. & Sexton H. (2011). Child effortful control as a mediator of parenting practices on externalizing behavior: evidence for a sexdifferentiated pathway across the transition from preschool to school, Journal Abnormal Child Psychology, 39(1):71-81. doi: 10.1007/s10802-010-9437-7. 2015 stitüsü, Enstitü Dergisi/E Dergi ISSI:13, 10, 120-1. Erozkan, A. (2012). Examination of relationship between anxiety sensitivity and parenting style in adolescents, Educational Sciences: Theory & Practice, 12(1), 52-57. Esther, F. (2013). Parental influence on social anxiety in children and adolescents: its assessment and management using psycho drama. Psychology, 4 (34), 246-253. Coln, L., Jordan. S.S. & Mercer, H. (2013). A unified model exploring parenting practices as mediators of marital conflict and children s adjustment, Child Psychiatry and Human Development. 44,419-429. Doi: 10. 1007/s10578-012-0336-8. Frank, G., Plunkett, S. & Otten, M. (2010).Perceived parenting, self- esteem and general self-efficacy of Iranian American adolescents, Journal of Child and Family Studies,19,738-746.doi: 10.1007/s10826010-9363. Costello, E. J., Egger, H., & Angold, A. (2005). 10-Year Research Update Review: The Epidemiology of Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Disorders: I. Methods and Public Health Burden. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 44(10), 972-986. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/01.chi.0000172552. 41596.6f. Hanlon, N. (2010). Information on social anxiety, Retrieved from: http:// www. Social anxiety.co.uk/ downloads/ information on Social Anxiety. Pdf. Hannum, J. & Dvorak, D. (2004). Effect of Family conflict divorce, and attachment patterns on the psychological distress and social adjustment of college freshmen, Journal of College Student Development,45(1),27-42. Darling, N., Cumsille, P., Caldwell, L.& Dowdy, B. (2008). Predicator of adolescents disclosure strategies and perceptions of parental knowledge, Journal Youth Adolescent, 35, 667 – 678. Heimberg, R. & Becker, R., (2002). Cognitive behavioral group therapy for social phobia, New York: Guilford Press. Darling , N., Cumsille, P. & Martinez, L. (2008). Individual difference in adolescences beliefs about the legitimacy of parental authority and their own obligation to obey: A longitudinal Investigation, Child Development, 79(4), 1103-1118. Kessler, R. C., Berglund, P. A., Demler, O., Jin, R., & Walters, E. E. (2005). Lifetime Prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the National Darling, N. & Steinberg, L.(1993). Parenting style as context an integrative model, Psychology, Bull, 113(3), 487 – 496. Co morbidity Survey Replication (NCS-R). Archives of General Psychiatry, 62, 593–602. DSM-V.; APA. (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed., Washington DC: APA. Kuhar, M. (2010). Parental authority styles In adolescent parent relationships. In: DiskursKindheits- und Jugendforschung, 5 (3), 321-336. URN: http://nbnresolving.de/urn:nbn: de:0168-ssoar354694. Driscoll, A., Russell, S., & Crockett, L. (2008). Parenting styles and youth well- being across immigrant generations, Journal of Family Issues, 29(2), 185 – 209. Leahy, R. & Holland, S. (2000). Treatment plans and interventions for depression and anxiety disorders. New York: Guilford Press. Edelmann, R. J. (2004). Coping with blushing (2nd ed.). England: Sheldon Press. Erkan, Z. (2002). Sosyal kaygı düzeyi yüksek ve düşük ergen- lerin ana baba tutumlarına ilişkin nitel bir çalışma. Çukurova Üniveritesi Sosyal Bilimler En- Lerner, R. M., Lerner, J. V., von Eye, A., Ostrom, C. W., Nitz, K., Tolwar–Soni, R., & Tubman, J. G. (1996). Continuity and discontinuity across the transition of early adolescence: A developmental contex- 726 Relationship between Social Anxiety and Parental Authority Hilal Al- Nabhani & Abdulhameed Hassan tual perspective. In J. A Graber, J. Brooks- Gunn, & A. C. Petersen (Eds.), Transitions through adolescence: Interpersonal domains and context (pp. 3-22). New Jersey: Law-fence Erlbaum 33. F. Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 Ruscio, A, M., Brown, A., Chin, T., Sareen, j., Stin, B., & Kessler, C. (2008). Social Fearsand social phobia in The USA: results from The National Comorbidity survey Replication psychological Medicine. 38. 15-20. Lieb, R., Wittchen, H., Hofler, M., Fuetsch, M., Stein, B. & Merikangas, K. R. (2000). Parental psychopathology, Parenting style, and the risk of social phobia in offspring a prospective- Longitudinal community study, Arch gen Psychiatry, 57(9),859-866. Doi: 10.1001/ archpsyc, 57(9),859. Smith, G. & College, D. (2007).Parenting effects on self-efficacy and self-esteem in late adolescence and how those factors impact adjustment to college. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Eastern Psychological Association, Philadelphia, and P.A. Sueda, K. (2009). Social anxiety remediation in Japan and the United States, Intercultural Relation’s , 92,110-138. Mannuzza, S., Klein, Moulton, L., Scarfone, N., Malloy, P., Vosburg, S. K. (2002). Anxiety sensitivity among children of parents with anxiety disorder: a controlled high risk study. Journal Anxiety Disorder, 16, 135-148. Taylor, S., Jang, L., Stewart, H., & Stein, B. (2008). Etio- logy of the dimensions of anxiety sensitivity: A behavioral-get- notice analysis. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 22, 899-914. Meunier, J., Roskam, I., stivenart, M. , Moorteie, G., Browne, D., & Kumar, A. (2011). Externalizing behavior trajectories: The role of parenting, sibling relationships and child personality. Journal of Applied Development Psychology, 32,20-33. Doi: 10-1016/j. appdev.2010.09.006.. Tiffany, F. (2008). The Effects of Mother's Physical and Emotional Unavailability on Emotion Regulation, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development,. 59(. 2/3), 208-227. Turner, E., Chadler, M., & Heffer, R. (2009). The influence of parenting style, achievement motivation and self – efficacy, on academic performance in college students, Journal of College Student Development, 50 (3), 337-346. McLeod, D., Wood, J., & Weisz, J. R. (2007). Examining the association between parenting and childhood anxiety: A metaanalysis. Clinical psychology review, 27(2), 155-172. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2006.09.0 02 Yavuzer, H. (2005). Ana-baba ve cocuk. İstanbul: Remzi Kitabevi. Olivares, J., Garcia-lopez, L. J., Hidalgo, M. D. & Caballo, V. E. (2004). Relations among social anxiety measures and their invariance, A Confirmatory factor analysis. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 20, 172-179. Vigil, A. (009) The factorial structure of the 41-item version of the Screen for child anxiety Related Emotional disorders (SCARED) in A Spanish population of 8 to 12 years –old, International Journal of Clinical and Health psychology, 9, 313 -327. Reitman, D., Rhode, P. C., Hupp, S. D. A. & Altobello, C.(2002). Validation of the parental authority questionnaire – revised, Journal of Psychology and Behavioral Assessment, 24(2), 119-127. Waitea, P., Whittingtonb, L. & Creswella, C. (2014). Parent-Child Interactions and Adolescent Anxiety: A Systematic Review. Psychopathology Review, 1(1), 51-76. ISSN 2051-8315 / DOI:10.5127/pr.033213 Rudolph, J. & Zimmer-Gembeck, M. J. (2013). Parent relationships and adolescent’s depression and social anxiety: indirect association via emotional sensitivity to rejection threat. Australian Journal of Psychology. 66,110-121. Wood, J., Mcloed, D., Sigman, M., Hwang, C., & Chu, C. (2007). Parenting and childhood anxiety: theory, empirical findings, & future directions, Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 134-151. Wood, J., McLeod, D., Sigman, M., Hwang, C., & Chu, C. (2003). Parenting and 727 Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Vol. 9 Issue 4 Oct.) childhood anxiety: Theory, empirical findings, and future directions. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44(1), 134151. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00106. Zimmer-Gembeck, J. & Nesdale, D. (2013). Anxious and angry rejection sensitive, social withdraw, and retribution in high and low ambiguous situation, Journal of Personality, 81 .29 -38. Doi:10.1111/j.14676494.2012.00792.x. 728 2015 Relationship between Social Anxiety and Parental Authority Hilal Al- Nabhani & Abdulhameed Hassan Appendix Anxiety Figure 1 Relationship between social anxiety and parental authority 729 Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Pages 730-737) Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 Mindfulness of Career Counselors within the Omani’s Context Muna A. Al-Bahrani* & Bakkar S. Bakkar Sultan Qaboos University, Sultanate of Oman ___________________________________________ Received: 26/8/2015 Revised: 28/9/2015 Accepted: 30/9/2015 _____________________________________________ Abstract: This study aimed to explore the variation of Omani career counselors‘ mindfulness level, in relation to their gender, qualifications, experience, age, and social status. The Mindful Attention Awareness Scale was used to assess the mindfulness level of 164 career counselors from the Sultanate of Oman. The findings showed that mindfulness level was high among career counselors. The results also revealed significant differences due to gender. Males‘ level of mindfulness was higher than females. No differences were found due to qualifications, experience, age, and social status on the level of mindfulness. Recommendations suggested further research to examine the relationship between mindfulness in career counselors and positive client outcomes. Keywords: Mindfulness level, career counselor, Omani context. الوعي العقلي لدى املرشدين املهنيني ضمن البيئة العمانية منى بنت عبداهلل البحرانية* وبكار سليمان بكار سلطنة عمان،جامعة السلطان قابوس _____________________________________________ هدفت هذه الدراسة إ ىل اللشف عن الفزوق يف مشتىى الىعٌ العقمٌ لدى املزشدين املونًني وعالقتوا:مشتخمص ٌوعٌ اجاتتااه العقم والعيز وااحالة اجاتتياعًة استخد مقًا، واملههن العميٌ وابخةر،النى مب تيارا ٌلتحديد مشتىى الىعٌ العقمٌ لدى املزشدين املونًني يف سمطنة عيان أظوز تتائج الدراسة بأن مشتىى الىع حًث كان،العقمٌ لدى املزشدين املونًني عالٌ كيا كشفت النتائج عن وتىد فزوق دالة تعزى ملتيار النى مشتىى الىعٌ العقمٌ عند الذكىر أعمى من اإلتاث ومل تلن هناك أية فزوق دالة يف مشتىى الىعٌ العقمٌ تعزى ٌ والعيز وااحالة اجاتتياعًة وارتلز التىصًا عمى إتزاء دراسا لتقًًم العالقة بني الىع، ابخةر: ملتيارا العقمٌ لدى املزشدين املونًني والنىاتج اجاجيابًة لمعيًن مشتىى الىعٌ العقمٌ املزشد املوين الاًئة العياتًة:اللميا املفتاحًة *munabh@squ.edu.om 730 Mindfulness of Career Counselors within the Omanis Context Muna Al-Bahrani & Bakkar Bakkar Mindfulness is a newly developed concept within the field of psychology. It has sparked growing interest, and its application as a counseling intervention is powerful and widely pertinent; whether being used to enhance traditional counseling approaches or as a prevention strategy (Brown, Marquis, & Guiffrida, 2013). Recent research demonstrates that mindfulness can gradually achieve outcomes like acceptance, letting go, trust, nonjudgmental attitude, and self-awareness (Birnbaum & Birnbaum, 2008). This awareness permits the individual to ―be present‖ to reality as it is, rather than to react to it or habitually process it through conceptual filters. Consciousness takes on a clarity and freshness that permits more flexible, more objectively informed psychological and behavior responses (Brown, Ryan, & Creswell, 2007; Kurash, & Schaul, 2006). Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 chotherapy, cohesion in group therapy, empathy, and collecting client feedback (Norcross, & Wampold, 2011).Core skills such as empathy are integrated with empirically supported interventions (Cormier, Nurius & Osborn, 2013). Empirical studies demonstrated that mindfulness is a predictor of counseling selfefficacy in master's-level and doctoral-level counseling students. Although empathy did not predict counseling self-efficacy, mindfulness did significantly predict empathy (Greason & Cashwell, 2009). The processes of mindfulness include concepts such as acceptance and contact with the present moment (Shapiro, Brown, & Biegel, 2007). Mindfulness and experiential acceptancebased approaches appear to be a viable means for cultivating levels of empathy. As individuals are more mindfully attentive to the their thoughts and feelings, they are more likely to find common ground and greater understanding in their relationships, to engage in higher levels of valued action, and increase their overall quality of life in the process—one moment at a time (Block‐Lerner, Adair, Plumb, Rhatigan, & Orsillo, 2007). Mindfulness may be an important tool not only for cultivating attention capacities in counseling students, but also for helping students learn how to be with clients. The counselor would be able to stay present, fully hear the client's story, and engage empathically (Greason, & Cashwell, 2009). It tolerates not knowing and clients‘ distress so that therapists‘ likelihood of acting out is reduced (Clgolla, & Brown, 2011; Stauffer & Pehrsson, 2012). The mindfulness practice has been described as a vital element of Buddhist, Christian, Hindu, Islamic, Jewish, and Taoist teaching. Mindful awareness practices have appeared in a variety of forms such as yoga and centering prayer. For example, centering prayer teaches individuals the need to wake up to the experiences of the present moment. Individuals learn how to quiet the inner talking and experience silence as well as to return to their intentions of focusing on God (Blanton, 2011). In addition to this, the practice of prayer in Islam teaches individuals to rest their minds from worldly distractions and to focus solely on the act of obedience and submission to Allah‘s will (Sayeed & Prakash, 2013). It was documented that a significantly higher alpha wave activity was recorded during the prostration position of the Muslim prayer (Doufesh, Faisal, Lim & Ibrahim, 2012). Mindfulness has the potential to influence positive emotional change in counselors, social workers, and people in general (Birnbaum. & Birnbaum, 2008). Therapists work requires dealing with clients and their own emotions on a regular basis and they are at risk of occupationally related psychological problems. Therefore, preparation for the role of therapist occurs on both professional and personal levels (Shapiro et al., 2007; Stauffer, 2007). Considering the nature of work, the practice of mindfulness is being used more often both to help clients and to facilitate counselor effectiveness (Brown et al., 2013, Reid, Farragher, & Ok, 2013; Rothaupt & Morgan, 2007). Mindfulness attitudes may moderate the depersonalization associated with burnout by helping counselors focus on the client‘s uniqueness in Changes in perception and ways of knowing reality are being expressed through increasing integration of traditions from east and west within the helping paradigm. Within these paradigmatic shifts, mindfulness has emerged as an essential practice to expand consciousness for the purpose of self-observation and knowing the world (Birnbaum & Birnbaum, 2008). The self or the person of the counselor as Reupert, (2006) noted is more essential that the orientation chosen or the intervention employed within the helping paradigm. Several relationship elements were demonstrably effective including alliance in individual psy- 731 Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Vol. 9 Issue 4 Oct.) the present moment and alleviate emotional exhaustion through accepting their current emotions and then letting them go (Kurash, & Schaul, 2006 ; Thompson, Amatea & Thompson, 2008). McCracken, 2007). 2015 Gauntlett-Gilbert & Vowles, Statement of the problem The effectiveness of mindfulness practice was highlighted within several helping paradigms (Birnbaum, 2008; Napoli & Bonifas, 2011; Tadlock-Marlo, 2011). For example, using mindfulness in supervision experiences appeared to improve empathy with the client‘s emotional experience and enhance awareness of functioning as a counselor (Andersson, King & Lalande, 2010). Further, mindfulness does have the potential to enhance an occupational counselor‘s ability to practice in a maximally effective from a wide variety of areas of practice. Active listening, non-judgmentally, and perspective taking are fundamental aspects of the client-centered practice philosophy that is embedded within the profession of helping paradigm (Reid & Farragher, 2013). The reported results showed that counselor trainees who do not avoid their own issues and who are highly attuned to their own emotions and vulnerabilities would be more attuned to the vulnerabilities of others (Trusty & Watts, 2005). Further, as a result of practicing mindfulness, counselors may be able to reflect the here-and-now of the experience and help clients to gain awareness of the uncomfortable issues that cause problem for them (Stauffer , 2008). The application of a mindfulness-based stress reduction program reported significant decline in stress, negative affect, rumination, state and trait anxiety, and significant increases in positive affect and self-compassion (Shapiro et al., 2007). Mindfulness and awareness may assist counselors to be more open to their emotional exploration as opposed to avoiding or surpassing emotion which may be detrimental to the quality of therapy (May & O‘Donovan, 2007; Stauffer, 2007). Therefore, the researchers called for integrating mindfulness into therapists‘ training curriculum in order to enhance their ability to be with their clients and strengthen the therapeutic relationship as well foster self-care (Clgolla, & Brown, 2011). Researchers support the importance of examining contextual variations in mindfulness such gender and age. The variations was found among females who showed higher levels of mindfulness. As indicated by Gilbert and Waltz (2010) females were significantly higer in the observatinal factor. This suggests that females are more influenced by contextual differences in the degree to which they are aware of the present moment in a nonjudgmental manner than males (Lilja et al., 2011; Luk, Holman, Kohlenberg, 2008). Females are more able to take the perspective of another person in comparison with men (Davis & Franzoi, 1991). In addition, in addressing the difficult topic of professionalism within the context of a systems based medical curriculum, females scored higher than males on selfreport awareness and emotional intelligence (Doherty, Cronin & Offiah, 2013). Conversely, males scored higher than females on a mindfulness‘ measure (Abdulla & Alshamsi, 2013). In terms of age, Lilja et al. (2011) noted that older participants obtained higher values on a measure of mindfulness than their younger counterparts. The researcher indicated that it would be interesting to see whether the result related to age, gender, and meditation experience can be replicated in other cultures. However, age, gender, and education were found to be unrelated to mindfulness (Hansen, Lundh, Homman & Wångby‐Lundh, 2009; Josefsson, Larsman, Broberg, & Lundh, 2011; It appears that ongoing practice of mindfulness may be a critical tool for counselors to sharpen their ability to be attentive. The growing interest to practice and explore mindfulness will provide benefits to the recipients of services and foster healthier approaches to life among professionals (Campbell & Christopher, 2012; O‘Driscoll, 2009; Rothaupt & Morgan, 2007). With a shortage of studies in this area, research is needed to explore the mindfulness construct among counselors because mindfulness has demonstrated great promise and warrants further investigation (Brown et al., 2013). Career counseling profession has only recently developed in Oman. As a response to the national need in increasing career awareness and boosting educational and employment opportunities, in 2008 the Ministry of Education assigned teachers who had Bachelor Degree in Education to be career counselors. To fully fill their career counseling duties, counselors have been exposed to a substantial in-service training workshops. Fur- 732 Mindfulness of Career Counselors within the Omanis Context Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 Muna Al-Bahrani & Bakkar Bakkar ther, they were prepared academically through a higher diploma program in career guidance and counseling within college of Education at Sultan Qaboos University. This program exposed those counselors to the theoretical and practical part of various topics such as techniques, theories including cognitive and developmental models of individual and group counseling, communication skills, vocational measurements and the design of career programs. ed among career counselors who work in high schools in Muscat Governorate during the first semester of the academic year 2013/2014. The participation was voluntarily and the participants who volunteered were given the questionnaires package to complete. The participants completed the questionnaire at their own pace and then returned it to the researcher within one month of distribution. Measure The Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS) was constructed by Brown and Ryan (2003). The scale consists of 15 items, all of which indicate lack of a mindfulness. I One example is ―I tend not to notice feelings of physical tension or discomfort until they really grab my attention.‖ The MAAS was positively correlated with measures such as wellbeing, including feelings of autonomy, competence, and positive relations with others. In addition, it was negatively correlated with anxiety, hostility, depression, selfconsciousness, and impulsivity (Brown & Ryan, 2003). Research Questions The main aim of this study was to understand the variation of career counselors‘ mindfulness level. Two primary questions were investigated: 1. What is the level of the participants’ mindfulness? 2. Are there any statistical significant differences in the participants’ mindfulness in relation to their gender, qualification, experience, age, and social status? Method The items are rated on a 6-point Likert scale (almost always =1, very frequently =2, somewhat frequently =3, somewhat infrequently =4, very infrequently =5, almost never =6) .Total scores can range from 15 to 90 and higher scores indicate more mindfulness. Participants The participants of this study were 164 high schools career counselors who are assigned to provide career counseling services including helping students in the process of selecting their careers. The sample varied in terms of qualification, experience, age, and social status. Table 1 illustrates the characteristics of the sample. In the present study, the researcher used the MAAS with a 5-point Likert scale (never =1, rarely =2, sometimes =3, often =4, always=5). The rationales of using the five point scale was to keep the options meaningful to the respondents, so it is clearer and more easily understood. The total score ranged from 15 to 75 Procedure The study was conducted based on the standard procedures that are applied within the Omani context. Questionnaires were distribut- Table 1 Demographic Characteristics of the Sample (N=164) Gender Qualification Bachelor Degree Higher Diploma Master Degree Experience Short (1-5years.) 30 31 61 Age Long (6+years) 20-30 years. 57 10 46 19 103 29 31-40 years. 69 53 122 41years and above Single 8 9 5 11 13 20 Married 78 87 66 77 144 164 Social status Total 733 Female 50 27 00 Total Male 67 12 8 117 39 8 2015 Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Vol. 9 Issue 4 Oct.) Results and the mean score is 45, which was considered as a cut- off point. A score of 15 indicates the highest level of mindfulness awareness, and a score of 75 denotes indicate lowest level of mindfulness. The scale‘s items were translated by the researcher into Arabic and were back-translated by another researcher in the psychology department. In responding to the questions, means and standard deviations were calculated. Both ttest and ANOVA were utilized to examine the effect of the independent variables (gender, qualification, experience, social status, and age) on mindfulness measure. To answer the first research question concerning the level of the participants‘ mindfulness, means and standard deviations were obtained as shown in Table 2. Reliability Cronbach‘s alphas ranging from .80 to .90 have been reported for MAAS (Brown & Ryan, 2003). In this study, reliability of the MAAS was estimated in two ways: test-retest reliability and internal consistency. To estimate test – retest reliability, a pilot study was conducted twice with an interval of 3 weeks on 20 career counselors who were pursuing their higher diploma in career counseling program at Sultan Qaboos University. The correlation coefficient between the first time scores and the second time scores was (0.82, p. < 0.01). Internal consistency was calculated by Cronbach Alpha for MAAS's items. Cronbach Alpha coefficient for the whole scale was 0.88. These values indicate appreciable and accepted reliabilities for the MAAS scale. Findings in table 2 reveal that mindfulness level is high, that is, the total scores of mindfulness for male and female counselors are 29.265, 32.403 respectively, and the total score for all is 30.738. These scores are below the cut-off point 45; which means that the level of mindfulness is high among career counselors. To answer the second research question regarding the differences in the participants‘ mindfulness in relation to their gender, qualification, experience, age, and social status, ttest and a Univeriate analysis were utilized. Table 3 represents the findings findings of ttest of experience, social status, and gender. Additionally, Table 4 illustrates the findings of Univeriate of age and qualification. Table 2 Means and Standard Deviations of Participants’ Scores on the MAAS Gender Qualification Age Male Bachelor Higher Diploma Master 20-30 31- 40 41 and above Total Mean 28.841 30.333 36.417 32.952 31.185 30.000 29.265 Female SD 1.548 2.381 2.571 2.323 1.326 2.509 7.047 Mean 33.323 34.169 36.631 31.228 32.333 32.403 Total SD 1.652 1.553 1.863 1.216 3.492 7.021 Mean 29.769 32.667 35.500 34.552 29.770 31.738 30.738 SD 7.212 6.884 4.598 7.509 7.044 4.973 7.187 Table 3 Findings of T-Test of Gender, Experience and Marital Status Experience Social status Gender Source Corrected Model Intercept Qualification Age Error Corrected Total Short Long Single Married Male Female N Mean SD t df Sig. 61 103 20 144 87 77 31.180 30.476 29.800 30.868 29.264 32.403 1.601 1.692 2.291 1.184 7.047 7.021 0.606 162 0.546 -0.622 162 0.535 -2.852 162 0.005 Table 4 Univeriate Analysis on the Effect of Independent Variables on the MAAS SS df MS F 2479.938 35954.005 140.767 126.578 5939.787 8419.726 32 1 2 2 131 163 77.498 35954.005 70.383 63.289 45.342 734 1.709 792.953 1.552 1.396 P 0.019 0.0001 0.216 0.251 Mindfulness of Career Counselors within the Omanis Context Muna Al-Bahrani & Bakkar Bakkar Findings in Table 3 shows that there were no significant differences in mindfulness level due to the experience and the social status. Ttest values are respectively: t (1, 162) = 0.606, (p = 0.546), t (1, 162) = -0.622, (p = 0.535). However, there were significant differences in mindfulness due to gender, t (1, 162) = -2.852, (p < 0.05). These differences favored males whose mean was 29.265, compared with female counselors whose mean was 32.403 which indicaes males' mindfulness awareness level is higher than the females' level. Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 students was significant on the mindfulness facet ‗non-judging‘, and that females were more influenced by contextual differences in the degree to which they were aware of the present moment in a nonjudgmental manner than males (Luk et al., 2008). Indeed, AlBahrani, Aldhafri, Alkharusi, Kazem, and Alzubiadi (2013) suggested that the tendency of female to engage in more nonproductive coping involves blaming one self, keeping to themselves, escaping, and anxiety because they are more subject to stressors than males. As Brown et al. (2007) has stated that cognitive, emotional, somatic, and behavioral factors can foster or inhibit mindful states, given what is known about the effects of stress, fatigue, lifestyle choices, and other factors on the quality of conscious states of mind. No statistically significant differences were found due to experience, social status, qualifications, and age on the level of mindfulness. Indeed, this finding is consistent with results reported by (Hansen et al., (2009); McCracken et al., (2007). Table 4 shows that there were no significant statistical differences in mindfulness among career counselors due to qualification and age. F values were respectively: F (1, 131) =0.908, (p = 0.342), F (1, 131) =0.105, (p = 0.747), F (2, 131) =1.552, (p = 0.216), F (1, 131) =0.639, (p = 0.425) and F (2, 131) =1.396, (p = 0.251). Discussion This study examined the level of mindfulness of career counselors within Omani culture and the level of mindfulness among career counselors was high. To some extent the result was expected because of the uniqueness of the sample. They were all career counselors who had been exposed to some knowledge and training in mindfulness within the cognitive theories such as acceptance and commitment therapy in their higher diploma program. Some components of mindfulness such as active listening, non-judgmentally, and perspective taking are fundamental aspects that are embedded within the profession of helping services such as occupational therapy (Reid & Farragher, 2013). Additionally, awareness of the present moment is a technique that is embedded in Muslims‘ lives. Islamic relaxation is a method that incorporates the Islamic tenets of prayer, recitation of the holy Qur‘an and Zikr, or recollection of Allah, to obtain a relaxed response of calmness and mindfulness (Syed, 2003). There are two primary limitations in this study. Firstly, this study relied exclusively on self-report measures. Secondly, the findings should be interpreted carefully because the measures of mindfulness are fairly new, and most, if not all, suffer from a scarcity of construct and predictive validation. Mindfulness can be assessed through declarative knowledge, meaning that individuals can directly report on those experiential qualities that constitute mindfulness to facilitate the validation of self-report measures of mindfulness (Brown et al., 2007). Despite the limitations of this study, there is sufficient evidence to support the idea that mindfulness is beneficial for clients and for counselors . Research suggests that personal practice of mindfulness by counselors will better position them in their work with clients (Takahashi et al., 2005) and provides them with better assessment of their own strengths and limitations (Cormier et al., 2013). Another significant finding of this study was related to gender. The difference in scores of mindfulness was found in favor of males compared to females. This finding consistent with the findings reported in studies by (Abdulla & Alshamsi, (2013); Thompson et al., (2008)). This echoes findings that disagree with what was reported by de Vibe et al., ( 2013) who found that the effect for female Recommendation and future research Based on the results of this study, several implications can be addressed. First, students as Stauffer (2007) stressed, should be provided with an opportunity to consider the use of self not only to raise self-awareness, but also to consider the ways in which the self might be usefully and professionally enacted during 735 Journal of Educational and Psychological Studies - Sultan Qaboos University (Vol. 9 Issue 4 Oct.) training, with their peers and instructors, and when working with clients across various contexts (Reupert, 2009). Second, further research to investigate if a direct relationship exists between 2015 Brown, A., Marquis, A., & Guiffrida, D. (2013). Mindfulness-Based Interventions in Counseling. Journal of Counseling & Development, 91, 96-104. Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 84(4), 822848. mindfulness in counselors and positive client outcomes would be of enormous relevance to the field (O'Donovan, & May, 2007). References Brown, K. W., Ryan, R. M., & Creswell, J. D. (2007). Mindfulness: Theoretical foundations and evidence for its salutary effects. Psychological Inquiry, 18(4), 211-237. Abdulla, A., M. & Alshamsi, A., A. (2013). Mindfulness among the students of the university. The Professor Journal, 205(2), 343-366. Campbell, J., & Christopher, J. (2012). Teaching mindfulness to create effective counselors . Journal of Mental Health Counseling, 34(31), 213-226. Al-Bahrani, M. Aldhafri, S., Alkharusi, H., Kazem, A., & Alzubiadi, A. (2013). Age and gender differences in coping style across various problems: Omani adolescents‘ perspective. Journal of Adolescence, 36(2), 303-309. doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2012.11.007 Josefsson, T., Larsman, P., Broberg, A. G., & Lundh, L. G. (2011). Self-reported mindfulness mediates the relation between meditation experience and psychological well-being. Mindfulness, 2(1), 49-58. Andersson, L., King, R., & Lalande, L. (2010). Dialogical mindfulness in supervision role-play. Counseling and Psychotherapy Research, 10 (4), 287-294. Kurash, S., & Schaul, J. (2006). Integrating mindfulness meditation within a university counseling centre setting. Journal of College Student Psychotherapy, 20 (3), 53-67. Baer, R. A. (2003). Mindfulness training as a clinical intervention: A conceptual and empirical review. Clinical psychology: Science and Practice, 10(2), 125-143. Lilja, J. L., Frodi-Lundgren, A., Hanse, J. J., Josefsson, T., Lundh, L., Sköld, C., & ... Broberg, A. G. (2011). Five Facets Mindfulness questionnaire—reliability and factor structure: A Swedish version. Cognitive Behavior Therapy, 40 (4), 291-303. doi:10.1080/16506073.2011.580367 Birnbaum, L. & Birnbaum, A. (2008). Mindful social work: From theory to practice. Journal of Religion & Spirituality in Social Work, 27, 87-104. 18. DOI: 10.1080/15426430802113913 Luk., J., Holman, G., Kohlenberg, R. (2008). Contextual variations of mindfulness an interpersonal and task-oriented contexts: The roles of gender and ethnicity. Psi Chi Journal of Undergraduate Research, 184-190. Birnbaum, L. (2008). The use of mindfulness training to create an ‗accompanying place‘ for social work students. Social Work Education, 27 (8), 837–852. Blanton, P. G. (2011). The other mindful practice: Centering prayer & psychotherapy. Pastoral Psychology, 60(1), 133-147. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11089010-0292-9. May, C., & O‘Donovan, A. (2007). The advantages of mindful therapists. Psychotherapy in Australia, 13(4), 49-53. McCracken, L. M., Gauntlett-Gilbert, J., & Vowles, K. E. (2007). The role of mindfulness in a contextual cognitive-behavior al analysis of chronic pain-related suffering and disability. Pain, 131(1), 63-69. Block‐Lerner, J., Adair, C., Plumb, J. C., Rhatigan, D. L., & Orsillo, S. M. (2007). The case for mindfulness‐based approaches in the cultivation of empathy: Does nonjudgmental, present‐moment awareness increase capacity for perspective‐ taking and empathic concern?. Journal of marital and family therapy, 33(4), 501-516. Napoli, M., & Bonifas, R. (2011). From theory toward empathic self-care: Creating a mindful classroom for social work students. Social Work Education, 30 (6), 635– 649. 736 Mindfulness of Career Counselors within the Omanis Context Muna Al-Bahrani & Bakkar Bakkar Norcross, J. C., & Wampold, B. E. (2011). Evidence-based therapy relationships: research conclusions and clinical practices. Psychotherapy, 48(1), 98. Vol.9 Issue 4, 2015 Mindfulness Competencies. (Doctoral dissertation). Oregon State University, Oregon. Syed, I. B. (2003). Spiritual medicine in the history of Islamic medicine. Journal of the International Society for the History of Islamic Medicine, 2, 45-49. O‘Driscoll, A. (2009). The growing influence of mindfulness on the work of the counseling psychologist; A review. Counseling Psychology Review, 24, 16-23. Tadlock-Marlo, R. (2011). Making minds matter: Infusing mindfulness into school counseling. Journal of Creativity in Mental Health, 6, 220–233. O'Donovan, A., & May, S. (2007). The advantages of the mindful therapist. Psychotherapy in Australia, 13(4), 46-53. Takahashi, T., Murata, T., Hamada, T, Omori, M., Kosaka, H., Kikuchi, M., Yoshida, H., & Wada, Y. (2005). Changes in EEG and autonomic nervous activity during meditation and their association with personality traits. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 55(2) 199-207. Reid, D., Farragher, J. & Ok, C. (2013). Exploring mindfulness with occupational therapists practicing in mental health contexts. Occupational Therapy in Mental Health, 29, 79–292. Reupert, A. (2006). The counselor‘s use of self in therapy; inevitable presence. International Journal for the Advancement of Counseling, 28, 95-105. Thompson, I., Amatea, A., & Thompson, E. (2008 ). Personal and Contextual Predictors of Mental Health Counselors‘ Compassion Fatigue and Burnout. Journal of Mental Health Counselling36 (1), 58–77. Reupert, A. (2009). Students' use of self: teaching implications. Social Work Education, 28(7), 765-777. Trusty, J. & Watts, R. E. (2005). Model of effects of adult Attachment on emotional empathy of counseling students. Journal of Counseling & Development, 83(1), 66-77. Rothaupt, J., & Morgan, M. (2007). Counselors ' and counselor educators' practice of mindfulness: A qualitative inquiry. Counseling and Values, 52, 40-54. Sayeed, S., & Prakash, A. (2013). The Islamic prayer (Salah/Namaaz) and yoga togetherness in mental health. Indian Journal of Psychiatry, S224-S230. doi:10.4103/00195545.105537 Shapiro, S. L., Brown, K. W., & Biegel, G. M. (2007). Teaching self-care to caregivers: Effects of mindfulness-based stress reduction on the mental health of therapists in training. Training and Education in Professional Psychology, 1(2), 105. Stauffer, M. D. (2008). Mindfulness in counseling and psychotherapy: A literature review and quantitative investigation of mindfulness competencies. ProQuest. Stauffer, M., & Pehrsson, D. (2012). Mindfulness Competencies for Counselors and Psychotherapists. Journal of Mental Health Counseling, 34, 227-239. Stauffer, M., D. (2007). Mindfulness in Counseling and Psychotherapy: A Literature Review and Quantitative Investigation of 737 زتً ١ايدزاضات ايرتبٚ ١ٜٛايٓفط١ٝ زتً ١دٚز ١ٜستهَُ ١تخصص ،١تصدز عٔ جاَع ١ايطًطإ قابٛعٚ ،تعٓى ٢بٓػىس ايدزاضىات ٚايث ىٛص اة ى ١ًٝايى تتىىٛا س ٗٝىىا َكَٛىىات ايث ى ايعًُىىَ ٞىىٔ فٝى و ىىاي ١ايفهىىسٚٚ ،اىىٛث اٝ ٗٓ،ىىٚ ،١دقىى ١ايتٝ ٛىىل، زتىىا ت آ،ىىاٖ ٚطىىسم ايتىىدزٜظٚ ،عًىىِ ايىىٓفظ ايرتبىىٚ ،ٟٛاوداز ٠ايرتبٜٛىى ،١نُىىا تٓػىىس اجملًىى ١تكىىازٜس ا،ىىواسات ٚايٓىىدٚات ايرتبٜٛىى،١ َٚساجعات ايهتب ايرتبٚ ١ٜٛايٓفط ١ٝاذتدٜثًَٚ ،١خصات ايسضا ٌ٥ادتاَع١ٝ زتا ت ايرتب ١ٝا،ختًف.١ زتًظ ايٓػس ايعًُٞ و .د .عاَسبٔ عً ٞايسٚاع ،زٝ٥ظ اجملًظ و.دَ .اٖس ستُد وبٖ ٛالٍ و.د .ستُد خإ و.د .ستُد ٚيد خاٙٚ و.د .بٛعالّ بًكامسٞ و.د .لمك بٔ ستُد ايًُهٞ و.د .خايد ْاج ٞداٟ دَٝ .ػ ٌٝنًريبٛدٜت و .عً ٞبٔ ْا س اذتضسَٞ ٖ ١٦ٝايت سٜس زٝ٥ظ ايت سٜس احمل ىىسز و.دَ .اٖسستُد وبٖ ٛالٍ و.د .عثد ايك ٟٛضامل ايصبٝدٟ وعضا ١٦ٖٝ ٤ايت سٜس و.د .عًَٗ ٞد ٟناظِ و.د .عثد اهلل بٔ مخٝظ وَثٛضعٝدٟ و.د. اذت ١عثد اهلل عٝطإ د .عً ٞستُد إبساِٖٝ د .عً ٞبٔ غسف اٛ،ضٟٛ د .ومحد بٔ محد ايسبعاْٞ دٚ .ج ١ٗٝابت ايعاْٞ د .ستُد ايطاٖس عثُإ د .ستُد عثد ايهس ِٜايعٝا س٠ د .ضًُٝإ بٔ عً ٞايػعًٞٝ د .فطني بٔ عً ٞارتسٞ ٚ إْتصاز ْا س (َطاعد ايت سٜس) ٖ ١٦ٝا،طتػاز ٜٔايعا١ٝ، و.د .دْٝظ َهٓسَْ ،ٞعٗد ْٖٛغ نْٛغ و.د .ومحد عٛد ،ٙجاَع ١ايريَٛى، و.دٗ .د ايدي ،ِٝجاَع ١اً،و ضعٛد، يًرتبْٖٛ ،١ٝغ نْٛغ ،ايصني إزبد ،اةزدٕ اًُ،ه ١ايعسب ١ٝايطعٛد١ٜ و.د.غانس عثد اذتُٝد ضًُٝإ، و.د .فٝإ مخٝظ ،ادتاَع ١اةَسٜه١ٝ و.د. اةنادمي ١ٝا،صس ١ٜيآلدا ،ايكاٖسَ ،٠صس بثريٚت ،بريٚت ،يثٓإ دَػل ،ضٛزٜا و.د .ومحد بطتإ ،جاَع ١ايهٜٛت، و.د .عثد ايسمحٔ ايطسٜس ،ٟجاَع١ و.د .ستُد عً ،ٞجاَع ١و اباضها، ايهٜٛت اً،و ضعٛد،اًُ،ه ١ايعسب ١ٝايطعٛد١ٜ نٓدا اجملًد ايتاضع -ايعدد ايسابع اذتٓ ١كس ،جاَع ١دَػل، -ونتٛبس 2015 - فكٛم ايطثىع © 2015ستفٛظى ١جملًى ١ايدزاضىات ايرتبٜٛىٚ ١ايٓفطىٚ . ١ٝاجملًى ١ريىري َطىوٚي ١عىٔ ارزا ٤آ،ػىٛزٗٝ ٠ىا فٝى وْى٘ اثٌ آزا ٤ايثافثني (ا،ويفني). ا غرتانات ايطٓ 15 :١ٜٛزٜا داخىٌ عُىإ 50ٚد ٚزا وَسٜهٝىا يًُػىرتنني َىٔ خىازا عُىإ .ا تصىا ت َىع اجملًى ١تهىَ ٕٛىٔ خالٍ َهتثٗا :زتً ١ايدزاضات ايرتبٚ ١ٜٛايٓفط ،١ٝنً ١ٝايرتب ،١ٝجاَعى ١ايطىًطإ قىابٛعَ ،32 : .: ،طىك – 123ضىًطٓ١ عُإ ٖ -اتف - )00968( 24143868 :انظ - )00968( 24143817 :بسٜد إيهرتjesedu@squ.edu.om :ْٞٚ قٛاعد ايٓػس( َٛجٗ ١يًثافثني ٚاحملهُني ) ايت هِٝ تسضٌ مجٝع ايث ٛص ا،كدَ ١يًٓػس إىل َتخصصني يت هُٗٝا فطب اة ٍٛايعًًُٜٚ .١ٝك ٢ايث ايٓٗا ٞ٥بعد وٕ جيس ٟايثاف ايتعدٜالت اي ايكثٍٛ ٜطًثٗا احملهُٚ .ٕٛايثافثَ ٕٛطوٚي ٕٛعٔ ستتٜٛات وعا ِٗ، ايث ٛص آ،ػٛز ٠تعرب عٔ ٚجْٗ ١عس ناتثٗٝا ٚيٝظ عٔ ٚجْٗ ١عس اجملً .١نُا وٕ ايث ٛص ا،سضً ١إىل اجملً١ ختضع يف ص وٚي ٞتك ّٛب٘ ٖ ١٦ٝايت سٜس ،يتكسٜس وًٖٝتٗا يًت هٚ ِٝايتصاَٗا بكٛاعد ايٓػسٚ .حيل هل١٦ٝ َٔ د ٕٚإبدا ٤اةضثا . ايت سٜس وٕ تعترز عٔ قث ٍٛايث غسٚط ايٓػس توند ٖ ١٦ٝايت سٜس عً ٢اسٚز ٠ا يتصاّ بػسٚط ايٓػس بػهٌ ناٌَ ،إذ وٕ ايث ٛص اي ايٓػس ضٛف ئ ٜٓعس ٗٝا ٚتعاد إىل و ابٗا َثاغس ٠فتٜ ٢تِ ايتكٝد بػسٚط ايٓػس. تكدّ ْطخ ١إيهرت َٔ ١ْٝٚايث .1 تًتصّ بػسٚط عً ٢بسْاَ َٔ Wordخالٍ إمي ٌٝاجملً١ َ jesedu@squ.edu.omع َساعا:٠ Simplified Arabicع ِ ٚ 12بٗٛاَؼ ف ِ وٕ تهتب ايث ٛص بايًغ ١ايعسب ١ٝباضتخداّ خ ايٛافد َٓٗا 2ضِ عًٚ ٟٛضفًٚ ٞوٜطس3ٚ ،ضِ ومئٚ ،تسى َطا ْٚ ١صف بني ايططٛز. وٕ تهتب ايث ٛص بايًغ ١اوجنًٝص ١ٜباضتخداّ خ Time New Romansع ِ ٚ 12بٗٛاَؼ ف ِ ايٛافد 2ضِ عًٚ ٟٛضفًٚ ٞومئ3ٚ ،ضِ وٜطس ٚتسى َطا ْٚ ١صف بني ايططٛز. .2 وٕ عٔ 8000نًُ ١مبا ٜصٜد عدد نًُات ايث ذيو اةغهاٍ ٚا،ساجع ٚادتداٚ .ٍٚوٕ ٜصٜد عدد ادتدا ٍٚعٔ 7جدا.ٍٚ .3 وَ ٚكدَاً يًٓػس وٕ ٜه ٕٛقد مت ْػس ايث زتً ١وخسٜٚ ;٣تعٗد ايثاف بريو ٚبعدّ تكدِٜ عث٘ يًٓػس إىل جٗ ١وخس ٣إىل وٕ ٜتِ اختاذ ايكساز آ،اضب بأْ٘ اطًع عً ٢غسٚط ايٓػس .4 .5 وٕ ٜه ٕٛايث ٖرا ايػإٜٔٚ.تعٗد ايثاف ايسٝ٥طٞ اجملًٚ ١ايتصّ بٗا. جصً٤ا َٔ نتا َٓػٛز . جيٛش ْػس ايث َهإ آخس ،بعد إقساز ْػسٙ و ٚوجصاَ٘ٓ ٤ زتً ١ايدزاضات ايرتب١ٜٛ ٚايٓفط ١ٝظاَع ١ايطًطإ قابٛع ،إ بعداذتص ٍٛعً ٢إذٕ نتاب ٞبريو َٔ زٝ٥ظ ايت سٜس . .6 َٛا ك ١ايثاف عًْ ٢كٌ فكٛم ايٓػس نا ١إىل اجملًٚ ،١إذا زريثت اجملً١ إٕ إعادْ ٠ػس ايث عًٗٝا وٕ حتصٌ عًَٛ ٢ا كَ ١هتٛب َٔ ١افث٘ . .7 جيب وٕ ٜس ل َع ايث ًَخصإ ٚافد بايًغ ١ايعسبٚ ١ٝآخس بايًغ ١اوجنًٝصٜ ٚ ١ٜصٜد عدد نًُات وُٜٗا عٔ 200نًُ.١ .8 ًٜتصّ ايثافثٕٛ نتاب ١عٚ ِٗ ٛعٓد تٝ ٛل َساجعِٗ بدي ٌٝايسابط ١اةَسٜه ١ٝيعًِ ايٓفظ – او داز ارتاَظ -عًُاً بإٔ ايث ٛص اي تًتصّ بٗر ايدي ٌٝضٛف ٜتِ إعادتٗا إىل و ابٗا دٕٚ حتهُٗٝا. .9 جملً ١ايدزاضات ايرتبٚ ١ٜٛايٓفط ١ٝاذتل ضٝاض ١ايٓػس .11 طًب فرف و ٚتعد ٌٜو ٟجص َٔ ٤ايث مبا ٜتفل َع اجملً.١ ٜثًّغ ايثافث ٕٛبكساز ٖ ١٦ٝايت سٜس ريض ٕٛال ١إىل ضت ١غٗٛز َٔ تازٜخ اضتالّ ايث . َع٘. .11 إز ام ودٚات ايث .12 تعسٜف ايثاف مبصادز دعِ عث٘ . َالفعٖ ١اَ :١يًث ٛص ا،كدَ ١بايًغ ١ايعسب ،١ٝتطتخدّ اةزقاّ اهلٓد ( ١ٜا،نت ٚادتداٚ ٍٚا،ساجع) إ فاي ١وٕ ا،سجع بايًغ ١اوجنًٝص ١ٜعٓدٖا تطتخدّ اةزقاّ ايعسب ،١ٝنُا جيب وٕ ته ٕٛايفا ً١ ايعػس ١ٜيألزقاّ اهلٓد ١ٜنُا ٖٚ ٛااح ا،ثاٍ ودْاٚ ،ٙضٛف ٜتِ إعاد ٠ايث ايرٟ تًتصّ وزقاَ٘ بٗرا ايٓعاّ. مثال( 76,32 :الفاصمة العشرية :شفت مع حرف الواو العربي)؛ أما الفاصمة في النص فتكون ( )،من شفت وحرف النون العربي. تٓػس اجملً ١ايث ٛص: .1اٝ،داْ / ١ٝاوَربٜك ١ٝاة ً.١ٝ .2ايٓٛع ١ٝايت ً.١ًٝٝ ًَ .3خصات ٚعسٚض ايهتب ادتدٜد.٠ .4ا،ساجعات ايٓكد ١ٜيألد ايرتبٚ ٟٛايٓفط.ٞ َتطًثات إعداد ايث : جيب وٕ تتضُٔ َطٛد ٠ايث .1 : ف َٓ ١فصً ١عًٗٝا اضِ ايثاف ٚعٓٛاْ٘ بعد عٓٛإ ايث َثاغس ٠بايًغتني ايعسبٚ ١ٝا جنًٝص.١ٜ ٚبرنس بسٜد ٙاويهرت.ْٞٚ .2 ًَخصني وفدُٖا بايعسبٚ ١ٝارخس باوجنًٝصٜ ١ٜت اٚش نٌ َُٓٗا َ ٦نًُ.١ .3 مخظ نًُات َفتاف.١ٝ .4 َطٛد ٠ايث ٚتته َٔ ٕٛاةجصا ٤ايتاي:١ٝ َكدَٚ :١تتضُٔ اوطاز ايٓعس ٟيًث ٚ ،ته ٕٛايدزاضات ايطابك ١جص٤اً َٓٗا َٓٚدزت١ جطِ ا،كدَ( ١و ٟبد ٕٚعٓٛإ َطتكٌ). َػهً ١ايدزاضٚ ١وٖدا ٗا ٚوضً٦تٗا /و ٚساٝاتٗا. وُٖ ١ٝايدزاضٚ ١ستدداتٗا. ايطسٜكٚ ١إجسا٤ات ايدزاضٚ :١تتضُٔ(اجملتُع ٚايعٚ ،١ٓٝودٚات ايدزاضٚ ، ١ايتعسٜفات اوجسا١ٝ٥ يًُصطً ات ٚ ،دم اةدا ٚ ٠ثاتٗا ٚإجسا٤ات ايدزاضٚ ،١طسٜك ١حتً ٌٝايثٝاْات). ايٓتاٜٚ ٥ػتٌُ ٖرا ايكطِ عًْ ٢تا ٥ايت ًٚ ٌٝادتداٚ ٍٚاةغهاٍ ٚايتعًٝل عًٗٝا. آ،اقػ :١ميهٔ بعض اةفٝإ دَ آ،اقػَ ١ع ايٓتا٥ ا،ساجع المالحق ،إن وجدت. .5 تدزا ايسض ّٛايثٝاْٚ ١ٝاةغهاٍ ايتٛا١ٝ ٝ ايٓصٚ ،ته ٕٛبايًْٛني اةبٝض ٚاةضٛد ٚتسقِ تسقُٝاً َتطًطالًٚ ،تهتب ومساؤٖا ٚعٓاٜٗٓٚا ٚا،الفعات ايتٛا ١ٝ ٝحتتٗا. .6 تدزا ادتداٍٚ ايٓص ٚتسقِ تسقُٝاً َتطًطالً ٚتهتب عٓاٜٗٓٚا ٛقٗا. وَا ا،الفعات ايتٛا ١ٝ ٝتهتب حتت ادتدَ .ٍٚثاٍٜ :عسض جدٜ ٚ ،1 ٍٚهتب ٜعسض ادتد،1 ٍٚ عٓٛإ ادتد.ٍٚ ا،نت ٚ ترنس اهلٛاَؼ َٚالفعات ٚتٛا ٝات ايثاف .7 غسٚط تطً ِٝايث آخس ايصف ١عٓد ايضسٚز.٠ : ٜ كدّ ايثاف ْطخ ١إيهرت َٔ ْ٘ٝٚايث َهتٛب ١عً ٢بسْاَ Windows Microsoft Word.عرب ايربٜد اويهرتjesedu@squ.edu.om ْٞٚ زضاي ١تتضُٔ ايسريث١ ْػس ايث تعٗد بعدّ تكد ِٜايث . إىل زتً ١وخسٚ ٣عدّ إزضاي٘ فتٜ ٢تِ اوْتٗا َٔ ٤حته ِٝايث ٚإ داز قساز بايٓػس و ٚعدَ٘. إجسا٤ات ايت هٚ ِٝايٓػس: .1تك ١٦ٖٝ ّٛايت سٜس مبساجع ١ايث ٛص ٚايدزاضات ٚاةعُاٍ اةنادمي ١ٝيًتأند َٔ اضتٝفاٗ٥ا ،عاٜري ايٓػس اجملً.١ .2تسضٌ ايث ٛص ٚايدزاضات ٚاةعُاٍ اةنادمي ١ٝا،طت ١ٝ ٛيًُعاٜري إىل ا ٓني َٔ احملهُني. ٜ .3هتب نٌ ستهِ تكسٜسا عٔ َد ٣الف ١ٝايث .4إذا اختًف احملهُإٜ ،سضٌ ايث يًٓػس . حملهِ اي يرتجٝح اذتهِٜٚ ،عترب فهُ٘ ْٗاٝ٥ا. ٜ .5ثًّغ ا،ويف/ا،ويف ٕٛبٓت ١ ٝايت ه ِٝخالٍ َد ٠ترتاٚث بني ال ١إىل ضت ١وغٗس َٔ تازٜخ اضتالّ ايث ٚبعد إجسا ٤ايتعدٜالت عً ٘ٝإٕ ٚجدت. .6خيضع تستٝب ايث ٛص ٚوٚيْ ١ٜٛػسٖا عتثازات ٓ ١ٝحتددٖا ٖ ١٦ٝايت سٜس. .7ميٓح ايثاف ْطختإ َٔ ايعدد ايرْ ٟػس ٘ٝعث٘ ْٚطخ ١إيهرت.١ْٝٚ منٛذا يًتٝ ٛل فطب ْعاّ ايسابط ١اةَسٜه ١ٝيعًِ ايٓفظ ( )APA ايٓص ٚقا ١ُ٥ا،ساجع مالحظة :البحوث التي ال تلتزم بدقة بهذا النظام تعاد إلى أصحابها بدون تحكيم. نتا (َويف ٚافد) قا ١ُ٥ا،ساجع: الفراجي ،هادي أحمد ( .)6002ترشيد استهالك المياه .الرياض :مكتب التربية العربي لدول الخميج. ايٓص: (الفراجي )6002 ،أو الفراجي ()6002 نتا (ونثس َٔ َويف ٚافد) قا ١ُ٥ا،ساجع: الزاممي ،عمي عبد جاسم؛ والصارمي ،عبد اهلل محمد ،وكاظم ،عمي مهدي ( .)6008مفاهيم وتطبيقات في التقويم والقياس التربوي .الكويت :مكتبة الفالح لمنشر والتوزيع. ايٓص: (الزاممي والصارمي وكاظم )7002 ،أو الزاممي والصارمي وكاظم (( ،)7002الزاممي وآخرون)7002 ، أو الزاممي وآخرون ()7002 صٌ نتا قا ١ُ٥ا،ساجع: طعيمة ،رشدي أحمد ( .)6002بين المفهوم والمصطمح .في رشدي أحمد طيعمة (محرر) .الجودة الشاممة في التعميم بين مؤشرات التميز ومعايير االعتماد :األسس والتطبيقات (ص ص .)66-98عمان :دار المسيرة. ايٓص: (طعيمة )7003 ،أو طعيمة ()7003 ع دٚز ١ٜعًَُ( ١ٝويف ٚافد) قا ١ُ٥ا،ساجع: الموسوي ،نعمان محمد صالح ( .)6002تطوير أداة لقياس إدارة الجودة الشاممة في مؤسسات التعميم العالي .المجمة التربوية -الكويت.997 -76 ،)26( 72 ، ايٓص: (الموسوي )7006 ،أو الموسوي ()7006 ع دٚز ١ٜعًُ( ١ٝونثس َٔ َويف ٚافد) قا ١ُ٥ا،ساجع: الغنبوصي ،سالم بن سميم؛ والحارثي ،حمود بن خمفان؛ وكاظم ،عمي مهدي ( .)6096تقويم برنامج إعداد المعمم بكمية التربية في جامعة السمطان قابوس من وجهة نظر الخريجين.مجمة الدراسات التربوية والنفسية_ جامعة السمطان قابوس.992-88 ،3 ، ايٓص: (ايغٓثٚ ٞ ٛاذتاز ٚ ٞناظِ )2012 ،و ٚايغٓثٚ ٞ ٛاذتاز ٚ ٞناظِ ()2012 بعد ا قتثاع اة( ٍٚايغٓثٚ ٞ ٛآخس )2012 ،ٕٚو ٚايغٓثٚ ٞ ٛآخس)2012( ٕٚ َكاٍ زتًَ( ١ويف ٚافد) قا ١ُ٥ا،ساجع: عثمان ،إبراهيم ( .)9872التغيرات في األسرة الحضرية في األردن.مجمة العموم االجتماعية952 ،)2(91، – .966 ايٓص: (عثمان)9872 ،أو عثمان ()9872 َكاٍ جسٜد( ١َٜٝٛ ٠بدَ ٕٚويف) قا ١ُ٥ا،ساجع: تزايد تداعيات الركود العالمي عمى ألمانيا ( ،29يناير .)6008 ،الوطن االقتصادي -سمطنة عمان ،ص.6 ايٓص: (”تزايد تداعيات الركود“)7002 ، َٛاقع إْرتْت َتخصص١ قا ١ُ٥ا،ساجع: هل المرح ينبع من هرمونات الذكورة ؟ ( ،62يناير .)6007 ،تاريخ االسترجاع 96 :مايو ،6008من: http://www.eparanm.org ايٓص: (رابطة اإلخصائيين النفسيين المصرية ]رانم[)7002 ، (رانم)7002 ، َكاٍ َٓػٛز إيهرتْٝٚا (ع َٓػٛز قاعد ٠بٝاْات) قا ١ُ٥ا،ساجع: عاشور ،يوسف جمعة (” .)6007حيث“ في القرآن الكريم استعماال وداللة .مجمة الجامعة االسالمية- غزة .972-919 ،)9( 73 ،تاريخ االسترجاع 96مايو ،6008من: http://www.iugaza.edu.ps/ara/research/articles ايٓص: (عاشور)7002 ،أو عاشور ()7002 احملتٜٛات ايثاف اٛ،اٛع ا،هإ ايرتب ٟٛا،فضٌّ يتعً ِٝايطًث ١ذ ٟٚاوعاقَٔ ١ ٚجْٗ ١عسو ا ايعالق١ ضًطٓ ١عُإ جالٍ فاا فطني ايصف ١ 644-628 َٓطكَ ١ادضٔ ايتعًٚ ١ٜ ٚ ،١ُٝٝضهاْطٔ ،ايٜ ٛات ا،ت د ٠ا َسٜه١ٝ عثد اذتا غ قاضِ ايػاٜب جاَع ١آٍ ايثٝت ،اةزدٕ إبساٖ ِٝايكسٜٛتٞ جاَع ١ايطًطإ قابٛع ،ضًطٓ ١عُإ ايعالق ١بني إدزاى ايطًث ١ايعُاْٝني ٚاضتخداّ مجع ١بٔ بط ٞايثٛضعٝدٚ ٟدٜٓا ادتٌُ اضرتاتٝ ٝات ايهتاب ٚ ١ودا ِٗ٥ايهتابٞ جاَع ١ايريَٛى ،اةزدٕ اةخطا ٤اوَال١ٝ٥ تعًِ ايًغ ١اوجنًٝص ١ٜعٓد ايطال ايعُاْٝني غٝخ ١بٓت عً ٞايربٜه١ٝ 659-645 676-661 ٚشاز ٠ايرتب ،١ٝضًطٓ ١عُإ عثد ٚستُد ا،خال جاَع ١ايطًطإ قابٛع ،ضًطٓ ١عُإ تطثٝل ايطًث ١ايعُاْٝني يًُعٝاز ايثاْ، ٞدزبٞ ايتهٓٛيٛجٝا ومحد ٜٛضف عثد ايسف ٚ ِٝطالٍ غعثإ عاَس بسْاَ اوذتام اٗ،ين جاَع ١ايطًطإ قابٛع ،ضًطٓ ١عُإ تأج ٌٝاوغثاع اةنادمي ٚ ٞعالقت٘ باحملددات ايدا ع١ٝ رب ٟعثد ايفتاث ٚض س ايػٛزظٞ ٚايت ص ٌٝايدزاضٚ ٞضاعات ا ضترناز يد ٟطال ا،سفً ١ايثاْ١ٜٛ جاَع ١ايطًطإ قابٛع ،ضًطٓ ١عُإ اطُ ١بٓت ٜٛضف ايثٛضعٝد١ٜ ٚايت دٜات جاَع ١ايطًطإ قابٛع ،ضًطٓ ١عُإ عالق ١ايكًل ا جتُاع ٞبطًط ١ايٛايد ٜٔيد ٣طًث١ ٖالٍ بٔ شاٖس ايٓثٗاْٚ ٞعثد اذتُٝد ضعٝد فطٔ جاَع ١ايطًطإ قابٛع ايٛع ٞايعكً ٞيد ٣ا،سغد ٜٔاٝٓٗ،ني أُ ايث١٦ٝ ايعُاْ١ٝ 711-691 ضًطٓ ١عُإ :حتً ٌٝا،طاز بساَ ايًغ ١ايعسب ١ٝيغري ايٓاطكني بٗا :ايصعٛبات نً ١ٝايرتب١ٝ 691-677 717-711 729-718 جاَع ١ايطًطإ قابٛع ،ضًطٓ ١عُإ َٓ ٢بٓت عثداهلل ايث ساْٚ ١ٝبهاز ضًُٝإ بهاز جاَع ١ايطًطإ قابٛع ،ضًطٓ ١عُإ 737-731