P1: JLS International Journal of Primatology [ijop] pp1259-ijop-489437 September 2, 2004 23:19 Style file version Nov. 18th, 2002 C 2004) International Journal of Primatology, Vol. 25, No. 5, October 2004 ( Male Masturbation in Free-Ranging Japanese Macaques Ruth Thomsen1,3 and Joseph Soltis2 Received November 5, 2003; revision February 5, 2004; accepted February 13, 2004 We studied masturbatory behavior of males over a 14-mo period in freeranging Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata yakui) on Yakushima island, Japan. We distinguished 2 types: (1) masturbation only and (2) masturbation with final ejaculation. Both types occurred in all 15 adult troop males. We calculated masturbation only as an index (M/h). It occurred over the whole year; however, it was more frequent during the mating than during the non-mating periods. Masturbation with ejaculation was restricted to the mating period. Male mating success correlated positively with social status. Both rate of masturbation only and masturbatory ejaculations increased as male social status and male mating success declined. Lower-ranking males had higher rates of masturbation only, i.e., they spent more time masturbating than higherranking males did. Lower-ranking males also had higher proportions of ejaculates for masturbation, while higher-ranking males used most of their ejaculates for mating. KEY WORDS: masturbation; Japanese macaques; Yakushima. INTRODUCTION Male masturbation occurs in captive nonhuman primate species, including gibbons (Hylobates spp.), chacma baboons (Papio ursinus), mangabeys (Cercocebus albigena), red spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi), chimpanzees 1 Institute for Zoo and Wildlife Research, Department of Evolutionary Ecology, AlfredKowalke-Str. 17, 10315 Berlin, Germany. 2 Wildlife Tracking Center, Animal Programs Administration, Disney’s Animal Kingdom, P.O. Box 10000, Lake Buena Vista, Florida 32830. 3 To whom correspondence should be addressed; e-mail: thomsen@izw-berlin.de. 1033 C 2004 Springer Science+Business Media, Inc. 0164-0291/04/1000-1033/0 P1: JLS International Journal of Primatology [ijop] 1034 pp1259-ijop-489437 September 2, 2004 23:19 Style file version Nov. 18th, 2002 Thomsen and Soltis (Pan troglodytes), lowland gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) and some macaque species (Bielert and van der Walt, 1982; Carpenter, 1942; Linnankoski et al., 1993; Maple, 1977; Mootnick and Baker, 1994; Nieuwenhuijsen et al., 1987). In a study on reproductive strategies in freeranging baboons (Papio cynocephalus anubis) Bercovitch (1989) noted that masturbation to ejaculation did not correlate with testicular size. Theory predicts that the amount of ejaculate is an important factor during sperm competition as a male should increase his chance to outcompete the sperm of rival males by increasing the amount of sperm inseminated into a female (Parker, 1970, 1982). This quantity model is supported by comparative studies that show that testicular size and the level of female promiscuity are highly and positively correlated within taxa (Gage, 1994; Harcourt et al., 1981; Ridley, 1999). In short, the quantity model suggests that males in species that face a high degree of sperm competition should possess large testes, produce large amounts of sperm, store sperm in large sperm storage organs, and finally inseminate large ejaculates into females. Recently 34 out of a total of 52 nonhuman primate species masturbated (65%). Masturbation only and masturbation to ejaculation, occurred more often in species in a multimale multifemale social system than in species living in other social systems (Thomsen et al., 2003). Masturbation to ejaculation could be beneficial if it improves ejaculate quality in spite of the costs of loosing sperm (Baker and Bellis, 1993). But it also could be costly if it reduces a male’s chance for further copulations in the case they run out of sperm because of previous masturbatory ejaculations. Primate males of middle or low social status are often excluded from mating due to successful mate guarding of females by males of higher social status (Berard et al., 1994; Soltis et al., 2001). Since sperm production is continuous throughout the breeding season (Matsubayashi and Enomoto, 1983) nonmating or less mating males of those species have 2 options regarding their sperm: (A) store sperm inside capus, corpus and cauda epididymis and vas deferens until an opportunity for copulation arises or (B) flush out sperm by masturbation. Our aim was (1) to get basic information of male masturbation in a free-ranging primate species, (2) to detect patterns of male masturbation during a whole year and (3) to test whether masturbation with and without ejaculation are related to male social status and male mating success. We choose Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata) as subject animals because masturbation only occurs in captive males (Tokuda, 1961) and we saw masturbation to ejaculation ad libitum in a free-ranging population in 1993. Furthermore, Japanese macaques are seasonally breeding primates, females mate with multiple males, and degree of sperm competition is high (Soltis, et al., 1999). P1: JLS International Journal of Primatology [ijop] pp1259-ijop-489437 September 2, 2004 Male Masturbation in Free-Ranging Japanese Macaques 23:19 Style file version Nov. 18th, 2002 1035 METHODS Definitions Mating: Japanese macaques are series mounters. Mating refers to terminal, ejaculatory mounts that are characterized by a rigid posture of the male and remaining ejaculate (white plug) on the female’s perineum (Soltis et al., 2000). Masturbation: Rhythmic manual rubbing of the erect penis for ≥5 sec. Masturbation with ejaculation: Rhythmic manual rubbing of the erect penis terminating in ejaculation. Study Animals and Sites We conducted observations on NINA-A-troop in the Hanyama study site on the Northwestern coast of Yakushima Island (Maruhashi, 1980). The troop has been continuously habituated to human observers since 1994 (Yakushima Research Group). During the study period it comprised ca. 50 individuals, including juveniles and infants. Troop composition changed between 2 mating periods (MP) due to natural demographic processes such as deaths, births, immigration and emigration. It consisted of 15 adult males and 15 adult females during MP 1997 (October–February) and 12 males and 13 females during MP 1998 (October-November). All adults could be recognized individually. We calculated social status (rank 1–15) via a dominance matrix. The age of each male (4–15 years) was known (Soltis et al., 2000). Behavioral Data Collection From October 1997 until November 1998 we collected data on both types of masturbation and on male mating success via focal animal sampling (Thomsen) and ad libitum observations (Thomsen and Soltis, Altmann, 1974). Total time of focal sampling (males and females) comprised 1204 h by Thomsen (Soltis et al., 2000). Total time of focal sampling of the 15 males by Thomsen is 435 h 30 min (mean ± s.d. = 30.1 ± 22.5 h/male, range: 6 h 10 min – 12 h). From time of detection of the troop in the morning the selected male was located and followed during the daytime over a 2-day period, if possible. If the focal male could not be detected within a 30-min period of initially finding the troop, we followed the male with the closest lower social status instead for the rest of the day. If a male was lost in the dense and steep forest, P1: JLS International Journal of Primatology [ijop] pp1259-ijop-489437 September 2, 2004 1036 23:19 Style file version Nov. 18th, 2002 Thomsen and Soltis we followed him again when he could be located. We included focal samples in the analysis if he was followed successfully for ≥6 h per observation day without interruptions for >5 min (N = 12 males in MP 1997, N = 13 males in MP 1998). To calculate amount of time a male spent masturbating per hour observation (independently of final ejaculation) we used one-zero sampling. Within 1-min-periods on the instance of the sample point it was recorded whether or not the focal male had masturbated during the preceding sample interval (Martin and Bateson, 2002). We calculated an individual masturbatory index by dividing the total number of observed masturbations by duration of total focal sampling time in h per male (M/h). Whenever we saw a male mating or masturbating with ejaculation we recorded exact time and social circumstances. Statistics We calculated nonparametrical statistics via SPSS version 9.0 for Windows. All tests are two-tailed and two-tailed alpha is 0.05 (Conover, 1980). RESULTS Seasonal Differences in Masturbation We could follow 13 of 15 males for ≥6 h during observation days. Time that males spent with masturbating reached a maximum from October until February when at least one troop female was in estrus (M/h: mean ± s.d. = 4.1 ± 1.9, N = 13 males). It was significantly reduced during the nonmating period between March and August 1998, i.e. when no female was in estrus (M/h: mean ± s.d. = 1.2 ± 0.9; Wilcoxon, z = −3.19, p < 0.001). Masturbation Only and Male Social Status Incidence of masturbation increased as male social status declined (Fig. 1). The lower a male’s social status the higher his masturbatory index (accuracy 0.1), i.e. the more time he spent with masturbation during the 14-mo period (rs = 0.75, N = 13, p < 0.05). This correlation applied to both mating (rs = 0.75, N = 13, p < 0.01) and nonmating periods (rs = 0.71, N = 13, p < 0.01). The smaller correlation coefficient of the nonmating period might be a result of fewer males present regularly in the troop during this period of the year. P1: JLS International Journal of Primatology [ijop] pp1259-ijop-489437 September 2, 2004 23:19 Male Masturbation in Free-Ranging Japanese Macaques Style file version Nov. 18th, 2002 1037 Fig. 1. Masturbatory index of free-ranging Japanese macaque males on Yakushima island, Japan, in relation to their social status. The lower a male’s social status the more time he spent masturbating. Masturbation to Ejaculation and Male Mating Success We observed a total of N = 213 ejaculations: 105 during mating and 108 finished a masturbatory event. We observed all 15 troop males to masturbate to ejaculation at least once. Masturbation with ejaculation occurred during different social circumstances. Males masturbated in the presence of a mating pair (N = 19, 17.6%). Males masturbated in the presence of an estrous female (N = 40, 37.0%). In 7 out of the 40 cases (17.5%), they mated successfully on the same day after they had masturbated with ejaculation. Males started masturbating after an estrous female had passed them (N = 14, 13.0%). Males masturbated in the presence of other masturbating males (N = 10, 9.3%). Males masturbated when staying solitarily in the forest (N = 20, 18.5%). We could not clearly assign 5 cases (4.6%) to any of these situations. With the exception of the lowest-ranking male (rank 15) all other 14 troop males mateed at least once with ejaculation (range: 1–28; mean ± s.d. = 7.0 ± 8.9). Total number of ejaculates inseminated by NINA-A-troop males into females increased with social status (rs = 0.934, N = 15, p < 0.01). P1: JLS International Journal of Primatology [ijop] pp1259-ijop-489437 September 2, 2004 1038 23:19 Style file version Nov. 18th, 2002 Thomsen and Soltis Proportion of Ejaculates Used to Mate and to Masturbate Proportion of all ejaculations occurring during mating increased with male social status (rs = 0.68, N = 15, p < 0.01). In contrast, proportion used for masturbation decreased with social status (rs = −0.67, N = 15, p < 0.01). For instance, 7 (21.2%) of the alpha-male ejaculations were masturbatory, whereas at least half (50%–100%) of ejaculations of males of rank ≥6 were masturbatory (Fig. 2). The 2nd-ranking male, which masturbated disproportionately often given his rank (57.5%) was displaced from the alpha position 3 days before data collection started in October 1997 (Soltis, personal observation). DISCUSSION We focused on male masturbation because female masturbation has not been observed in the population. All 15 males of NINA-A-troop, males of 3 surrounding troops (Miki Matsubara, Hideki Sugiura, Sachiko Hayakawa, personal communication) and 4 non-troop males (own observations) masturbated during the study period. Masturbation also occurs in Japanese macaques in a provisioned troop at Arashiyama, Japan (Michael Huffman, unpublished data) and in males in solitar cages (own observations). The male masturbatory index was higher during the mating (mean M/hour = 4.2) than during the nonmating periods (mean M/hour = 1.2). Fig. 2. Proportion of ejaculates which NINA-A-troop males (N = 15) used for mating and for masturbation in relation to their social status. Higher-ranking males used a higher proportion of ejaculates for mating while mid- and low-ranking males used higher proportions for masturbation. P1: JLS International Journal of Primatology [ijop] pp1259-ijop-489437 Male Masturbation in Free-Ranging Japanese Macaques September 2, 2004 23:19 Style file version Nov. 18th, 2002 1039 During the mating period, NINA-A-troop males masturbated 4.2 times within 60 min independently from ejaculations. This would be 50.4 times within one 12-h day. There is no comparative datum from other free-ranging primate species available yet, however, the value seems to be high. Macaques in general may masturbate more frequently than chimpanzees or other nonseasonal or less promiscuous species do. Seasonality might be important for the amount of masturbation due to the presence of visible cues, i.e. estrous females or mating pairs. The masturbatory index in our subjects is related to male social status. Males of low social status spent more time masturbating than males of high social status did. In rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) male social status correlated positively with median mount series duration (Manson, 1996). Brief and hidden copulations of sneaker males are perhaps an adaptive response to the threat of mount series disruption by higher-ranking males (Dunbar, 1984; Manson, 1996; Ohsawa et al., 1993). In humans, there is a linear relationship between motile sperm concentration and motile sperm number and the time taken to produce a masturbatory ejaculate within 5– 30 min (Pound et al., 2002). In macaques masturbation only could function to increase precopulatory sexual arousal of lower-ranking males that might increase sperm number in the cauda epididymis and vas deferens before mounting. Proportion of all ejaculations used for mating increased with social status, while in low- and middle-ranking males ejaculations were largely masturbatory. Although masturbation to ejaculation occurred during differing social circumstances, the majority of 67.6% of cases occured in the presence of an estrous female, which may reflect the same pattern of masturbation stimulated by visual cues in captives (Bielert and van der Walt, 1982; Linnankoski et al., 1993). Furthermore, in 7 cases, sneaker males masturbated only a few times (2–17 min) before mating. The behavior also occurs in rhesus macaques (Carpenter, 1942) and savanna baboons (Bercovitch, 1989; Ransom, 1981). Masturbation to ejaculation before mating could also be accidental, i.e. The quantity model to improve number of motile sperm by masturbating proximate to mating might fail and males simply lost their ejaculates. Or it could lead to lower ejaculate volume and reduced sperm number, but to a better ejaculate quality because the next ejaculate that is used for mating may contain fewer but quicker moving sperm. This interpretation of the observed pattern would support the quality model of Baker and Bellis (1993). Finally, describing patterns of masturbation in wild populations might inform future models of sperm competition in primates, which should incorporate observations of mating behaviors in addition to data on quantity and quality of sperm. P1: JLS International Journal of Primatology [ijop] pp1259-ijop-489437 September 2, 2004 1040 23:19 Style file version Nov. 18th, 2002 Thomsen and Soltis ACKNOWLEDGMENTS We thank all members of the Yakushima Research Group in Japan for their interest and active discussion during this study. Special thanks go to Juichi Yamagiwa, Kiyoaki Matsubayashi and Hideyuki Ohsawa from Primate Research Institute of the Kyoto-University for their support. RT is grateful to Gerhard Neuweiler, Department Biology II, University of Munich and to Heribert Hofer, Institute for Zoo and Wildlife Research, Berlin, who enriched this work with critical comments. The study was financed by the German Academic Exchange Service (HSP III, D/97/16290). REFERENCES Altmann, J. (1974). Observational study of behavior: Sampling methods. Behaviour 49: 227–265. Baker, R. R., and Bellis, M. A. (1993). Human sperm competition: Ejaculate adjustment by males and the function of masturbation. Anim. Behav. 46: 861–885. Berard, J. D., Nurnberg, P., Epplen, J. T., and Schmidtke, J. (1994). Alternative reproductive tactics and reproductive success in male rhesus macaques. Behaviour 129: 177–201. Bercovitch, F. B. (1989). Body size, sperm competition, and determinants of reproductive success in male savanna baboons. Evolution 43: 1507–1521. Bielert, C., and van der Walt, L. A. (1982). Male chacma baboon (Papio ursinus) sexual arousal: Mediation by visual cues from female conspecifics. Psychoneuroendocrinology 7: 31– 48. Carpenter, C. R. (1942). Sexual behavior of free ranging rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). J. Comp. Psychol. 33: 143–164. Conover, W. J. (1980). Practical Nonparametric Statistics, Wiley, New-York. Dunbar, R. I. M. (1984). Reproductive decisions: An economic analysis of gelada Baboon strategies, Princeton University Press, Princeton NJ. Gage, M. J. G. (1994). Associations between body size, mating pattern, testis size and sperm lengths across butterflies. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 258: 247–254. Harcourt, A. H., Harvey, P. H., Larson, S. G., and Short, R. V. (1981). Testis weight, body weight and breeding system in primates. Nature 293: 55–57. Linnankoski, I., Grönros, M., and Pertovãara, A. (1993). Eye contact as a trigger of male sexual arousal in stump-tailed macaques. Folia primatol. 60: 181–184. Manson, J. (1996). Male dominance and mount series duration in Cayo Santiago rhesus macaques. Anim. Behav. 51: 1219–1231. Maple, T. (1977). Unusual sexual behavior of non-human primates. In Money, J. and Musaph, H. (eds.), Handbook of Sexology, Exerpta Medica, Amsterdam. Martin, P., and Bateson, P. (2002). Measuring Behaviour, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK. Maruhashi, T. (1980). Feeding behavior and diet of the Japanese monkey (Macaca fuscata yakui) on Yakushima Island, Japan. Primates 21: 141–160. Matsubayashi, K., and Enomoto, T. (1983). Longitudinal studies on annual changes in plasma testosterone, body weight and spermatogenesis in adult Japanese monkeys (Macaca fuscata fuscata) under laboratory conditions. Primates 24: 521–529. Mootnick, A. R., and Baker, E. (1994). Masturbation in captive Hylobates (Gibbons). Zoo. Biol. 13: 345–353. Nieuwenhuijsen, K., de Neef, K. J., van der Werff, J. J., Bosch, T., and Slob, A. K. (1987). Testosterone, testiz size, seasonality, and behavior in group-living stumptail macaques (Macaca arctoides). Horm. Behav. 21: 153–169. P1: JLS International Journal of Primatology [ijop] pp1259-ijop-489437 Male Masturbation in Free-Ranging Japanese Macaques September 2, 2004 23:19 Style file version Nov. 18th, 2002 1041 Ohsawa, H., Inoue, M., and Takenaka, O. (1993). Mating strategy and reproductive success of male patas monkeys (Erythrocebus patas). Primates 34: 533–544. Parker, G. A. (1970). Sperm competition and its evolutionary consequences in the insects. Biol. Rev. 45: 525–567. Parker, G. A. (1982). Why are there so many tiny sperm? Sperm competition and the maintenance of two sexes. J. Theor. Biol. 96: 281–294. Pound, N., Javed, M. H., Ruberto, C., Anis Shaikh, A., and Del Valle, A. P. (2002). Duration of sexual arousal predicts semen parameters for masturbatory ejaculates. Physiol. Behav. 76: 685–689. Ransom, T. W. (1981). Beach Troop of Gombe, Bucknell University Press, Lewisburg, PA. Ridley, M. (1999). Book review: Sperm competition and sexual selection. Nature 397: 576–577. Soltis, J., Mitsunaga, F., Shimizu, K., Yanagihara, Y., and Nozaki, O. (1999). Female mating strategy in an enclosed group of Japanese macaques. Am. J. Primatol. 47: 263–278. Soltis, J., Thomsen, R., Matsubayashi K., and Takenaka, O. (2000). Infanticide by resident males and female counter-strategies in wild Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 48: 195–202. Soltis, J., Thomsen, R., and Takenaka, O. (2001). The interaction of male and female reproductive strategies and paternity in wild Japanese macaques, Macaca fuscata. Anim. Behav. 62: 485–494. Thomsen, R., Soltis, J., and Teltscher, C. (2003). Sperm competition and the function of male masturbation in non-human primates. In Jones, C. B. (ed.), Sexual Selection and Reproductive Competition in Primates: New Perspectives and Directions, ASP-Book Series, Special Topics in Primatology, Vol. 3: pp. 436–453. Tokuda, K. (1961). A study of the sexual behavior in the Japanese monkey troop. Primates 3: 1–40.