Communication in its broadest sense: working with deaf children and their communities Bencie Woll In memory of Sheila Wirz • This talk reflects on research and interventions with deaf children over the past 25 years which have aligned with the priorities which Sheila Wirz brought to the field: – low cost, community-based – broadly-defined perspectives on communication and language – with the involvement of deaf people. 2 From the Brazilian reseearch group London 1993! First meeting between Brazil and England, represented by Ida Lichtig and Sheila Wirz. The awakening of a common aim towards a community based rehabilitation, placing speech therapy in a new and wide social context. A scientific and international link starts between the University of São Paulo and the University of London, with the participation of various researchers from both countries. Brazil 1995! All the Brazilian group (researchers and students ) are waiting for the new English researcher, Dr. Sheila Wirz. After her arrival she meets them and starts straightaway her enquiring based on the research questions : How many deaf are there in Brazil ? How many in São Paulo city? How many deaf children are in schools ? How many are attending speech-therapy sessions? How.....? Where.....? Why ......? People looked at each other and this look meant “this one really came to work ...she is a workaholic”…. At lunchtime her choice was walking in the woods instead of the car ride; “it’s healthier ,” she said. First lesson: “Down with sedentarism She looked disappointed when asked about São Paulo ... If we would be able to read her thoughts they would transmit something like this: “The city is too big. It is crowded with well dressed people, beautifull cars and buildings !!! Where are the snakes? and the monkeys? This is not the Brazil which needs our help, our support … Then the students arrive, the teachers, sign language interpreters, and the deaf instructors …. Confusion and embarrassment due to the difficulty in communicating in different languages. Parents arrive… reality is shown completely and crudely: unpreparedness to deal with so many difficult situations!!! Sheila cheers up and seems to find a real reason now for her coming to Brazil. She wanted to know and learn more ... as much as she could about: slums, beaches, concerts, the Brazilian way of being ... The serious researcher gives place to a glad woman, eager to learn more about the Brazilians´ social, educational and cultural reality. Light and happy she dances , sits on the floor and is touched by everything she sees. This is Sheila!!! A generous person, competent and righteous in her 3 way of being and teaching !!! Thanks for the received generosity, hospitality ,teachings and affection. 4 The Sao Paulo Project • Challenges – A country with well-developed services for deaf children – But - limited access for poorer families to those services • The project – designed to identify deafness – provide a service delivery plan to support deaf children and their families • used a low cost tool developed in Zimbabwe (Wirz et al., 2001) to identify hearing impairment in children • Developed tools for assessing communication skills as part of the first stage of a programme of intervention – Created an intervention programme involving members of the Deaf community 5 Assessing deaf children’s communication • Following recognition by the Brazilian government in 2002 of Brazilian Sign Language (also known as LIBRAS) as the official language of the Brazilian Deaf community, it is now used more widely in educational and health settings • However, 95% of deaf children are born to hearing parents, who usually do not know sign language before their child's diagnosis, and who also find it difficult to communicate with their deaf child by means of spoken language • As a consequence the child may not fully develop either SpL or SL but may communicate in a mixture of signs, words and gestures. • “Modality-free” measures were developed to provide an assessment of communication which do not depend only on language proficiency measures 6 The programme for children • 23 prelingually deaf children, ranging in age from three to six years old and their parents • All children were resident in low income neighbourhoods of São Paulo city and had hearing parents who did not know LIBRAS. • Formal education begins in Brazil at 7 and the majority of children were not enrolled in other pre-school programmes at the beginning of the programme. By the end of the programme, most were attending pre-school on a part-time basis. • The programme comprised 10 sessions • For the children – 2 groups: a 45 minute LIBRAS activity group, followed by a spoken Portuguese activity group – A break, where refreshments were available for the children, parents and team. – A storytelling workshop for the children led by the Deaf staff member – A final free play session where children could choose toys and interact freely. 7 The programme for parents • LIBRAS classes led by the Deaf staff member, including the Deaf staff member talking about experiences of deafness, growing up, and adulthood • Sessions led by the SaLT and Deaf staff member discussing deafness, the parents’ needs, and answering questions about their child’s development • Observations by the parents of the sessions led by the Deaf staff member. For many of them the first time they had seen their child able to communicate freely and fully, profoundly changing their views of their children as disabled • This experience in turn helped break down prejudices against sign language and encourage their efforts to learn LIBRAS. 8 The programme for SaLTs • The children were visited three times in their homes during the programme by a Deaf member of staff or LIBRAS interpreter, together with a speech and hearing therapist. The team: – gained an understanding of the children’s home circumstances – observed their relationship with family members – were able to match the intervention programme to the reality of the children’s home lives. • The home visits also had the aim of improving the relationship between the staff and the families, by giving family members the opportunity to express their problems, doubts and feelings 9 A deaf-hearing partnership • The vast majority of intervention teams are directed by and include only hearing people • Mixed deaf and hearing teams of service providers face enormous challenges in developing effective working relations, including the language used between deaf and hearing team members and the consequences of that communication Part of Sheila’s commitment to the community as the basis of intervention 10 Research at DCAL with deaf children and their families 11 What is DCAL? • Research centre funded by ESRC • The largest research centre in this field in Europe with over 30 staff and research students, about 1/3 of whom are deaf • DCAL places sign language and deaf people in the centre of our general understanding of how language and communication work within linguistics, psychology and child development 12 Some of our research studies • Assessing early language development: spoken and signed • Language, interaction and socio-cognitive development 13 Deaf children’s language development 14 Deafness and development • 90% of deaf children learn language in atypical ways • Many deaf children do well but language delay is still common • Even with early diagnoses and cochlear implants, language development is still an issue for deaf children • Emphasis on speech can lead to other areas being neglected 15 The advantages of bilingualism • Educators and policy makers have often suggested that knowing 2 languages is bad for a child’s academic and intellectual development • New insights: • Being bilingual can have a profound effect on your brain, improving cognitive skills not related to language and even shielding against dementia in old age • In a bilingual’s brain, both language systems are always active 16 • The bilingual experience improves executive function — a command system that directs the attention processes used for planning, solving problems and performing various other mentally demanding tasks • These include ignoring distractions to stay focussed, being able to switch attention, and holding complex information in mind • Bilingual people not only perform better on many cognitive tasks, but they also do so with less activity in parts of the brain involved in monitoring: their brains are more efficient 17 Assessing Language Correct score (%) 100 80 60 40 20 0 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 Age (years) 18 The MacArthur Bates CDI • to assess early child language. • parents indicate vocabulary production and comprehension by ticking items on a list of words grouped into categories such as ‘animals’, ‘toys’ and ‘actions’ • translated and adapted into many languages • Widely used in educational and clinical settings – Children with autism, developmental language disorders – deaf children (including those with cochlear implants 19 The BSL-CDI: 569 items in 22 categories • The entire checklist is available in BSL on the project website – to clarify the intended meaning of the English words used on the checklist – to give examples of the BSL signs • The list has also since been widely used by hearing parents, teachers and therapists http://www.ucl.ac.uk/HCS/research/EBSLD/ 20 Sample of the BSL CDI website 21 Sample checklist category: People PEOPLE (U – UNDERSTANDS, S = SIGNS) U S U S U S 1. aunt 12. doctor 23. people 2. baby 13. fireman 24. people 3. babysitter 14. friend 25. pet’s name 4. babysitter’s name 15. girl 26. police 5. boy 16. grandma 27. Father Christmas 6. brother 17. grandpa 28. sister 7. child’s own name 18. Indian 29. teacher 8. child/kid 19. postman 30. uncle 9. clown 20. man 31. woman 10. cowboy 21. mummy 11. daddy 22. nurse Other words 22 Positive Support Project • Parent-led monitoring of key outcomes for deaf children in the first years of life • Relating these to type and extent of specific interventions • Key outcomes – Language – Communication – Social behaviour – Family functioning – Motor and physical development 23 Use with the Positive Support Sample • English language (Oxford CDI standardised on British English-speaking children) • BSL CDI • One or both completed by parents when children reached 24m or at end of project (if child less than 24m). Repeat data 12 months later 24 Current analyses: bilingual DCHP • Comparing language development in English and BSL • Effect of degree of hearing loss on BSL and English language development • Comparing vocabularies in English and BSL 25 Mean age (months) % Moderate-severely deaf % Severely deaf % Profoundly deaf Bilingual (N = 16) 18-24 23.2 29 21 43 7 Monolingual English (N = 13) 14-24 20.6 53 14 29 4 % Moderately deaf Age (months) Sample Groups. 26 English and BSL in bilingual DCHP at 24m 300 250 200 UK MacArthur-Bates CDI 150 BSL CDI 100 50 0 Comprehension Production No significant differences between English and BSL skills in either Comprehension or Production 27 Bilingualism and the cumulative lexicon CI and mother-child interaction • The hearing mother and implanted deaf child share a sensory world to a greater extent than a child with an (ineffective) HA. • However, this benefit may depend on previously developed effective vision and touch-based interaction • Currently, the dominant method of clinical preparation for CI in relation to communication skills focuses on ‘auditory-verbal’ preparation, and explicitly excludes non-auditory interactions • A focus on parent-child communication may have better cognitive outcomes 29 Socio-cognitive development • Many studies report delays in Theory of Mind understanding (from 2-3 years to lifelong) • This might be due to language demands of the tasks used but even nonverbal measures show difficulties • Why would deafness lead to ToM difficulties? 30 False Belief tests 31 Incorrect location Correct location True and false belief conditions Results - Cat and mouse 33 Results - Cat and mouse 34 Conversational study • 19 hearing children (9 female, mean age 28 months) • 20 deaf children (16 female, mean age 28 months) • All had hearing parents. 16 had CIs and 4 had hearing aids. Morgan, Meristo, Geraci, Mann, Iozzi, Hjelmquist, Surian, Siegal (2014) 35 Categories of talk 36 Mother’s talk to their children DoH HoH 20 18 proportion of utteran 16 14 12 10 8 6 4 2 0 desire emotion mod.assert think/know elab. links ch. Life Less connected and more failed conversation turns when children are deaf 37 Language delay and socio-cognitive development • Deaf children experience poorer early interaction involving connected conversation about the mind. • ToM delays appear early in spontaneousresponse measures • Delays in ToM are only overcome late • Evidence for a remaining atypical pattern 38 Implications for hearing parents of deaf children – and for professionals • Importance of early and sufficient exposure to language and communication – Role for signing at the pre-implant stage – Disadvantages of leaving sign language until after evidence of spoken language failing • Language mixing as normal bilingual behaviour • Helping families focus on communication – not speech 39 Thank you – and thank you, Sheila DCAL, University College London 49 Gordon Square, London WC1H 0PD www.dcal.ucl.ac.uk Email: dcal@ucl.ac.uk 40