Reviewed Article Folia Primatol 2004;75:295–316 DOI: 10.1159/000080208 Received: May 14, 2003 Accepted after revision: January 15, 2004 Nigerian Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes vellerosus) at Gashaka: Two Years of Habituation Efforts Volker Sommera Andrew Fowler a Jeremiah Adanub Isabelle Faucher a a Department of Anthropology, University College London, London, UK; Department of Forestry and Wildlife Management, Federal University of Technology, Yola, Nigeria b Key Words Nigerian chimpanzee ` Habituation ` Call pattern ` Party size Abstract Cross-population comparisons of chimpanzees can shed light on the pathways of hominid evolution. So far, no eco-ethological data exist for the recently recognized subspecies Pan troglodytes vellerosus. We report on the first 2 years of a new long-term study from what is perhaps their last remaining stronghold: the Gashaka Gumti National Park, Nigeria. The mosaic habitat (woodland, lowland and gallery forest) receives 1,826 mm rain/year, with 4–5 months being completely dry. Primates at Gashaka are not hunted, and the chimpanzees are therefore relatively tolerant of human observers. We focused on the GashakaKwano community, investing 3,000 h of patrols. A total of 95 sightings were achieved which lasted for an average of 27 min (range 1–190 min). Party size averaged 3.7 animals (range 1–17) but was, similarly to encounter length, susceptible to a wide range of methodological, social and ecological factors. The Kwano community comprises at least 35 members which occupy a home range of at least 26 km 2, yielding a density of 1.3/km2. The area represents the West African equivalent of a chimpanzee site similar to the forest-woodland habitat in which early humans might have evolved. Copyright © 2004 S. Karger AG, Basel Ó2004 S. Karger AG, Basel 0015–5713/04/0755–0295$21.00/0 Fax + 41 61 306 12 34 E-Mail karger@karger.ch www.karger.com Accessible online at: www.karger.com/fpr V. Sommer, Department of Anthropology University College London, Gower Street London WC1E 6BT (UK) Tel. +44 20 7679 7030, Fax +44 20 7679 7728 E-Mail v.sommer@ucl.ac.uk Introduction Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) have been studied across Africa since the 1960s [reviews in Goodall, 1986; Wrangham et al., 1994; Boesch and BoeschAchermann, 2000; Boesch et al., 2002]. Attempts to reconstruct the probable behavioural suite of the last common ancestor of apes and humans [Moore, 1996] found that both chimpanzee and human societies exhibit tool use, hunting, territorial aggression, war-like raiding and cultural differences which are inexplicable by ecological variables [Manson et al., 1991; McGrew, 1992; Whiten et al., 1999]. Chimpanzees [for the following, see reviews in Goodall, 1986; Boesch and Boesch-Achermann, 2000; Boesch et al., 2002] are agile arborealists, but can also knuckle-walk rapidly on the ground. Their omnivorous diet consists primarily of fruit and plant parts, but includes insects and various mammals such as monkeys, duikers and bush pigs. Chimpanzees live in large multi-male/multi-female groups, so-called communities, of typically 30–80 members. Chimpanzee social life is characterized by fusion-fission in that the larger community separates daily into smaller parties which travel, forage, rest and nest together. Parties communicate by vocalizing and drumming. Numerous ecological (e.g. food distribution, rainfall), social (e.g. oestrous females or infants present) and spatial factors (e.g. location within the home range) correlate with party size. Nevertheless, the underlying causes are often obscure, and party sizes vary considerably between sites. Understanding the flexibility in chimpanzee behaviour and ecology requires data from diverse ecological locations [Wrangham et al., 1994; Whiten et al., 1999]. However, relatively few long-term studies exist, and sites where chimpanzees are habituated to human observers are even rarer. Moreover, wild chimpanzees are increasingly driven into local extinction due to habitat destruction and hunting [Pearce and Ammann, 1995; Sommer and Ammann, 1998]. Recently, based on genetic analyses from hair samples, a fourth chimpanzee subspecies has been recognized: Pan troglodytes vellerosus, the Nigerian chimpanzee [Gonder et al., 1997], distributed between the confluence of the Benue and Niger in Nigeria and the Sanaga river in Cameroon. However, the apes have already been displaced from most of these areas by human activity. Detailed data on their behaviour or ecology are lacking. Several mostly unpublished reports describe distribution and nest-building activity in what is perhaps the last remaining stronghold of P. t. vellerosus, the Gashaka area in Nigeria [Dunn, 1993; Harcourt and Ellerton, 1995; Hogarth, 1997; Adanu, 1997, 1998; Foster, 1998; Lameed, 2002]. Our paper reports on the first 2 years (2000–2001) of a new long-term study at Gashaka, providing details about acoustic signals, party size and the progress of habituation efforts. This supplements knowledge about other subspecies and will hopefully improve the understanding of chimpanzee and human evolution. Material and Methods The Gashaka Primate Project The Gashaka Gumti National Park (GGNP) lies in Eastern Nigeria on the border with Cameroon (06°55’–08°13’ N and 11°13’–12°11’ E; fig. 1). GGNP was gazetted from two game reserves in 1991 and is, with about 6,500 km2, Nigeria’s largest national park (for the 296 Folia Primatol 2004;75:295–316 Sommer/Adanu/Faucher/Fowler Fig. 1. Location of GGNP/Nigeria (courtesy Y. Warren). following, see Dunn [1999]). Human population in the park has declined over the last decade, due to relocation and migration towards more easily accessible areas. The southern (Gashaka) sector of the park has rugged terrain from about 300 m up to 2,419 m at Chappal Waddi (Gangirwal), Nigeria’s highest mountain. It is an important water catchment area for the Benue river. Abundant rivers flow continuously, even throughout the markedly dry season. The park area around the village of Gashaka in southern Taraba state harbours a great diversity of wildlife, including 6 species of diurnal primates. It is probably the only region where a sizable population of P. t. vellerosus has a long-term prospect of survival. The Gashaka Primate Project was established in 1999 with the aims of (a) understanding how environmental factors influence the structure of primate societies and (b) contributing to primate conservation. The Gashaka Primate Project works with students and researchers from Africa and western countries and is logistically supported by GGNP and the Nigerian Conservation Foundation. A research station is available in Gashaka (elevation 320 m), a village of about 600 people on the edge of the park. The major study site is inside the park at Kwano (583 m; 07°19’ N, 11°35’ E), an abandoned settlement, 10.5 aerial km from Gashaka. A field station Nigerian Chimpanzees at Gashaka Folia Primatol 2004;75:295–316 297 Fig. 2. Climate during the 2000/2001 study period. Recorded by Nigerian Conservation Foundation weather station in Gashaka village (elevation 320 m). Humidity measured at 16.00 h. was erected at Kwano in 2001. An ancient footpath connects Gashaka with Kwano, leading to the highlands and on to Cameroon. The path (traffic approx. 1 person/h) was widened in 1999 so that vehicles can reach Kwano during the dry season. However, during rains, a walk of about 3 h provides the only access. Serti, the nearest town and site of GGNP headquarters, is 40 km away. Vehicles connect Serti and Gashaka during the dry season, but the wet season requires precarious river crossings with canoes. Conservation Status The Gashaka-Kwano area is not a pristine forest but experienced anthropogenic influence throughout centuries. Most notable is the deliberate yearly burning of grass (from December to January) by locals and park authorities which likely turned large parts of previous semi-deciduous forest into grassy woodland [Louppe et al., 1995]. Large-scale logging does not exist, although some timber and non-timber forest products are extracted. From time to time, Fulani pastoralists will graze cattle. All hunting is prohibited in GGNP although ungulates and pigs are illegally hunted. However, during 4 years of study at Kwano (1999–2003), we did not find any snare although they are occasionally encountered around Gashaka. One adult female chimpanzee is known to be missing a hand; she was probably accidentally caught in a snare. Monkeys or chimpanzees are not hunted in the Gashaka-Kwano region, because of religious taboos connected with Islam. Primates will not react discernibly if a gun is pointed at them, indicating that they have not been hunted for a long time. These taboos do not extend to crop-raiding baboons or tantalus monkeys near villages who are sometimes shot or poisoned [Dunn, 1993; pers. observation]. Primates are poached in other parts of the park – particularly near the Cameroonian border –, typically as part of a trade in bushmeat by non-indigenous hunters. Nevertheless, Gashaka-Kwano is an area with considerable conservation potential, particularly for non-human primates. Climate Weather data were recorded at Gashaka village. Average temperatures should be about 3°C less at Kwano, given the higher altitude. The climate fluctuates half-yearly between a dry and a wet season (fig. 2). Rains may start in mid March. The average number of rainy days per month during the heavy downpours from mid April to mid October was 18 298 Folia Primatol 2004;75:295–316 Sommer/Adanu/Faucher/Fowler Fig. 3. Woodland savannah and lowland tropical rain forest cover the undulating hills in the Gashaka-Kwano area (photo: V. Sommer). (range 11–22). The wettest day (August 2001) saw 144 mm rain. The yearly average reached 1,826 mm (year 2000: 1,875 mm during 127 rainy days; year 2001: 1,777 mm during 124 days). No rain falls during at least 4 months/year (November–February). The mean monthly humidity (at 16.00 h) had a minimum of 15% (February 2001) and a maximum of 78% (September 2001). The dry season coincides with the Harmattan period (November–March), when a dry dusty wind blows from the Sahara. The mean minimum temperature was 21°C, the coolest day 13°C (January 2001), the mean maximum temperature 33°C, and the hottest day 41°C (March 2000). Flora The Gashaka-Kwano area is located in the sub-Saharan Guinea zone, representing a mosaic of habitats. Montane forests, open (montane) grassland and swamps can be found outside the study area; the Kwano region itself is characterized by woodland, lowland and gallery forest (fig. 3) [for details, see Dunn, 1999; Chapman and Chapman, 2001]. Woodland – also known as Guinea savannah – is dominated by a ground cover of coarse grasses 2–3 m high (e.g. Andropogon spp., Hyparrhenia spp.). Trees are relatively small, dispersed and often fire-resistant, including Lophira lanceolata, Daniellia oliveri, Afzelia africana, Crossopteryx febrifuga, Piliostigma thonningii, Entada africana, Prosopis africana, Annona senegalensis, Combretum spp., Terminalia spp., Parkia biglobosa. Families most commonly found in the often stratified lowland forest include Leguminosae, Sterculiaceae, Apocynaceae and Moraceae. Emergents may grow up to 40 m high and include Khaya grandifolia, Milicia excelsa as well as trees with characteristic buttress roots such as Terminalia superba or Ceiba pentandra. At 15–35 m, species such as Trilepisium Nigerian Chimpanzees at Gashaka Folia Primatol 2004;75:295–316 299 madagascariense, Pseudospondias preussi and Afzelia spp. may form a closed canopy. Trees found in the next lower storey include Celtis zenkeri, Albizia spp., Erythrophleum suaveolens, Campylospermum spp. and Psydrax spp. Below grow small trees of 2–8 m and shrubs like Cnestis ferruginea, Connarus griffonianus, Psychotria peduncularis, Celtis philippensis, Campylospermum flavum, Psydrax subcordata, Monodora tenuifolia and Olax subscorpoidea. The forest floor may be covered by a herbaceous layer and mosses, ferns and orchids. Lianas and epiphytes are abundant throughout the forest. Gallery forest along muddy stream banks includes Pandanus candelabrum and Costus spp., while those along rivers and streams contain Brachystegia eurycoma, Berlinia grandiflora, E. suaveolens, Blighia unijugata and Phoenix reclinata. Fauna The Gashaka-Kwano habitat supports a wide variety of large animals (table 1), particularly mammals, including 6 diurnal species of non-human primates. Potential predators of primates include leopards and monkey-hunting raptors such as the crowned eagle (Stephanoaetus coronatus). Ungulates are not abundant because of past rinderpest epidemics [Dunn, 1999] and poaching. Methodology Patrol teams traversed the Kwano area from 6.00 to 18.00 h to gather information about chimpanzees during 111 days in the year 2000 and during 247 days in the year 2001. Patrols covered 23/24 study months (table 2). The combined recording time for 2000 was 678 h, whereas 2,316 h were invested in 2001, yielding a total of 2,994 h. Patrols were scheduled to cover 12 h of a given day (6.00–6.59, 7.00–7.59,.... 16.00– 16.59, 17.00–17.59 h). We obtained records for 269 (97.4%) of the 23 × 12 = 276 individual hours of days. The number of patrols was 515. The number of persons in a given patrol – students, researchers, local field assistants, volunteers – could vary (1 = 19.6%, 2 = 53.8%, 3 = 18.1%, 4 = 6.4%, 5 = 1.2%, 6 = 0.6%, >6 = 0.4%). The patrols walked paths and specially cut trails or sat at elevated observation points. They recorded time and location of any chimpanzee vocalization (typically pant-hoots, barks or screams) and drumming of buttress roots or tree boles [Goodall, 1986]. The patrols used auditory contact or recently constructed chimpanzee nests to find the apes. They were instructed (a) not to approach further when they saw chimpanzees, (b) not to hide upon visual contact but to make their approach known to the apes, e.g. by talking in a quiet voice, and (c) to mock-chew vegetation once visible to the apes to convey nonaggressive intentions. The Gashaka-Kwano chimpanzees would often tolerate human observers and at other times retreat more or less quickly. The patrols remained near the chimpanzees until they changed location. To follow the apes in the rugged terrain was not normally possible. Researchers or labourers would also meet chimpanzees by coincidence, usually near the footpath. Information about these opportunistic encounters was included in the analysis. Acoustic signals (vocalization, drumming) were analysed in a one-zero fashion, employing 1-min intervals. Rates were calculated for each hour of the day for each given month. For example, a total of 1,680 min (28.0 h) of patrols fell into the period from 10.00 to 10.59 h in April 2001 when acoustic signals were recorded during 25 min, yielding a rate of 0.89/h. The mean hourly values were summed up and averaged, resulting in a mean hourly score per month. Acoustic signals are often repetitive, occurring in ‘bouts’. Such bouts are not clustered in an obviously discrete pattern, i.e. it is ambiguous when they begin and end. We defined bouts through a statistical criterion [Martin and Bateson, 1986]. An individual signal was defined as lasting as long as successive one-zero 1-min intervals had an entry. A bout criterion interval can then be derived from a frequency histogram of intervals between signals (no entry for up to 1 min, up to 2 min, 3 min etc.). From this, a graph is developed where the X-axis represents interval length (min) and a logged Y-axis the cumulative percent of individual intervals (1 min, 2 min, 3 min...). The resulting log survivorship curve is expected to 300 Folia Primatol 2004;75:295–316 Sommer/Adanu/Faucher/Fowler Table 1. Large animals in the Gashaka-Kwano chimpanzee home range region (2000–2001) Taxon Common name Latin name Hausa name Sightings1 Primates Chimpanzee Pan troglodytes vellerosus Papio anubis Cercopithecus nictitans Biri mai ganga see text Gogo Bakinbiri C C Gimchiki Kirikaa Biri mai roro Pangolins Rodents Carnivores Baboon Putty-nosed monkey Mona monkey Tantalus monkey Guereza Galago Tree pangolin Derby's flying squirrel Gambian sun squirrel Brush-tailed porcupine Crested porcupine Gambian giant rat Spot-necked otter African civet Leopard Golden cat Wild dog2 Lion2 Spotted hyaena Aardvarks Ungulates Reptiles Birds 2 Aardvark Rock hyrax Red river hog Warthog Cercopithecus mona Cercopithecus aethiops Colobus guereza Galago spp.? Phataginus tricuspis Anomalurus derbianus Dankunya Kurege C O C R R R Heliosciurus gambianus Kurege C Atherurus africanus Beguwa O Hystrix cristata Cricetomys gambianus Lutra maculicollis Civetticus civetta Panthera pardus Beguwa Gafiya Karenruwa Tunkun juda Damisa Felis aurata Lycaon pictus Panthera leo Kawondamisa Kerkeshi/ Karendaji Zaki O Probably present R O heard (R), scratch marks (R) R R Crocuta crocuta Kura Orycteropus afer Procavia ruficeps Potamochoerus porcus Phacochoerus africanus Giant forest hog Hylochoerus meinertzhageni African buffalo Syncerus caffer Bushbuck Tragelaphus scriptus Red-flanked duiker Cephalophus rufilatus Yellow-backed duiker Cephalophus rufilatus Waterbuck Kobus ellipsiprymnus Hartebeest Alcelaphus buselaphus Nile monitor lizard Varanus niloticus 2 Nile crocodile Crocodylus niloticus Many3 Dabganya Agwada Jan alde Alden daji/ Alhanzir Bakin alde R (seen at Yakuba, 15 km away) R (heard at Selbe, 15 km away) C (burrows) O O O O Bauna Mazo Makurna Boka Gwombaza Kanki O O O O O O Guza Kada R R 1 Actual sightings by fieldworkers only; C = common; O = occasional; R = rare. Not at Kwano but in surrounding area (<20 km distance). 3 More than 431 species identified in GGNP. 2 have two discernible parts: a rapidly declining portion, which reflects the short within-bout intervals, and a slowly declining portion – the tail of the curve – representing the long between-bout intervals. The bout criterion interval is estimated to be the point at which the slope changes most rapidly. With this, bouts of chimpanzee acoustic signals can theoretically be discerned. Nigerian Chimpanzees at Gashaka Folia Primatol 2004;75:295–316 301 Table 2. Patrols of fieldworkers in the Gashaka-Kwano chimpanzee home range Days with patrols Days with opportunistic records1 Year 2000 January February March April May June July August September October November December Subtotal 7 13 18 0 10 14 8 8 11 5 10 7 111 0 0 1 5 6 8 3 4 0 0 0 0 27 ,46 ,63 ,40 ,46 ,56 ,38 ,58 ,40 ,678 Year 2001 January February March April May June July August September October November December Subtotal Grand total 8 10 16 26 28 26 21 17 18 27 24 26 247 358 4 0 0 0 4 0 1 2 y1 1 0 2 15 42 ,87 ,88 ,92 ,265 ,293 ,290 ,265 ,200 ,151 ,183 ,204 ,200 2,316 2,994 1 Recording time, h ,70 ,116 ,106 Other fieldworkers, labourers, porters. Each sighting of chimpanzees was treated as an ‘encounter’, defined as lasting from when the first chimpanzee was visible until the last disappeared. For encounters of only a few seconds, a minimum length of 1 min was used. At least 3 h had to pass before a new party count would enter into the analysis. However, in reality, a given party was almost never found again during a given day once it went out of sight. The maximum number of chimpanzees visible during an encounter was recorded, as well as their major activity and the party’s age-sex class composition (adult male, AM; adult female, AF, with or without swelling; adult of unknown sex; juvenile of either sex; infant of either sex). It is assumed that the exact number of individuals could be determined in 78% of the encounters. In the remaining 22%, movements in the vegetation, calls or drumming indicated that more animals were present. In these cases, 1 additional individual was added to the number of visible animals. All counts were lumped for the analysis. Following conventions [Mitani et al., 2002], infants were excluded from calculations of party sizes, but juveniles more or less independent from their mothers were included. 302 Folia Primatol 2004;75:295–316 Sommer/Adanu/Faucher/Fowler Fig. 4. Home range of the Gashaka-Kwano chimpanzee community as inferred from acoustic signals (1,705 calls, drumming) and direct sightings (95 encounters) during 2001– 2002. Quadrate distribution not corrected for listening and observation times. A two-tailed non-parametric Kolmogorov-Smirnov test detected differences between context-specific samples of party size or encounter length. Correlations were subject to the Spearman rank correlation test. The level of significance was set at p < 0.05. Given that sample sizes were often small, we labelled values of p < 0.10 as ‘trend’ and of p < 0.20 as ‘tendency’. Results The current analysis is based on 1,705 acoustic signals (calls, drumming) and 95 direct sightings (encounters) of chimpanzees recorded in 2000–2001 (fig. 4). They occurred in 30 quadrates (grid size 934 m × 934 m) around the Kwano field station which correspond to 26.2 km2. Nigerian Chimpanzees at Gashaka Folia Primatol 2004;75:295–316 303 Fig. 5. Diurnal distribution of acoustic signals (based on 1,577 calls and drumming). Hour of day: 6 = 6.00–6.59 h, 7 = 7.00–7.59 h, etc. Fig. 6. Annual distribution of acoustic signals (April 2000 = no data). 304 Folia Primatol 2004;75:295–316 Sommer/Adanu/Faucher/Fowler Fig. 7. Logarithmic plot of intervals between acoustic signals. WBI = Within-bout intervals; BBI = between-bout intervals; BCI = bout criterion interval. Acoustic Signals Records include 1,481 min in which only vocalizations were heard, 142 min with vocalizations + drumming and 82 min with only drumming. The proportions of vocalizations (87%), vocalizations + drumming (8%) and drumming (5%) were very similar between observation periods, indicating a fair degree of interobserver reliability (January–December 2000/January–June 2001/July–December 2001: vocalizations 90/87/84%; vocalizations + drumming 5/8/13%; drumming 4/6/4%). Chimpanzees were seen using feet and one or two hands to produce drum beats, focusing particularly on buttress roots. The number of beats per drumming, as recorded during 90 events, was 1 (3.3%), 2 (63.3%) and 3 (33.3%). Acoustic signals were heard throughout the day with clear peaks in the very early morning and evening (fig. 5). Chimpanzees call and drum throughout the year, but monthly rates vary dramatically (fig. 6). There is a tendency towards a negative correlation with monthly rainfall (rs = –0.301, n = 23, p = 0.163). We attempted to determine the length of bouts of acoustic signalling by calculating the log survivorship curve for intervals between signals, separately for 3 observation periods and averaged (fig. 7). However, none of the 4 series of cumulative proportions resulted in the expected initial rapid decline of the curve, as exemplified by the following values for the average: up to 1 min 100%, 2 min 91%, 3 min 82%, 4 min 74%, 5 min 67%, 6 min 61%, 7 min 58%, 8 min 55%, 9 min 51%, 10 min 48%,..., 15 min 36%, 20 min 28%,..., 30 min 17%,..., 40 min 12%,..., 50 min 8%,..., 60 min 5%. Thus, the log-transformed plot of these values with their Nigerian Chimpanzees at Gashaka Folia Primatol 2004;75:295–316 305 Fig. 8. Party size of Gashaka-Kwano chimpanzees (based on 95 encounters). rather steady decline did not indicate the expected breaking point between withinbout and between-bout intervals. Party Size and Composition Chimpanzees were seen 95 times during 23/24 months. In about half of all encounters only 1 or 2 individuals were recorded, while the maximum was 17 (fig. 8). The mean across all individual parties was 3.7, and across monthly averages it was 4.1. Different age/sex classes (AM, AF, juvenile, infant) were recognized to a varying degree. Individuals were aged a total of 303 times, yielding a party composition of 81% adults, 9% juveniles and 10% infants. To determine the adult sex ratio, we excluded the many records of encounters with adults of unknown sex. Sightings in which all adults could be sexed yielded an overall ratio of AM/AF of 0.9 (43/48). This includes cases in which only AM or AF were present as well as mixed parties with AM and AF. The ratio corresponds well with the figure of 0.8 for mixed parties only (10/13, derived from the combinations AM/AF = 1/1, 1/1, 1/2, 2/2, 1/2, 2/2, 2/3). Unisexual parties were recorded with the following frequencies: 1 AM (n = 5), 2 AM (n = 3); 1 AF (n = 2), 1 AF with 1 infant (n = 5), 1 AF with 1 juvenile and 1 infant (n = 1), 2 AF with 1 juvenile and 2 infants (n = 1), 3 AF with 2 infants (n = 1). A fine-tuned analysis revealed that party size was sensitive to methodological, temporal, spatial and social factors (for the following, see table 3). Party size had a trend to decline from the first (mean 5.6) to the second year of study (mean 3.1). Likewise, larger parties (mean 5.4) were significantly more tolerant of observers than smaller ones (mean 2.4). Parties tended to be larger, if infants 306 Folia Primatol 2004;75:295–316 Sommer/Adanu/Faucher/Fowler Table 3. Correlates of party size and encounter length in Gashaka-Kwano chimpanzees Party size, n Encounter length, min mean SE median SD min. max. n Overall (years 2000–2001) 3.7 0.4 2 3.5 1 17 95 Year 2000 Year 2001 5.6 3.1 0.9 0.3 4 2 4.6 2.7 1 1 16 17 25 70 test1 + (p = 0.079) Enforced as result of patrol Opportunistic Chimpanzees tolerant Chimpanzees nervous/flee Dry season (15 Oct–14 Apr) Wet season (15 Apr–14 Oct) mean SE median SD min. max. n 27 4 10 39 1 190 88 42 22 11 4 30 7 52 33 1 1 190 149 22 66 29 7 4 6 11 1 40 13 1 1 149 30 83 5 45 7 32 44 1 190 39 16 7 1 34 1 149 27 5.4 0.7 4 4.4 1 17 40 2.4 0.4 1.5 2.0 1 7 28 3.6 0.5 3 3.1 1 17 35 40 8 22 49 1 190 34 3.8 0.5 2 3.7 1 16 60 20 4 6 29 1 149 54 73 20 60 61 1 190 9 22 4 7 32 1 149 79 50 18 10 4 35 5 48 31 1 1 190 149 25 63 55 9 32 52 1 190 31 12 2 4 16 1 60 57 Estrous female(s) present Estrous female(s) absent 5.6 1.2 5 3.7 1 14 9 3.5 0.4 2 3.4 1 17 86 Infant(s) present Infant(s) absent 4.7 3.4 0.8 0.4 3 2 4.3 3.1 1 1 16 17 26 69 Party size below average Party size above average * (p = 0.046) + (p = 0.093) x (p = 0.141) Core area of range Periphery of range2 3.8 3.5 0.5 0.6 2 2.5 3.6 3.0 1 1 17 15 65 30 29 24 6 5 7 13 43 27 1 1 190 103 61 27 Daytime 6.00–8.59 h 9.00–11.59 h 12.00–14.59 h 15.00–17.59+ h 3.7 3.7 3.8 3.4 0.6 0.6 0.9 1.5 3 2 2 2 3.3 3.3 3.7 4.5 1 1 1 1 17 14 16 15 30 32 19 9 33 30 19 17 8 8 7 7 12 11 4 5 43 45 27 20 1 1 1 1 149 190 105 59 30 30 17 9 Travel3 Rest3 Forage3 5.1 4.4 3.4 0.8 1.1 0.5 4 4 3 4.5 4.1 2.4 1 1 1 16 17 9 29 15 25 34 27 33 10 8 7 14.5 20 22 49 30 36 1 1 1 190 95 128 26 15 25 Nest group size (May 01–Jan 02)4 5.7 0.8 5 5.2 1 23 38 1 Kolmogorov-Smirnov test; * p < 0.05, significant; + = trend (p < 0.10); x = tendency (p < 0.20). Periphery = outermost quadrates in figure 4 with entries of calls, drumming and/or encounters. 3 Activity at onset of encounter. 4 A. Fowler, unpubl. data. 2 Nigerian Chimpanzees at Gashaka Folia Primatol 2004;75:295–316 307 test1 x (p = 0.109) x (p = 0.151) * (p = 0.017) x (p = 0.117) * (p = 0.030) * (p = 0.014) * (p = 0.014) Fig. 9. Annual distribution of party size (August 2000, November 2000 = no data). were present (mean 4.7) than if infants were absent (mean 3.4), and those including females with anogenital swellings (mean 5.6) had a tendency to be larger than parties without (mean 3.8). A higher proportion of dry season parties contained swollen females (7/39 = 18%) than wet season parties (2/56 = 4%). Nevertheless, monthly sizes fluctuated little since 64% of all months had means of 2–4 (fig. 9). Consequently, party sizes were remarkably similar during the dry (mean 3.6) and wet seasons (mean 3.8). Similarly, there was no discernible influence of monthly rain on party size (rs = 0.114, n = 22, p = 0.614) or of monthly party size on hourly rates of acoustic signals (rs = –0.041, n = 21, p = 0.860). Party sizes recorded in the core area (mean 3.8) differed also little from those in the range periphery (mean 3.5). Sizes did likewise not fluctuate much throughout the day (mean 3.4–3.8) or in relation to activities such as travel (mean 5.1), resting (mean 4.4) or foraging (mean 3.4). Encounter Length Sightings lasted on average 27 min with a maximum of 190 min (for the following, see table 3). Forced encounters – i.e. those, where a patrol team actively located chimpanzees by e. g. following their calls – lasted on average 29 min. Opportunistic encounters, e.g. by personnel carrying out other research, tended to be much shorter (mean 7 min). Not surprisingly, tolerated encounters were significantly longer (mean 45 min) than those terminated by flight (mean 17 min). Interestingly, encounters also had a tendency to last longer if more humans were included in a patrol team (1 person = 21 min; 2 = 23 min; 3 = 40 min; 4+ = 28 min; rs = 0.800, n = 4, p = 0.200). 308 Folia Primatol 2004;75:295–316 Sommer/Adanu/Faucher/Fowler Fig. 10. Length of encounters as a function of party size. Fig. 11. The duration and frequency of chimpanzee encounters as a function of cumulative patrol time. Nigerian Chimpanzees at Gashaka Folia Primatol 2004;75:295–316 309 As with party size, numerous other factors influenced encounters. Sightings tended to be longer during the dry (mean 40 min) than during the wet season (mean 20 min). Encounters lasted significantly longer if at least 1 infant was present (mean 50 min) compared to parties without infants (mean 18 min). Encounters with parties that included swollen females (mean 73 min) were also more extended compared to situations when swollen females were absent (mean 22 min). Moreover, we saw larger than average parties significantly longer (mean 55 min) than below-average parties (mean 12 min). This is also reflected by the highly significant positive correlation between length of sightings and increasing party size (fig. 10). Although encounter length was virtually halved as the day progressed, from 33 min in the mornings to 17 min in the late afternoon, this did not reach statistical significance. Likewise, there was little difference between parties at travel (mean 34 min), rest (mean 27 min) or foraging (mean 33 min). Spatial factors also had little influence given the small difference between the core area (mean 29 min) and the periphery (mean 24 min). Finally, we assessed if duration and frequency of sightings increased with patrol effort, i.e. whether or not a habituation effect could be detected. There was clearly no discernible correlation (fig. 11). Somewhat ironically, the longest sighting ever – 190 min – was the very first one, and it almost appeared as if the chimpanzees became less tolerant of observers after about 2,000 h of patrol given the long period with only relatively short encounters. Also, encounter frequency was more than double (n = 69) during the first 1,500 h of patrol efforts than during the second half (n = 26). Similarly, the ratio of tolerated encounters versus those terminated by flight decreased from 3.5 (14/4) in the year 2000 to 1.1 (26/24) in 2001. Consequently, encounters had a tendency to be twice as long in the first (mean 43 min) compared to the second study year (mean 22 min). The Kwano Community: Size and Population Density Chimpanzees were heard or seen and nests were found in about 26.2 km2 around the Kwano field station (fig. 4). Steep and barren hillsides which lead into open grassland to the west and north, the proximity of Gashaka village in the east, as well as the relatively broad Ngiti river in the south-east seem to represent (natural) barriers. Vocal exchanges between chimpanzees were heard across the banks of the Ngiti river. Thus, the area covered during the present study likely represents the core of what we believe is inhabited by a single chimpanzee group, named the ‘Kwano’ community. We estimated its minimum size via encounters and counts of nest groups [A. Fowler, unpubl.]. The maximum number of chimpanzees was recorded on January 27, 2001, when a group of schoolchildren and field assistants walking along an eastern trail were confronted with 17 apes who watched the humans from the ground and trees over a period of 95 min. Two other nearby patrol teams heard or saw three other parties estimated to comprise 4, 10 and 4 apes, thus totalling 35 animals. The largest nest group ever counted was 23 (July 2001). Given that about 20% of the study population were infants or juveniles who sleep with their mothers, we arrive at a size of 28 for this nesting party. Assuming that some members slept elsewhere, this supports a minimum community size of 35. 310 Folia Primatol 2004;75:295–316 Sommer/Adanu/Faucher/Fowler Both the area in which chimpanzees are found as well as the head count is likely to increase with increasing study time. However, the described barriers in the range may buffer our figures against underestimates of magnitude. Given that chimpanzees were sighted in 26 km2, we therefore calculate the population density as 1.3 apes/km2. Discussion Our study is the first to provide detailed eco-ethological information for the newly recognized chimpanzee subspecies P. t. vellerosus from what might be the only viable population. Vegetation and climate at Gashaka (fig. 2) is similar to mixed-vegetation Tanzanian sites (Gombe: 1,775 mm rain during 152 days, mean max. temp. 28°C, mean min. temp. 19°C; Mahale: 1,836 mm rain during 141 rainy days, mean max. temp. 27°C, mean min. temp. 19°C). The climate thus occupies a middle position between very dry (Assirik/Senegal: 954 mm rain) and wet sites (Boussou/Guinea: 2,230 mm [Hunt and McGrew, 2002]). Gashaka has a larger set of predators (leopard, lion, hyaena, wild dog) compared to most other chimpanzee study sites. The area therefore represents the West African equivalent of a chimpanzee site similar to the hypothesized forest-woodland habitat in which early humans lived [Hunt and McGrew, 2002]. Acoustic Signals At new sites – such as Gashaka – researchers rely on acoustic signals [Goodall, 1986] to locate the apes [Hunt and McGrew, 2002]. Chimpanzees vocally express emotions and convey information, e.g. about food sources and danger. Drumming against tree buttresses – the most prominent non-vocal sound signal – earned Nigerian chimpanzees the Hausa name ‘biri mai ganga’ (‘monkey with the drum’). Loud calls, particularly pant-hoots, are often given when parties meet during nesting and at the beginning of the day [Goodall, 1986; Hunt and McGrew, 2002]. The evening peak (fig. 5) might therefore reflect the joining of smaller day parties (mean 3.7) for night nest groups (mean 5.7). The early morning peak seems to reflect decisions about party composition and travel directions. Calls and drumming tended to decrease with increasing rainfall. Denser vegetation which muffles sounds as well as observer fatigue may have led to underreporting during rains, particularly because party sizes hardly differed between the dry and wet season (table 3) and because rates of acoustic signals did not correlate with party size. We were unable to assess the expected breaking point between within-bout and between-bout intervals of acoustic signalling because the log-transformed graph declined monotonously (fig. 7). This is probably because we could not normally distinguish signals from different parties – i.e. discern intra-party and distance signals – particularly when heard from afar. Thus, the signal from a second party would often fill in and mask the between-bout interval of a first party. As a result, we are unable to determine a bout criterion interval. Short of being able to identify individual callers [Boesch and Boesch-Achermann, 2000], observers would Nigerian Chimpanzees at Gashaka Folia Primatol 2004;75:295–316 311 have to travel continuously with the chimpanzees to at least reliably record the signals of individual parties. Party Size Overall party size varied greatly with, however, a skew toward the right (fig. 8). Consequently, the median (2) was smaller than the mean (3.7) – a result in line with other studies [Mitani et al., 2002]. The average of the monthly mean party sizes was greater (4.1) and nest group size even larger (mean 5.7). Thus, comparisons with other sites are problematic, particularly since methodological, ecological, social and spatial factors cause further fluctuations (see below). Nevertheless, it is known that the smaller the community, the larger the relative party size (i.e. mean party size/community size × 100 [Boesch and BoeschAchermann, 2000]). If we assume a size of 35 for the Gashaka-Kwano community, party sizes of 3.7, 4.1 and 5.7 translate into relative sizes of 11, 12 and 16%, respectively. These values are well within the observed cross-population range and e.g. close to Budongo/Uganda (12%) or Taï/Ivory Coast (13%). Thus, our data might confirm that smaller communities are socially less fluid, indicating that not only food competition influences fission-fusion, but perhaps also predation pressure [Boesch and Boesch-Achermann, 2000], which could be considerable at Gashaka (table 1). Our data on party composition are scarce since many parties had unknown composition (see Lanjouw [2002] for a similar problem). The overall socionomic sex ratio (AM/AF = 0.90) is within the variation at other sites [Boesch et al., 2002]. However, juveniles and infants at Gashaka make up only 19% of the community, half the proportion for e.g. Kanyawara/Uganda (63% adults, 13% juveniles, 24% infants [Wrangham, 2002]). Probably, as elsewhere during initial study years (e.g., Taï/Ivory Coast [Boesch and Boesch-Achermann, 2000]), lactating females were particularly wary of observers, leading to underreporting. Indeed, females with infants tended to be found in larger than average parties which were more tolerant of humans (table 3). Potential underreporting not withstanding, lactating females still represent the highest proportion of unisexual parties at Gashaka, perhaps because scramble competition restricts their travel and sociability (see Williams et al. [2002] for Gombe/Tanzania). Party size had a trend to increase when estrous females were present, and encounters lasted significantly longer (table 3), in line with many other reports [Hashimoto et al., 2001; Anderson et al., 2002; Matsumoto-Oda, 2002; Mitani et al., 2002; Wrangham, 2002; Wallis, 2002]. It is typically hypothesized that party sizes increase because males seek sexually receptive females or because such females seek copulations [Wallis, 2002; Wrangham, 2002]. A tendency for cycle onset during the dry season is reported from Budongo/Uganda and Gombe/Tanzania, probably as a result of feeding conditions which stimulate hormonal activities [Wallis, 2002]. The Gashaka data support this, since 18% of dry season parties but only 4% of wet season parties contained swollen females. Mean party size, however, did not differ between the wet and dry seasons (table 3), and monthly rainfall and party size were not correlated. It is often assumed that rain stimulates plant productivity so that rain can be used as a proxy for food availability (review in Mitani et al. [2002]). Nevertheless, while the dry season may correspond to food scarcity (Taï/Ivory Coast [Doran, 1997]), this is not 312 Folia Primatol 2004;75:295–316 Sommer/Adanu/Faucher/Fowler necessarily the case (e.g. Mitani et al. [2002] for Ngogo/Uganda). Moreover, party size may increase with fruit abundance at some sites [Mitani et al., 2002] but not at others (Kalinzu/Uganda) [Hashimoto et al., 2001]. We were unable to replicate findings for Taï/Ivory Coast [Anderson et al., 2002] that parties were smaller (a) while travelling, (b) during mid-day and (c) in the range core where aggressive territorial encounters are less likely (table 3). Admittedly, our data are of lower quality. On the other hand, the Gashaka-Kwano community might have immediate neighbours only in the south-west of their range where the forest is continuous. Here, a hostile interaction between males was indeed observed during 2002 [A. Fowler, pers. observation]. Encounter Length Various socio-ecological and methodological factors influenced the durations of chimpanzee sightings (table 3). For example, opportunistic encounters tended to be shorter than those resulting from dedicated patrols, most likely because opportunistic observers had no particular reason to stay with or follow the chimpanzees they had encountered by chance. Somewhat surprisingly, encounters were extended if observation teams were larger. Chimpanzees will at times watch travellers on the footpath from trees in the vicinity. Thus, relatively noisy groups of humans are probably associated with less danger than single observers who may be perceived as trying to furtively approach. Clearly, encounter length depended on how relaxed the chimpanzees remained. Flight was particularly likely if observers came across chimpanzees unexpectedly and thus without warning. Sightings tended to be shorter during the rains, perhaps because it is much more difficult to see or follow chimpanzees in dense and wet vegetation, often with heavy downpours. Moreover, due to reduced visibility, observers can inadvertedly startle chimpanzees, causing them to flee. Encounters lasted significantly longer if parties contained infants or swollen females. Such parties were also larger, which seems to increase tolerance to observers since encounters with larger than average parties lasted significantly longer and the length of sightings increased with party size (fig. 10). Habituation Efforts Chimpanzees at Gashaka are not extremely wary towards observers because they are used to see humans walking on foot through their habitat. Nevertheless, party size had a trend to be smaller during our second study year (table 3). One might conclude that the chimpanzees became less tolerant of researchers. However, it is also possible that smaller parties later on in the study allowed observers to approach more often. In addition, observers probably became increasingly better in tracking down smaller groups which are harder to locate. Similarly, both frequency and length of encounters decreased markedly over 2 years (fig. 11, table 3). However, instead of concluding that our research did ‘dehabituate’ the chimpanzees, one has to consider that, over the course of the study, observers patrolled wider and wider areas in more rugged terrain. Consequently, the success of seeing and remaining with chimpanzees decreased. Finally, chimpanzees regularly passed through camp in the beginning of the study but ceased doing so later on, similar to bonobos at Lomako/DRC [G. Hohmann, pers. commun.]. Nigerian Chimpanzees at Gashaka Folia Primatol 2004;75:295–316 313 On average, a single sighting was achieved after 31.5 h (2.6 days). Assuming a community size of at least 35 and a mean party size (including infants) of 4.0 individuals, it takes 8.8 sightings until all members encounter an observer. Thus, the average interval between the sightings of an individual chimpanzee is currently 22.8 days. Clearly, we will have to improve rates of sightings and increase the degree of habituation. Only a fraction of the large home range could be monitored during a given day. Chimpanzee calls do not travel across several hillsides that dissect the range and we did therefore sometimes not hear calls for several successive days. Moreover, patrols could not alert each other to the presence of chimpanzees since hand-held radios did not carry signals across hilltops either. A recently erected transmitter station does now allow us to place radioequipped patrols on elevated observation points who report calls back to the field station. Researchers then target specific segments of the home range instead of conducting more or less random searches. We are confident that this strategy will increase the rate and duration of chimpanzee sightings considerably. Population Density Our research indicates that the Gashaka-Kwano chimpanzee community comprises at least 35 members ranging over at least 26 km2 which translates into a population density of 1.3 apes/km2. If these are close to the actual figures, then the Gashaka chimpanzees would have a relatively small community in a relatively large range, compared to Tanzanian chimpanzees which live in similar climate and habitat: Gombe, 40–45 animals (Kasakela), range 4–24 km2; Mahale, 83 animals (M group), 7–14 km2 [Hunt and McGrew, 2002]. However, while our data are preliminary, it is conceivable that food supplies are more scarce at Gashaka. Previous census work in Gashaka Gumti National Park generated variable figures for chimpanzees per square kilometre (Dunn [1993]: 1.1; Hogarth [1997]: 0.75; Foster [1998]: 0.24–1.12; Adanu [1998]: 0.2–2.1; Lameed [2002]: 2.02). The divergence reflects different techniques of nest-counting (straight line vs. nonrandom transects) and surveyed habitats (lowland forest, mountainous forest, woodland). These densities translate to a total of 900–1,700 weaned chimpanzees in GGNP. Such figures depend on assessments of suitable habitat within the park. This is a difficult task, given the mosaic nature of plant cover and lack of updated vegetation maps based on remote sensing. Nevertheless, GGNP and the GashakaKwano area in particular are certainly premier sites which could ensure the survival of P. t. vellerosus in the wild. Outlook Forthcoming publications will focus on the plant composition of the Kwano forests, feeding ecology, nesting pattern and material culture of the GashakaKwano chimpanzees (tools, potential use of medicinal plants [McGrew, 1992; Huffmann, 1997]). Detailed behavioural studies have to rely on more direct observations, but we are hopeful that the degree of chimpanzee habituation will increase in the future. Long-term research at Gashaka will improve the understanding of the ecological and cultural flexibility of chimpanzees [McGrew et al., 1996; Whiten et al., 1999], given that very little is known about P. t. vellerosus. Moreover, studies at a 314 Folia Primatol 2004;75:295–316 Sommer/Adanu/Faucher/Fowler West African mosaic chimpanzee habitat should broaden our knowledge about the conditions under which Mio-Pliocene hominids lived [Hunt and McGrew, 2002]. Acknowledgment The Nigeria National Park Service kindly granted a research permit to the Gashaka Primate Project. Particularly helpful in the development of the project was the advice of Andrew Dunn and Richard Barnwell. Gashaka Gumti National Park, the Nigerian Conservation Foundation and WWF-UK provided vital logistical support. The fieldwork was sponsored by generous conservation grants from the North of England Zoological Society/ Chester Zoo, as well as by the Leakey Foundation/San Francisco and Leventis Ltd. & ProNatura International/Lagos. Additional support came from the Leakey Fund/London (to A.F.), the Dean’s Travel fund of UCL (to V.S.) and Primate Conservation Inc. (to J.A.). Claire Bracebridge, Julia Greenslade, Asako Saegusa, Judith Bovensiepen, Jennifer Rogan, Maurice Melle Ekane, Ymke Warren, Umaru Buba and Yakubu Wakirwa volunteered in data collection. The work would have been impossible without local field assistants, in particular Hammaunde Guruza, Salamu Waziri, Buba Bello, Bobbo Buba and Ali Tappare. We thank William McGrew and two anonymous reviewers for helpful comments. References Adanu J (1997). Determination of Life-Span of Chimpanzee Nests (Nest-Aging) in Gashaka-Gumti National Park. Project report. Gashaka, Nigerian Conservation Foundation/WWF-UK. Adanu J (1998). Distribution and Abundance of Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) in Gashaka Gumti National Park, Nigeria. UG dissertation, Yola. Anderson DP, Nordheim EV, Boesch C, Moermond TC (2002). Factors influencing fission-fusion grouping in chimpanzees in the Taï National Park, Côte d’Ivoire. In Behavioural Diversity in Chimpanzees and Bonobos (Boesch C, Hohmann G, Marchant LF, eds.), pp 90–101. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press. Boesch C, Boesch-Achermann H (2000). The Chimpanzees of the Taï Forest: Behavioural Ecology and Evolution. Oxford, Oxford University Press. Boesch C, Hohmann G, Marchant LF (eds.) (2002). Behavioural Diversity in Chimpanzees and Bonobos. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press. Chapman JD, Chapman HM (2001). The Forests of Taraba and Adamawa States, Nigeria: An Ecological Account and Plant Species Checklist. Canterbury, University of Canterbury/NZ, WWF/DFID. Doran D (1997). Influence of seasonality on activity patterns, feeding behaviour, ranging and grouping patterns in Taï chimpanzees. International Journal of Primatology 8: 18–206. Dunn A (1993). The Large Mammals of Gashaka-Gumti National Park, Nigeria: Line Transect Surveys from Forest and Savannah. Project report. Gashaka, Federal Ministry of Agriculture, Nigeria, Nigerian Conservation Foundation/WWF-UK. Dunn A (1999). Gashaka Gumti National Park: A Guidebook. Lagos, Gashaka Gumti National Park, NCF/WWF-UK. Foster KL (1998). Censusing Chimpanzees in Gashaka-Gumti National Park, Taraba and Adamawa States, Nigeria. Project report. Gashaka, Federal Ministry of Agriculture, Water Resources and Rural Development, Nigeria, Nigerian Conservation Foundation/WWF-UK. Gonder K, Oates J, Disotell T, Forstner M, Morales J, Melnick D (1997). A new West African chimpanzee subspecies? Nature 388: 337. Goodall J (1986). The Chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of Behaviour. Cambridge, Harvard University Press. Harcourt CS, Ellerton NG (1995). A Brief Look at the Primates of Gashaka-Gumti National Park, Nigeria. Project report. Gashaka, Nigerian Conservation Foundation/WWF-UK. Hashimoto C, Furuichi T, Tashiro Y (2001). What factors affect the size of chimpanzee parties in the Kalinzu forest, Uganda? Examination of fruit abundance and number of estrous females. International Journal of Primatology 22: 947–959. Hogarth SA (1997). The Distribution and Abundance of the Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) of the Lowland Gallery Forest of Gashaka-Gumti National Park, Nigeria. Project report. Gashaka, Federal Ministry of Agriculture, Water Resources and Rural Development, Nigeria, Nigerian Conservation Foundation/WWF-UK. Nigerian Chimpanzees at Gashaka Folia Primatol 2004;75:295–316 315 Huffman MA (1997). Current evidence for self-medication in primates: A multidisciplinary perspective. Yearbook of Physical Anthropology 40: 171–200. Hunt KD, McGrew WC (2002). Chimpanzees in the dry habitats of Assirik, Senegal and Semliki Wildlife Reserve, Uganda. In Behavioural Diversity in Chimpanzees and Bonobos (Boesch C, Hohmann G, Marchant LF, eds.), pp 35–51. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press. Lameed GA (2002). Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) population density and structure in Gashaka Gumti National Park. ROAN, The Journal of Conservation 1: 88–93. Lanjouw A (2002). Behavioural adaptations to water scarcity in Tongo chimpanzees. In Behavioural Diversity in Chimpanzees and Bonobos (Boesch C, Hohmann G, Marchant LF, eds.), pp 52–60. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press. Louppe D, Oattara N, Coulibaly A (1995). The effects of brush fires on vegetation: The Audrville fire plots after 60 years. Commonwealth Forestry Review 74: 288–292. McGrew WC (1992). Chimpanzee Material Culture: Implications for Human Evolution. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press. McGrew WC, Marchant LF, Nishida T (eds) (1996). Great Ape Societies. New York, Cambridge University Press. Manson JH, Wrangham RW (1991). Intergroup aggression in chimpanzees and humans. Current Anthropology 32: 369–390. Martin P, Bateson P (1986). Measuring Behaviour. Cambridge, University of Cambridge Press. Matsumoto-Oda A (2002). Social relationships between cycling females and adult males in Mahale chimpanzees. In Behavioural Diversity in Chimpanzees and Bonobos (Boesch C, Hohmann G, Marchant LF, eds.), pp 168–180. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press. Mitani J, Watts DP, Lwanga JS (2002). Ecological and social correlates of chimpanzee party size and composition. In Behavioural Diversity in Chimpanzees and Bonobos (Boesch C, Hohmann G, Marchant LF, eds), pp 102–112. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press. Moore J (1996). Savanna chimpanzees, referential models and the last common ancestor. In Great Ape Societies (McGrew WC, Marchant LF, Nishida T, eds.), pp 275–292. New York, Cambridge University Press. Pearce J, Ammann K (1995). Slaughter of the Apes: How the Tropical Timber Industry Is Devouring Africa’s Great Apes. London, World Society for the Protection of Animals. Sommer V, Ammann K (1998). Die Grossen Menschenaffen: Orang-utan, Gorilla, Schimpanse, Bonobo. Munich, BLV. Wallis J (2002). Seasonal aspects of reproduction and sexual behavior in two chimpanzee populations: A comparison of Gombe (Tanzania) and Budongo (Uganda). In Behavioural Diversity in Chimpanzees and Bonobos (Boesch C, Hohmann G, Marchant LF, eds.), pp 181–191. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press. Whiten A, Goodall J, McGrew WC, Nishida T, Reynolds V, Sugiyama Y, Tutin CEG, Wrangham RW, Boesch C (1999). Cultures in chimpanzees. Nature 399: 682–685. Williams JM, Liu H-Y, Pusey AE (2002). Costs and benefits of grouping for female chimpanzees at Gombe. In Behavioural Diversity in Chimpanzees and Bonobos (Boesch C, Hohmann G, Marchant LF, eds.), pp 192–203. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press. Wrangham R (2002). The costs of sexual attraction: Is there a trade-off in female Pan between sex appeal and receiving coercion? In Behavioural Diversity in Chimpanzees and Bonobos (Boesch C, Hohmann G, Marchant LF, eds.), pp 204–215. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press. Wrangham RW, McGrew WC, de Waal FBM, Heltne PG (eds.) (1994). Chimpanzee Cultures. Cambridge, Harvard University Press. 316 Folia Primatol 2004;75:295–316 Sommer/Adanu/Faucher/Fowler