Chapter 8 Experiments & Experiences Torgeir Holen, Hovedfagsoppgave, Bergen, 1998 8.1 The Negative Experiences The negative experiences is a mostly neglected field in biochemistry laboratories. The negative data are seldom reported in scientific journals (because of extreme shortage of space) or even in this kind of novice thesis (because of the assumed lack of professional style). This practice, I believe, results in novices and professionals alike having to deal with their mistakes with no guidance from any theoretical framework. To clarify matters, I do recognize that there exists a practical framework to convey these negative experiences, for example by lab courses and consultations with tutors during the laboratory work. As I understand this chapter is somewhat unusual, I will in the following paragraphs try to illuminate my point of view, before I proceed to some of the experiences from my experiments, as opposed to the results of the same experiments. Thereafter I will discuss different ways to implement reports of negative experiences in laboratories. 8.2 Negative, but Necessary The negative experiences should not here be confused with some kind of bad experience. We are talking about a state of confusion in connection with failed experiments that is very familiar to everyone working in laboratories. Everyone speaks about the problems one faces in an unfamiliar situation, but there seems to be no theorization around the phenomenon. Now, some even claim that this kind of state is beneficiary because the confused ideas might lead to new ideas, where an exact clarity is sterile for further insight. I agree it is necessary for everyone to face this confusion during one’s training to gain the insight of how to gain the information and clarity that is desired, but when one has been a bit battered, so to speak, there should be a theoretical framework to be consulted. It is said that cleverness is to learn from one’s own mistakes, but true intelligence is to learn from others’. Then the information about others’ failures must be available. 8.3 The Oral Tradition There is no organized theoretical framework for these negative experiences that I am aware of. That is not to say that the knowledge about the negative experiences is not existing or even unavailable. Indirectly, the experiences are included in the extensive method literature in molecular biology, but the statements only state what works. The oral tradition in a lab, or a scientific community, contains these negative experiences and there is much to be learned from consultation with colleagues. Still it is not structured or organized in any theoretical way, except maybe in old peoples biographies an in tutors’ internal discussions, and as such the access to the knowledge therefore is random and limited. Also, the knowledge in an oral tradition is very easily lost or distorted. This is a very familiar fact to people working in the framework of any organization. It is probably no coincidence that basic protocols vary so much between labs and that even high-tech scientists can be superstitious. 8.4 The Question Now assume that these negative experiences exist and are important to the everyday life in the laboratory, and therefore should be theorized about. This is not an unproblematic assumption as some will claim that everybody has to learn constantly. We are learning creatures and we all meet new situations every day. Some will even say that writing about these experiences would be stating something trivial. To this is to say firstly, that the Emperor might in fact have no clothes, so learning about learning might be useful, in my opinion valuable experiences are daily lost because there are no mechanisms of preservation. Secondly, I am not writing a sterile, metaphysical treatise on learning in general, but on how to master a biochemical laboratory situation as quickly and painlessly as possible. If we assume that these negative experiences are factual, it is interesting to note that older colleagues seem to master a new situation faster than novices. My question is why this is so. 8.5 The Master’s Experience The obvious, and therefore mistrusted, answer should be that these elders are more experienced. Yes, but what is the nature of these experiences and how are they generally acquired? Another explanation is that these are the people that are talented in this way of making a living and as Masters of their trade they are naturally better at their work than the through-going masses of novices. The ones that survive the lab-environment will tend to stay and replace the old Masters. This Unnatural Selection, so to speak, is an attractive mechanism and is probably part of the answer, but it avoids answering the real question. What kind of acquired or innate talent made these Masters better than the average novice? The third explanation is that these Masters have read and understood the theoretical biochemical framework better than the average novice, and therefore is superior in the lab. This explanation is missing my point entirely, as theoretical knowledge as opposed to practical lab-knowledge are not necessarily overlapping sets. I have no direct answers to the main question, but a suspicion that it is a mindset, consisting of simple rules of thumb and recognition of certain situations, a recognition that is acquired through many years of working in a lab. These simple rules and general observations should be written down and be published for the greater good. Then again, it is possible that the rules for describing a general, fruitful approach to laboratory life might become so general as to be self-defeating. This is discussed in the implementation part below. 8.6 This is not Science, is it? I think most people would at this stage agree that there might be some philosophical or sociological interest to these questions, but object that it is too vague and distant to have anything to do with practical lab work for a novice like me. Furthermore, that Science is something entirely different, and this chapter should not be in a thesis on molecular biology. I beg to differ. Firstly, the everyday problems faced by a novice is not really about hypothesis testing, it is not about whether a protein interact with DNA, and certainly not whether the latest articles in our field have some relevant new discovery. These are all very interesting theoretical problems, but the novice just want to know why this protocol does not work, and has not worked for weeks. This point has been fiercely disputed by my tutors, so I will try to clarify matters. Rein Aasland and Litta Olsen, in their text “Experimental Design” (Rein Aasland, pers. com.) model every activity in the laboratory as a micro hypothesis – an element of a larger program – to reach the position where major hypotheses can be tested. This can be viewed as some sort of Popperian position where science is modeled exclusively as hypothesis testing. I do not dispute that it is possible to view science in this light. I even tend to agree that it is desirable to do science this way. But I refuse to believe that this is the whole story. Thomas Kuhn initiated the understanding that many activities in science are too mundane to be considered hypothesis testing. I find the view that much of science is problem solving (or puzzle solving) very attractive, and my view is that this is not in direct conflict with the thesis that a central point of science is hypothesis testing. Aasland & Olsen’s position is normative, mine is descriptive. Usually the novice has done some faulty assumption, and it is the ability to make the correct assumptions that is my central problem, and not the debugging of the protocol per se. We do not have time to debug all the procedures and protocols all the time. The Masters have somehow learned to avoid the major problems, while the novices are spending huge parts of their time debugging apparently simple protocols or just keep on pounding their heads against the wall repeating the experiments. The novices, and maybe the ordinary scientist too, need some rules of thumb to recognize the kind of trouble they are in and consult the right sources, something that the Masters probably do quite subconsciously after years of practice. The second objection, that these problems are not Science (with a capical S) is harder to deal with. Of course one could ask what science really is, but then again one would have to deal with Popper, Kuhn, Feyerabend, Latour and Merton, and very few of the world’s hundreds of thousands of scientists do. They just know what Science is, because they have been working in a laboratory and been reading Science and Nature for quite a while. There has been no adequate general description of science. The theories from philosophers of science are generally found too simple or distorted as seen by the scientists themselves (Harald Trefall, lecture 1993). More recently and specifically, there has until now been no working philosophy of biochemistry (or molecular biology), and least not in the biochemistry itself (Roger Strand, 1997). So it might be that this is not science, but I believe that these will make my experiments more reproducible. This point alone probably justifies these considerations in a molecular biology thesis as some kind of methodological discussion. I believe that true science is deeply immersed in these practical problems all the time. I believe that is important for my lab, my fellow future students and for my tutors to read about some of the experiences that I have gained in the lab that are not producible in the table or figure of the usual experiments. 8.7 The assumptions I have noticed that it is hard for skillful people to truly comprehend what novices in their field are facing. The skillful have problems understanding why the central questions are not formulated. The skillful are often not aware of the basic assumptions that have been transcended on their behalf, like learning to ride a bicycle then never thinking of it again. But the basic assumptions are the basis of their very action. Of course there is an hermeneutical effect as one has to have some experience to gain an overview. Chess instructors, for example, never answer questions in the start. They just tell the novices to play. When some experience is gained, there is time to set these in perspective, a theoretical framework. Then further experiences can be organized and further insight gained more easily. In the laboratory there are so many assumptions as molecular biology delves deeper into the mysteries of the cells, and all cannot be thought about at the same time. In the laboratory, as in other walks of life, some experiences should be done alone, but then one should consult some theoretical framework. I will in the continuation treat three different episodes in my thesis work as examples in the light of our discussion of subconscious assumptions. 8.8 Examples Here follows three examples from my own experience with my thesis work, and what I think I learnt from them. I think these experiences are soon forgotten and replaced by other, revised memories (see section 8.5) so it is important to preserve them in this detailed, original form. 8.8.1 Example One: The First Restriction Experiment My first (very simple) autonomous experiment was to establish the size of the insert in the EST plasmid (see Chapter Two), and as I at that moment was very naive – tabula rasa in the ways of the lab – it might be a good introduction example. I was given instruction by my lab colleagues and also consulted my journals from my earlier lab courses. The techniques used were the SRS web system, which gives information about the EST plasmids from the databases, Promega tables of restriction enzymes and an agarose gel. The specific restriction performed is Fig.8.1. Restriction mapping of various not important per se. Just have a look at EST plasmids. the gel picture (Fig.8.1). Now I were to interpret this gel picture. What does the picture mean? At first I remember running into all kinds of problems, because there were so many bands. Especially the weak, lower bands were difficult to reconcile with some kind of cutting pattern. My brand new tutor Rein Aasland threw away all my calculations and immediately pointed out what bands were “real” and what bands were “ghost bands” or “artifacts”. The results were tabulated (see Table 2.1, Chapter 2). My first assumptions about bands being bands had been mistaken. This recognition of what is real and what is just artifacts is a typical skill the Masters have. Another assumption was that since according to the database-data the cDNA fragment were originally cut and cloned into the EST-plasmid with EcoRI and HindIII, there were not EcoRI sites in the fragment. This implies that the fragment coming out when cutting with these restriction enzymes is full-length, that is, that my tabulated 0.7 kb value (Table 2.1) was correct (section 2.1.1). Later I ran into problems, because the sequences pieces and sequences from the databases made up a map that was larger than 0.7 kb. The inconsistency between the size of the HuPcl1 cDNA and the EST-fasproject (see Chapter Two), remained not realized and not looked into for a long time, due to the idea that size estimation on a gel can be somewhat faulty. Maybe I should have realized the inconsistency earlier, but I think a more fundamental problem is at large here. My conception of a HuPcl1 cDNA of 0.7 kb was one of the ideas and concepts that are not daily criticized or thought of, but one which consciously or unconsciously, one uses or bases ones work on, with the accompanying faulty conclusions. The assumption is not questioned, because it can be defended by other assumptions, as here about the limits to fragment size estimation. Even this simple storytelling is becoming revisionistic as I also thought of the vector of HuPcl1 as pBS-II-KS(+) instead of pBS-II-SK(-) (identical, but with the MCS reversed). That is, the problems are multifactorial. Often, when none of several techniques are familiar, the mistakes made in one technique lead to problems in the next. This leads to some subtle inconsistencies that can be ignored for a long time. 8.8.2 Lesson 1 What do I feel that I learnt from this? First something about interpreting data: The gel, even one as simple as this, was very difficult to understand unless one used the “conceptglasses” that let us distinguish between “real” and “ghost” bands. Secondly, I see that my sequencing strategy was based upon a faulty assumption (the HuPcl1 fragment being 1000 bp, not 700 bp), which had long term implications for my thesis work. Thirdly, there was enough data available to solve the problem at the time. They were just not used (in fact I probably was unable to use them yet, and this efficient exploitation of all the available data is certainly one of the Masters’ virtues). The sequence from the ESTdatabase included the EcoRI site. I just had not cared to learn about the EGCC (see Methods 9.30) tools yet. The parallel gel show that the 0.7 kb fragment from the XhoI/EcoRI and EcoRI lane are the same size, that is, an indication of an EcoRI site, but this was not considered important. To cite Fleming on his discovery of the famous penicillin colony on the bacteria plate: “Before you can notice any strange happening you have to be a good workman, you have to be a master of your craft.” (Strand 1997, citing Ludovici, 1952). The conclusion must be that it is not enough to have the data available. One must also have training in interpreting them, and when to believe in them. The fourth and last point there is something about self-esteem. One reason the problems were not realized was because the inconsistencies (those that remained when one had taken into account the limits of the method) were explained as non-orderly conduct my myself. One is not so naive that one does not know one is a novice. Things do go wrong in the lab, and one blames oneself, with very weighty reasons of course. But I think that these phenomena, in company with the usual problems of working with biological problems, lead to obscuring of the central problems with the method. Novices make mistakes, but when the experiment has gone wrong several times, there are weighty reasons of suspecting something more. My tutor Rein Aasland’s advice is that if something does not work quite soon, one should introduce many controls and check “everything”. I would like to add...then you check the system itself and your own assumptions. Trust no one. Especially not yourself. 8.8.3. Example Two: The Probes To investigate the expression of HuPcl1 and HuPcl2 radioactive probes were prepared to be used on Håvard Valvatne’s Northern blots (see Chapter 4). Here first two RNA probes were tried, without any result. Then a hot DNA probe approach was tried. The first time there was only intense background signal (Fig.8.2). To illustrate that exposure to the film was just the final step in a long series of steps, I will describe them here: The plasmids were isolated – cut – fragments isolated – probe made – membranes incubated with probe – membranes washed – membranes exposed to film. This procedure could take 3-4 days (7 if Fig.8.2 A quite spotty-looking film. What has caused it? one include plasmid isolation and fragment preparation). It is hard to see where something goes wrong, especially if you are a novice. Also in this case I was working with radioactivity, which tends to make me nervous. Then I suddenly got a fine signal (see Chapter), but after this I was not able to repeat it. The same probe on mouse-Northern blots turned out blank. New probes made with HuPcl1 and then twice with HuPcl2 gave blank films, even after 4 days of exposure. The questions were many. Was the washing procedure too stringent? Was something wrong with the hybridizing buffers? Was there something wrong with the developing machine again? Did I forget to heat the probe in my nervousness in the isotope room? Was there something wrong with the DNA fragment? Was there something wrong with the enzyme that should make the probe? I found out – doing science on my lack of results, instead of on the biological problem, a common lab situation – that there was in fact no probe made, as most of the radioactivity stayed in solution, and did not precipitate out in ethanol. When I returned to the making of the HuPcl1 probe for use in the Lambda Phage System (see Chapter 5), the problems of the poor probes persisted. The Fig.8.3. Control gel for DNA problem was solved at a lunchtime chat, when Rune fragment before probe synthesis. In Male, and experienced colleague, pointed out that hindsight the band has much less my gelbands (the QIAGEX fragment control, see than 100 ng DNA. Fig.8.3) probably could not contain the 100 ng DNA I had estimated. Thus the synthesis of the probes over the last four months appeared to have been made difficult by a systematic mistake. The DNA concentration had been based on the absorbance of 260 nm UV-light. When compared to DNA concentration as estimated on the gel, there was an inconsistency of circa 3 to 10 times. When the fragment concentration was increased 15 times, the probe synthesis was much more efficient, as judged by the semi-quantitative ethanol precipitation method. The (main) problem had been that I had used absorbance at 260 nm to estimate the DNA concentration of my DNA preparations. This is an exact method as long as the DNA is pure. If not, it is not trustworthy. I knew this. I knew that a ratio of 1.83 (absorbance at 260 nm versus 280 nm) was ideal, but that for example 1.89 and 1.76 was all right too. My hypothesis at the time, because there is never time to solve any problem longer than to get the method to work, is that RNA and protein (which raise and lower the ratio respectively) cancel each other out and quite impure DNA can then have a ratio close to 1.83. * I had been using too little DNA by a factor of 5-10. But to estimate concentration on gel, one also needs some experience, but that is another story.** 8.8.4 Lesson 2 What can be learnt here? I had been working in the lab for 7-10 months when I had these problems. I had solved some major problems in sequencing and knew quite a bit about that method. But my experience was not generalized, something I felt so had when I entered the new field of making radioactive probes. This generalization-of-experience idea is also one motivation for this chapter. There must be some way to convey these experiences without having to use four months of your life and much research money. Firstly, I learnt that a DNA concentration is not necessarily a hard fact. It is a measurement-number that refers to a method. And sometimes different methods give different facts, something that should make one suspect foul play. Secondly, in hindsight, there were some hints in the laboratory practice that should have warned me. Litta Olsen, the experienced lab technician, never used UV spectroscopy, because she said she did not like the equipment (a Pharmacia Biotech, GeneQuant II RNA/DNA Calculator), while Håvard Valvatne, the lab’s PhD student, used it and liked it (he even calibrated it against pure oligonucleotide DNA). So in the lab there were in fact two schools of thought about this particular machine. Some hated it, but I liked the machine, so I never questioned the method behind my DNA estimations, especially since the method was well described in our Bible, that is Maniatis. Yet again, the assumptions were faulty. The problems different people encountered were explained by other phenomena, for example that the machine was not easy to use, so any inconsistencies were seen as mistakes or just a bad day for the machine. Thirdly, I would recommend using a positive control for the first runs of any system, even if it is a very system or a very expensive one. It is worth it in the long run. 8.8.5 Example Three: A plasmid was constructed... I wished to construct two pair of yeast plasmids (pGBT-HuPcl1, pGBT-HuPcl2, pGADHuPcl1, and pGAD-HuPcl2) to express the cDNA fragments (especially the PHD fingers) as hybrid proteins in the two-hybrid system, and thereby search for proteinprotein interactions (see Chapter 6). The construction of the two pair of plasmids turned into a very lengthy affair. The origin of the confusion was a combination of poor sequence data, difficult plasmids (pGBT9 and pGAD424), data errors and of course the learning process belonging to each battery of techniques that had to be mastered (agarose gels, QIAEX, restriction enzymes, Klenow, phosphatases, blun-end-ligation, electroporation, MiniPreps, GeneConstructionKit, sequencing etc...). In scientific literature one can frequently read, “A plasmid was constructed...”, if the procedure is mentioned at all. The use of the passive voice hides the fact that the plasmid must be made by someone. And that takes skill. When I constructed these two-hybrid constructs I ran into all kind of problems. This was blamed upon old and impure plasmidpreparations, and in turn five different preps were used (two 1 year old MaxiPreps, one MidiPrep, one MaxiPrep and one MiniPrep). Restriction analysis with enzymes unique to the MCS, like EcoRI and BamHI, which was essential to the cloning strategy, gave a fragment of 1000 bp where only the linearized vector was expected. This inconsistency can be caused by many things: The enzymes can be the wrong one (switched by mistake). The buffer can be old (leading to unspecific cutting). The cutting strategy can be wrong (one can overlook a restriction site). The maps (GCK-files) can be wrong. The incubation can be too long, again leading to unspecific cutting. The plasmids can be the wrong ones (we work with many plasmids). I concluded (or assumed) however, after repeatedly getting the same results with many different DNA-preps, that the 1000 bp band was caused by star activity of EcoRI or BamHI, caused by some impurity in the DNA prep. Even our purest and quite trusted MaxiPres had been thrown into doubt (we even speculated about trying out CsCl2 ultracentrifugation purification of DNA), because the some other problems with MaxiPreps earlier (too dense or too much DNA from MaxiPreps can inhibit restriction enzymes, and of course we had the fact from Example Two (section 8.8.3)), that our DNA in the lab was not wholly pure). But when different preps had been tried, and then plasmid had been fingerprinted, when all the enzymes, buffers and incubations steps had been changed, one suddenly realized (after the usual days of despair), that there was one assumption that had not been questioned, namely the Promega Catalogue enzyme preferences. When I correlated the Promega 1996 Catalogue and the New England Biolabs Catalogue on EcoRI, Promega insisted that EcoRI is a medium ionic strength enzyme (100 % activity in the Buffer Multicore, that is, 135 mM total salt, while in high salt (Buffer D, 150 mM NaCl) the activity was reported to 50-75 %), while New England Biolabs preferred high salt (160 mM total salt.) There must be a star-activity site in pGBT9 some 1000 bp from the MCS ***, which is recognized by EcoRI under sub-optimal conditions. The fault is of course not Promega’s, as there might be some difference between specificity and “percentage activity”. But where New England Biolabs mentions that EcoRI does have potential star activity on low ionic strength, Promega mentions that EcoRI has star activity in the absence of NaCl. This seems strange in the light of the MultiCore-buffer containing only 50 mM NaCl. When using high salt buffer (Promega buffer D), the problem disappeared. In addition there were thee usual problems, for example, that at cloning strategy turned out to be wrong (first discovered upon control sequencing afterwards), and that my pGBT-sequencing file contained a vital error. These unrelated problems of course add to the confused atmosphere when I was confronting the problem of the 1000-bp band. The recognition of the essential problem among dozens of factors is always harder there and then. In hind-sight, everything seems simple. 8.8.6 Lesson 3 A wise biochemist once said that two months in the laboratory often saves two hours in the library. This joke is not as paradoxical as one should think. Long days in the lab with endless protocols induce the wish to cut down on at least some of the steps. If can save five minutes on each centrifuge step, days will be saved over a whole year. And this is also a sound strategy as protocols sometimes have unnecessary steps (the protocols are not perfect, they have the essential feature that they work, that is if you do them right). If you can shave off the right parts of the protocol, then your understanding of the procedure increase. But understanding is also needed to know where you must be meticulous and where there is room for variation. These changes to the protocols can also turn to sloppiness, especially if one is tired. And in biochemistry the consequences of your detour form the protocol might be only be seen three days later. And then, as our examples show, they can easily be interpreted as caused by something else. Thus, the same conclusions can be drawn: In the general confused atmosphere of the lab, the problems might not be recognized as real, but thought to be a result of other factors, or your own sloppiness. When you start debugging protocols, instead of doing real science (as in working on the thesis hypothesis itself), some vital assumption is not recognize before most other factors have been checked. In this case the assumption was one seldom mentioned, namely that the Promega Catalogue have their weaknesses too. Furthermore, the sequences from the databases can contain errors (and when mistakes are transferred into protocols and lab-files, it takes real effort to hunt them down – a longer story, not to be told here). A vital understanding is that it is not hard to see the problems in hindsight (if you for example look for salt concentrations and have the New England Biolab tables to compare with), and that sequence mistakes are not to hard to spot, if you first suspect that they might be there. Another vital understanding is to recognize that these controls are easy (and fast) to do if you just are aware that such problems might appear, and that you regard them as an important, real problem. As I conclude this third example, I stress that this last point will lead to a major topic in my discussion, namely that many of the problems encountered, are easily avoidable if you just had heard about them previously. 8.9 Discussion In the following I will discuss what can be learnt from these three examples in general, and who this knowledge can be introduced into the lab life of a novice or a scientist. 8.9.1 The Recognition of Trouble My original question was, how do the Masters tread to avoid all the traps? Do they have some rules of thumb that they incessantly mumble beneath their breath as they work? Probably not. They have some rules, but they are taken out of context. For example, “it is important to have controls.” Sure... A seemingly trivial point. And that is sad, because this rule is so true. However, one cannot have controls for everything, as there are so many steps in the protocols and so many protocols. So it is important to know when, where and against what does one need controls. In other words, the rule of thumb must be set in a context. This context is the wordless understanding that I think the Masters have gained through years of experience. They have seen (and heard about) so many situations where it was necessary to use this or that rule of thumb. The important thing is in a way not the rule itself, but more about what the rule is directed against, the misfortunes not to be encountered. One need to know about what mistakes can be made, preferably in quite some detail. 8.9.2 Wordless Experience is not Worthless Experience My examples contain many words. I truly believe that experiences like these are very common in laboratories around the world. Everybody has these experiences, but I am not sure if they are remembered in words. They are remembered when needed, but not even then in many words, but in a feeling of what would be the right thing to do, plus some rule of thumb, often phrased almost like a cliché: “Run the experiment again!”, “Change all the reagents!”, Check all your calculations!”, “Reread the protocol!”, “Go ask if something is wrong with the machine these days!” etc... 8.9.3 Implementations If I truly have a point in stating that the negative experiences from failed experiments ought to be conserved for posterity and the greater good, who should this then be implemented in practice? One way would be to have the ordinary scientist read more biographies and philosophy of science, which do sometimes consider these or similar problems, but this is quite unrealistic as the barriers between disciplines even inside the sciences are remarkably high at time, not to speak of the “fuzzy stuff” in the humanities. A local solution, however, here at the local Faculty of Science, would be to make the compulsory Examen Philosophicum contain more of these everyday science experiences. Still, these approaches tend to theorize too much, and one is left in practice with almost sterile Popper-and-Kuhn storytelling. 8.9.4 The Sterile Articles A major leap in the method of science was implemented when the first secretary of The Royal Society of London for Promotion of Natural Knowledge, Henry Oldenburg, started the journal Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London in 1660 (Day, 1995, Online Britannica). IMRAD organization of articles (Introduction, Methods, Results and Discussion) have because of large amount of information and a competitive atmosphere, won support after the science boost succeeding World War II (Day, 1995). The format will ease the burden for reviewers and readers, especially if one shall browse many articles. Still, I think that although the format is great, it might be a bit sterile if one is not only interested in the facts of the experiments. Scientific papers that considered the basic assumptions, general confusion and human fallibility, would be much more fruitful for the major goal of scientific papers, namely reproducibility and further insight. This goal would be more easily attained if one managed to convey some of the real, confusing atmosphere of the lab. But if one take one look at the atrophied Methods & Materials parts of modern articles, one understands that this is quite unrealistic. 8.9.5. The LabLore Site As the understanding and experience we want to convey is hard to express in words (the general rules seemingly become useless clichés), it might even not be desirable to include a part in articles named, for example, “Major Problems Encountered”. So one other solution is the way of conveying understanding by indirect storytelling. My main suggestion is the initiation of a peer reviewed internet site, where stories, dialogues and examples, like the three examples I gave, will be collected in a searchable form, that is, on topics and disciplines. Links to valuable information sources like PubMed, databases, discussion groups, library services etc, should be collected. One example of an existing information collection site is Rein Aaslands web site www.uib.no/aasland. A new website, called LabLore, could be an accessible, easy way to other peoples negative, and positive, experiences. This could be another crucial tool for the student of molecular biology. * Post script: Long afterwards I discovered that the MiniPreps of Aasland lab were probably systemically contaminated with trace amounts of phenol – which do absorb 260 nm UV-light very strongly. Furthermore, my attempt at explanation is based on a common lab myth – as written about extensively in the latest edition of Sambrook (aka. Maniatis) lab manuals from Cold Springs Harbor Laboratories – it is true that DNA can contaminate protein isolates and raise the absorbance ratio above 1.6, but the inverse is simply not true, since proteins absorb so little UV light compared with DNA or RNA. ** PPS: Remarkably, the exact same story of faulty DNA quantification – people claming the UV spectrometer being faulty, and the gel estimation of bands more reliable – played out in the Seeberg laboratories in Oslo in 2003. What we learn from history is that people never learn, even when told in very clear terms. *** PPPS Years later, using plasmids from a later generation of yeast two-hybrid plasmids and from Xenopus expression plasmids, the concept of difficult plasmids prop up again and again. Star activity and correct restriction analysis is as important as ever, as months are lost if everything is not perfect. And nobody ever tells you anything about the plasmids FedEx’ed around.