5 TOWARDS PREDICTIVE MODELS OF BIRD MIGRATION SCHEDULES: THEORETICAL AND EMPIRICAL BOTTLENECKS Bruno J. Ens, Theunis Piersma and Joost M. Tinbergen DECISIONS REFUEL? STOP L MIGRATION ? DEPART ? v / : GOAL ■ REPRODUCE PREPARE FOfiV ■ S U R V IV E MIGRATION ?J PREY ECOLOGY Nederlands Instituut voor Onderzoek der Zee © 1994 This report is not to be cited without acknowledgement of the source. Netherlands Institute for Sea Research (NIOZ) P.O. Box 59, 1790 AB Den Burg, Texel, The Netherlands Netherlands institute for Forestry and Nature Research (IBN-DLO), P.O. Box 167, 1790 AD Den Burg, Texel The Netherlands Behavioural Biology, The Zoological Laboratory University of Groningen, P.O. Box 14, 9750 AA Haren The Netherlands ISSN 0923-3210 Cover design: H. Hobbelink TOWARDS PREDICTIVE MODELS OF BIRD MIGRATION SCHEDULES: THEORETICAL AND EMPIRICAL BOTTLENECKS Bruno J. Ens1, Theunis Piersma2 and Joost M. Tinbergen3 Netherlands Institute for Forestry and Nature Research (IBN-DLO), P.O. Box 167,1790 AD Den Burg (Texel), The Netherlands N etherlands Institute for Sea Research (NIOZ), RO. Box 59, 1790 AB Den Bureg (Texel), The Netherlands 3Behavioural Biology, The Zoological Laboratory, University of Groningen, P.O. Box 14, 9750 AA Haren, The Netherlands Report of a w orkshop made possible through grants from the Dutch National Research P rogram m e on G lobal A ir and Clim ate Change (NOP), the Institute for Forestry and Nature Research (IBN-DLO) and the Netherlands Institute for Sea Research (NIOZ) NETHERLANDS INSTITUTE FOR SEA RESEARCH NIOZ-RAPPORT 1994 -5 A few words on how this meeting came about and what made it possible may be of interest. The idea for the meeting arose during a visit to Oxford. The combined presence of Peter Hicklin and BE prompted Alasdair Houston to organize a small meeting to discuss models of migration. This impromptu meeting was also attended by Richard Caldow, John McNamara, Tony Moodey, Bill Sutherland, Thomas Weber and Wolfgang Weisser. In part because the meeting worked so well, it seemed that more people could have profited from attending. Also, the discussions might have been even more lively with a somewhat greater audience and a greater share of die-hard empiricists, willing to harp on every possible weakness in the models. Finally, there was a feeling that the study of migration strategies was rapidly gaining momentum and that this provided an important window of opportunity to forge a link between the empirical and theoretical studies of migration and thereby set them on a common course. Phoning around made it clear that many people were happy to attend such a “theoreticians-meet-empiricists" meeting. Initially envisaged as a small meeting of at maximum 35 sci­ entists it grew to a meeting of almost double that size through word of mouth in combination with a deliberate policy to let enthusiasm have its way. Wim Wolff showed great enthusiasm at the first mention of the idea and helped to organize funds. These funds were used to pay for the travel expenses of some dearly wanted partic­ ipants, mainly from the other side of the Atlantic, and for the publication of this report. Without the willingness of the majority of the participants to pay for their own expenses, the meeting would not have been possible though. Funds were obtained from the National Research Programme on Global Air and Climate Change, the Institute for Forestry and Nature Research and the Netherlands Institute for Sea Research. The cooperative attitude of Joke Hart, Michaela Scholl and Gjalt Steenhuizen during the meeting substantially contributed to its success. We are also grateful for the many replies that we received to our request for comments on an earlier version of this report. Comments were received from: Thomas Alerstam, Herbert Biebach, Bruno Bruderer, Dan Cristol, Peter Evans, John Goss-Custard, Anders Hedenström, Richard Holmes, Alasdair Houston, Lukas Jenni, Susi Jenni-Eiermann, Marcel Klaassen, Felix Liechti, Äke Lindström, Jaap van der Meer, Frank Moore, Martin Morton, Berry Pinshow and Thomas Weber. Especially gratifying were the comments of Ellen Ketterson, a participating non-participant, and the long letters of Paul Dolman, full of healthy disagreement. Henk Hobbelink enthusiastically engineered a cover for this report. The report was produced by Anneke Bol and Nelleke Krijgsman. TOWARDS PREDICTIVE MODELS OF BIRD MIGRATION SCHEDULES: THEORETICAL AND EMPIRICAL BOTTLENECKS Bruno J. Ens, T h e u n is P ie rsm a & Jo o st M. T inb erg en "The relation of theory to experiment in biology has been an uneasy one. The word “theoretical" has generally had perjorative connotations, and the right to the­ orize was the reward for years o f laboratory and field work. In fields where progress depends mostly on the refinement of technique in order to facilitate more accurate description, this perhaps did not matter too much, but other areas suf­ fered from an indigestion o f facts, while data was collected without reference to problems. In these circumstances, theoretical work often diverged too far from life and became exercises in mathematics inspired by biology rather than an analysis o f living systems. “ (L e v in s , 1968) INTRODUCTION T ake the a rch etypa l w ader, b re ed ing on th e arctic tundra, using in te rtida l m u d fla ts in the te m p e ra te zone as a s to p o v e r site during m ig ration and w in te r­ ing on in te rtida l m ud flats in the tropics. W h a t w ill h a p ­ pen to the birds if so m e o f th e ir h a bita ts are m odified by m an fo r exp lo itation , like reclam atio n o f the in te r­ tidal m u d fla ts o r oil-d rillin g on th e tu n d ra ? D esired a n sw e rs w o u ld a lm o st ce rta in ly in clud e exp ected cha ng es in th e nu m b e r and tim in g o f birds using the affected areas, as w ell as ch a n g e s in th e ir pattern of m ass gain. It w ill n o t be ea sy to g ive such answ ers. It w ill be even m ore d ifficu lt to p re d ict w h a t w ill happen if all h a bita ts are s im u lta n e o u sly affe cte d by global c li­ m ate change. O ne w a y to go a b o u t an sw e rin g the se q u e stio n s is to tre a t the m as th o u g h t exp e rim e n ts w h ere o u r a b ility to co m e up w ith an a n sw e r in dica tes the cu rre n t s ta te o f o u r kno w le dg e. So, w h a t is the cu rre n t state o f o u r kno w le dg e? T h e re is no d o u b t th a t stu d ie s o f the m ig ratory m ove m e n ts o f birds have a long history. A ltho ug h ea rly stu d e n ts o f m ig ra tio n did n o t fail to th e o rize (see review in G a u t h r e a u x 1982), a tte m p ts to co n stru ct form al m a th e m a tica l m odels, a n e ce ssa ry p re re q u i­ site fo r pre cise pre dictions, are o f m uch m ore rece nt origin. In the late sixtie s and e a rly seve ntie s P e n n y c u ic k (1969) and T u c k e r (1973) p re sen te d basic flig h t m e ch a n ica l th e o rie s a b o u t bird m ig ration . In the s e ve n tie s fo rm a tio n flig h t and w ind effects w e re e v a l­ uated (P e n n y c u ic k 1978, A ler s t a m 1979). In the eig h tie s ga m e th e o ry (see M ay n a r d S m it h 1982) was used to stud y the co n d itio n s fo r pa rtial bird m igration (L u n d b e r g 1987) and sta b le g e o g ra p h ica l po pulation s e g re g a tio n (L u n d b e r g & A le r s t a m 1986). In the nine ties s im p le o p tim iza tio n p ro ce d u re s w ere used to pre d ict op tim al d e p a rtu re fa t lo ad s (A lerstam & L in d ­ s t r ö m 1990). In ad dition in cre a sin g use is being m ad e o f sto ch a stic d yn a m ic pro g ra m m in g to handle m ore co m p le x d yn am ical a sp e cts o f m igration. The u se fuln ess o f th is m a th e m a tica l te ch n iq u e in the stu d y o f fo ra g in g and life histo ry pro blem s w as bro u g h t to the fo re by M c N a m a r a & H o u s t o n (1986) and M a n g e l & C l a r k (1986). A s th e o rigin al flig ht m ech an ical th e o rie s w e re a lso co n tin u a lly im p roved and adde d upon th ro u g h o u t th is perio d (R a y n e r 1988, P e n n y c u ic k 1989), th e re can be no do ub t tha t m athe m atica l m od els th a t a cco u n t fo r th e variou s asp e cts o f m ig ration are d e ve lo p e d a t an in crea sing rate. A ltho ug h th is is a h e alth y d e ve lo p m e n t, th e re is a risk th a t the th e o re ticia n s w ill b e co m e so excite d by each o th e r th a t th e y d isa p p e a r en m asse in to o u te r space, instead o f con fro ntin g th e m ore e m p irica lly m inded stu d e n ts o f m ig ration w ith pe n e tra tin g q u e s ­ tion s. S uch qu e stio n s can be o f g re a t help in w a rdin g o ff the e ve r-p re se n t d a n g e r o f d ro w n in g in th e c o m ­ plexitie s o f nature. In return, th e o re tic ia n s w ill profit from rem aining a n cho red to th e real w o rld, even th o u g h it m ay be p ro fo u n d ly irrita ting to be told the fa cts o f life by m e m b e rs o f the scie n tific com m u nity w ith m ud on th e ir boots. U sing m ore d e fe re n tia l la n ­ gu a g e it am o un ts to the c o n victio n th a t scie ntific pro gre ss is ha m pe red w h en th e o re tica l a d va n ce s are n o t a cco m pa nie d by c o m p a ra b le a d va n ce s on the em p irica l sid e o f th e rese arch pro gra m . O n ly th ro u g h close co o pe ration will it be po ssib le to a ch ie ve ad van ces on both sides. A cle a r e xa m p le o f w h a t m ay go w ro ng is niche theory, w h ich so u g h t to e xp la in the pa tte rns in anim al co m m u n itie s as a re su lt o f in te rsp e cific co m p e titio n (M a c A r t h u r 1972). W h ile th e o ry be cam e in cre a sin g ly co m p le x, in su fficie n t ad van ces w e re m ade in th e d e v e lo p m e n t o f a m e th ­ od o lo g y th a t allow ed the actu al m e a su re m e n t o f lim it­ ing reso urce s in the field. A s a result, th e rese arch pro gra m w a s not proven w ro ng o r su b sta n tia lly m o d i­ fied on the ba sis o f em p irica l evide nce, b u t sim p ly a b a n do ned by th e m a in stre a m o f e co lo gists. It is th e re fo re to be ho pe d th a t the w o rksho p, o rg an ized on Texel fro m 6 to 8 Ju ly 1993, su cce e d e d in esta b lish in g firm links be tw ee n th e o re tic ia n s and e m p iricists, th ro u g h the d ire ct co n fro n ta tio n o f ideas w ith facts and vice versa, as w ell as th ro u g h the e sta b lish m e n t o f pe rson al co n ta cts. D e spite the large n u m b e r o f p a rticip ants the m ee ting retained its in te nd ed inform al character. T hough discussio ns w e re lively, p a rticip ants sp e n t th e ir energy in exch a n g in g ideas instead o f d isp la y be ha viou r in te nd ed to im press, o r cla im priority. T his sug ge sts th a t clo se personal con tacts are a rem e dy a g a in st the trend noted by M a d d o x (1993) th a t in te nsifying social co m p e titio n am o ng scie n tists is in cre a sin g ly crip pling scie nce th ro u g h im p ed ing the fre e flo w o f ideas. The re m e d y relies on th e o b serva tion th a t scientists, like o th e r anim als, ca n n o t esca pe being p riso n e rs o f th e ir social relatio nships. S in ce lim its to th e tim e b u d g e t o f an anim al w ill lim it th e tim e a va ila ble fo r social c o n ­ tacts, the nu m b e r o f su fficie n tly in tim ate relatio nships w ill be lim ite d too, su g g e stin g th a t the rem e dy does not w o rk w h e n the n u m b e r o f scie ntists is too large. DEFINING THE PROBLEM Birds are on ly ca p a b le o f com p le ting a full m ig ratory cycle if th e y possess fo rm id a b le p o w e rs o f n a vig a ­ tion. No w o n d e r th a t m uch rese arch e ffort has co n ­ cen tra ted on trying to un de rsta nd th e m ech an ism s o f na vig ation (Papi & W a l r a f f 1982). S im ilarly, s u c ­ cessful com p le tion o f the m ig ra to ry cycle req uire s a pro pe r tim in g. M ainly throu gh the w o rk o f G w in n e r (e.g. 1990) e xp e rim e n ts w ith m ig ra to ry birds have provided so m e o f the be st de m o n stra tio n s o f the existe nce o f e n d o g e n o u s circa nn ual rhythm s s y n ­ chro nized by en viro n m e n ta l Z e itg e b e r s . H ow ever, an u n de rsta nd ing o f som e o f the abilities th a t a llo w birds to m ake th e ir m ig ra to ry jo u rn e ys is a fa r cry from a th e o ry pre dicting th e de tails o f a m ig ration sche du le o f a p a rticu la r bird spe cie s (P ie rsm a 1987, A le r s ta m & L in d s trö m 1990, Ens et al. 1990). B a k e r (1978) fo rm u la te d a ve ry general m odel and cou che d it in such a b stra ct term s th a t it rem ains "u nfam ilia r to m o st11acco rding to K e t t e r s o n & N olan (1983). T hese au thors s u b se q u e n tly state th a t “w h ile the co m p re h e n s ive n e ss of the m odel m akes it a d m i­ rable in the abstract, in practice it m ay ren de r p re dic­ tion s un testable". T h e alte rn a tive to con stru cting a ve ry general and co m p re h e n sive th e o ry e n co m p a ss­ ing all possible typ e s o f m ove m en t in the en tire a n i­ m al kin gd om is to build m odels fo r w e ll-d e fin e d and th e re fo re n e ce ssa rily lim ited problem s. T his w as m ore o r less the ap p ro a ch im p licitly o r e xp licitly take n by the p a rticip ants o f the w o rksho p. Like B a k e r (1978) it w as g e n e ra lly a ssu m ed tho ugh th a t the de tails o f a m ig ration sch e d u le are sha pe d by natural selection. T he fa c t th a t a t p re se n t none o f the pa rticip ants see m ed to be co n stru ctin g a grand un ifyin g the ory should n o t be take n to m ean th a t such a th e o ry is not w anted. In con tra st, an im p o rta n t goal o f the w o rk ­ shop w as to id en tify how the va rio u s studies o f s p e ­ cific m ig ratory d e cisio n s fit together. A fter all, the re can be no do ub t th a t so lu tio n s to o n e pro blem c o n ­ strain the po ssib le so lu tio n s to o th e r pro b le m s. To avo id m isu n d e rsta n d in g s it is im p o rta n t to m a ke cle ar th a t in th is re p o rt the w ord d e cisio n is used in the se n se o f M c F a r l a n d (1977) fo r case s w h e re seve ral m utua lly e xclu sive b e ha viou ral o p tio n s e x is t fo r the anim al: "the re m u st be so m e pro cess o r m ech an ism th a t d e te rm in e s w h ich activity is to ha ve prio rity at any p a rticu la r tim e, and such a p ro ce ss is c o n v e n ­ ie ntly term ed a de cisio n pro cess". T hus, the use o f the w ord de cisio n is in no w a y in te nd ed to im p ly c o n ­ scio us ch o ice by th e anim al. R g u re 1 d e p icts an id e ­ alized de cisio n sch e m e o f a m ig ra to ry bird. The sim p licity o f th e sch e m e e ffe ctive ly h id e s the m an y discu ssio n s be tw ee n the th re e o f us d u rin g its g e s ta ­ tion. A cco rd in g to the sch e m e the m ig ra to ry jo u rn e y o f an in dividu al bird can be broken do w n into fo u r m ajor d e cisio ns: (1) the de cisio n to s ta rt p re p a ra tio n s ne cessa ry fo r m ig ration like building up fu e l stores, (2) th e de cisio n to d e p a rt on the m ig ra to ry flig ht, (3) the de cisio n to in te rru p t th e m ig ra to ry flig h t fo r re fu e l­ ling and (4) the de cisio n to end m ig ration a lto g e th e r and sea rch fo r a good site to bre ed o r su rvive the no n -b re e d in g season. T h e se d e cisio n s lead to on e o f three phases: the fue llin g phase, th e flig h t ph a se and the re sid e n t phase. D uring each phase co n tin u o u sly a m ultitu de o f d e cisio n s has to be m ad e in resp on se to the pre vailin g co n d itio n s in o rd e r to reach the ap p ro p ria te state ch a ra cte rizin g th e phase. During the fu e llin g and refue lling pe rio ds the a n im a l has to d e cid e on the rate o f m ass gain in re sp o n se to local foo d co n d itio n s and pre da tion risks. D u ring the m ig ra ­ to ry flig h t the an im a l has to d e cid e on th e po sitio n in th e flock, the flig h t spe ed and th e flig h t a ltitu d e in resp on se to the w e a th e r co n d itio n s en route. Finally, the bird has to settle dow n, ta kin g h a b ita t q u a lity and th e p re se n ce o r ab sen ce o f c o m p e tito rs in to account. T he sch e m e has bro ken dow n the pro blem in to a set o f decisions, allow in g an e n q u iry into th e costs Major m&E^jDRy DEÔ sioM S s PREY - J ecoLc&y ~ * Fig. 1. Idealized scheme of the major migratory decisions faced by a migrating bird (see text for a detailed discussion). and be n e fits (w hich m ust u ltim a te ly be e xp resse d in te rm s o f fitne ss), as w e ll as th e u n de rlyin g m e ch a ­ nism o f each decision. A t th e sam e tim e the sche m e high lig hts th e in te rd e p e n d e n ce o f the de cisio ns: tho ugh th e va rio u s de cisio n s m a y be stud ied as in d e ­ p e nd en t p ro b le m s in th e ir ow n right, th e final solution req uire s th a t th e resu lts o f th e se stu d ie s fit in the sam e fram e. W h a t the s c h e m e do es n o t d e p ict is the d ire ctio n a l co m p o n e n t o f som e o f th e de cisio ns. A p a rt from d e cid in g w h a t to do w hen, th e anim al m ust also de cid e w h e re to go. F urthe rm o re, th o u g h the sche m e pre sen ts the problem as one o f the in dividu al bird fa c ­ ing a seq ue nce o f b e ha viou ral decisions, refere nce to a lo w er level o f in te gra tion, i.e. p h ysio log ica l p ro c­ e sse s in side the anim al, an d a h ig h e r level o f in te g ra ­ tion, i.e. po pu latio n pro cesse s, w ill be necessary. P h ysio lo g y fo r instance, d e te rm in e s how fa r a bird can fly w ith a given fa t load a t a given w ind speed, w h ile the costs and be n e fits in te rm s o f fitne ss o f a pa rticu la r m ig ra to ry de cisio n w ill often depend on the m ig ra to ry b e h a vio u r o f co n sp e cifics, i.e. on p o p u la ­ tion p ro cesse s. In fact, in ste ad o f de scrib ing the m ig ration sc h e d u le as being ad h e re d to by a h yp o ­ th e tica l sin g le individu al m an y o f us u ltim a te ly w a n t to in clud e the n u m b e r o f in dividu als in the de scrip tion o f a m ig ration schedule. D weight Power, P(watt) THE MIGRATORY FLIGHT FLIG H T M ECHANICS F lig h t m e ch a n ics is a ba sic e le m e n t in th e o rie s o f bird m ig ration (A le rs ta m 1991). A cen tra l e le m e n t of flig h t m e ch a n ics is the p o w e r curve, w h ich predicts th a t e n e rg y e xp e n d itu re w ill be high a t both low and high flig h t s p e e d s and reach a m in im um at in te rm e d i­ ate sp e e d s (P e n n y c u ic k 1989). T h is p o w e r curve can be use d to fin d th e flig h t spe ed th a t m in im ize s the e n e rg y co st per u n it tim e, th e flig h t spe ed fo r m in im iz ­ ing th e e n e rg y co sts p e r u n it o f d ista n ce covered (the m a xim u m ra n g e speed) and th e op tim al flig h t speed fo r m in im izing th e total d u ra tio n o f th e m ig ra to ry jo u r­ n e y (Fig. 2). A le r s ta m e l at. (1993) o b serve d fo r a range o f s e a b ird s th a t a ve ra g e a irsp e e d s fell betw een th e m in im um po w e r and m a xim u m ran ge sp e e d s e s ti­ m a te d from a e ro d yn a m ica l theory. B r u d e r e r & W e itn a u e r (1972) sh o w e d th a t C o m m on S w ifts rem a in a irb o rn e du rin g s le e p in g flig h ts a t a b o u t the th e o re tica l spe ed o f m in im um po w e r and clo sely Rate of fuel deposition, K (watt) Fig. 2. Power curve estimated for the Arctic Tern according to the theory of bird flight mechanics. The mechanical power output indicated on the power axis should be divided by the conversion efficiency (normally assumed to be about 23%) to give the total chemical energy metabolized by a flying bird. The rate of fuel deposition is converted into its mechanical equiv­ alents to be compatible with the flight power given. V mp is the speed that minimizes energy costs per unit of time. Vmr is the speed that minimizes the energy costs per unit of distance covered. V , is the optimal flight speed for minimizing the total duration of a migratory journey, including the periods of fuel deposition. Vmjqr is the maximal overall speed of migration that can be attained. From A lerstam (1991). Reprinted with permission from Elsevier Tredn Journals. Power: r max Speed: V = f (Vz ) Time: *1 w to ” 2 x2 (V mr + W ) E nergy: Fig. 3. Schem atic migration flight with an initial clim bing phase to altitude = z w hen distance is covered and a cruising phase when distance x , is covered. W e have indicated the qualities needed to derive the total energy cost (E, + E2) for the entire migration flight, which is the currency being m inimized with respect to clim b rate (see H e d e n strö m & A le rs ta m , 1994 for details) match maximum range speed during migratory flights. L ie c h t i (1992) showed that small passerines adjust their airspeed to wind conditions in close agreement with the theoretical curve. Anders Hedenström applied the power curve to cal­ culations on climbing flight. A simple model predicting the optimal rate of climb for birds setting out on a migratory flight was presented (Fig. 3). The model uses an energy minimization argument for the entire flight (climbing and cruising phases) and gives the climb rate resulting in minimum cost of transport. Some data in support of the model was presented of shorebirds departing from NW Iceland during spring migration (H e d e n s t r ö m & A l e r s t a m 1994). Compar­ ing species, a negative relation between climb rate and body mass was predicted, and observed. A neg­ ative correlation between maximum fuel load capac­ ity, i.e. the fuel load at which climbing flight is still possible, and body mass was also predicted and observed. As the maximal flight range is determined by the maximal fuel load capacity it follows that potential flight ranges are size-dependent (HEDEN­ STRÖM & A l e r s t a m 1992). Bruno Bruderer (in press) reported on the first study combining on-site radio sonde measurements with reliable radar observations on height distribution of migrants crossing the Negev desert in Israel. In addition, the data were gathered under extreme tem­ perature and humidity conditions, and by this, ena­ bled Bruderer and co-workers to test existing hypotheses on the influence of temperature and humidity on altitude distribution. A statistical model, developed in cooperation with Les Underhill (B r u d ­ e r e r et al. in press), indicated that choice of height stratum was primarily affected by the amount of tail wind, contradicting suggestions (see later) that noc­ turnal migrants above desert areas have to adjust their flight level to minimize evaporation of water. The model, which assumes that the birds compare the meteorological conditions in the adjoining height strata and then switch to the more profitable layer with a certain estimated probability, yielded a close fit to the empirical data: the change in tailwind speed predicted on average 63% of the autumn and 56% of the spring distributions, while none of the other varia­ bles had a significant influence. Fig. 4 provides an example. Felix Liechti combined the model of P e n n y c u ic k (1969, 1989) for maximum range speed with A l e r s t a m 's (1979) model on optimal drift strategy, calcu­ lating the optimal adjustment of heading and airspeed to compensate for changeable winds with respect to a specific goal (L ie c h t i 1992, 1993 and in press). In this case flying the maximum range is regarded optimal (Fig. 5). Under most wind conditions the proposed statistical model on optimal behaviour came close to the observed behaviour, but large deviations from the model occurred under strong side winds (above 5 m/ s). E N E R G Y A N D O T H E R P O T E N T IA L L Y L IM IT IN G R E S O U R C E S D U R IN G M IG R A T IO N Under the assumption that only energy is critical, maximal flight range can be calculated for a given fat load from the previously discussed power curve. However, the assumption may be wrong. There may be conditions (i.e. ambient temperatures) where 14./ 15.9.91 km 4 Fig. 5. Optimal flight behaviour: m inim izing energy exp e nd i­ ture during one flight stage in relation to a specific goal. The distance flown in one stage (At.Vg) is the sum of the work done by the bird (At.Va) and the displacem ent by the wind (At.Vw). For an optimal behaviour the rem aining distance after the flight stage (X) m ust be minimal. (S1 ^starting point, S2=stopover site, G=goal, D=initial distance to the goal, ra=wind direction, ß=direction of heading, Va=airspeed, Vw=wind speed, Vg=groundspeed, At=flight tim e). From LlECHTl (1992). □ water loss, instead of energy expenditure, limits flight range (Y a p p 1 9 6 2 , B ie s e l & N a c h t ig a l l 1 9 8 7 ). In a recent model C a r m i et al. (1 9 9 2 ) have sought to quantify these conditions. Marcel Klaassen (in press) subsequently refined the model of Carmi et al. with respect to the feedback of the difference between metabolic water production and total water loss (i.e. net water loss) on the flight costs. The flow chart of the modified model is presented in Fig. 6. ENERGY O WATER 0 5 10 15 20 25 30 35 “ I I I I I I-----------1 10 birds/km3 Fig. 4. C om parison of predicted (shaded bars) and observed (white bars) height distribution of nocturnal bird m igration in autum n (14/15.9.1991) over the Arava Valley (Israel). Pre­ dictions are based on the difference in tailwind between neighbouring height levels. The circular diagram in the upper right corner indicates the wind direction and speed (length o f the bar) for three height intervals (inner circle 0 to 1000; m edium 1001 to 2000; outer 2001 to 3000 m above ground level) and throughout the night (white 20:00 h, black 24:00 h, shaded 4:00 h). From B r u d e r e r et a i (in press). Fig. 6. R ow chart o f the physiological processes underlying the com puter model of Carmi et at. (1992) and including the refinem ent of K la a sse n (in press). Since the bird loses mass continuously during the flight the program perform s iterations over sm all tim e steps. The variables affected by the refinem ent of KlAASSEN (in press) are indicated with a *. When meteorological conditions are known, the water loss model, which necessarily includes a calcu­ lation of the energy balance, allows the prediction of the flight altitude that maximizes flight range. Radar observations by Biebach and Klaassen on migrants crossing the Egyptian desert showed that observed flight ranges corresponded more closely to the pre­ dictions from the water loss model than to the predic­ tions from the energy balance model alone. The water loss model also predicts that flight range can be increased by interrupting the trans-Sahara flight dur­ ing the day. Analysis of radar observations on the pat­ terning of the passage of migrants at different locations by Herbert Biebach indicates that many migrants do indeed rest during the day, as suggested by B ie b a c h et al. (1991). Further support for such interruptions was provided by the observations with tracking radar of B r u d e r e r et al. (in press). However, since in Bruderer's data on migrants crossing the Negev desert the birds did not adjust their flight levels according to the temperature or humidity profile, they did not seem to be limited by water loss (B r u d e r e r et al. in press). Though the water loss model has met some empiri­ cal success it was generally agreed that many impor­ tant parameters and processes were poorly known. The following two questions were identified as the most burning: (1) how much water can a bird lose before suffering physiological damage, (2) what is the rate of water loss under various conditions? Given that, apart from energy, the water balance may some­ times be crucial in determining flight range, it was felt that it might pay to take an even broader perspective and also consider the role of protein turnover and the need for sleep. In fact, there was a general consen­ sus that fat, protein and water constituted a complex of components potentially limiting the migratory flight. THE MIGRATORY JOURNEY: (RE)FUELLING, DEPARTING AND FLYING In the above we have discussed decisions that need to be made during the flying phase. In the following we will discuss decisions and physiological adapta­ tions during the fuelling phase alone, or in combina­ tion with the flying phase. P H Y S IO L O G Y O F M A K IN G A N D U S IN G N U T R IE N T STORES glycerol FFA bound to albumin Fig. 7. Sim plified model of fatty acid transport from adipose tissues to the muscles. Black arrows indicate the usual path­ way. Double-lined arrows indicate the pathway, which is suggested to help to increase fatty acid delivery to the flight muscles during flight in small migrants. FFA = free fatty acids; TG = triglycerides; VLD L = very low density lipopro­ teins. Based on Jenni-E ierm ann & Jenni (1992). Lukas Jenni reported on his physiological studies on migrant passerines caught when crossing the Alps (J e n n i -E ie r m a n n & J e n n i 1991, J e n n i & J e n n i -E ie r ­ m a n n 1992). Measurement of the plasma level of trig­ lycerides (the form in which fat is normally transported to the adipose tissue) suggests that small passerines resynthesize triglycerides in order to achieve a high rate of fat utilization during migration. This hints at an additional metabolic pathway not nor­ mally used by mammals during exercise (J e n n i -E ie r ­ mann & J e n n i 1992; Fig. 7). Furthermore, day migrants seemed to rely less on fat compared to night migrants. Thus, it appears that differences in meta­ bolic physiology are closely linked to the migratory strategy of a species. S u s i J e n n i - E ie r m a n n & J e n n i (in press) showed that plasma triglyceride levels in the blood of birds during stopover can be used to predict the amount of mass change, i.e. the amount of fat stored, during that day. This allowed them to conclude on the basis of measurements on birds caught only once that insectivorous birds store less fat during days when feeding conditions seem poor, i.e. after rainfall and at high wind speeds (J e n n i & J e n n i -E ie r m a n n unpub­ lished). This biochemical technique may prove a pow­ erful tool for evaluating strategies of fuel storage in the field as it circumvents many of the estimation problems discussed by Z w a r t s et al. (1990). C O N S IS T E N C Y IN A L L O M E T R IC R E L A T IO N S H IP S O F P H Y S IO L O G IC A L A N D E C O L O G IC A L V A R IA B L E S Jaap van der Meer, in collaboration with Theunis Piersma, had embarked on an evaluation of various allometric relationships between a dimension of body size and variables like maximal intake rate, energy expenditure during resting and foraging and energy cost of thermoregulation. When these allometric rela­ tionships are formalized in a consistent manner, it can be investigated in what way the shape of some selected relationships constrain the shape of the other relationships, assuming the animal retains energy balance. It is also possible to enquire into the conditions under which maximum fat deposition rate shows a negative correlation with lean body mass, as reported by L in d s t r ö m (1 9 9 1 ). O P T IM A L D E P A R T U R E FAT LO A D S All models that seek to explain departure fat loads as optimal decisions necessarily include assumptions about the flight phase. The marginal value, with respect to flight distance, of adding an extra gram of fuel will decrease with increasing fuel loads due to the increased flight costs. From this the optimal departure fat load can be calculated for a given fat deposition rate and a given optimization criterion (Fig. 8, A l e r ­ s t a m & L in d s t r ö m 1 9 9 0 ). Possible criteria are mini­ mizing the time spent on the migratory journey, or minimizing the energy spent on covering a given dis­ tance. Though the positive relationship between departure fat load and fat deposition rate predicted by the time minimization hypothesis was found in a field experiment with Bluethroats, the observed departure fat loads remained below the predicted levels (L in d STRÖM & A l e r s t a m 1 9 9 2 ). This could be due to not all individuals having the same expected speed of migration, or to variations in the expected speed of migration along the migration route. Assuming that increasing fat loads increase the risk of predation, smaller departure fat loads are also expected when birds attempt to maximize safety from predation during the migratory journey (A l e r s t a m & L in d s t r ö m 1 9 9 0 ). Experiments by Äke Lindström to test if captive Chaffinches and Bramblings would put on less fat when regularly confronted with a model Sparrowhawk yielded mixed results. Crucial for the theory developed by A l e r s t a m & L in d s t r ö m (1 9 9 0 ) is a time or energy cost during a settling phase preceding fat deposition in a stopover site. During the discussions it was questioned if such a phase really exists. The study of Frank Moore on a barrier island (see M o o r e et al. 1 9 9 0 ) where migrants are caught upon arrival and immediately fol­ lowed using telemetry, seemed ideally suited to obtain the necessary insights on this settling phase. The model of Alerstam and Lindström is most appli­ cable to birds that migrate over areas where suitable Fuel load, f Fig. 8. Prediction of optim al fuel loads, (a) The relationship between flight range (Y) and fuel load (f) at departure will be negatively accelerated because of increased flight costs with increasing fuel loads, (b) As a consequence the m ar­ ginal rate of gain in potential flight distance (dY/dt) for a bird m aintaining a constant rate of fuel deposition on stopover will drop as the bird attains progressively larger fuel loads (f). This relationship is shown for a low -quality (1) and highquality (2) stopover site. Given an expected speed of m ove­ m ent along the m igration route according to the horizontal broken line, the optimal fuel load for m axim izing speed of m igration is f-j for departure from the poor site 1 , and f2 for departure from the high quality site 2. From A le rs ta m (1991). Reprinted with perm ission from Elsevier Trend Jo u r­ nals. habitat is more or less widespread, as may be the case for most passerines. G u d m u n d s s o n et al. (1991) show how the idea of time minimization must be applied when only a few, discrete, stopover sites are at hand, which probably is the case for many wader and waterfowl species. When a good knowl­ edge exists of the conditions at various potential stop­ over sites (which is rarely the case), detailed predictions about a complete migratory journey can be derived. O P T IM A L R E T U R N JO U R N E Y The expectation that the speed of migration, or some other parameter characterizing migration, is opti­ mized, derives from the belief that migration is shaped by natural selection. Yet, natural selection maximizes fitness. Thus, it would be nice if it was possible to evaluate the fitness consequences of migratory decisions directly. This is exactly what Fig. 9 does by proposing that to each combination of arrival date and arrival mass a fitness value can assigned. Building on the knowledge on flight costs and the physiology of fuel storage discussed above, the technique of stochastic dynamic programming (M c N a m a r a & H o u s t o n 1986, M a n g e l & C l a r k 1986, M a n g e l & C l a r k 1988) then allows us to find the optimal decisions concerning mass gain and departure for the return journey. Thomas Weber showed the first results of such an analysis. When stochasticity was made to increase along the migra­ tion route, the last staging site was sometimes skipped. Perhaps not surprisingly, departure weights exceeded the weight necessary to reach the next stopover site when there was stochasticity in the expenditure during flight over a given distance. It may be possible to estimate the extent of this stochasticity from data on Knots which encounter quite variable wind conditions between years when migrating from the Banc d'Arguin to the Wadden Sea in spring (P ie r s m a & v a n d e S a n t 1992). In unfavourable years many birds were apparently forced to use an intermediate stopover site, while arrival in the Wad­ den Sea was delayed. A THE GOAL -^r Fig. 9. Schem atic presentation of the link between the ultim ate goal (maximizing fitness, in this case equated to reproductive success on the breeding grounds) and the problem of how mass gain should be patterned during spring m igration (based on P ie r s m a & E ns 1992). O P T IM A L D E C IS IO N S A N D S T O C H A S T IC IT Y Of course, stochastic dynamic programming is not without its problems either. The technique is non-analytic and often involves many parameters. This makes it hard to understand the exact causes as well as the generality of the simulation results. Alasdair Houston used his work on the trade-off between gaining energy and avoiding predation to discuss the condi­ tions under which different types of models were most appropriate to problems of dynamic optimization (H o u s t o n et at. 1993). When an effect of state (e.g. fat load) is present and stochasticity is negligible, analytic solutions can often be found. However, when the foraging environment is stochastic, simple analyti­ cal models are most likely to fail and stochastic dynamic programming seems the only theoretical avenue (e.g. H o u s t o n & M c N a m a r a 1993). The vari­ ous possibilities are depicted in Fig. 10. A striking example was on the buildup of fat stores prior to migration in a variable environment. At low reserve levels risk averse behaviour, meaning preference of a smaller variance in intake rate at a same mean, is expected when the latest possible departure date is still a long way off, but it switches to risk prone behav­ iour, i.e. preference of a higher variance in intake rate at a same mean, either if reserves are higher or if the time until departure is reduced (Fig. 11, B e d n e k o f f & Fig. 10. Diagram depicting the conditions under which differ­ ent types of optim ization m odels are m ost appropriate (Alasdair Houston). Fig. 11. Graph showing for each com bination o f reserve le v­ els (maximum 80) and tim e steps before final tim e T w hether a hypothetical m igrant birds should be either risk prone (pre­ ferring a high variance in intake at a sam e mean) or risk averse (preferring a low variance in intake at a sam e mean). In the m odel final tim e T represents the last opportunity to migrate effectively. Successful m igration is only possible with m aximum reserve levels. Sim ulations are based on model 2 (prem igratory fattening under predation hazard) of B e d n e k o f f & H o u s t o n (in press). 80 70 60 = = = - = 3= Risk Prone := 333333= 3=3=33::r 50 Reserves 40 30 £ Risk Prone = 20 Risk Averse 200 400 G00 800 Time 1000 1200 1400 H o u s to n in press). Laboratory experiments by M o o r e & S im m (1986) demonstrate risk prone behav­ iour in a migrating bird. Indeed, it may well be that migrating birds offer excellent opportunities for the study of the risk proneness of foraging behaviour, since the penalty is not death, but delayed arrival, or the skipping of a breeding season. That is to say, migration offers a situation where risk-prone behav­ iour can be advantageous without death being very likely. L o r ia & M o o r e (1990) and M o o r e (1992) demonstrated differences in foraging behaviour between fat and lean migratory birds in field studies on a stop-over site. The next challenge consists of actually measuring that the observed foraging behav­ iour is risk prone or risk averse. Enormous environmental stochasticity was appar­ ent in the study by Martin Morton of the Mountain White-crowned Sparrow. On the same date the breeding grounds were sometimes covered by sev­ eral meters of snow, or no snow at all, depending on the year. For every 2 days delay in breeding, due to snow conditions at the start of the season, there was a 1 day delay in departure at the end of the season, up to a point where departure was not delayed any further (Fig. 12). There was no link between the date of egg laying and the onset of moult, but fattening prior to autumn migration started when moult had ended. Fig. 12. Relationship of autum nal m igration departure schedules of M ountain W hite-crowned Sparrows in relation to reproductive schedules. Seven years of data (Martin M or­ ton). M E A S U R IN G T H E F IT N E S S C O N S E Q U E N C E S O F A R R IV A L D ATE M odels th a t seek to ca lcu la te th e op tim al tim in g o f the return jo u rn e y to the b re ed ing g ro u n d s m u st s ta rt from a re la tio n sh ip be tw ee n fitn e ss and a rriva l date a n d /o r fitn e ss and arrival co n d itio n (Fig. 9). T hou gh the lo gic o f the m od els req uire s a d op ting a life history ap proach, so far, stu d e n ts o f m ig ra tio n ha ve not attem p te d to a ctu a lly m ea sure th e fitn e ss c o n s e ­ qu en ces o f alte rn a tive m ig ra to ry d e cisio ns. T h is m ay well be due to the fa c t th a t "tra d itio n a l" life histo ry th e ­ ory ha s co n ce n tra te d on stu d yin g h o w de cisio ns take n once a ye a r a ffe ct m ortality and rep ro d u ctio n (see review s in Le s s e l l s 1991 and S t e a r n s 1992). S ta te -d e p e n d e n t life -h isto ry th e o ry (e.g. M c N a m a r a & H o u s t o n 1992) see ks to b ridg e the g a p be tw ee n beha viou ral eco lo g y and life histo ry theory. Still, fit­ ness m ust be m ea sure d. In o rd e r to e stim a te fitne ss co n se q u e n ce s of m ig ration p a tte rn s w e sh o u ld kno w the relation be tw ee n re p ro d u ctive succe ss, arrival date and n u trie n t stores upon arrival. In Fig. 9 the vie w is ta ke n th a t o p tim al arrival da te is in d e p e n d e n t o f nu trie n t stores. H ow ever, th e re p ro d u ctive succe ss as a fu n ctio n o f arrival date is re p re se n te d by th e sum o f the re p rod uctive va lu e o f th e o ffsprin g and the residual re p rod uctive va lu e o f th e parents. W hen the sha pe o f th e fitn e ss cu rve ove r arrival tim e differs betw een offsprin g and parents, a tra d e -o ff o ve r arrival tim e exists. W hen, in addition, th e fitn e ss o f th e p a r­ e n t but n o t the o ffsprin g (or vice versa) d e p e n d s on the n u trie n t sto re s in d e p e n d e n tly o f arrival tim e, than the op tim al arrival tim e w ill s h ift w ith n u trie n t stores. F itness co n se q u e n ce s o f arrival date and con dition ca n n o t be estim a te d re lying on natural variatio n be tw ee n individuals. J u s t as in th e stu d y o f clutch size (e.g. T in b e r g e n & D a a n 1990), e xp e rim e n ts aim ed a t m a n ip u la tin g arrival tim e to e stim a te fitne ss co n se q u e n ce s fo r both p a re n ts and o ffsp rin g are needed. Even though the data are only observational, the review by D a a n et al. (1989) leaves little doubt that for single-brooded bird species the reproductive value of an egg declines in the course of the season. It is much less clear why this is so. There could be a link with competitive advantages related to the benefits of prior residence for either young competing on the wintering grounds, or adults competing on the breed­ ing grounds, or both. When effects of prior residence are important, late-fledged chicks are at a clear com­ petitive disadvantage during their first winter. Also, we would expect that early arriving breeding birds would be in the best position to take up the best territories (M y e r s 1981). However, reviewing the evidence, K e t t e r s o n & N o la n (1983) suggest that "priority of arrival is not sufficient to establish an indefeasible claim when the claim is contested by a former owner". On the one hand there can be no doubt that local sta­ tus is maintained from one year to the next in many 70- ■3> potential competitors. The experiments of Dan Cristol (MS) go some way towards this goal. He showed that experimentally delayed female Red-winged Black­ birds (i.e. held in captivity for a while) were less often the first to breed in their harem and initiated clutches later, even though all females were released well before the first clutches were laid (Fig. 13). These negative effects on fitness, thought to result from a decline in social status, were much less severe when compared to the effects of a similar experiment on males (in a different population though), explaining why the polygynous male Red-winged Blackbirds tend to arrive earlier and much more synchronized than the females. b - b: 60 g 501 £ 40 <u w 1J J - -' -o''n 0 BD Ô S' c 30 20 i 10 WHICH MODELS ARE BEST? 0 807060 5040 30 ° C o n tro ls • Experim entais: natural arrival ° Experim entais: delayed arrival ----- R eg re ssio n o f co n tro ls 2010- 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 Day o f arriva l Fig. 13. Date of arrival and first egg (in days since first fem ale arrived) for A) control and B) experim ental fem ales for 1990 and 1991 combined. Regression equation (dashed line, y=40.7+0.41x) was com puted for controls only, and is also shown in B) for com parison. Open circles represent natural arrival and first egg date for experim ental females. Solid circles represent the dates of release from captivity and first egg for the sam e experim ental females. (C r is t o l MS) migratory long-lived species like the Oystercatcher. On the other hand it would seem that preempting potential usurpers by arriving early on the breeding grounds is less costly than evicting established usurpers (ignoring the cost of departing early from the wintering grounds). The longer Great Tits are experi­ mentally removed from their territory the more likely it is that they will not succeed in regaining their territory (K r e b s 1 9 8 2 ). Thus, methods should be found to measure the cost of arriving a certain number of days later than Fig. 9 depicts the timing of arrival as a simple optimi­ zation problem. However, the above discussion strongly suggests that the concepts of game theory (M a y n a r d S m it h 1982) need to be applied, since what matters is not so much the absolute date of arrival, but the relative timing with respect to the arrival of potential competitors. This poses further problems for fitness measurements as well as for model builders. A t this stage we must ask ourselves how to proceed as there seems a clear danger that the enterprise will lead to a forbidding graveyard of abandoned and underdeveloped models none of which are rigorously tested. We suggest that the the­ oreticians should see it as their task to make clear under what conditions the more complex, but suppos­ edly more realistic, dynamic optimization models yield the same predictions as the more elegant and analyt­ ically tractable rate-maximization models. The empiri­ cists on the other hand should only abandon models if they consistently fail empirical tests. The model of A l e r s t a m & L in d s t r ö m (1990) has inspired some good tests (L in d s t r ö m & A l e r s t a m 1992) and should inspire more. THE FULL ANNUAL CYCLE AS AN INDIVIDUAL DECISION Noël Holmgren and Anders Hedenström were the only ones brave enough to tackle the full annual cycle in their application of dynamic programming to define the conditions when moult should take place. Pilfering from the abstract of the paper by Noël and Anders on this topic (chapter 3 in H o l m g r e n 1993): "The sched­ uling of moult was basically a result of a trade-off between having a high feather quality during breeding versus during the non-breeding period. When feather quality was of major importance for survival, summer moult tended to be the optimal policy. In the opposite situation, when feather quality was more important during breeding, winter moult tended to be optimal. A high impact of feather quality on survival rate in com­ bination with low costs of moult resulted in biannual moult (summer and winter moult). Winter moult became more likely as the survival rate per se increased. Moult duration, migration costs and repro­ ductive success p e rse were found to have no impact on the timing of moult." Though the assumption that feather quality affects fitness seems quite reasona­ ble, the participants of the workshop knew of little direct evidence, let alone estimates of the magnitude of the effect. Since then, the work of Dale Clayton and coworkers on feather lice, heat loss and mate choice (B o o th et al. 1993) was brought to our attention by Ellen Ketterson. POPULATION DYNAMICS OF MIGRATORY SPECIES So far we have considered a migration schedule as a sequence of individual decisions. What we have to compare our models with though, is the migratory behaviour of a population of individuals. This sug­ gests that a complete understanding requires that we are also able to predict the number of birds that follow a particular migration strategy. This, in turn, forces us to ask how migratory birds affect each other. We already touched upon this question when investigat­ ing the effect of arrival time on fitness. Though there are many ways in which the migra­ tory decisions of one individual may be affected by the decisions of other individuals (due to the benefits of flying in a flock for instance; see discussion in P ie r s m a et al. 1990) the workshop concentrated on the effects of intraspecific competition on the breed­ ing and wintering grounds. In his book "Populations in a seasonal environment" F r e t w e l l (1972) graphi­ cally investigated the consequences of densitydependent reproduction on the breeding grounds, and density-dependent survival on the wintering grounds. Here, density-dependent means that, with increasing population density, reproduction in sum­ mer or survival in winter are negatively affected. He showed that, depending on the shape of the relation­ ships, regulation of the population occurred primarily in winter, during summer or in both seasons (Fig. 14). G o s s -C u star d (1980) developed an essentially simi­ lar simulation model. Remarkably, it is only now that these models are being followed up by attempts to obtain empirical estimates of the parameters and more penetrating theoretical investigations, like inclu­ sion of the cost of migration, and attempts to derive the population model from knowledge of the behav­ iour of individuals. John Goss-Custard presented the results of a col­ laborative effort to amass all available data on breed­ ing and survival of the European populations of the Oystercatcher (G o s s -C u star d et al. in press (a); G o s s -C u star d et at. in press (b)). Assuming strong Fig. 14. Graph of a very simple model of population regula­ tion in a seasonal environment. Production occurs during the breeding season with rate b, while deaths are confined to winter and occur with rate d. The population is stable if d=b/(1+b), i.e. if the death rate equals the “replacement rate” g. The equilibrium autumn population size N(1 +b)eq is found by plotting both g and d against the autumn population size N(1+b). From F r e t w e l l (1972). density dependence in summer, it was calculated that populations would decline following habitat loss on the wintering grounds, if wintering densities exceeded those where winter mortality became density-depend­ ent. With steeper density dependence more birds would be lost. Though the database is impressive, the most important parameters, i.e. those describing important aspects of the density-dependent relation­ ships, are least well known. To derive these popula­ tion parameters from knowledge of the behaviour of individuals (see G o s s -C u s tar d & D u r e l l 1990) a game theory model of how Oystercatchers distribute themselves over the musselbeds in the Exe estuary was constructed. From this, a relationship between winter mortality and population size could be derived. The conceptual framework of the model presented by Paul Dolman was essentially similar, but its imple­ mentation differed. Instead of splitting the problem into several submodels, each with empirically esti­ mated parameters, Paul Dolman presented a general simulation model incorporating all the processes believed to be relevant (Fig. 1 5, S u t h e r l a n d & D o l ­ m an in press). Below, the pros and cons of these con­ ceptually similar models are investigated through a close examination of the components, confronting the assumptions with the relevant insights of other partic­ ipants to the workshop. DENSITY-DEPENDENT REPRODUCTION Density-dependent reproduction will occur when with increasing population size an increasing proportion of birds settle in habitat of poor quality or do not breed, FRAMEWORK B R E E D IN G GROUNDS WINTERING GROUNDS Fig. 15. Diagram depicting the presumed nature of the den­ sity-dependent processes on the breeding and wintering grounds (Paul Dolman). the so-called buffer effect ( K lu ijv e r & T in b e rg e n 1953). Detailed long-term studies of the American Redstart and the Blackthroated Blue Warbler by Rich­ ard Holmes and colleagues provided evidence for variation in habitat quality affecting fitness, leading to competition for high quality habitats, where food is abundant and nest predation low (H o lm e s e t al. 1992; S h e r r y & H o lm e s 1992). In Oystercatchers, intense competition exists for territories of high qual­ ity, where transport costs of food to the chicks are low (Ens e t al. 1992). E n s e t al. (M S2) suggest that potential recruits face a trade-off between immedi­ ately settling in a territory of poor quality or queuing for a territory of high quality. Thus, the only freedom left to the newly arriving nonbreeders when faced with intense competition is the decision on how to cut their losses: losing fitness in time (by queuing for a high quality territory) or losing fitness in space (by settling in a poor quality territory). Queuing must result from the advantage enjoyed by prior residents in the com­ petition for space and the impossibility of maintaining high status over a large area (E ns e t al. MS1). Despite the evidence for competition (see also the section on the fitness consequences of arrival date), the shape of the curve relating reproduction to the number of potential settlers remains elusive. Whereas differences between individuals are thought to be of critical importance in models describing density-dependent winter survival, such individual differ­ ences were not explicitly incorporated in the submodels of John Goss-Custard and Paul Dolman dealing with density-dependent reproduction. There is a cle ar need to de rive th e d e n sity -d e p e n d e n t re la ­ tio n s h ip s from the a c cu m u la tin g kn o w le d g e on the m ech an ism o f social c o m p e titio n du rin g th e bre ed ing season. DENSITY-DEPENDENT W IN TER SURVIVAL W in te r su rviva l will be d e n sity -d e p e n d e n t w h en food stocks can be de pleted , or w h en in te rfe re n ce forces an in cre a sin g n u m b e r o f b ird s in to h a b ita ts o f poor qu ality w ith in cre a sin g p o p u la tio n size. E vid e n ce for m assive d e p le tio n o f foo d stocks by va rio u s spe cie s of w a te rfo w l on m ig ration w a s p ro vid ed by M en no bart van E erden. In som e e xa m p le s fo o d w a s d e pleted to a sam e th re sh o ld level ea ch year, b e fore th e birds m ove d on, w h ile in o th e r e xa m p le s th e th re sh o ld level w as va ria b le (van E e rd e n 1984; D r e n t & P rin s 1987). T he la tte r m ay have been d u e to va ria b ility in th e fo o d su p p ly a t p re ce d in g sta g in g sites, a n d /o r the need to a rrive on th e w in te rin g s ite a t a ce rta in date. In contrast, w in te rin g O yste rca tch e rs rare ly rem ove m ore than 30% o f the stan din g sto c k o ve r the w inter, b ut in te rfe re n ce is a w e ll-e sta b lish e d p h en om en on , in clud in g the o b se rva tio n th a t in d ivid u a ls diffe ring in d o m in a n ce s u ffe r to a d iffe re n t d e g re e from the p re s ­ en ce o f co n sp e cifics (Ens & G o s s - C u s ta r d 1984; G o s s - C u s ta r d e t al. 1984). S u b s e q u e n t th e o rie s on th e d istribu tion o f un eq ua l c o m p e tito rs have coined th e term "co m p e titive a b ility" to d e scrib e th e se d iffe r­ en ce s betw een in d ivid u a ls ( S u t h e r la n d & P a r k e r 1985; P a r k e r & S u t h e r la n d 1986). H ow ever, this c o n ce p t m ay n o t a p p ly in th e fie ld (see a p p e n d ix I). For instance, it is hard to id e n tify th e b e tte r c o m p e tito r w ith re sp e ct to the a cq u isitio n o f foo d w h en th e re is a tra d e -o ff be tw ee n fo ra g in g a b ility a n d fig h tin g ability. G ood fo ra g e rs m a y have h ig h e r su rviva l cha nce s w hen food s u p p lie s are se ve re ly de pleted , w h ile good fig h te rs can e xclu d e o th e r birds from th e b e st fe e d in g areas. Potentially, such a tra d e -o ff m ig h t exp la in w hy S h e r r y & H o lm e s (MS) did n o t find s ig n ific a n t s u rv i­ vorsh ip diffe re n ce s be tw ee n A m e rica n R e d sta rts w in ­ te rin g in d iffe re n t ha bita ts, eve n th o u g h rem oval e xp e rim e n ts sh o w e d th a t m ales d e sp o tica lly e xclud ed fe m a le s from certain h a b ita ts ( M a r r a e t al. 1993). F urthe rm o re, social d o m in a n c e is not n e c e s ­ sa rily so le ly de te rm in e d by re la tive fig h tin g ability, but m a y a lso be de te rm in e d by p rio r re side nce, e s p e ­ cia lly w h en in d ivid u a ls sh o w a stro n g site fidelity. P rese nt e vid e n ce ce rta in ly a llo w s th e su g g e stio n tha t O yste rca tch e rs w in te rin g on m u sse lb e d s "q ue ue" fo r p o sitio n s o f high local d o m in a n ce (E ns & C a y fo r d , in press). T h e e xp e rim e n ts o f Dan C ristol on D a rk-eyed Ju n co s le ft little do ub t th a t be h a vio u ra l d o m in a n ce is in flue nced by situ a tio n a l factors, like a rrival date (prior re sid e n ce in crea ses do m in a n ce , C r i s t o l e t al. 1990), h u n g e r (lean birds a re in itia lly d o m in ant, C r is ­ t o l 1992) and th e fa m ilia rity w ith a m ore d o m in a n t bird, as w ell as a ttrib u te s o f th e b ird s like sex. D etailed stu d ie s o f th e W h ite -th ro a te d S pa rrow s depletion. T h is m ad e it n e ce ssa ry to assu m e th a t the ideal fre e distrib u tio n w as reached a t ea ch tim e step. T h e co n d itio n s un d e r w h ich th is a s su m p tio n holds have been th e o re tica lly in ve stig a te d by et al. (1988, 1991). E m pirical e vid e n ce s u g g e stin g clo se tra ckin g o f flu ctu a tio n s in h a b ita t q u a lity o ve r a fa irly w ide g e o g raph ica l a re a w as pro vid ed by M e n n o b a rt van E erd en throu gh an im p re ssive e xa m p le on Teal. In som e yea rs one fifth o f the E uro pe an p o pu latio n co n ce n tra te d in the n a ture re se rve th e O ostvaa rde rspla ssen , w h ile in o th e r ye a rs n u m b e rs w ere ve ry low, in line w ith flu ctu a tio n s o f the foo d sup ply re lated to the m a n a g e m e n t o f th e w a te r le ve ls (Fig. 17). Bernstein X1000 Total population attem pting to settle Fig. 16. The proportion of O ystercatchers predicted to die on the Exe estuary in w inter in relation to the total num ber arriv­ ing there in autum n (means of several m odel runs, bars rep ­ resent one SE). The threshold intake rate values required for a bird to rem ain on the estuary were either 200 mg AFDM /5 min (open circles) or 150 mg AFDM /5 min (solid cir­ cles). The solid triangles show the effects of rem oving the best 30% o f the total m ussel area, assum ing a threshold intake rate of 150 mg AFDM /5 min. From G o s s - C u s ta r d et al. (in press (e)). underline the importance of site-dependency as well as age and sex in determining dominance in the field (Piper & Wiley 1989; Piper & Wiley 1990; Piper 1990). The presented population models did not incorpo­ rate these details of the competitive process. John Goss-Custard derived the relationship between over­ winter mortality and autumn population size from a game theory model of how Oystercatchers differing in their susceptibility to interference and in foraging effi­ ciency distribute themselves over the musselbeds of the Exe (Goss-Custard e t at. in press (c,d & e)). The model assumed that each individual would move to the musselbed where it would achieve the highest intake rate and that there were no costs to moving, in line with the essence of the assumptions of the ideal free distribution model (Fretwell & Lucas 1970) according to Kacelnik e t at. (1992). Though the model will not be the last word on the subject (GossCustard ef a/.in press (e)) the exercise demonstrated beyond doubt that the precise shape of the densitydependent winter mortality can be derived from knowledge of the behaviour of individuals in combina­ tion with measurements of the distribution of resources (Fig. 16). In the model presented by Paul Dolman, density dependence in winter mortality not only resulted from interference, but also from prey Fig. 17. The annual peak num ber of Teal in the freshw ater marsh O ostvaardersplassen, Netherlands, plotted against the annual seed production of Ranunculus sceleratus. Large differences between years are due to m anipulations of the w ater level. From v a n E e rd e n & M u n s te r m a n (M S). T H E C O S T O F M IG R A T IO N M ig ration m u st be costly, but w h a t a re th e co sts and ho w high are th e y? F or p o pu latio n m o d e ls o f m ig ra ­ to ry birds it is im p o rta n t to kno w if m o rta lity on m ig ra ­ tion sho uld be m od elled a s in cre a sin g o n ly w ith dista nce , o r as being de n sity d e p e n d e n t as well. It has on ly re ce n tly be com e po ssib le to e stim a te the co n trib u tio n o f the e n e rg y s p e n t on m ig ra tio n to the an nu al e n e rg y b u d g e t ( D r e n t & P ie r s m a 1990; P ie r s m a e t al. 1991; W ie r s m a & P ie rs m a , 1994). W ie r s m a & P ie r s m a (1994) have used h e ated ta xide rm ic m ou nts to p re d ict sta n d a rd o p e ra tive te m p e ra ­ ture, or, in verse ly, m a in te n a n ce m etab olism , for d iffe re n t m icro h a b ita ts and w e a th e r con ditions. T his m e th o d o lo g y allow ed them to co n clu d e th a t if island ica Knots, the s u b sp e cie s th a t ha b itu a lly w in te rs on the in te rtida l m ud flats o f W e ste rn E urope, w e re to m ove to W e s t A frica, h a b itu a lly occu p ie d b y ca n u tu s K nots, th e y w o u ld in cu r a saving o f 1.1 W on th e ir m a in te n a n ce m etab olism and pa y an extra 0 .1 -0 .2 W to c o ve r th e co st o f tra ve l (Fig. 18). T h e p ro blem is Fig. 18. M aintenance m etabolism on the tem perate Dutch W adden Sea and on the tropical Banc d'Arguin, Mauritania, W est Africa predicted for islandica Knots on the basis of m eteoro­ logical data and m icrohabitat specific equations. These equations were calibrated with the use of heated taxiderm ie m ounts. From W ie rs m a & P ie rs m a (1994). that it is not clear how energy expenditure relates to survival and prospects for reproduction. To calculate how birds from different breeding grounds should distribute themselves over wintering grounds at different distances, Paul Dolman simply assumed the existence of a critical intake rate, neces­ sary to achieve the breeding grounds. The critical intake rate was thought to increase linearly with dis­ tance between the wintering and breeding grounds. Instead of assuming that birds with low intake rates do not reach the breeding grounds at all, it seems more likely that they will reach the breeding grounds later, probably at the cost of a reduction in reproduc­ tive success (Fig. 9). Both assumptions lead to density-dependence in either mortality or reproduction following migration for which there exists no empirical evidence whatsoever. An alternative assumption could therefore be that the crossing of a certain dis­ tance carries a fixed mortality risk. Estimating this risk is difficult. Peter Evans concluded that for several species of waders the actual mortality during the migratory journey is low relative to the mortality on the wintering and the breeding grounds (Pienkowski & Evans 1985; Evans 1991). The reverse seems to be the case for the small migratory passerines stud­ ied by Dick Holmes and colleagues (Holmes et al. 1989; Holmes & Sherry 1992). Martin Morton reported that there existed no differences in age-specific survivorship curves between a migrant and a nonmigrant subspecies of the White-Crowned Spar­ row. A problem with all these survival estimates is that it is not possible to separate lack of fidelity to the wintering or breeding area from mortality. The only conclusive solution to this problem is to study a signif­ icant part of the population throughout the year, as can be done in some species of geese (e.g. Owen & Black 1989; Ebbinge et al. 1991). This will also help in testing the suggestion of Pienkowski & Evans (1985) that an important reason for minimizing the flight distances on the first migration is to reduce the risk of failing to complete the journey to a suitable “wintering" site, i.e. adults may not suffer high losses on migration, but the inexperienced juveniles may. PREDICTING THE EFFECTS OF HABITAT CHANGE An im p o rta n t in cen tive in seve ral o f the p re vio usly d iscusse d stu d ie s w as th e d e sire to u n d e rsta n d and, if possible, p re d ict the e ffe c t o f m a n -in d u ce d ha bita t cha ng es on the p o pu latio n dyn a m ics o f the m ig ratory birds. S uch m a n -in d u ce d ch a n g e s ra n g e from local reclam atio n o f so m e o f the m u d fla ts in an e stu a ry to global clim a te change. A u n ifyin g th e m e in th e stud ies o f D ick H o lm es and c o -w o rke rs fo r in sta n ce has been th e de sire to id e n tify th e ca u se s o f the dra m atic d e clin e in the n u m be rs of n e o tro p ica l m ig ra n t p a s s e r­ ines in press). et al. in pre ss (a) used th e ir p o p u la tio n m od el to c a lc u ­ late the e ffe ct o f loss o f w in te rin g h a b ita t on th e e q u i­ librium p o pu latio n size o f th e E u ro p e a n p o pu latio n of O yste rca tch e rs et al. in pre ss (b). T hey a rrived a t the startlin g co n clu sio n th a t the coa stal su b p o p u la tio n s w o u ld be s tro n g ly ne g a tive ly affected by h a b ita t loss, w h ile n e g a tive e ffe cts on the inlan d b re ed ing s u b p o p u la tio n s o n ly b e ca m e a p p a r­ e n t a fte r th e coa stal su b p o p u la tio n s w e re de cim a te d. T h e re is a cle a r need to te s t th e u n d e rlyin g a s s u m p ­ tio n th a t coa stal and inland birds co m p e te on equal te rm s on the w interin g grounds. (Holmes & Sherry 1992; Sherry & Holmes 1992; Sherry & Holmes Goss-Custard (Goss-Custard When the ESS migration model of Sutherland & Dolman (in press) was used to predict the effect of habitat change on the numbers of birds using differ­ ent migration routes, it was assumed that the birds can adapt to the new circumstances, culturally or genetically, i.e. a new ESS can be achieved. Berthold & Querner (1981) and Berthold (1988) have shown the existence of genetic variation in the migratory behaviour of the Blackcap. Berthold et al. (1992) provide evidence that this species has evolved a new migration route over the past 30 years. Instead of assuming that a new ESS is reached in response to habitat loss without problems, Paul Dolman (unpublished) therefore theoretically investigated the conditions under which a species with a genetically determined migration strategy could survive a drastic change of habitat. The model showed that for the migration strategy to track the changing environment, it was important to have sufficient genetic variance in the population. The model also showed that assortative mating can allow a much more rapid response, allowing tracking even when the level of genetic vari­ ation is initially very low. CONCLUSIONS S O M E IM P O R T A N T A N D H O P E F U L L Y A M E N A B L E PR O BLEM S W hile d iscussin g the va rio u s m ig ra to ry d e cisio n s in turn, a host o f p ro blem s w a s e n cou ntere d. It see m s pru de nt to m ake a sh o rt list o f the m ost im p ortan t ones. • T hou gh e n e rg y is rig htly d e scrib e d as the fire o f life 1961), an anim al m u st also retain w a te r and protein ba la nce during th e m ig ra to ry flight, to p ­ ics th a t d e se rve and a ttra ct in crea sing attention. • T here can be little do u b t th a t the po w e r cu rve is cen tra l to pre d ictio n s on speed, a ltitu de and range o f th e m ig ratory flight, its e lf a crucial e le m e n t o f any m ig ration sche du le. T hou gh th e re w a s general a g re e m e n t th a t the po w e r curve m u st be U -shaped, it w as fe lt th a t m uch m ore em p irica l w o rk w as needed to confirm th e pre cise sh a p e fo r a given species, a ne ce ssa ry p re re q u isite fo r reliab le q u a n ­ tita tive predictions. O n th e basis o f e m p irica l e v i­ d e n ce it is im p ossib le to d e cid e be tw ee n the m od els o f (1989) o r (1988), b u t th e fo rm e r has fe w e r and m ore e a sily m ea sure d pa ra m e te rs tha n the latter. • M ig ration m u st be costly, but w h a t are th e costs and how high are the y? It is in tu itive ly a p pe aling to assu m e th a t high e n e rg y e x p e n d itu re re d u ce s s u r­ vival (e.g. e t al. 1990), but nothing con clu sive is known. For po pu latio n m od els o f m ig ra to ry birds it is im p o rta n t to kn o w if m o rta lity on m igration s h o u ld be m o d elled as fixed fo r a given distance, or as d e n sity depe nd ent. (Kleiber Pennycuick DAAN Rayner • T h o u g h the re is an in cre a sin g a w a re n e ss th a t p o p ­ ulatio n m od els sh o u ld be de rive d from th e in te ra c ­ tio n s be tw ee n individuals, th e m o d e ls discu sse d con tain ed a ssu m p tio n s ab o u t th e c o m p e titive p ro c ­ ess th a t m a y be un tenable. O ne of th e a u th o rs o f th is re p o rt fe e ls ra th e r s tro n g ly th a t e m p irica l s tu d ­ ies o f the c o m p e titive p ro ce ss w o uld b e n e fit from a c la rifica tio n o f c o n ce p ts (see ap p e n d ix I). M IG R A T O R Y B IR D S P R O V ID E G O O D R E S E A R C H SYSTEM S In th e a b ove w e have listed w h a t w e re co n sid e re d to be th e m o st im p o rta n t problem s, w ritten from the p e r­ sp e ctive o f a scie n tist w h o has decided, fo r w h a te ve r reason, th a t solving th e m yste ry o f bird m ig ra tio n is w h a t he w a n ts to do w ith his life. H ow ever, scie ntists w a ntin g to solve a d iffe re n t problem sho uld in no w ay fee l exclud ed. If the stu d y o f m ig ration s tra te g ie s is to becom e a se rio u s u n de rta king d ire ct fitn e ss m e a s u re ­ m ents m ust be take n. T hus, the re is a cle a r need fo r a clo se r link betw een m ig ration and bre ed ing b io lo g i­ cal stud ies o f the sam e po pu latio ns. In fact, it is even p o ssib le th a t scie ntists w ith d iffe re n t in te re sts w ill find th a t m ig ra to ry birds are p a rticu la rly su ita b le fo r a n s ­ w e ring th e ir questions. T h e fo llo w in g su g g e stio n s cam e forw ard: • B irds p re pa ring fo r m ig ration m ay o ffe r go od o p p o r­ tu n itie s fo r stud ying w h e th e r forag in g b e h a vio u r is risk prone or risk averse. T h e pro blem see m s th a t field w o rke rs lack a sea rch im a ge fo r such b e h a v ­ iour: the th e o re ticia n s sho uld co m e forw a rd w ith su g g e stio n s o f w h a t to look for. • M ig ratory birds m ay o ffe r be tte r o p p o rtu n itie s fo r stud ying sea son al tim in g o f m o u lt and rep ro d u ctio n than se d e n ta ry birds. A tte n tio n should fo cu s on arrival tim e and the e xp e rim e n ta l m a n ip u la tio n o f th a t arrival tim e. PRO SPECTS T h e w o rksh o p le ft the p a rticip a n ts w ith little d o u b t th a t the stu d y o f bird m igration, in p a rticu la r th e stu d y o f m ig ration sche du les, is rap id ly g a in ing m om e ntum th ro u g h th e a p p lica tio n o f th e sa m e id ea s and te c h ­ niques, first d e velope d by eco no m ists, so s u c c e s s ­ fu lly a p plied in o th e r fie ld s o f b e ha viou ral eco lo gy. To m aintain m om e ntum it is n e ce ssa ry th a t th e m ore e m p irica lly m in de d and the m ore th e o re tic a lly m inded stud ents o f m ig ration c o n tin u e to rem a in in clo se c o n ­ tact. T o m in im ize th e d a m a g e o f exce ssive social co m p e titio n it is n e ce ssa ry th a t this co n ta ct is n o t only e sta blished throu gh the scie n tific literature, but ab ove all th ro u g h p e rson al con tacts. A clo se c o o p e ra tio n be tw ee n th e o rists and em p iricists w o u ld fit the tra d i­ tion o f b e h a vio u ra l eco lo gy. It w ill be im p o rta n t th o u g h to avo id re p ea ting a com m o n m ista ke o f m an y b e h a v ­ ioural e co lo gists, w h o have ne g le cte d th e stu d y o f m ech an ism a t th e e xp e n se o f th e stu d y o f th e e v o lu ­ tio n a ry co sts and b e ne fits 1993). (Huntingford REFERENCES A le r s ta m , T., 1979. Optim al use of wind by m igrating birds: com bined drift and overcom pensation. J. theor. Biol. 79: 341-353. — , 1991. Bird flig h t and optim al m igration. Trends Ecol. Evol. 6: 210-215. A l e r s t a m , T., G .A . G u d m u n o s s o n & B. L a r s s o n , 1993. Flight tracks and speeds of Antarctic and Atlantic sea­ birds: radar and optical m easurem ents. Phil. Trans. R. Soc Lond. B 340: 55-67. A le r s ta m , T. & Â L in d s tr ö m , 1990. Optimal bird migration: the relative im portance of tim e, energy, and safety. In: E. G w in n e r. Bird Migration, Physiology and Ecophysiology. Berlin: Springer Verlag: 331 -351. B a k e r , R.R., 1978. The Evolutionary Ecology of Animal Migration. Holm es and Meier, New York. BEDNEKOFF, P.a. & A.I. H o u s t o n , in press. Risk-sensitive foraging, m ass-dependent predation, and prem igratory fattening. Ecology. B e r n s te in , C., A.J. K a c e ln ik & J R. K re b s , 1988. Individual decisions and the distribution o f predators in a patchy environm ent. J. Anim. Ecol. 57: 1007-1026. B e r n s te in , C ., A.J. K a c e ln ik & J.R. K re b s , 1991. Individual decisions of predators in a patchy environm ent. II. The influence of travel costs and structure of the environ­ m ent. J. Anim . Ecol. 60: 205-225. B e r t h o l d , P., 1988. Evolutionary aspects of m igratory behavior in European warblers. J. evol. Biol. 1: 195209. B e r t h o l d , P., A.J. H e lb ig , G. M o h r & U. Q u e r n e r , 1992. Rapid m icroevolution of m igratory behaviour in a wild bird species. Nature 360: 668-670. B e r tv io ld , p . & U. Q u e r n e r , 19 8 1 . G enetic basis of m igra­ tory behavior in European warblers. Science 212: 7779. B ie b a ch , H. W. F r ie d r ic h , G. H eine , L. J e n n i, S. J e n n iE ie rm a n n & D. S c h m id l, 1991. The daily pattern of autum n bird m igration in the northern Sahara. Ibis 133: 414-422. B ie s e l, W. & W. N a c h t ig a ll, 1987. Pigeon flight in a wind tunnel. IV. therm o-regulation and water homeostasis. J. comp. Physiol. B 157: 117-128. B o o t» , D.T., D.H. C la y t o n & B .A . F lo c k , 1993. Experim en­ tal dem onstration o f the energetic cost o f parasitism in free-ranging hosts. Proc. R. Soc Lond. B 25 3: 125-129. B r u d e r e r , B., in press. Nocturnal bird migration in the Negev - A tracking radar study. Ostrich 65. B r u d e r e r , B., L.G. U n d e r h i l l & F. U e c h ti, in press. A lti­ tude choice of night m igrants in a desert area predicted by m eteorological factors. Ibis 136. B r u d e r e r , B. & E. W e itn a u e r , 1972. Radarbeobachtungen über Zug und N achtflüge des M auerseglers (Apus apus). Rev. Suisse Zool. 79: 1190-1200. C a rm i, N., B. P in s h o w , W . P. P o r t e r & J. J a e g e r, 1992. W ater and energy lim itations on flight duration in small m igrating birds. Auk. 109: 268-276. C h r is t y , J.H. & M. S a l m o n , 1984. Ecology and evolution of m ating system s of fiddler crabs (genus Uca). Biol. Rev. 59: 483-509. C r is t o l , D.A., 1992. Food deprivation influences dom i­ nance status in dark-eyed juncos, Junco hyemalis. Anim . Behav. 43: 11 7 -1 2 4 . C r is t o l , D.A., MS. Experim entally delayed arrival of early fem ale red-winged blackbirds. C r is t o l , D.A., V .N . j r N o l a n & E.D. K e t t e r s o n , 1990. Effect of prior residence on dom inance status o f dark­ eyed juncos, Junco hyemalis. Anim . Behav. 40: 5 8 0 586. D a a n .S., C. D ij k s t r a , R.H. D r e n t & T. M e ij e r , 1989. Food supply and the annual tim ing of avian reproduction. In: Proceedings of the XlX th International Ornithological Congress, pp. 3 9 2 -4 0 7 . O ttawa, O ttaw a Press I. D a a n , S., C. D ij k s t r a & J.M . T in b e r g e n , 19 90. Family plan­ ning in the kestrel (Falco tinnunculus): the ultim ate con­ trol of covariation of laying date and clutch size Behaviour 114: 8 3 -1 1 6 . D r e n t , R.H. & T. P ie r s m a , 1990. An exploration of energet­ ics of leap-frog m igration in arctic breeding waders, pp. 3 9 9 -4 1 2 In: E. G w in n e r . Bird m igration: physiology and ecophysiology. Springer-verlag, Berlin. D r e n t , R.H. & H.H.T. P r in s , 1987. The herbivore as a p ris ­ o n e r o f its fo o d s u p p ly . C h a p te r 9 (pp. 1 3 1 -1 4 7 ). In: J. A n d e l et al. D is tu rb a n c e in G ra s s la n d s . Dr. W . J u n k P u b lis h e rs , D o rd re ch t. E b b in g e , B .S ., J .B . v a n B ie z e n & H. v a n d e r V o e t , 1991. E s tim a tio n o f a n n u a l a d u lt s u rv iv a l ra te s o f B a rn a cle G e e se Branta leucopsis u sin g m u ltip le re s ig h tin g s o f m a rke d in d ivid u a ls. A rd e a 79: 7 3 -1 1 2 . E e r d e n , M.R. v a n , 1984. W aterfowl m ovem ents in relation to food stocks. In: P R. E v a n s , J.D. G o s s -C u s t a r d & W .G . H a l e . Coastal waders and w ildfow l in winter, pp. 8 4 -1 0 0 . Cam bridge Univ. Press, Cam bridge. E e r d e n , M.R. v a n & M.J. M u n s t e r m a n , MS. Seed density van and waterlevel fluctuations determ ining food uptake and bird num bers in Teal (Anas crecca) stopping over at two freshw ater m arshes in the Netherlands. E n s , B.J. & J. C a y f o r d , in press. Feeding with other oystercatchers. In: J.D. G o s s -C u s t a r d . The behavioural ecology o f the oystercatcher. Oxford Univ. Press, Oxford. E n s , B.J. & J.D. G o s s -C u s t a r d , 1984. Interference among oystercatchers, Haem atopus ostralegus, feeding on m ussels, M ytilus edulis, on the Exe estuary. J. Anim. Ecol. 53: 2 1 7 -2 3 1 . E n s , B.J., H. vd Jeugd & D. Heg. M S 1 . Territory acquisition in the oystercatcher: vital preparations for a breeding career. E n s , B .J., M, K e r s t e n , A . B r e n n in k m e ij e r & J.B . H u l s C h e r , 1992. T e rrito ry q u a lity , p a re n ta l e ffo rt a n d re p ro ­ d u c tiv e s u c c e s s o f o y s te rc a tc h e rs ( Haem atopus ostralegus). J. A n im . E col. 6 1 : 7 0 3 -7 1 5 . E n s B.J., T. P ie r s m a , W.J. W o l f f & L. Z w a r t s , 1990. Hom eward bound: problem s waders face when m igrat­ ing from the Banc d'Arguin, M auritania, to their north­ ern breeding grounds in spring. Ardea 78: 1-16. E n s , B.J., F.J. W e is s in g & R.H. D r e n t , MS2. The despotic distribution and deferred m aturity: two sides of the sam e coin. E v a n s , P .R ., 1991. Seasonal and annual patterns of m ortal­ ity in m igratory shorebirds: som e conservation im plica­ tions. In: C.M. P e r r in s , J..D L e b r e t o n , G .J.M H ir o n s . Bird Population Studies: relevance to conservation and m anagem ent. Oxford: Oxford University Press: 3 4 6 359. F r e t w e l l , S., 19 72. Populations in a seasonal environm ent. Princeton: Princeton University Press. — , 1980. Evolution o f m igration in relation to factors regulat­ ing bird numbers. In: A. K e a s t , E.S. M o r t o n . Migrant Birds in the Neotropics: Ecology, Behavior, D istribu­ tion, and Conservation. W ashington: Smithsonian Press: 517-527. F r e t w e ll, S. & H.L. Lucas j r , 1970. On territorial behaviour and other factors influencing habitat distribution in birds. Acta, biotheoretica 19: 16-36. G a u t h r e a u x , S.A., 1982. The ecology and evolution of avian migration systems. In: D.S. F a r n e r , J.R. K in g , C.R. PARKES. Avian Biology, Vol IV. New York: A ca ­ demic Press: 93-168. GOSS-CUSTARD, J.D., 1980. Com petition for food and inter­ ference am ong waders. Ardea 68: 31-52. G o s s -C u s t a r d , J.D . & S . E.A. D u r e l l , 1988. The effect of dom inance and feeding m ethod on the intake rates of O ystercatchers, Haem atopus ostralegus feeding on m ussels, M ytilus edulis. J. Anim. Ecol. 57: 827-844. G o s s -C u s t a r d J.D . & S.E.A. D u r e l l , 1990. Bird behaviour and environm ental planning: approaches in the study of w ader populations. Ibis 132: 273-289. G o s s -C u s t a r d , J.D ., R.T. C l a r k e & S . E.A. D u r e l l , 1984. Rates of food intake and aggression o f oystercatchers H aem atopus ostralegus on the m ost and least pre­ ferred m ussel M ytilus edulis beds of the Exe estuary. J. Anim. Ecol. 53: 233-245. G o s s -C u s t a r d , J.D., R.G. C a l d o w , R.T. C la r k e & A.D. WEST, in press (e). Deriving population param eters from individual variations in foraging behaviour: II. Model tests and population param eters. J. Anim. Ecol. G o s s -C u s t a r d , J.D., R.G. C a l d o w , R.T. C l a r k e , S . E.A. D u r e l l & W .J. S u t h e r l a n d , in press (c). Deriving pop­ ulation param eters from individual variations in forag­ ing behaviour: I. Empirical gam e theory distribution m odel of oystercatchers H aem atopus ostralegus fe e d ­ ing on m ussels M ytilus edulis. J. Anim. Ecol. G o s s -C u s t a r d , J.D ., R.T. C l a r k e , K.B. B r ig g s , B.J. E n s , K-M. Exo, C .J. S m it , A.J. B e in t e m a , R.W.G. C a l d o w , C a t t DC , N. C l a r k , S . E.A. D u r e l l , M.P. H a r r is , J.B. H u l s c h e r , P .L. M e in in g e r , N. P ic o z z i , R. P r y s -J o n e s & U. S a f r ie l , in press (a). Modelling the effect of habi­ tat loss on the European population of the oystercatcher Haem atopus ostralegus: I. Estim ating m odel param eters. J. Appl. Ecol. G o s s -C u s ta rd , J.D., R.T. C la rk e , S.E.A. D u r e ll, R.W.G. C a ld o w RW G & B.J. Ens, in press (b). Predicting the effect of winter habitat loss on the European population of the oystercatcher Haem atopus ostralegus'. II. Model predictions. J. Appl. Ecol. G o s s -C u s t a r d J.D., A.D. W e s t , R .W .G . C a l d o w S.E.A. D u r e l l & S. M c G r o r t y , in press (d). An empirai opti­ mality m odel to predict the intake rates of oystercatch­ ers (Haem atopus ostralegus) feeding on mussels (Mytilus edulis). Ardea G r a f e n , A., 1987. The logic of divisively asym m etric con­ tests: respect for ow nership and the desperado effect. Anim. Behav. 35: 4 6 2 -4 6 7 . G u d m u n d s s o n , G..A, Â. L in d s t r ö m & T. A l e r s t a m , 1991. Optimal fat loads and long distance flights by migrating Knots Calidris canutus, Sanderlings C. alba, and T urn­ stones A renaria interpres. Ibis 133: 140-152. G w in n e r , E., 1990. Circannual rhythm s in bird m igration: control of tem poral patterns and interactions with pho­ toperiod. In: E. G w in n e r . Bird Migration, Physiology and Ecophysiology. Berlin: Springer Verlag: 257-268. H e d e n s t r ö M, A. & T. A l e r s t a m , 1992. Climbing perform ­ ance of migrating birds as a basis for estimating limits for fuel-carrying capacity and muscle work. J. exp. Biol. H e d e n s trö M , A. & T. A le r s ta m , 1994. Optimal clim bing flight in m igrating birds: predictions and observations of knot and turnstone. Anim. Behav., in press. H o lm e s , R.T. & T.W . S h e r r y , 1992. Site fidelity of m igratory w arblers in tem perate breeding and neotropical w inter­ ing areas: im plications for population dynam ics, habitat selection and conservation: 5 6 3 -5 7 5 . In: J.M. H a g a n III & D.W. J o h n s to n . Ecology and C onservation of Neo­ tropical M igrant Landbirds. Sm ithsonian Press, Wash. D.C. H o lm e s , R .T., T .W . S h e r r y & L. R e its m a , 19 89. Population structure, territoriality, and overw inter survival of two m igrant warbler species in Jamaica. Condor 91: 5 4 5 561. H o lm e s , R.T., T.W. S h e r r y , P.P. M a r r a & K.E. P e t it , 1992. M ultiple-brooding and annual productivity of a Neotropical m igrant passerine, The Black-throated Blue W arbler (Dendroica caerulescens) in an unfrag­ m ented tem perate forest. Auk 109: 321 -333. H o lm g re n , N., 1993. Patch selection, conflicting activities and patterns o f migration in birds. Doctoral Disserta­ tion, Lund University. H o u s to n , A.I. & J.M . M c N a m a r a , 1993. A theoretical in ve s­ tigation of the fa t reserves and m ortality levels of small birds in winter. O m is Scand. 24: 2 0 5 -2 1 9 . H o u s to n , A.I. & J.M . M c N a m a ra & J .M .C . H u tc h in s o n , 1993. General results concerning the trade-off between gaining energy and avoiding predation. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 3 4 1 : 3 7 5 -3 9 7 . H u n t in g f o r d , F., 1993. Behavioural m echanism s in evolu­ tionary perspective. Trends Ecol. Evol. 8: 81 -84. Je n n i, L. & S. J e n n i-E ie rm a n n , 1992. M etabolic patterns of feeding, overnight fasted and flying night m igrants d u r­ ing autum n migration. Ornis Scand. 23: 251 -259. J e n n i-E ie rm a n n , S. & L. Je n n i, 1991. M etabolic responses to flight and fasting in night-m igrating passerines. J. Com p.Physiol. B 161: 4 6 5 -4 7 4 . J e n n i-E ie rm a n n , S. & L. J e n n i, 19 92. High plasm a triglycer­ ide levels in sm all birds during m igratory flight: a new pathway for fuel supply during endurance locom otion at very high m ass-specific m etabolic rates? Physiol. Zool. 65: 1 1 2-12 3. J e n n i-E ie rm a n n , S. & L. Je n n i, in press. Plasm a m etabolite levels predict individual body-m ass changes in a small long-distance m igrant, the Garden W arbler. Auk. K a c e ln ik , A., J.R. K re b s & C. B e r n s te in , 1992. The ideal free distribution m odel and predator-prey populations. Trends Ecol. Evol. 7: 50 -55. K e t t e r s o n , E.D. & v. N o l a n J r , 19 83. The evolution o f dif­ ferential bird migration. Current O rnithology I: 3 5 7 -4 0 1 . K la a s s e n , M., in press. W ater and energy lim itation on flight range. Auk. K le ib e r , M., 1 9 6 1 . The fire of life. W iley, New York. K lu ijv e r , H.N. & L. T in b e rg e n , 1953. Territory and the regu­ lation of density in titm ice. Arch. Néerl. Zool. 10: 2 6 6 287. K r e b s , J.R., 1982. Territorial defence in the G reat T it (Parus major): Do residents always win? Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 11: 18 5-19 4. L e s s e lls , C.M., 19 91. The evolution of life histories. In: J.R. K re b s & N.B. D avies. Behavioural Ecology, an e vo lu ­ tionary approach. Third edition, pp: 4 7 1 -4 8 8 . Blackwell Scientific Publications, Oxford. Le vin s, R., 1968. Evolution in changing environm ents: som e theoretical explorations. Princeton Univ Press, Prince­ ton. LlECHTl, F., 1992. Flugverhalten nächtlich ziehender Vögel in A bhängigkeit von W ind und Topographie. PhD th e ­ sis, U niversity o f Basel. — , 1993. N ächtlicher Vogelzug im Herbst über S üddeutsch­ land: W inddrift und Kom pensation. J. Orn. 134: 373404. — , in press. Nocturnal m igrants adjusting air speed in rela­ tion to wind. Ibis 136. L in d s t r ö m , Â., 1991. M aximum fat deposition rates in m igrating birds. Ornis Scand 22: 12-19. L in d s t r ö m , A . & T. A l e r s t a m , 1992. Optimal fat loads in m igrating birds: a test of the tim e-m inim ization hypoth­ esis. A m . Nat. 140: 477-491. L o r ia , D.E. & F.R. M o o r e , 1990. Energy dem and of m igra­ tion on red-eyed vireos, Vireo olivaceous. Beh. Ecol. 1: 24-35. LUNDBERG, P., 1987. Partial bird m igration and evolutionary stable strategies. J. theor. Biol. 125: 351-360. LUNDBERG, S. & T. A l e r s t a m , 1986. Bird m igration patterns: conditions for stable geographical population segrega­ tion. J. theor. Biol. 123: 403-414. M a c A r t h u r , R.H., 1972. G eographical Ecology: patterns in the distribution o f species. New York: Harper & Row. M a d d o x , J,, 1993. Com petition and the death o f science. Nature 363: 667. M a n g e l , M. & C .W . C l a r k , 1986. Towards a unified fo ra g ­ ing theory. Ecology 67: 1127-1138. M a n g e l , M. & C .W . C l a r k , 1988. Dynam ic modeling in behavioural ecology. Princeton: Princeton University Press. M a r r a , P.P., T.W . S h e r r y & R.T. H o l m e s , 1993. Territorial exclusion by older m ales in Neotropical m igrant bird in winter: rem oval experim ents in Am erican Redstarts (Setophaga ruticilla). Auk. 110: in press. M a y n a r d S m it h , J „ 1982. Evolution and the theory of games. Cam bridge University Press, Cambridge. M c F a r l a n d , D .J. 1977. D e c is io n -m a k in g in a n im a ls . N atu re 269: 15 -21. M c N a m a r a , J.M . & A .I. H o u s t o n , 1986. T h e c o m m o n c u r­ re n c y fo r b e h a v io u ra l d e c is io n s . A m . N at. 127: 358- 378. M c N a m a r a , J.M. & A.I. H o u s t o n , 1992. State-dependent life-history theory and its im plications for optim al clutch size. Evol. Ecol. 6: 170-185. M o o r e , F.R., 1992. Ecophysiological and behavioral response to energy dem ands during migration. Acta X X Congressus Internationalis Ornithologie: 753-760. M o o r e , F.R., P. K e r l in g e r & T. S im o n s , 1990. Stopover on a G ulf coast barrier island by spring trans-G ulf m igrants. W ilson Bull. 102: 487-500. M o o r e , F.R. & P.A. S im m , 1986. Risk-sensitive foraging by a m igratory bird (D endroica coronata). Experientia 42: 1054-1056. MYERS, J.P., 1981. A test of three hypotheses for latitudinal segregation of the sexes in wintering birds. Can. J. Zool. 59: 1527-1534. O w e n , M. & J.M. B l a c k , 1989. Factors affecting the survival o f barnacle geese on m igration from the breeding grounds. J. Anim. Ecol. 57: 37-47. P a p i , F. & H.G. W a l r a f f (eds), 1982. Avian Navigation. Ber­ lin: Springer. P a r k e r , G .A ., 1974. A s s e s s m e n t s tra te g y an d th e e v o lu tio n o f fig h tin g b e h a v io u r. J. th e o r. Biol. 47: 2 2 3 -2 4 3 . P a r k e r , G.A. & W .J. S u t h e r la n d , 1986. Ideal free d istribu­ tions when individuals differ in com petitive ability: p he­ notype-lim ited ideal free models. Anim. Behav. 34: 1222-1242. P e n n y c u ic k , C.J., 1969. The m echanics of bird migration. Ibis 111: 525-556. — , 1978. Fifteen testable predictions about bird flight. Oikos 30: 165-176. — , 1989. Bird R ig h t Perform ance. Oxford: Oxford University Press. P ie n k o w s k i, M.W. & P.R. E v a n s , 1985. T h e ro le o f m ig ra ­ tio n in th e p o p u la tio n d y n a m ic s o f b ird s. In: R.M. S ib le & R.H. S m ith . B e h a v io u ra l E co lo g y: e c o lo g ic a l c o n s e ­ q u e n c e s o f a d a p tiv e b e h a v io u r. O xfo rd : B la ckw e ll S c i­ e n tific P u b lica tio n s: 331-351. P ie rs m a , T., 1987. Hop, skip or jum p? Constraints on m igra­ tion of arctic waders by feeding, fattening, and flight speed. Lim osa 60: 185-191. [In Dutch with English summ ary], P ie rs m a , T., R. D r e n t & P. W ie rs m a , 1991. Tem perate ve r­ sus tropical wintering in the w orld's northernm ost breeder, the knot: m etabolic scope and resource levels restrict subspecific options. Acta XX Internat. Ornithol. Congr. (Christchurch): 761-772. P ie rs m a , T. & B.J. E n s, 1992. O ptim al m igration schedules: reserve dynam ics as constraint. W ader Study Group Bull. 64: 17-18. P ie rs m a , T. & S. v a n de S a n t, 1992. Pattern and predicta­ bility of potential wind assistance for waders and geese m igrating from W est Africa and the W adden Sea to Siberia. Ornis Svecica 2: 55-66. P ie rs m a , T., L. Z w a r t s & J.H. B ru g g e m a n , 1990. Behav­ ioural aspects of the departure of waders before long­ distance flights: flocking, vocalizations, flight paths and diurnal tim ing. A rdea 78: 157-184. P ip e r, W .H., 1990. Site tenacity and dom inance in wintering white-throated sparrows Z onotrichia albicollis (Passeriform es: Em berizidae). Ethology 85: 114-122. P ip e r, W .H. & R.H. W ile y , 1989. C orrelates of dom inance in wintering white-throated sparrows: age, sex and lo ca ­ tion. Anim . Behav. 37: 298-310. P ip e r, W .H . & R.H. W ile y , 1990. C orrelates of range size in wintering white-throated sparrows, Zonotrichia albicol­ lis. Anim. Behav. 40: 545-552. R a y n e r, J.M .V ., 1988. Form and function in avian flight. Curr. Ornithol. 5: 1 -77. S h e r r y , T.W . & R.T. H o lm e s , 1992. Population fluctuations in a long-distance Neotropical m igrant: dem ogrpahic evidence for the im portance of breeding season events in the Am erican Redstart: 431-444. In: J.M. H a g a n III a n d D.W. J o h n s to n . Ecology and C onservation of Neotropical Migrant Landbirds (J.M. Hagan III and D.W. Johnston, eds.). Sm ithsonian Press, W ash. D.C. S h e r r y , T.W. & R.T. H o lm e s , in press. Sum m er versus w in­ ter lim itation of Neotropical m igrant landbird popula­ tions: w hat are the issues and w hat is the evidence? In: T. M a r t in & D. F in c h . Ecology and M anagem ent of N eotropical M igratory Birds: a Synthesis and Review of the Critical Issues. O xford University Press, Oxford. S h e r r y , T.W. & R.T. H o lm e s , M S. Patterns of winter habitat use in Nearctic-Neotropical m igrant birds and their im plications for population dynam ics and conservation. S te a r n s , S .C ., 1992. The evolution o f life histories. Oxford Univ. Press, Oxford. S u t h e r la n d , W .J. & P.M. D o lm a n , in press. Com bining behaviour and population dynam ics with applications for predicting consequences of habitat loss. Proc Royal Soc Series B. S u t h e r la n d , W.J. & G .A. P a r k e r , 1985. Distribution of une­ qual com petitors. In: R.M. S ib ly & R.H. S m ith . Behav­ ioural Ecology: ecological consequences of adaptive behaviour. Oxford: Blackwell Scientific Publications: 255-274. T in b e rg e n , J.M . & S. D aan, 1990. Fam ily planning in the great tit (Parus m ajoi): optim al clutch size as an inte­ gration o f parent and offspring fitness. Behaviour 114: 161-190. T u c k e r , V .A ., 1973. Bird m etabolism during flight: evalua­ tion of a theory. J. Exp. Biol. 58 : 689-709. WlERSMA, P. & T. PlERSMA, 1994. Effects o f m icrohabitat, flocking, clim ate and m igratory goal on energy expend­ iture in the annual cycle o f red knots. Condor 9 6 : 257279. Y app, W .B., 1962. Some physical lim itations in bird m igra­ tion. Ibis 104: 86-89. Z w a r t s , L., B.J. E n s, M. K e r s te n & T. P ie rs m a , 1990. Moult, m ass and flight range of waders ready to take off for long-distance m igrations. Ardea 78: 339-364. A P P E N D IX I: C O M P E TITIV E CO NCEPTS D uring th e w o rksh o p and the fo llo w in g g e sta tio n o f th is re p o rt it be ca m e c le a r th a t m y d is lik e o f so m e term s a nd co n c e p ts w a s n o t sha red by the m a jo rity o f pa rticip ants. S in ce m u d dled co n ce p ts w ill lead to m ud dled in vestig ation s, I fe lt in spire d to w rite th is ap pe nd ix, w h ere I try to m ake cle a r on w h a t basis I distin g u ish be tw ee n useful and co n fu sin g con cep ts. A c o n ce p t is con sid ered useful w h en it has a c le a r de fin itio n and, in princip al, a llo w s ta kin g m ea su re m e n ts. A co n ce p t is con sid ered co n fu sin g w h e n it has b e en use d in m a n y d if­ fe re n t w ays, even th o u g h it w a s o rig in a lly c le a rly defined. A co n ce p t is a lso co n sid e re d co n fu sin g if it has no cle a r m e a ning w h en a p plied to a real w orld situa tion , even tho ugh it m a y be p re cise ly d e fin e d in a m a th e m a ti­ cal m odel atte m p tin g to d e scrib e th a t real w o rld situation. USEFUL CONCEPTS social dominance Aggregate measure of success in winning encounters at a given location against a specified set of opponents, probably depending on both relative fighting abilities and payoffs of focal animal and oppo­ nents. It is often wrongly thought of, not as a relationship, but as the ability of an individual to dominate others. This is due to the habit of calculating dominance scores without making reference to the oppo­ nents or to the location (e.g. Ens & Goss -Custard 1984). fighting ability T he a b ility o f an in dividu al to w in fig h ts w h en payoffs and po sitio nal a d va n ta g e s are c o n tro lle d for. It sho uld not be co n fu se d w ith social dom inance, w h ich refers to a re la tio n sh ip and not a tru e a b ility o f an in dividu al. T hou gh the co n ce p t is e a sy to grasp, it is ra th e r eva sive w h en it co m e s to actu al m e a s u re ­ m ent. T his is be cau se it is ve ry hard to control fo r all the c o n fo u n d in g factors. payoff to winning a contest Increase or reduction in the reproductive potential of an individual when it either wins or loses a contest; should be measured as the change in Expected Future Reproductive Success (EFRS); see (1987). Though quickly understood on paper, it is very hard to actually measure. Measurement inevita­ bly involves many additional assumptions. it foraging ability T he a b ility o f an in dividu al to find food. It can be derived from w h a t Grafen Goss-Custard & Durell (1988) have ca lle d the Inte rfe re n ce Free Intake R ate (IFIR) by co n tro llin g fo r prey d e nsity. IFIR is m e a su re d as the rate o f fo o d in ta ke th a t an in dividu al ach ie ves in a certain lo catio n w h en it is n o t h in d e re d by oth er in dividu als. T here cou ld w e ll be a tra d e -o ff be tw ee n forag in g a b ility and fig h tin g ability, sin ce de sig nin g an a n im a l th a t is g o o d a t fig h tin g m ay co n flict w ith d e m a n d s th a t m ake the a n im a l g o o d a t e xp lo iting resources. In fid d le r cra b s fe m a le s have tw o sm all cla w s fo r p icking up food, w h ile in m ales on e o f the se fe e d in g cla w s is e n la rged to such an exte nt th a t it can no lo n g e r be used fo r fee d in g , b u t it is very effective in fig h tin g w ith con spe cifics, as well as in attractin g fe m a le s (Christy & Salmon 1984). It is m y hunch th a t in m a n y species, m ales m ay be be tte r a t figh ting o v e r reso urce s and less e ffic ie n t a t e x p lo it­ ing th e se reso urce s a t lo w reso urce le vels co m p a re d to fem ales. susceptibility to interference (STI) The rate at w h ich foo d in ta ke de clin e s w h en the nu m b e r o f co m p e tito rs in crea ses, m o s t o fte n m e a s ­ (Goss-Custard & Durell 1988). Ideally, it sho uld be co n tro lle d fo r prey density, b u t to m y kn o w le d g e th is has n e ve r been ured as the slo pe o f th e re la tio n sh ip be tw ee n intake rate and co m p e tito r d e n sity done. CONCEPTS CAUSING CONFUSION resource holding potential (RHP) W hen P a r k e r (1974) laun ched his th e o ry th a t the o u tcom e o f e n co u n te rs w ill de p e n d on both th e fig h t­ ing a b ilitie s (e.g. w e a p o n ry o r physical streng th) and the pa yoffs to the in dividu als involved, he used the w ord R e so u rce H olding Potential (RHP) instead o f figh ting ability. T hou gh in te nd ed as a cla rificatio n, the term R H P has given rise to m uch m isu n d e rsta n d in g s in m y o p in ion and sho uld, perhaps, be dro pp ed. A s fa r as I u n de rsta nd it, th e reason fo r a d op ting th e term RH P is th a t it also in clu d e s p o s i­ tion al a d van ta ge s. T w o kn ig h ts m ig h t b e n e fit e q u a lly from gaining acce ss to a p rince ss and m ig h t be e q u a lly good figh ters, but if one ha s to fig h t up hill the od ds are a g a in st him . S im ilarly, a m ale to a d m ay w ard o ff rival m a le s m ore e a sily w h en it is in a m p le xu s w ith the fe m a le tha n w h en he sw im s n e a r to her. Thus, w h e n using the w o rd fig h tin g ab ility instead o f reso urce ho ld ing potential, it is im p o rta n t to keep in m ind th a t the re could be positio nal a d van ta ge s, w h ich sho uld be un re la te d to the pa yoffs though. competitive ability M y princip al problem w ith the co n ce p t o f co m p e titive a b ility is th a t it crea te s th e im p re ssio n th a t it can be m e a su re d as a sin g le ch a ra cte r o f an individual. I thin k th a t to be high ly unlikely. As used by S u t h ­ e r l a n d & P a r k e r (1985), co m p e titive a b ility in dica tes the a b ility to acq uire re so u rce s w h e n com p etin g w ith o th e r in dividu als and it is m ore o r less eq ua te d to su sce p tib ility to in te rfe ren ce, i.e. th e rate at w h ich foo d in ta ke de clin e s w hen th e nu m be r o f c o m p e tito rs in crea ses. S u sce p tib ility to in te rfe re n ce d e pe nd s on local d o m in a n ce and it is ve ry likely th a t local d o m in a n ce d e pe nd s on both pa yoffs and fig h tin g ability. W h e re a s figh ting a b ility is a tru e a b ility o f an individual, so cia l d o m in a n ce is not. W e m ig h t d e cid e to e q u a te com p etitive a b ility to figh ting ability, but I th in k little w o uld be ga in ed from tw o te rm s fo r a sin g le concept. F urtherm ore, the p o in t a b o u t c o m p e titive a b ility is th a t it d e scrib e s th e s u c ­ cess in com p etin g for resources. W hen scram b le co m p e titio n is th e m o st im p o rta n t process, fo ra g in g ability, in ste ad o f figh ting ability, is w h a t m atters. W hen both co n te s t and scra m b le c o m p e titio n m atter, w e need to m ea sure both forag in g a b ility and figh ting a b ility and the po ssib le tra d e o ff be tw ee n th e se tw o a b ilitie s w o uld d e stro y a n y hope o f m ea surin g c o m p e titive a b ility as a sin g le cha racter. In fact, a tra d e o ff see m s in evita ble to me, since the a ttrib u te s th a t m ake an an im a l g o od a t fig h tin g are d iffe re n t from th e a ttrib u te s th a t m ake an an im a l good a t forag in g. (w ritten by B runo Ens) A P P E N D IX II: LIST O F PARTICIPANTS S. Â k e s s o n , D e p a rtm e n t o f A nim al E cology, U n ive rsity o f Lund, S -2 2 3 62 Lund, S w eden. P r o f . D r . T. A le r s t a m , D e p a rtm e n t o f A nim al E cology, U n ive rsity o f Lund, E kolog ihu set, S -2 2 3 62 Lund, S w eden. D r s . H. B a p t is t , R ijksw a te rsta at, DG W , P.O. Box 8039, 4 3 3 0 EA M iddelburg, T he N e th erland s. D r . J.J. B e u k e m a , NIO Z, P.O. Box 59, 1790 A B Den Burg (Texel), T he N e th erland s. D r . H. B ie b a c h , M ax P la nck In stitu t fü r V e rh a lte n sp h ysio lo g ie , D -8 13 8 A ndechs, G e rm a n y D r s . D. B o s , R ijksw a te rsta at, DG W , P.O. Box 8039, 4 3 3 0 EA M iddelburg, T he N e th erland s. T. B r e g n b a ll e , N ational E nviron m ental R e sea rch Institute, D e p ta rtm e n t o f W ild life E cology, Kalo, 8410 Ronde, D enm ark. D r . B. B r in k m a n , Instituu t v o o r Bos- en N a tu u ro n d e rzo e k (IB N -D LO ), P.O. Box 167, 1790 A D Den Burg (Texel), T h e N etherlands. D r . B. B r u d e r e r , S ch w e ize risch e V og elw arte, C H -6 2 0 4 Sem pach, S w itze rlan d D r s . J.H. B r u g g e m a n n , D e p a rtm e n t of M arine Biology, U n iversity o f G roningen, P.O. B ox 14, 9 7 5 0 A A Haren, The N etherlands. L. B r u in z e e l , Z o o lo g ica l La boratory, U n iversity o f G roningen, P.O. B ox 14, 9750 A A G ro n in g e n , T he N e th e r­ lands. D r s . L.S. B u u r m a , L u ch tm a ch tsta f hoofd se ctie n a tu u rlijk m ilieu, P.O. B ox 20 703, 2 5 0 0 ES Den Haag, The N e th erland s. D r s . N. C a d é e , R W S d ie n s t g e tijd ew a te ren , afd eling biologie, P.O. B ox 8039, 4 3 3 0 EA M id de lbu rg, T h e N e th ­ erlands. D r . R. C a l d o w , Institute o f T e rre stria l Ecology, F urzeb roo k R e sea rch S tation, W a reh am , D o rse t B H 2 0 5AS, UK. D r . D.A. C r is t o l , D e p a rtm e n t o f B iology, U n ive rsity o f Indiana, B lo o m in g to n IN 47405, Indiana, USA. D r . N. D a n k e r s , Instituu t vo o r Bos- en N a tu u ro n d e rzo e k (IB N -D LO ), P.O. Box 167, 1 7 90 AD D en Burg (Texel), T h e N etherlands. A. D e k in g a , N IO Z, P.O. Box 59 1790 A B Den Burg The N etherlands. S. D ie t r ic h , A m H e lge ha us 12, D -3 57 5 K irch ha in 7, G erm any. P.M . D o l m a n , S cho ol o f B io lo gica l S ciences, U n iversity o f East A nglia, N orw ich, N o rfo lk N R 4 7TJ, UK. D r s . M. v a n E e r d e n , RW S direktie F levoland, P.O. Box 600, 8 2 00 AP Lelystad, T he N e th erland s. D r s . M. E n g e lm o e r , V a kg ro e p G ed ra gsb iolog ie, R ijksu n ive rsite it G roningen, P.O. B o x 14, 97 50 A A Haren, The N etherlands. D r . B.J. E n s , In stitu u t voo r B o s -e n N a tu uro nde rzoe k (IBN -D LO ), P.O. Box 167, 1790 A D Den Burg (Texel), The N etherlands. P r o f . D r . P.R. E v a n s , U n ive rsity o f D urham , D e p a rtm e n t o f B io lo gica l S ciences, D urham DH1 3LE, UK. J. van G il s , V a kg ro e p G ed ra gsb iolog ie, R ijksu n ive rsite it G roningen, P.O. B ox 14, 9 7 5 0 A A H aren, T he N e th ­ erlands. D r s . P. d e G o e ij, NIO Z, P.O. Box 59, 1790 A B Den Burg (Texel), The N etherlands. D r . J.D. G o s s - C u s t a r d , In stitu te o f T e rrestria l Ecology, F urzeb roo k R esearch S tation, n e ar W a re h a m , D o rset B H 205 AS, UK. A. H e d e n s tr ö m , D e p a rtm e n t o f T h e o re tica l E cology, U n iversity o f Lund, E kolog ihu set, S -2 2 3 62 Lund, S w e ­ den. P r o f . D r . R.T. H o l m e s , D e p a rtm e n t o f B iological S ciences, D a rtm o uth C ollege, H anover, N e w H a m psh ire 03 75 5-35 76 , USA. D r . N. H o lm g r e n , D e p a rtm e n t o f T h e o re tica l E cology, E cology B uilding, U n ive rsity o f Lund, S -2 2 3 62 Lund, S w eden. D r . A.I. H o u s t o n , S cho ol o f B io lo gica l S ciences, W ood la nd Road, Bristol BS8 1UG, UK. H. H u is m a n n , F achb ere ich B io lo gie /C he m ie, U n iversitä t O snabrück, P.O. B ox 4469, 4 5 0 0 O sna brück, G e r­ m any. D r . L. J e n n i , S ch w e ize risch e V og elw arte, C H -620 4 Sem pach, S w itze rlan d. D r . S. J e n n i-E ie rm a n n , S ch w e ize risch e V og elw arte, C H -620 4 S em pach, S w itze rlan d. M. K e s t e n h o lz , S ch w e ize risch e V ogelw arte, C H -620 4 S em pach, S w itze rlan d. N. K j e l l é n , D e p a rtm e n t o f A nim al E cology, U n iversity o f Lund, S 2 2 3 62 Lund, S w eden. D r . M. K l a a s s e n , M ax P la nck Institut fü r V e rh a lte n sp h ysio lo g ie , D -8 23 46 A nd ech s, G erm any. A. K o o lh a a s , N IO Z P.O. B ox 5 9, 1 7 9 0 A B Den Burg (Texel), T h e N etherlands. D r . F. L ie c h ti, S ch w e ize risch e V og elw arte, C H -6 2 0 4 S em pach, S w itze rlan d. D r . Â . L in d s t r ö m , D e p a rtm e n t o f A nim al E cology, U n ive rsity o f Lund, E kologihuset, S -2 2 3 62 Lund, S w eden. D r s . J. v a n d e r M e e r , NIO Z, P.O. Box 59, 1790 A B Den Burg (Texel), The N etherlands. D r . C .D .T . M in t o n , R AO U , 21 G lad stone St., M oo ne e P onds, M elbourne, Vic. 3150, A ustralia. I. M i t c h e l l , D e p a rtm e n t o f B io lo gica l S ciences, South Road, D urham DH1 3LE, UK. T. M o o d e y , NER O U nit o f B ehavioural Ecology, D e p a rtm e n t o f Zoology, S ou th P arks Road, O xfo rd 0 X 1 3PS, UK. D r . F.R. M o o r e , D e p a rtm e n t o f B iological S ciences, U n ive rsity o f S o u thern M ississippi, B ox 5018, H a ttie s­ burg, M ississippi 39 40 6-50 18 , USA. P r o f . D r . M .L. M o r t o n , D e p a rtm e n t o f Biology, O ccide ntal C ollege, Los A ng eles C A 90 04 1-33 92 , USA. D r . A. v a n N o o r d w ijk , NIO O C e ntre fo r T errestria l Ecology, P.O. B ox 40, 66 66 ZG H eteren, T he N etherlands. D r . M .E. P e r e y r a , D e p a rtm e n t o f Biology, O ccide ntal C ollege, Los A ng eles, C A 90 041-3392, USA. D. P e t e r , S ch w e ize risch e V og elw arte, C H -620 4 S em p ach , Sw itzerland. D r . T. P ie rs m a , NIO Z, P.O. Box 59, 1790 A B Den Burg (Texel), T h e N etherlands. D r . B. P in s h o w , B la ustein Institute fo r D esert R esearch, S ed e B o q e r C am pus, 8499 3 Israel. D r s . M. P o o t , NIO Z, P.O. Box 59, 1790 A B Den Burg (Texel), T he N etherlands. J. S a lv ig , N ational E nviro n m e n ta l R esearch Institute, G renavej 12, Kalo, D K -8 41 0 Ronde, D enm ark. D r . I. S c o t t , D e p a rtm e n t o f B iological S ciences, S outh Road, D urham DH1 3LE, UK. H. S it t e r s , Institute o f T e rre stria l E cology, F urzeb roo k R esearch S tation, F urzeb roo k, W areh am , D orset B H 2 0 5AS, UK. D r s . C.J. S m it , Instituu t vo o r Bos- en N a tu u ro n d e rzo e k (IB N -D LO ), P.O. Box 167, 1790 A D Den B urg (Texel), T he N etherlands. R. S p a a r , S ch w e ize risch e V og elw arte, C H -6 2 0 4 Sem pach, S w itzerland. H. S t a r k , S ch w e ize risch e V og elw arte, C H -620 4 S em p ach , Sw itzerland. D r . J.M . T in b e r g e n , V a kg ro e p G ed ra gsb iolog ie, R ijksu n ive rsite it G roningen, P.O. B ox 14, 9 7 50 A A Haren, T h e N etherlands. D r s . I. T u l p , NIO Z, P.O. Box 59, 1790 A B Den Burg (Texel), T he N etherlands. K. U h le n b u s c h , F achb ere ich B io lo gie /C he m ie, U n iversitä t O snabrück, P.O. Box 4 4 6 9 , 4 5 0 0 O sna brück, G e r­ m any. A .J. U r f i , Institute o f T errestria l E cology, F u rze b ro o k R esearch S tation, F urzebrook, W a reh am , D o rse t B H 20 5AS, UK. D r s . N. V e r b o v e n , N IO O -C TO , B ote rhoe ksestr. 22, P.O. B ox 40, 6666 ZG H eteren, T he N e th erland s. D rs . Y. V e r k u il , NIO Z, P.O. B ox 59, 1790 A B Den Burg (Texel), T he N etherlands. T.P. W e b e r , N E R C U nit o f B ehavioural E cology, D e p a rtm e n t o f Zoology, South P arks Road, O xford 0 X 1 3PS, UK. D r s . P. W ie rs m a , V a kg ro e p G ed ra gsb iolog ie, R ijksu n ive rsite it G roning en, P.O. B ox 14, 9 7 5 0 A A Haren, T he N etherlands. P r o f . D r . W .J. W o l f f , IB N -D LO , P.O. B ox 46, 3956 ZR Leersum , T h e N etherlands. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 Reto Spaar Jacob Saivig Clive Minton Dan Cristol Theunis Piersma Kerstin Uhlenbusch Nils Kjéllen Marcel Klaassen M ennobart van Eerden Bert Brinkman Sabine Dietrich Paul Dolman Herbert Biebach Yvonne Verkuil Frank Moore Bruno Bruderer Karin Krijgsveld Cor Smit Thom as Bregnballe Luit Buurma Jan van der Kamp Jaap van der Meer 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 Ian Mitchell Martin Poot Ian Scott Maria Pereyra Martin Morton Sussanne Âkesson Tony Moodey Anita Koolhaas Eric Stienen Noël Holmgren Paul Ruiters Petra de Goeij Niels Cadée Meinte Engelm oer Susi Jenni-Eierm ann Alasdair Houston Hans Schekkerman Thomas W eber Daan Bos Âke Lindström Humphrey Sitters Lukas Jenni 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 61 62 63 64 65 66 Arie van Noordwijk Bruno Ens W im W olff Richard Caldow Dick Holmes Heike Huism ann Jan van Gils John Goss-Custard Leo Bruinzeel Popko W iersm a Felix Liechti Ingrid Tulp Dieter Peter Peter Evans Abdul Jamil Urfi Herbert Stark Henrich Bruggemann Berry Pinshow M athias Kestenholz Joost Tinbergen Thom as Alerstam Anders Hedenström CONTENTS Preface......................................................................................................................................................................1 Towards predictive models of bird migration schedules: theoretical and empirical bottlenecks....................3 Introduction........................................................................................................................................................ 3 Defining the problem .........................................................................................................................................4 The migratory flig h t...........................................................................................................................................5 Flight mechanics..........................................................................................................................................5 Energy and other potentially limiting resources during m igration.......................................................... 6 The migratory journey: (re)fuelling, departing and flying.............................................................................. 8 Physiology of making and using nutrient stores...................................................................................... 8 Consistency in allometric relationships of physiological and ecological variables...............................9 Optimal departure fat loads........................................................................................................................9 Optimal return jou rn e y..............................................................................................................................10 Optimal decisions and stochasticity.........................................................................................................11 Measuring the fitness consequences of arrival date............................................................................. 12 Which models are best?.................................................................................................................................13 The full annual cycle as an individual decision............................................................................................13 Population dynamics of migratory species...................................................................................................14 Density-dependent reproduction..............................................................................................................14 Density-dependent winter survival.......................................................................................................... 15 The cost of m igration................................................................................................................................16 Predicting the effects of habitat change.......................................................................................................17 Conclusions................................... ................................................................................................................. 18 Some important and hopefully amenable problem s............................................................................. 18 Migratory birds provide good research systems....................................................................................18 Prospects...................................................................................................................................................18 References...................................................................................................................................................... 19 Appendix I: Competitive concepts...................................................................................................................... 23 Useful concepts..............................................................................................................................................23 Concepts causing confusion..........................................................................................................................24 Appendix II: List of participants...........................................................................................................................22