Inaugural Article: Parallel information processing channels created in retina The MIT Faculty has made this article openly available. Please share how this access benefits you. Your story matters. Citation Schiller, Peter H. “Parallel information processing channels created in the retina.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 107.40 (2010): 17087 -17094. © 2010 by the National Academy of Sciences As Published http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1011782107 Publisher National Academy of Sciences (U.S.) Version Final published version Accessed Wed May 25 18:21:25 EDT 2016 Citable Link http://hdl.handle.net/1721.1/62298 Terms of Use Article is made available in accordance with the publisher's policy and may be subject to US copyright law. Please refer to the publisher's site for terms of use. Detailed Terms INAUGURAL ARTICLE Parallel information processing channels created in the retina Peter H. Schiller1 Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA 02139 This contribution is part of the special series of Inaugural Articles by members of the National Academy of Sciences elected in 2007. In the retina, several parallel channels originate that extract different attributes from the visual scene. This review describes how these channels arise and what their functions are. Following the introduction four sections deal with these channels. The first discusses the “ON” and “OFF” channels that have arisen for the purpose of rapidly processing images in the visual scene that become visible by virtue of either light increment or light decrement; the ON channel processes images that become visible by virtue of light increment and the OFF channel processes images that become visible by virtue of light decrement. The second section examines the midget and parasol channels. The midget channel processes fine detail, wavelength information, and stereoscopic depth cues; the parasol channel plays a central role in processing motion and flicker as well as motion parallax cues for depth perception. Both these channels have ON and OFF subdivisions. The third section describes the accessory optic system that receives input from the retinal ganglion cells of Dogiel; these cells play a central role, in concert with the vestibular system, in stabilizing images on the retina to prevent the blurring of images that would otherwise occur when an organism is in motion. The last section provides a brief overview of several additional channels that originate in the retina. midget channels | ON/OFF channels | parasol channels I n the past 120 years, great strides have been made in uncovering how the brain processes visual information. Several of the discoveries were quite surprising and unexpected. It was found that, in the retina, the photoreceptors faced away from the incoming light and were closest to the pigment epithelium. All the other cell types in the retina were positioned between the photoreceptors and the vitreous humor. This meant that, except for the central foveal region, the incoming photons had to pass through all these cells and their processes before impinging on the photoreceptors. It was also discovered that there are two distinct photoreceptor types, the rods and the cones. When Schultze established this in the 1860s with anatomical methods he had developed (1), his findings were met with considerable skepticism. Undaunted, he went on to ask what the reason might be for having two different kinds of photoreceptors. He noted that the rods are absent in the fovea and that night vision is poor when incoming light is confined to this region. So he proposed that the rods are for night vision, a fact now fully accepted. Further studies have identified several distinct classes of cells in the retina that included the horizontal cells, the bipolar cells, the amacrine cells, and the ganglion cells that send their axons to the central nervous system exiting the eye at the optic disk. Studies have yielded yet another unexpected discovery, showing that there are several distinct classes of retinal ganglion cells. The use of the Golgi stain by its inventor, Camillo Golgi, and by Santiago Ramon y Cajal, was central in establishing these facts (2). For the numerous remarkable findings they made about the organization of the brain, Golgi and Cajal received the Nobel Prize in 1906. The subsequent emergence of physiological methods has made it possible to examine the functions of the neurons in the retina. The first to do so was Haldan Keffer Hartline, who developed procedures that enabled him to record the electrical activity of www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1011782107 individual retinal ganglion cells (3). Doing so, he discovered three classes of retinal ganglion cells: “ON” cells that discharged vigorously when the retina was illuminated, “OFF” cells that discharged when the light was turned off, and ON/OFF cells that responded transiently to both the onset and the termination of light. He also established that each cell is sensitive to only a small area of illumination on the retinal surface, a region called the receptive field of the cell. Hartline received the Nobel Prize for these discoveries in 1967. Subsequently, Steven Kuffler discovered in the cat, as did Horace Barlow in the frog, that the receptive field of each ganglion cell is actually composed of two regions that are concentrically arranged: an excitatory center area and an antagonistic surround region (4, 5). Stimulating the center with a small spot elicited a vigorous response, whereas a larger spot that impinged also on the surround produced an attenuated response. Stimulation of the surround alone with an annulus produced a weak response of the opposite polarity. The ON/OFF cells gave vigorous transient responses to both the onset and the termination of light when confined to the center of their receptive fields; in these cells, the responses were also attenuated when the size of the stimulus was increased. Similar results were obtained subsequently in many species suggesting that the ON, OFF, and ON/OFF cells of the retina and their antagonistic center-surround organization form a basic ground plan for the visual systems of all vertebrates (6–9). ON and OFF Channels Origins of the ON and OFF Channels in the Retina. In the 1960s, it became possible to place finely drawn micropipettes inside the various cell types of the retina (10, 11). Such intracellular recordings provided accurate measurements of the graded potentials produced by the neurons and made possible their subsequent anatomic identification by minute dye injections. Studies using these procedures, beginning with the pioneering work in John Dowling’s laboratory, revealed that the basic organization of the retina in vertebrate species is quite similar (12–16). The photoreceptors in all cases hyperpolarize to light and use the neurotransmitter glutamate, which acts differently on the two major classes of bipolar cells: OFF bipolars, which have sign-conserving ionotropic receptors (iGluRs), and ON bipolars, which have sign-inverting metabotropic receptors (mGluR6). Thus, the OFF cells polarize in the same manner as do the photoreceptors whereas the ON bipolars polarize in the direction opposite to the photoreceptors. Fig. 1A shows this arrangement schematically. The majority of cones in central retina connect with at least two bipolar cells, one of which is ON and the other OFF. The ON and OFF bipolars in turn connect with ON and OFF retinal ganglion cells, respectively. Further complexities in this arrangement will be described later. Author contributions: P.H.S. designed research, performed research, and wrote the paper. The author declares no conflict of interest. Freely available online through the PNAS open access option. 1 E-mail: phschill@mit.edu. This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10. 1073/pnas.1011782107/-/DCSupplemental. PNAS | October 5, 2010 | vol. 107 | no. 40 | 17087–17094 NEUROSCIENCE Contributed by Peter H. Schiller, August 13, 2010 (sent for review July 10, 2010) A B C Fig. 1. (A) The basic connections a cone makes with ON and OFF ganglion cells via ON and OFF bipolar cells. The ON bipolar cells have sign inverting synapses as described in the text. (B) The basic connections rods, cones, bipolar cells, horizontal cells, and amacrine cells make in the retina. [Reprinted from Progress in Retinal and Eye Research, Vol 15, Schiller PH, The ON and OFF channels of the mammalian visual system, 173–195, Copyright (1995), with permission from Elsevier]. (C) The responses of an ON and OFF monkey LGN cell before, 17088 | www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1011782107 The intracellular recording studies have also revealed that photoreceptors, horizontal cells, and bipolar cells produce only graded potentials. Center-surround organization appears at the level of the bipolar cell (11). Action potentials are first seen in some amacrine cells and are produced by all ganglion cells. The axons of the ganglion cells project to a number of different nuclei in the brain, the largest of which, in primates, is the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) of the thalamus. This is a structure with several lamina in which the left and right eye inputs are segregated. In addition, there is a propensity for the various retinal ganglion cell types to terminate in different layers (6, 8, 9, 17). In a few species, there is a clear or partial segregation of the ON and OFF cells in the LGN (8, 9, 18, 19). Neurons from the LGN project to the visual cortex. Although the receptive field characteristics of LGN neurons are similar to those of retinal ganglion cells, in the cortex several new properties are found (6, 20); the majority of cells in primary visual cortex show selectivity for the orientation of edges, and many cells respond selectively to the direction of motion of these edges. Anatomical studies have revealed that the ON and OFF bipolar cells are distinct structurally and make connections in the inner retina in different subregions of the inner plexiform layer (IPL), as depicted in Fig. 1B; the OFF bipolars make contact with OFF retinal ganglion cell arbors in sublamina a of the IPL and ON bipolars make contact with ON ganglion cells in sublamina b of the IPL (7, 16, 21). The layout of the ON and OFF retinal ganglion cells has been effectively examined in retinal whole mounts, in which the retina is viewed as a flattened preparation. The dendritic arbors of the ON and OFF ganglion cells, once properly labeled, can readily be identified because they come into focus at different depths within the specimen. Using such methods, Heinz Wässle and collaborators discovered that the spacing of the arbors within each of the ON and OFF cell classes, but not between them, is highly orderly (22). This systematic arrangement suggests that the ON and OFF systems provide separate, and probably independent, coverage of the visual field. The synapses bipolar cells make in the inner retina are glutamatergic. Horizontal and amacrine cells come in many different subtypes and use a variety of neurotransmitters that include gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA), glycine, and acetylcholine. The diversity of horizontal and amacrine cell types appears to decrease in higher mammals in which more complex visual analysis is increasingly relegated to neocortex. That nature has gone to such complexities to create the ON and OFF systems suggests that great benefits must be reaped for the analysis of visual information. The initial ideas were that the ON system signals the onset of light and the OFF system the termination of light. As first suggested by Jung (23), another idea that has gained some degree of acceptance was that the ON system gives rise to the perception of brightness and the OFF system to the perception of darkness. A third consideration was advanced by more physiologically minded investigators, who suggested that the ON and OFF systems have evolved to provide for the antagonistic center-surround organization of receptive fields in retinal ganglion cells and in the cortex (24). Two major schemes have been proposed as to how the preganglionic neural elements of the retina connect to the ganglion cells to yield their antagonistic center/ surround organization. According to the first, the surround mechanism is supplied by the lateral connections of the horizontal and amacrine cells of the retina. According to the second, the center-surround arrangement of the retinal ganglion cells is produced by convergent connections from ON and OFF bipolars (8, 9, 25). The data gathered during the past few years in primates favor the idea that those cells of the retina that can be unequivocally identified as being ON-center or OFF-center types form separate pathways and do not interact extensively. The surround mecha- during, and after the infusion of APB into the eye. APB blocks the ON cell response and has no significant effect on the OFF cell (25). (Reprinted by permission from Macmillan Publishers Ltd: Nature, 297:580–583, copyright 1982.) Schiller Physiological Studies Using 2-Amino-4-Phosphonobutyrate. The use of a glutamate neurotransmitter agonist has provided an important line of support for the claim that the ON- and OFF-center ganglion cells form independent pathways to the central nervous system. This substance, 2-amino-4-phosphonobutyrate (APB), was discovered by Slaughter and Miller (32) to act differently on the ON and OFF bipolar cells of the mudpuppy (Necturus maculosus) retina. By decreasing membrane conductance, APB produces prolonged hyperpolarization in the ON bipolar cells, as a result of which they become unresponsive to subsequent light stimulation (32, 33). Current evidence shows that ON bipolar cells have a specific metabotropic glutamate receptor termed mGluR6 that is coupled to a G protein. The activation of this receptor leads to the closing of cGMP-gated cation channels by increasing the rate of cGMP hydrolysis by a G protein-mediated process (34). The mGluR6 receptor responds selectively to L-2-amino-4-phosphonobutyrate (35). In a remarkable recent study, Masu et al. (36) have succeeded in generating KO mice that lacked mGluR6 expression and hence lacked the ON system. This work further attests to the unique and distinct nature of the ON bipolar cells and the ON system in the mammalian retina. Schiller PNAS | October 5, 2010 | vol. 107 | no. 40 | 17089 INAUGURAL ARTICLE Except for a transient initial excitatory response, the OFF bipolars are largely unaffected by APB. As there is no ready uptake mechanism for this artificial substance, it is effective in small quantities and is broken down slowly. It seems to have little or no direct effect on the synaptic networks of the inner plexiform layer. Effect of APB on the responses of single cells in the mammal. To determine the effect of APB on the mammalian visual system, several investigators have applied this substance to the retina while recording from various visual structures. It became immediately evident that the effects of APB noted in the mudpuppy are similar in the mammalian retina. When APB is administered in appropriate concentrations to the monkey eye, the ON response in retinal ganglion cells is abolished whereas the OFF response is largely unaffected (9, 24, 25, 37). Fig. 1C shows recordings made from an ON and OFF cell in the LGN before, during, and after the infusion of APB into the retina. The ON cell stops responding to the visual stimulus presented in the receptive field of the cell whereas the OFF cell continues to respond. Having established that APB blocks the ON system in the monkey as it does in the mudpuppy, investigators moved on to determine to what extent the ON and OFF bipolar cells make interactive connections with the retinal ganglion cells to produce their antagonistic center-surround organization. To do so, the center and the surround of receptive fields can be selectively stimulated before and after APB administration. Fig. 2 shows a typical result obtained by recording in the parvocellular LGN of a monkey. The cells shown had color-opponent characteristics and were consequently stimulated with colored spots that preferentially activated the center and the surround mechanism. The center mechanism was activated with a small spot of the appropriate wavelength composition; the surround mechanism was activated briefly both when the center was stimulated and during the off cycle using a large spot of a different wavelength composition appropriate for the surround. The inhibitory effect of the surround stimulation is evident for both the ON and the OFF cell under normal conditions. Following administration of APB, the responses of the ON cell are eliminated whereas the responses of the OFF cell, including the center-surround antagonism produced by the stimulus that activated the surround mechanism, are mostly unaffected. These findings support the idea that the center-surround organization of retinal ganglion cells and LGN cells is not a product of ON and OFF cell interactions, but rather a product of the lateral networks of the retina that involve the horizontal and perhaps the amacrine cells. Thus the model of retinal organization these findings support is that the ON- and OFF-center ganglion cells, most of which send their axons to the LGN, form separate pathways (25, 37, 38). Also favoring this model is an elegant study by Mangel and Miller (39) in which hyperpolarizing current, injected intracellularly into rabbit horizontal cells, produced a decrease in the firing rate of ON ganglion cells and an increase in OFF ganglion cells. Effects of APB on ON/OFF cells. The ON/OFF cells that respond transiently to both light increment and light decrement comprise the third category of retinal ganglion cells that had been identified by Hartline (3); these cells are thought to arise as a result of convergent input from the ON and OFF bipolar cells onto individual retinal ganglion cells where the dendritic arbors of the ON/OFF ganglion cell are bistratified. In the primate retina, the ON/OFF retinal ganglion cells project extensively to the superior colliculus (40). In many species, such cells also project into the geniculostriate system (6, 41). Retinal administration of APB eliminates the response to light increment but not to light decrement in collicular cells (42). Effects of APB on single cells in area V1. The retinal input through the LGN to primary visual cortex, commonly referred to as V1, is transformed in a number of ways, as already noted, yielding neurons that are orientation and direction-specific (6, 20). Many of the neurons receive convergent input from the left and right eyes. V1 neurons are most effectively activated by edges with specific orientations. The characteristics of V1 neurons were first revealed by Hubel and Wiesel (20), who received the Nobel Prize in 1981. NEUROSCIENCE nism of these cells is a product of the lateral connections in the retina rather than a product of an interactive connection between the ON and OFF bipolar cells. In the following sections, three lines of evidence will be considered in support of this view, which come from anatomical, physiological, and behavioral studies. When Ramon y Cajal et al. (2) examined the organization of the retina after the duality of the rod and cone photoreceptors had been established by Schultze (1), the evidence he gathered suggested to him that these two systems form separate pathways to the central nervous system. Subsequent studies have established, however, that, for the most part, where the rods and cones coexist in any region of the retina, their signals converge upon the retinal ganglion cells (26, 27). Furthermore, current studies have shown that, unlike for the cones, the rod system in some mammals—including the cat, monkey, and human—has only ON-type rod bipolars. This has raised some interesting questions about the manner in which the rods might make their connections with the retinal ganglion cells. The nature of this connectivity has been worked out in some detail recently. The rod photoreceptor cells form sign-inverting synapses with the rod ON bipolars (28). Examination of the connections of the rods has also established that each rod ON bipolar cell contacts many rod photoreceptors; thus the rods have been said to pool their signals and, by doing so, form relatively large receptive field centers (29). In the inner retina of the cat and probably the monkey, the rod bipolars terminate in the inner portions of the inner plexiform layer, but do not contact ganglion cells directly. Instead, they terminate on amacrine cells, most commonly on the AII type. Each of these amacrine cells in turn makes two major kinds of contacts: one is a gap synaptic junction with ON cone bipolar cells and the other is a glycinergic synaptic junction with OFF ganglion cells, as depicted in Fig. 1B (28–30). This arrangement appears to accomplish for the rods in the inner retina what is accomplished for the cones in the outer retina: it turns the single-ended photoreceptor system into a double-ended one. As a result of these connections, under darkadapted conditions when the cones are unresponsive, the ON ganglion cells are excited by light increment and the OFF ganglion cells by light decrement. The organization of the receptive fields under light- and darkadapted conditions is quite different, as was first shown by Barlow et al. (31). This difference, which was subject to considerable incredulity at the time, is now readily understandable. The receptivefield center under dark-adapted conditions is driven by the rods through the bipolar and amacrine cells. The surround mechanism is the product of those portions of bitufted horizontal cells that make lateral connections only with the rods (16). As a result of this arrangement, several superimposed receptive fields can be created for any given patch of retina. 10 0 On-Center LGN Cell -- Normal 80 60 40 20 time 5 00 1000 1500 2000ms On-Center LGN Cell -- APB 100 80 60 40 Number of spikes 20 ti me 100 500 1 0 00 15 00 2000ms Off-Center LGN Cell -- Normal 80 60 40 20 time 100 500 1000 1 50 0 2000ms Off-Center LGN Cell -- APB 80 60 40 20 ti me 5 00 10 00 1500 2000ms R F c e n te r s tim u la tio n R F s u r r o u n d s t im u l a tio n Fig. 2. The responses of an ON and an OFF cell in the monkey LGN before and after APB infusion into the eye when the center and the surround of the receptive fields are stimulated with red and green spots of different sizes. The surround activated by the large green spot is turned on briefly during both the on and off cycles of the small red spot. APB application (Lower) silences the ON ganglion cell for both center and surround stimulation. The response of the OFF ganglion cell is unaffected; the cell retains its centersurround antagonism (25). (Reprinted by permission from Macmillan Publishers Ltd: Nature, 297:580–583, copyright 1982.) Two major classes of cells they have identified in area V1 are the simple and complex cells. Simple cells have receptive fields in which there are spatially distinct ON and OFF regions, whereas in complex cells the ON and OFF regions overlap spatially (6, 20, 43). When a properly oriented bar is swept across the receptive field of V1 complex cells, a response is elicited by both the light and dark edges of the bar as it sweeps across the receptive field. This is shown in Fig. S1A. When the ON channel is inactivated by APB infusion into the eye, the light edge response is eliminated but the dark edge response remains. This indicates that the light edge response is caused by the input from the ON system and the dark edge response to a result of the input from the OFF system. It has been proposed that the direction and orientation specificity of V1 neurons is a product of the interaction between the ON and OFF systems. An alternative hypothesis proposed was that inhibitory intracortical circuitry achieves these attributes in V1 cells (6, 20, 43). Inactivation of the ON channel with APB shows that direction specificity and orientation selectivity remain unaffected. Fig. S1B shows the responses of a complex cell in monkey V1 that under normal conditions responds preferentially to 17090 | www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1011782107 downward motion. After APB infusion, the leading light edge response is blocked but the directional selectivity of the cell persists. In Fig. S1C, orientation tuning curves are shown before and after APB infusion. The orientation tuning function is unaffected by the APB block. These findings indicate that orientation and direction specificities are a product of intracortical circuitry rather than interactions between the ON and OFF channels. That retinal APB infusion does not significantly affect orientation and direction selectivities in area V1 has also been established in the cat by Sherk and Horton (44). Behavioral Studies of ON and OFF Channels Using APB. Further insights into the role of the ON and OFF channels in vision come from behavioral studies in which the visual capacities of animals were assessed before and after the ON channel was blocked with APB. In monkeys, the most notable deficit observed under lightadapted conditions was for the detection of stimulus spots made visible by virtue of increases in light energy; that is, spots that are perceived as brighter than the background (9, 24, 45, 46). There was little or no deficit in the detection of spots that were darker than the background. Examples of this are shown in Fig. 3, in which both percent correct and reaction-time data are presented. Following APB administration the percent correct responses for the detection of light increments decreases dramatically, which is accompanied by large increases in the response latencies; performance with stimuli seen by virtue of light decrement is unaffected. These observations fit with the idea that the ON channel is sensitive to light incremental stimuli and signals lightness, whereas the OFF channel is sensitive to light decrement and signals darkness—an idea, as already noted, that had been advanced earlier by Jung (23). Behavioral studies in monkeys have also shown that, under dark-adapted conditions, when only the rods are functional, APB blocks the detection of stimuli seen by virtue of either light increment or light decrement; in other words, the rod system is blocked in its entirety and, consequently, the animal becomes night-blind (9, 47). These findings provide additional support for the claim that the rod bipolars in the primate are all ON types. Midget and Parasol Channels Anatomy and Physiology of Midget and Parasol Systems. In 1966, Enroth-Cugell and Robson (48) published an influential article showing that, in the cat, they distinguished three distinct classes of retinal ganglion cells, which they called the X, Y, and W. Subsequently, it has been found that similar subclasses of retinal ganglion cells exist in most mammalian species (49, 50). In the primate, two of these classes were named the midget and parasol systems. The ganglion cells of the midget system, as the name implies, are quite small. The parasol cells, by contrast, are much larger, with their dendritic arbors and receptive field diameters being approximately three times greater than the midget cells. The axons of the parasol ganglion cells are larger and have faster conduction velocities than do the axons of the midget cells. Fig. 4A provides a cross-sectional view of the midget and parasol cells as created by Polyak (51). Fig. 4B shows the dendritic arbor arrangement of these cells as seen in a whole-mount preparation by Watanabe and Rodiek (52). The midget and parasol cells come in both ON and OFF subvarieties. The input to the receptive field center of the midget cells of the monkey is composed of a single cone in the central retina. Thus, a single cone will give rise to both an ON and OFF bipolar cell, as shown in Fig. 1 A and B. By contrast, the receptive field center of the parasol cells is comprised of several cones that are not segregated according to type (16, 50). The receptive field sizes of these two classes of cells are quite different as are their inputs. The excitatory center of the midget cells in central retina is comprised of a single cone, thereby rendering them color-selective. The excitatory center of the parasol ganglion cell receptive fields is comprised of several cones, as is the surround, indicating that these cells cannot convey information about color. The parasol cells, however, are not insensitive to stimuSchiller Midget cells Parasol cells INAUGURAL ARTICLE A Light Increment 100 Before APB injection 60 60 40 40 30 20 20 Mean = 253ms 10 After APB injection 60 100 80 60 50 30 20 20 Mean = 406ms 10 100 200 300 400 500 600 700 Light Decrement Percent Correct Detection 80 Saccadic latency distribution Before APB injection 60 50 60 40 40 30 20 20 Mean = 249ms 10 After APB injection 60 100 80 parasol cell dendritic arbor Fig. 4. (A) Cross-section of Golgi-stained midget and parasol cells (51). [Reprintedwith permission from Polyak SL (1957) The vertebrate visual system: Its origin, structure, and function and its manifestations in disease with an analysis of its role in the life of animals and in the origin of man, preceded by a historical review of investigations of the eye, and of the visual pathways and centers of the brain (University of Chicago Press, Chicago).] (B) The dendritic arbors of a midget and parasol cell at comparable eccentricities (52). [Reprinted from Watanabe M, Rodieck RW (1989) Parasol and midget ganglion cells of the primate retina. J Comp Neurol 289:434–454 (Copyright 1989 with permission from Wiley).] Reaction Time in milliseconds 100 midget cell dendritic arbor 40 40 50 60 40 40 30 20 20 Mean = 251ms 10 100 200 300 400 500 600 700 Reaction Time in milliseconds Fig. 3. Percent correct performance and eye-movement reaction times made to light and dark stimuli presented in various locations of the visual field to which a trained rhesus monkey had to make saccadic eye movements to be rewarded. Following APB administration, percent correct performance declines and reaction times increase to a bright spot. Percent correct responses and reaction times to a dark spot are unaffected. lation by different wavelengths; they are capable of responding to chrominance changes as long as the stimuli contain reasonable color contrast. The signals so generated can lead to the perception of stimuli without leading to specific color sensations. One might imagine that, to the parasol system, the world looks a bit like a black-and-white movie. It is generally believed that the midget and parasol systems have their own set of ON and OFF bipolars. This arrangement in the midget and parasol cells makes it evident that there is a much higher number of bipolar cells in the primate retina than there are cone photoreceptors, yielding a ratio greater than three to one (16, 53). The receptive fields of the parasol ganglion cells at any given retinal eccentricity are approximately three times Schiller B 50 larger in diameter, as are their dendritic arbors, and they respond to visual stimuli in a transient fashion; midget cells, by contrast, produce a rather sustained response. The ability to experience different colors lies within the domain of the midget system (54–58). Until recently it was believed that the midget system is organized in a pure color-opponent fashion. Thus it was assumed that any given midget cell whose receptive field center is made up of a long-wavelength cone, has a surround that is driven by middle-wavelength cones. Serious questions have been raised during the past few years about the validity of this inference. It has been shown that the horizontal cells of the retina, even for the midget system, are “promiscuous”: they make contact with all cones within the area they sample (16). If color opponency is the product of interaction between the center and the surround mechanism created by the horizontal cells, it appears that the opponency is not pure; it does not arise by pitting red cones against green ones (red/green opponency) and blue cones against red plus green ones (blue/yellow opponency), but by pitting individual short-, middle-, and long-wavelength cones forming the center mechanism against all cones that feed into the surround mechanism. That this is a computationally viable alternative has been established by Lennie et al. (59). It must be recognized, however, that possible interconnections in the inner retina could provide a clear color-opponent system. The midget and parasol cells of the retina project predominantly to the LGN in the trichromatic primate, where the midget cells make connections within the parvocellular layers and the parasol cells within the magnocellular layers. In turn, the cells in these two sets of layers project to the striate cortex, where they terminate predominantly in layers 4cα and 4cβ, respectively. Within the striate cortex, some cells, after a cascade of connections, receive a convergent input from these two systems (60). Some of the cortical cells, however, are driven exclusively by one or the other system, although there is commonly a convergence of the ON and OFF subtypes, as has already been noted. Recent studies of the connections from the striate cortex to extrastriate PNAS | October 5, 2010 | vol. 107 | no. 40 | 17091 NEUROSCIENCE Percent Correct Detection 80 Saccadic latency distribution regions have shown that the input to area MT is dominated by the parasol system, whereas the input to area V4 is mixed (61, 62). The ratio of the midget and parasol cells changes notably as a function of eccentricity. In the center of the fovea (sometimes called the foveola), there are virtually no parasol cells. Near the fovea, the midget cells outnumber the parasol cells by approximately eight to one. This ratio changes gradually until, in the far peripheral representation, they become equally represented (63). This arrangement is clearly reflected in the LGN. In central representation, out to approximately 18°, the LGN most commonly has six layers. The dorsal four layers are called parvocellular and contain medium-sized cells. These layers receive input from the retinal midget cells (64, 65). The ventral two layers are called magnocellular and contain large cells that receive input from the retinal parasol cells. Beyond 18° of the visual field representation in the LGN, the layers are reduced to four: two parvocellular and two magnocellular ones. Thus, in central representation, the midget cells are far more numerous than the parasol cells, whereas in peripheral representation, they are equal in number (60). Behavioral Studies Examining Functions of Midget and Parasol Systems. A variety of behavioral experiments have been carried out that examined the functions of the midget and parasol systems. Some of these experiments have used visual stimulus conditions that selectively activated the midget or parasol systems (50, 66–68). Another set of experiments blocked the midget or parasol systems by making selective lesions in the parvocellular and magnocellular portions of the LGN (49, 50, 67). To assess the role of these two systems in vision, monkeys had been trained to perform a variety of visual tasks using detection and discrimination tasks. Each trial began with a fixation spot appearing in the center of a monitor the monkey faced. Upon fixation, as determined by monitoring the monkeys’ eye movements, either a single stimulus appeared (the detection paradigm) or an array of stimuli was presented, one of which, the target, was different from the others, called distractors (i.e., oddity task). The monkey was rewarded for making a saccadic eye movement directly to the target. The contrast, size, color, and/or shape of the target or the distractors was systematically varied. The target was presented in intact regions of the visual field or in regions where the midget or the parasol system was blocked (50, 67). Fig. S2 shows data obtained from a monkey that assessed color, texture, and pattern processing. Fig. S2A shows data obtained in a color discrimination task using the oddity task. The target in this case had a different color from that of the isoluminant distractors of a different color. The data show that, after a midget system block, the monkey was no longer able to make color discriminations. Conversely, the parasol system block did not have much of an effect on color discrimination. These findings establish the fact that color processing is accomplished by the midget system. Fig. S2B shows the texture detection task. A single target appeared on a textured background with the target consisting of a small square area within which the diagonal lines were reversed relative to the background. Blocking the midget system brought performance to chance, whereas blocking the parasol system had no effect on texture detection. Fig. S2C shows a pattern discrimination task; the target is different from the distractors by having checkerboards of a lower spatial frequency. The data show that a midget system block eliminates the ability of the monkey to discriminate fine differences in the perception of patterns whereas a parasol system block has no significant effect on pattern perception. In contrast with these dramatic effects, when monkeys were tested for brightness discrimination, it was found that this capacity was unaffected by a midget or parasol block, indicating that both these systems can process this capacity. Fig. S3 shows data obtained on stereoscopic depth perception, motion detection, and flicker detection. Stereoscopic depth perception has been studied extensively using random-dot stereograms as initially developed by Julesz (68, 69). To study this in monkeys, the monkeys were trained to look through a stereoscope. 17092 | www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1011782107 A small region in the stereogram was arranged to appear in depth by virtue of the disparity introduced. A saccadic eye movement made to this region was rewarded. The top section of Fig. S3 shows data obtained using this procedure. Following a midget system block, the monkeys’ ability to perform on this task was devastated. The deficit was complete at the lowest disparity. There was no deficit when the parasol system was blocked. These findings suggest that the midget system plays a central role in stereopsis, especially at high spatial frequencies and low disparities. The parasol system, however, seems to be able to contribute to this mechanism at low spatial frequencies and high disparities. To study motion, the display consisted of an array of randomly placed small dots. On each trial in a small region of the visual field, the dots were set in motion. A saccadic eye movement made to that location was rewarded. The contrast of the dots was systematically varied. The data in the center section of Fig. S3 show that a midget system block had no effect on motion discrimination whereas the block of the parasol system created a major deficit. In another study, depth analysis based on motion parallax was examined, which showed that the parasol system plays a central role in processing this information (68). The bottom section of Fig. S3 shows data obtained when the task was to discriminate flicker from nonflicker. To do so, an array of light-emitting diodes (LEDs) was used, the flicker rate of which could be varied over an extensive range. The data show that, when the parasol system is blocked, a major deficit arises in flicker perception. These data establish that the parasol system plays a central role in temporal processing that includes motion and flicker. Based on these data, we can conclude that the midget system plays a central role in color, pattern, texture, and stereoscopic depth perception whereas the parasol system plays a central role in motion perception, flicker perception, and depth processing based on motion parallax. Both systems contribute to brightness perception. As a result of this work, we believe the midget and parasol systems have evolved to extend the range over which visual information can be effectively processed. The midget system extends it in the high spatial frequency and wavelength domains whereas the parasol system extends it in the temporal domain. Accessory Optic System The accessory optic system (AOS) originates in the retina. Its ganglion cells project to three target nuclei in the anterior portion of the midbrain: the dorsal, medial, and lateral terminal nuclei (70–72). The morphology of retinal ganglion cells projecting to the terminal nuclei was first studied in the pigeon by Karten and collaborators in the 1970s (72–74). They showed that these were the displaced ganglion cells of Dogiel whose distribution on the retinal surface was remarkably orderly. In the rabbit, which has approximately 350,000 ganglion cells, there are approximately 6,000 to 7,000 directionally selective cells of Dogiel that project to the nuclei of the AOS (75). The responses of direction-specific retinal ganglion cells in the rabbit retina were first studied by Barlow and Levick (76). Based on their work and that of others, it was established that those direction-selective cells of the rabbit retina that respond best to very slow velocities of image motion (0.1–1°/s) are all ON types (75). They have a trilobed spatial distribution of preferred excitatory directions (77). Striking about these observations was the discovery that the three axes of direction selectivity correspond to the planes of three principal axes of the semicircular canals. The neurons in the terminal nuclei of the rabbit to which the displaced ON-type direction selective cells of Dogiel project have similar properties but have much larger receptive fields. The preferred direction of cells in the dorsal terminal nucleus was found to be from posterior to anterior in the horizontal plane. Cells in the medial and lateral terminal nuclei had upward or downward excitatory directions, both with a posterior component. Inhibition plays a prominent role in the AOS as approximately 50% of the cells have positive glutamic acid decarboxylase reactions (78, 79). The terminal nuclei project to the dorsal cap of Schiller 1. Schultze M (1866) Zur anatomie und physiologie der retina. Arch Mikrosk Anat 2:175–286. 2. Ramon y Cajal S, Craigie EH, Cano J (1937) Recollections of My Life (American Philosophical Society, Philadelphia). 3. Hartline H (1938) The responses of single optic nerve fibers of the vertebrate eye to illumination of the retina. Am J Physiol 121:400–415. 4. Barlow HB (1953) Summation and inhibition in the frog’s retina. J Physiol 119:69–88. 5. Kuffler SW (1953) Discharge patterns and functional organization of mammalian retina. J Neurophysiol 16:37–68. 6. Bishop P (1984) Processing of visual information within the retinostriate system. Handbook of Physiology, ed Darien-Smith I (American Physiological Society, Bethesda, MD), pp 341–424. 7. Rodieck RW (1973) The Vertebrate Retina; Principles of Structure and Function (Freeman, San Francisco). 8. Schiller PH (1986) The central visual system. Vision Res 26:1351–1386. 9. Schiller PH (1990) The On and Off Channels of the Visual System. Vision and the Brain: The Organization of the Central Visual System, eds Cohen B, Bodis-Wollner I (Raven Press, New York), pp 35–41. 10. Bortoff A (1964) Localization of slow potential responses in the Necturus retina. Vision Res 4:627–635. 11. Werblin FS, Dowling JE (1969) Organization of the retina of the mudpuppy, Necturus maculosus. II. Intracellular recording. J Neurophysiol 32:339–355. 12. Ehinger B, Ottersen OP, Storm-Mathisen J, Dowling JE (1988) Bipolar cells in the turtle retina are strongly immunoreactive for glutamate. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 85:8321–8325. 13. Marc RE, Liu WL, Kalloniatis M, Raiguel SF, van Haesendonck E (1990) Patterns of glutamate immunoreactivity in the goldfish retina. J Neurosci 10:4006–4034. 14. Masland RH (1986) The functional architecture of the retina. Sci Am 255:102–111. 15. Miller R, Slaughter M (1986) Excitatory amino acid receptors of the retina: Diversity of subtypes and conductance mechanisms. Trends Neurosci 9:211–218. 16. Wässle H, Boycott BB (1991) Functional architecture of the mammalian retina. Physiol Rev 71:447–480. 17. Poggio G (1980) Central neural mechanisms in vision. Medical Physiology, ed Mountcastle VB (Mosby, St. Louis), Vol 1, pp 544–585. 18. Conway JL, Schiller PH (1983) Laminar organization of tree shrew dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus. J Neurophysiol 50:1330–1342. 19. Schiller PH, Malpeli JG (1978) Functional specificity of lateral geniculate nucleus laminae of the rhesus monkey. J Neurophysiol 41:788–797. 20. Hubel DH, Wiesel TN (1962) Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat’s visual cortex. J Physiol 160:106–154. 21. Famiglietti EV, Jr., Kaneko A, Tachibana M (1977) Neuronal architecture of on and off pathways to ganglion cells in carp retina. Science 198:1267–1269. 22. Wässle H, Boycott BB, Illing RB (1981) Morphology and mosaic of on- and off-beta cells in the cat retina and some functional considerations. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 212:177–195. 23. Jung R (1961) Korrelationen von neuronentatigkeit und sehen. Neurophysiologie und Psychophysik des Visuellen Systems, eds Jung R, Kornhuber H (Springer-Verlag, Berlin), pp 410–435. 24. Schiller PH (1992) The ON and OFF channels of the visual system. Trends Neurosci 15: 86–92. Schiller PNAS | October 5, 2010 | vol. 107 | no. 40 | 17093 INAUGURAL ARTICLE Other Parallel Channels Originating in the Retina In addition to the systems discussed so far, several others have been found to originate in the retina. These include the so-called W system, directionally selective cells other than those that project to the terminal nuclei, the pupillary system, a small complement of cells that project to the hypothalamus, and the so-called koniocellular (K) system. The W-type cells of the retina, described in the cat and monkey by a number of investigators, appear to be a “grab-bag” category that actually consists of several subtypes and seems to include the ON/OFF retinal ganglion cells (90). Anatomically, these cells are likely to be of the δ and γ types (16). Many of these cells come in both ON and OFF subvarieties and, as already noted, project extensively to the superior colliculus and also to the pretectal nuclei. Prominent among the pretectal nuclei is the nucleus of the optic tract that receives input from those direction-selective retinal ganglion cells that respond to higher velocities and come in both ON and OFF subtypes. Another visually driven structure in the pretectum is the olivary nucleus, which is involved in the generation of the pupillary reflex. As studied in the rat and rabbit (42), it appears that the cells of the olivary pretectal nuclei are all excited, in a tonic fashion, to light increment, suggesting that they are exclusively of the ON type. This is supported by the finding that, in the rabbit, retinal administration of APB eliminates responsiveness to light in the olivary pretectal nucleus. However, when the pupillary reflex was examined in rabbit and monkey following APB blockage of the ON system in the retina, the reflex was only mildly impaired. This suggests that other pathways and the OFF system can make a contribution to light activation of the ciliary muscles (42). Yet another class of retinal ganglion cells that has been identified are the K cells that express αCAM kinase/calbindin. This system has been extensively studied; several reviews describe it in detail (91–98). Many of the K ganglion cells have bistratified dendritic arbors, as a result of which they receive both ON and OFF inputs from bipolar cells. Some of them are driven by blue cones. A subgroup of these cells project to the intralaminar layers of the LGN, from which the projections are predominantly to the upper layers of area V1 (91–93, 95). On the basis of the foregoing it is evident that, from the single layer of photoreceptors, a series of parallel pathways are created by the stunningly complex and elaborate circuitry of the retina. There are many different retinal ganglion cells that project selectively to a variety of central nuclei and are involved in different aspects of visual information processing. Furthermore, in some of these systems, the same set of retinal ganglion cells can carry several kinds of different messages. For example, the midget cells, when excited in the daytime by the cones, can carry information about both color and luminance; at night, when they are driven by the rods, the receptive fields of these same cells take on a different configuration and convey messages about only luminance. This arrangement renders these cells multifunctional. NEUROSCIENCE Kooy of the inferior olive (80, 81), which gives rise to climbing fibers projecting to the flocculonodular lobe of the cerebellum. The dorsal cap of the inferior olive also receives a projection from the nucleus of the optic tract, which has been studied by Collewijn and others (82, 83). This structure, like the terminal nuclei, receives input from direction selective cells of the retina, but of a different class that gives both ON and OFF responses and responds best to higher velocities of stimulus motion. Electrical stimulation of these regions elicits nystagmic eye movements akin to those obtained during optokinetic nystagmus, and lesions produce deficits in the tracking of moving stimuli. As, in the rabbit, the retinal direction selective cells projecting to the terminal nuclei are all ON types, it is possible to assess the role of these cells in the generation of optokinetic nystagmus by applying APB to the retina while the animal is exposed to slowly moving vertical stripes. When this was done by Knapp et al. (84), they found that the optokinetic nystagmus was abolished. This finding provided additional support for the idea that the ON direction-selective cells of the rabbit retina provide the essential input, via the terminal nuclei, for the generation of tracking eye movements for large stimulus constellations. This curious arrangement of having only ON-type cells that project into the AOS in the rabbit is not found in all species. Although in the turtle and zebrafish, similar results have been obtained (85, 86), this was not the case in the cat or monkey. In these species, only mild effects of APB were found on OKN, which was restricted to high spatiotemporal frequencies (84). This suggests that in these species both the ON and OFF channels contribute to the OKN. In the goldfish, APB also failed to abolish the OKN (87); it was subsequently discovered, however, that in teleosts there are two mechanisms of ON-bipolar generation, one of which is not sensitive to APB (88). On the basis of these observations, a specific function for the AOS emerges as proposed by Simpson et al. (89). The AOS signals self-motion as reflected in the slip of the visual world over the retinal surface. The detection of this slip by the direction- and velocity-selective cells of the AOS triggers a chain of activity through the pathways of the AOS that produces corrective eye movements, resulting in the stabilization of the retinal image. At higher velocities such stabilization is typically accomplished by the vestibular system. Hence, the AOS nuclei and the vestibular apparatus form two complementary systems geared toward the detection of self-motion and the appropriate corrective action for image stabilization, thereby allowing for the rapid and accurate analysis of objects that appear in central vision. 25. Schiller PH (1982) Central connections of the retinal ON and OFF pathways. Nature 297:580–583. 26. Daw NW, Jensen RJ, Brunken WJ (1990) Rod pathways in mammalian retinae. Trends Neurosci 13:110–115. 27. Kolb H, Famiglietti EV (1974) Rod and cone pathways in the inner plexiform layer of cat retina. Science 186:47–49. 28. Müller F, Wässle H, Voigt T (1988) Pharmacological modulation of the rod pathway in the cat retina. J Neurophysiol 59:1657–1672. 29. MacLeod DI, Chen B, Crognale M (1989) Spatial organization of sensitivity regulation in rod vision. Vision Res 29:965–978. 30. Nelson R, Famiglietti EV, Jr., Kolb H (1978) Intracellular staining reveals different levels of stratification for on- and off-center ganglion cells in cat retina. J Neurophysiol 41: 472–483. 31. Barlow HB, Fitzhugh R, Kuffler SW (1957) Change of organization in the receptive fields of the cat’s retina during dark adaptation. J Physiol 137:338–354. 32. Slaughter MM, Miller RF (1981) 2-amino-4-phosphonobutyric acid: A new pharmacological tool for retina research. Science 211:182–185. 33. Shiells RA, Falk G (1990) Glutamate receptors of rod bipolar cells are linked to a cyclic GMP cascade via a G-protein. Proc Biol Sci 242:91–94. 34. Nawy S, Jahr CE (1990) Suppression by glutamate of cGMP-activated conductance in retinal bipolar cells. Nature 346:269–271. 35. Nakajima Y, et al. (1993) Molecular characterization of a novel retinal metabotropic glutamate receptor mGluR6 with a high agonist selectivity for L-2-amino-4phosphonobutyrate. J Biol Chem 268:11868–11873. 36. Masu M, et al. (1995) Specific deficit of ON response in visual transmission by targeted disruption of the mGluR6 gene. Cell 10:757–765. 37. Schiller PH (1984) The connections of the retinal on and off pathways to the lateral geniculate nucleus of the monkey. Vision Res 24:923–932. 38. Horton JC, Sherk H (1984) Receptive field properties in the cat’s lateral geniculate nucleus in the absence of on-center retinal input. J Neurosci 4:374–380. 39. Mangel SC, Miller RF (1987) Horizontal cells contribute to the receptive field surround of ganglion cells in the rabbit retina. Brain Res 414:182–186. 40. Schiller PH, Malpeli JG (1977) Properties and tectal projections of monkey retinal ganglion cells. J Neurophysiol 40:428–445. 41. Norton TT, Casagrande VA (1982) Laminar organization of receptive-field properties in lateral geniculate nucleus of bush baby (Galago crassicaudatus). J Neurophysiol 47: 715–741. 42. Knapp A (1985) The visual role of the rabbit's ON channel. Ph.D. Dissertation in Neuroscience. September, 1985. (Massachusetts Institute of Technology Cambridge, MA). 43. Schiller PH, Finlay BL, Volman SF (1976) Quantitative studies of single-cell properties in monkey striate cortex. I-V. J Neurophysiol 39:1288–1386. 44. Sherk H, Horton JC (1984) Receptive field properties in the cat’s area 17 in the absence of on-center geniculate input. J Neurosci 4:381–393. 45. Schiller PH, Sandell JH, Maunsell JH (1986) Functions of the ON and OFF channels of the visual system. Nature 322:824–825. 46. Smith EL, 3rd, Harwerth RS, Crawford ML, Duncan GC (1989) Contribution of the retinal ON channels to scotopic and photopic spectral sensitivity. Vis Neurosci 3: 225–239. 47. Dolan RP, Schiller PH (1989) Evidence for only depolarizing rod bipolar cells in the primate retina. Vis Neurosci 2:421–424. 48. Enroth-Cugell C, Robson JG (1966) The contrast sensitivity of retinal ganglion cells of the cat. J Physiol 187:517–552. 49. Merigan WH, Maunsell JH (1993) How parallel are the primate visual pathways? Annu Rev Neurosci 16:369–402. 50. Schiller PH, Logothetis NK (1990) The color-opponent and broad-band channels of the primate visual system. Trends Neurosci 13:392–398. 51. Polyak SL (1957) The Vertebrate Visual System; Its Origin, Structure, and Function and Its Manifestations in Disease with an Analysis of Its Role in the Life of Animals and in the Origin of Man, Preceded by a Historical Review of Investigations of the Eye, and of the Visual Pathways and Centers of the Brain (Univ Chicago Press, Chicago). 52. Watanabe M, Rodieck RW (1989) Parasol and midget ganglion cells of the primate retina. J Comp Neurol 289:434–454. 53. Schein SJ (1988) Anatomy of macaque fovea and spatial densities of neurons in foveal representation. J Comp Neurol 269:479–505. 54. Boynton R (1979) Human Color Vision (Holt, Rinehart and Winston, New York). 55. Valois RD (1973) Central mechanisms of color vision. Handbook of Sensory Physiology, Central Processing of Visual Information, ed Jung R (Springer-Verlag, Berlin), Vol 7, pp 209–253. 56. Lennie P (1984) Recent developments in the physiology of color vision. Trends Neurosci 7:243–248. 57. Mollon JD, Sharpe LT (1983) Colour Vision: Physiology and Psychophysics (Academic Press, London). 58. Wiesel TN, Hubel DH (1966) Spatial and chromatic interactions in the lateral geniculate body of the rhesus monkey. J Neurophysiol 29:1115–1156. 59. Lennie P, Haake P, Williams D (1989) Chromatic opponency through random connections to cones. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 30(suppl):322. 60. Malpeli JG, Schiller PH, Colby CL (1981) Response properties of single cells in monkey striate cortex during reversible inactivation of individual lateral geniculate laminae. J Neurophysiol 46:1102–1119. 61. Ferrera VP, Nealey TA, Maunsell JH (1992) Mixed parvocellular and magnocellular geniculate signals in visual area V4. Nature 358:756–761. 17094 | www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1011782107 62. Maunsell JH, Nealey TA, DePriest DD (1990) Magnocellular and parvocellular contributions to responses in the middle temporal visual area (MT) of the macaque monkey. J Neurosci 10:3323–3334. 63. Erwin E, Baker FH, Busen WF, Malpeli JG (1999) Relationship between laminar topology and retinotopy in the rhesus lateral geniculate nucleus: Results from a functional atlas. J Comp Neurol 407:92–102. 64. Creutzfeldt O, Lee BB, Valberg A (1986) Colour and brightness signals of parvocellular lateral geniculate neurons. Exp Brain Res 63:21–34. 65. Derrington AM, Krauskopf J, Lennie P (1984) Chromatic mechanisms in lateral geniculate nucleus of macaque. J Physiol 357:241–265. 66. Livingstone MS, Hubel DH (1987) Psychophysical evidence for separate channels for the perception of form, color, movement, and depth. J Neurosci 7:3416–3468. 67. Schiller PH, Logothetis NK, Charles ER (1990) Role of the color-opponent and broadband channels in vision. Vis Neurosci 5:321–346. 68. Schiller PH, Slocum WM, Weiner VS (2007) How the parallel channels of the retina contribute to depth processing. Eur J Neurosci 26:1307–1321. 69. Julesz B (1971) Foundations of Psychopean Perception (Univ Chicago Press, Chicago). 70. Hayhow WR (1959) An experimental study of the accessory optic fiber system in the cat. J Comp Neurol 113:281–313. 71. Hayhow WR (1966) The accessory optic system in the marsupial phalanger, Trichosurus vulpecula. An experimental degeneration study. J Comp Neurol 126:653–672. 72. Hayhow WR, Webb C, Jervie A (1960) The accessory optic fiber system in the rat. J Comp Neurol 115:187–215. 73. Karten JH, Fite KV, Brecha N (1977) Specific projection of displaced retinal ganglion cells upon the accessory optic system in the pigeon (Columbia livia). Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 74:1753–1756. 74. Britto LR, Hamassaki DE, Keyser KT, Karten HJ (1989) Neurotransmitters, receptors, and neuropeptides in the accessory optic system: an immunohistochemical survey in the pigeon (Columba livia). Vis Neurosci 3:463–475. 75. Simpson JI (1984) The accessory optic system. Annu Rev Neurosci 7:13–41. 76. Barlow HB, Hill RM, Levick WR (1964) Retinal ganglion cells responding selectively to direction and speed of image motion in the rabbit. J Physiol 173:377–407. 77. Oyster CW, Barlow HB (1967) Direction-selective units in rabbit retina: distribution of preferred directions. Science 155:841–842. 78. Giolli RA, et al. (1985) GABAergic neurons comprise a major cell type in rodent visual relay nuclei: an immunocytochemical study of pretectal and accessory optic nuclei. Exp Brain Res 61:194–203. 79. Penny G, Conley M, Diamond I, Schmechel D (1983) A comparative study of the thalamic distribution of GAD immunoreactive neurons. Soc Neurosci Abstr 9:921. 80. Mizuno N, Mochizuki K, Akimoto C, Matsushima R (1973) Pretectal projections to the inferior olive in the rabbit. Exp Neurol 39:498–506. 81. Mizuno N, Nakamura Y, Iwahori N (1974) An electron microscope study of the dorsal cap of the inferior olive in the rabbit, with special reference to the pretecto-olivary fibers. Brain Res 77:385–395. 82. Collewijn H (1975) Direction-selective units in the rabbit’s nucleus of the optic tract. Brain Res 100:489–508. 83. Hoffman K-P (1981) Neuronal Responses Related to Optokinetic Nystagmus in the Cat’s Nucleus of the Optic Tract. Progress in Oculomotor Research, eds Fuchs A, Becker W (Elsevier, Amsterdam), pp 443–454. 84. Knapp AG, Ariel M, Robinson FR (1988) Analysis of vertebrate eye movements following intravitreal drug injections. I. Blockade of retinal ON-cells by 2-amino-4phosphonobutyrate eliminates optokinetic nystagmus. J Neurophysiol 60:1010–1021. 85. Ariel M (1987) Synaptic drugs injected into the vitreous affect the retinal control of turtle eye movements. Soc Neurosci Abstr 13:1053. 86. Emran F, et al. (2007) OFF ganglion cells cannot drive the optokinetic reflex in zebrafish. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104:19126–19131. 87. DeMarco PJ, Jr., Nussdorf JD, Brockman DA, Powers MK (1989) APB selectively reduces visual responses in goldfish to high spatiotemporal frequencies. Vis Neurosci 2:15–18. 88. Grant GB, Dowling JE (1996) On bipolar cell responses in the teleost retina are generated by two distinct mechanisms. J Neurophysiol 76:3842–3849. 89. Simpson JI, Soodak RE, Hess R (1979) The accessory optic system and its relation to the vestibulocerebellum. Prog Brain Res 50:715–724. 90. Stone J (1983) Parallel Processing in the Visual System (Plenum, New York). 91. Callaway EM (2005) Structure and function of parallel pathways in the primate early visual system. J Physiol 566:13–19. 92. Casagrande VA (1994) A third parallel visual pathway to primate area V1. Trends Neurosci 17:305–310. 93. Dacey DM, Lee BB (1994) The ‘blue-on’ opponent pathway in primate retina originates from a distinct bistratified ganglion cell type. Nature 367:731–735. 94. Hendrickson AE, Wilson JR, Ogren MP (1978) The neuroanatomical organization of pathways between the dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus and visual cortex in Old World and New World primates. J Comp Neurol 182:123–136. 95. Hendry SH, Yoshioka T (1994) A neurochemically distinct third channel in the macaque dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus. Science 264:575–577. 96. Hendry SH, Reid RC (2000) The koniocellular pathway in primate vision. Annu Rev Neurosci 23:127–153. 97. Casagrande VA, Yazar F, Jones KD, Ding Y (2007) The morphology of the koniocellular axon pathway in the macaque monkey. Cereb Cortex 17:2334–2345. 98. Shostak Y, Ding Y, Casagrande VA (2003) Neurochemical comparison of synaptic arrangements of parvocellular, magnocellular, and koniocellular geniculate pathways in owl monkey (Aotus trivirgatus) visual cortex. J Comp Neurol 456: 12–28. Schiller