Outcomes in a Program that Offers Financial Rewards for Weight Loss John Cawley Joshua A. Price Cornell University Acknowledgement We would like to thank the Economic Research Service of the USDA on Behavioral Economics Research on Dietary Choice and Obesity Weight Loss Attempts For obese, benefits of weight loss substantial: Health benefits of modest weight loss (defined as 5-10% of starting weight) include decreased blood pressure and cholesterol and a 25% reduction in mortality risk for type 2 diabetics (Vidal, 2002) Possible labor market benefits; e.g. healthy-weight white females earn 11% more than obese white females (Cawley 2004) Healthy weight incur $125 lower annual out-of-pocket health care costs than the obese; Finkelstein et al. (2003) In U.S., 46% of women and 33% of men trying to lose weight (Bish et al. 2005) Most attempts at weight control fail; in one community-based study of weight gain prevention (Crawford et al., 2000): Most (53.7%) participants gained weight in the first twelve months Three-quarters gained weight over three years Only 4.6% lost weight and maintained the loss for three years Why do Most Weight Loss Attempts Fail? Salience: benefits of weight loss are not salient Opportunity costs not visible so frequently overlooked (Bastiat 1850) Immediacy: benefits of weight loss are not necessarily immediate The effectiveness of rewards declines with their delay from time of choice; Ainslie (1975) Time inconsistency: time discounting may be hyperbolic (Ainsley 1975) People want to lose weight but tend to succumb to temptation Thaler and Shefrin (1981) : decision-making is battle between farsighted planner (who wants to diet and be active) and a myopic doer (who wants to eat and be sedentary) Financial Rewards for Weight Loss May Offer Solution to These Problems Salience: financial rewards can be known with certainty in exchange for well-defined objectives Even small financial incentives can be effective because of “bracketing” – tradeoffs considered in isolation, not compared to income or wealth (Read et al. 1999; Kahneman and Tversky 1979) Immediacy: can be paid sooner than other benefits of weight reduction may be realized Time-inconsistency: incentives can be structured to serve as precommitment device to help people overcome time-inconsistent preferences Forfeitable bonds effective because people exhibit loss aversion – dislike losing own money more than like winning other people’s money (Tversky and Kahneman 1991; Camerer 2005) Our Project and Contributions Examine outcomes in a program that offers various types of financial rewards for weight loss Program features include: fixed payments, lotteries, refundable bonds Outcomes we examine attrition and weight loss (lbs, %) NIH (1993) recommends using attrition and weight loss (e.g. 10% at 6 mos, 1 yr) to evaluate weight loss interventions Also test hypotheses about who is most likely to attrite, lose more weight Advantages over previous literature: Large sample (N=2,407) Long-term (1+ years) Real-world intervention The Intervention Employer hires Company X to provide weight-loss advice, document weight loss, pay incentives to employees Kickoff sessions: provide information, encourage signup Baseline weigh-in: up to week after kickoff Daily emails provide info about diet, portion size, physical activity Must weigh in once per quarter at kiosk located at worksite Data uploaded to Company X and to enrollee’s personal website Website: view own weight trajectory, chat room, information resources Call center for live coaching Standard Incentive Schedule: Quarterly Payments Weight Loss (as % of Baseline Weight) Dollar Reward Per Month (Paid Quarterly) 1 1 2 2 3 3 4 4 5 5 10 10 15 15 20 25 25 35 30 50 Modified Incentive Schedule: Lottery, Bond, and Bonuses Weight Loss (as % of Baseline Weight) Greater than zero Reward (Lottery Quarterly, Year-End Payment) Entered into quarterly drawing for gift certificates: ten $50 gift cards each quarter and ten $50 salon vouchers each quarter. 5 Complete reimbursement of monthly fees (11 * $9.95), paid at end of year 10 Complete reimbursement of monthly fees (11 * $9.95) plus $100 bonus, paid at end of year “Biggest loser” (as % of baseline) at worksite $250 gift certificate, awarded at end of year Control Group Received all program elements (daily emails, website resources, call center options, quarterly weigh-ins) ….except were paid no incentives for weight loss Were paid $20 / year for weighing in quarterly Which presumably affects attrition, perhaps enrollment Variable Means by Group Control Standard Incentives (N=1,513) Modified Incentives (N=765) (N=129) Male 16% 21% 36% Age 46.5 43.0 44.1 Baseline BMI 32.9 32.8 31.3 Overweight 40% 38% 52% Obese 31% 31% 26% Morbidly Obese 29% 30% 22% Variable Research Questions What is attrition in such programs and how does it vary with incentive schedule? What is weight loss in such programs and how does it vary with incentive schedule? Methods Attrition Report unconditional attrition rates, compare across groups Estimate hazard model of attrition Weight Loss Report unconditional mean weight change (lbs, % baseline) Estimate OLS regression of weight loss (lbs, % baseline) Estimate Probit models of losing 5%, 10% of baseline weight Attrition Hypothesis I: those that are unsuccessful at weight loss are more likely to drop out Rationale: selection, loss aversion Hypothesis II: lower attrition in programs that require participants to post a refundable bond Rationale: selection, loss aversion Attrition Quarter 1 2 3 4 Standard Incentives (N=1,513) 0.512 0.621 0.720 0.764 Modified Incentive (N=765) 0.248 0.335 0.393 0.574 Control Group (N=142) 0.256 0.395 0.450 0.481 Standard incentives group has higher attrition than in any published RCT on financial rewards for weight loss – max was 57.9% at 13 months (PaulEbhohimhen and Avenuell, 2007) Results from the Hazard Model Lagged weight loss reduces probability of dropping out 5% lower probability for each percent of baseline weight lost Individuals in the standard incentive group are twice as likely to drop out of the program as those in the control group Modified incentive group are no more likely to drop out than those in the control group Obese and morbidly obese are marginally more likely to drop out than overweight individuals Weight Loss Hypothesis I: Weight loss will be greater for those who are offered financial rewards for weight loss Hypothesis 2: In quarters 1-3 weight loss will be greater in standard incentive group than modified incentive group, in quarter 4 weight loss is greater in modified incentive group Unconditional Mean by Group and Quarter Standard Incentive Modified Incentive Control Group Quarter Ignoring Dropouts Baseline CarryForward 1 4.6 2.2 4.2 3.2 3.4 2.6 [2.31%] [1.13%] [2.06%] [1.55%] [1.73%] [1.29%] 5.5 2.1 4.9 3.3 3.1 1.9 [2.73%] [1.04%] [2.38%] [1.58%] [1.62%] [0.98%] 7.8 2.2 4.1 2.5 3.0 1.7 [3.68%] [1.03%] [2.00%] [1.21%] [1.49%] [0.82%] 6.1 1.4 8.4 3.6 3.2 1.7 [2.75%] [0.64%] [4.15%] [1.77%] [1.68%] [0.87%] 2 3 4 Weight Loss in lbs. [Percent weight loss] Ignoring Baseline Carry- Ignoring Baseline CarryDropouts Forward Dropouts Forward OLS Estimates Standard incentive group lost more weight in each quarter than the control group (1.6 to 4.4 lbs) (0.8 to 2.1 percentage points) Modified incentive group has no difference in weight loss compared to control group until the fourth quarter (4.8 lbs) (2.37 percentage points) Males lose more weight than females Morbidly obese lose more pounds but same percentage as overweight individuals Percent Losing 5% of Baseline Weight Standard Incentive Modified Incentive Control Group Quarter Ignoring Dropouts Baseline CarryForward Ignoring Dropouts Baseline CarryForward Ignoring Dropouts Baseline CarryForward 1 14.1% 7.6% 16.5% 11.9% 11.9% 9.2% 2 19.4% 7.9% 23.8% 15.8% 13.2% 8.4% 3 24.9% 7.3% 23.0% 13.8% 26.5% 15.5% 4 24.3% 4.1% 45.7% 19.3% 23.4% 12.7% Percent Losing 10% of Baseline Weight Standard Incentive Modified Incentive Control Group Quarter Ignoring Dropouts Baseline CarryForward Ignoring Dropouts Baseline CarryForward Ignoring Dropouts Baseline CarryForward 1 1.9% 1.0% 2.7% 1.9% 0.9% 0.7% 2 4.8% 2.0% 6.5% 4.3% 4.4% 2.8% 3 9.0% 2.6% 6.8% 4.1% 6.0% 3.5% 4 9.8% 1.7% 16.5% 6.9% 7.8% 4.2% Probit Results Modified incentive group was 27.2 percent more likely to lose 5% of weight than the control group No significant difference in probability of losing 5% of baseline weight between standard and control group No significant difference in probability of 10% weight loss between standard and modified groups compared to control group Modified group significantly more likely than standard group to lose 10% of baseline weight Conclusion Attrition Those that are unsuccessful at weight loss are more likely to drop out Lower attrition in programs that require participants to post a refundable bond Weight Loss Weight loss is greater for those who are offered financial rewards for weight loss In quarters 1-3 weight loss will be greater in standard incentive group than modified incentive group, in quarter 4 weight loss is greater in modified incentive group Suggestions on How to Modify Such Programs Program should reward loss of fat, not weight BMI / pounds poor measure of fatness (Burkhauser and Cawley 2008) Current program has disincentive for muscle-building exercise Pay immediately and often; Ainslie (1975) More frequent weigh-ins with immediate gratification Should always allow posting of bonds Requiring them discourages sign-up (Jeffrey 1978) Large single payment may be better than monthly fees Consider paying for participation Control group paid $20 for the year had lower attrition than the standard incentives group Beware unintended consequences Employer with “modified” incentives discovered employees were using unhealthy methods to achieve weight loss just before quarter 4; revised program for the next year