ORIGINAL ARTICLE doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.2007.00077.x LINEAGES WITH LONG DURATIONS ARE OLD AND MORPHOLOGICALLY AVERAGE: AN ANALYSIS USING MULTIPLE DATASETS Lee Hsiang Liow1,2,3 1 Committee 2 E-mail: on Evolutionary Biology, University of Chicago, 5734 S. Ellis Avenue, Chicago, Illinois 60637 l.h.liow@bio.uio.no Received July 31, 2006 Accepted December 6, 2006 Lineage persistence is as central to biology as evolutionary change. Important questions regarding persistence include: why do some lineages outlive their relatives, neither becoming extinct nor evolving into separate lineages? Do these long-duration lineages have distinctive ecological or morphological traits that correlate with their geologic durations and potentially aid their survival? In this paper, I test the hypothesis that lineages (species and higher taxa) with longer geologic durations have morphologies that are more average than expected by chance alone. I evaluate this hypothesis for both individual lineages with longer durations and groups of lineages with longer durations, using more than 60 published datasets of animals with adequate fossil records. Analyses presented here show that groups of lineages with longer durations fall empirically into one of three theoretically possible scenarios, namely: (1) the morphology of groups of longer duration lineages is closer to the grand average of their inclusive group, that is, their relative morphological distance is smaller than expected by chance alone, when compared with rarified samples of their shorter duration relatives (a negative group morpho-duration distribution); (2) the relative morphological distance of groups of longer duration lineages is no different from rarified samples of their shorter duration relatives (a null group morpho-duration distribution); and (3) the relative morphological distance of groups of longer duration lineages is greater than expected when compared with rarified samples of their shorter duration relatives (a positive group morpho-duration distribution). Datasets exhibiting negative group morpho-duration distributions predominate. However, lineages with higher ranks in the Linnean hierarchy demonstrate positive morpho-duration distributions more frequently. The relative morphological distance of individual longer duration lineages is no different from that of rarified samples of their shorter duration relatives (a null individual morpho-duration distribution) for the majority of datasets studied. Contrary to the common idea that very persistent lineages are special or unique in some significant way, both the results from analyses of long-duration lineages as groups and individuals show that they are morphologically average. Persistent lineages often arise early in a group’s history, even though there is no prior expectation for this tendency in datasets of extinct groups. The implications of these results for diversification histories and niche preemption are discussed. KEY WORDS: Average morphology, fossil record, lineage duration, prolonged stasis, survivorship. Variation is rampant in the biological world. This variation is present in lineage (species and higher taxa) durations: it has been repeatedly observed that lineage durations of natu3 Current address: Centre for Ecological and Evolutionary Synthesis (CEES), Department of Biology, P.O. Box 1066, Blindern, 0316 Oslo, Norway. E-mail: l.h.liow@bio.uio.no C 885 ral groups resemble hollow curves (Simpson 1944, 1953; Stanley 1979; Levinton and Ginzburg 1984). In any given clade or paraclade (sensu Raup 1985, p. 44, e.g., monophyletic birds or paraphyletic nonavian dinosaurs), most lineages have shorter durations and a few have longer durations. This observation leads naturally to the question: do lineages that outlive their relatives without becoming extinct or evolving into separate C 2007 The Society for the Study of Evolution. 2007 The Author(s). Journal compilation Evolution 61-4: 885–901 LEE HSIANG LIOW lineages have distinctive properties that could aid their prolonged survival? Factors that affect lineage durations or survivorship may be categorized as extrinsic (environmental) or intrinsic (biological), although the two categories often overlap. Biological characteristics may operate on different lineages in different ways during time intervals of varying environmental conditions (e.g., mass or background extinction regimes), resulting in different lineage durations or survivorship (Jablonski 1986a, 1994; Jablonski and Raup 1995). Broad geographic ranges (Jackson 1974; Boucot 1975; Chen et al. 2005; Jablonski 2005, Liow 2007), the possession of planktotrophic larvae (Hansen 1978; Jablonski 1986b; Jeffery and Emlet 2003) as well as deeper and wider depth distribution of marine organisms (Buzas and Culver 1984; Oji 1996), high site occupancy (fossil locality coverage; Jernvall and Fortelius 2004), generalist feedings strategies (Baumiller 1993), broader niche breadth (Kammer et al. 1997, 1998), and greater ecological tolerances (Jackson 1974; Schopf 1994) are some biological characteristics that promote lineage persistence and/or damp lineage differentiation. Morphology affects the functioning and performance of organisms (Koehl 1996) and reflects aspects of physiology and ecology (Wainright and Reilly 1994). Therefore, morphology could serve as a proxy for ecology, which in turn affects not only the longevity of individual organisms but also the survivorship of lineages. Yet, little is known about the distribution of morphology in relation to lineage duration, much less the processes that contribute to it. Previous studies have focused on the relationship between morphological complexity and duration with mixed results: the correlation of complexity with durations is dependent on the clades analyzed and complexity metrics used (Flessa et al. 1975; Anstey 1978; Ward and Signor 1983; Boyajian and Lutz 1992). Lineages with longer durations might be morphologically distant from the average morphology of their broader systematic grouping because being different may confer a competitive advantage (a positive morpho-duration distribution; Fig. 1A). If several related (and hence ecologically and/or morphologically similar) lineages overlap in space and time, then those lineages that are able to substantially differentiate themselves ecologically may out-compete and survive beyond the average life spans of their relatives. Conversely, lineages with longer durations might also be morphologically closer to the average morphology of their broader systematic grouping than expected because generalists may be able to survive and persist through a greater range of environmental changes (Simpson 1944; Liow 2006; Fig. 1B, a negative morpho-duration distribution). Finally, lineages with shorter or longer durations may not have significantly different distributions of morphology, indicating that factors unassociated with morphology may be operating more strongly in influencing survivorship (Fig. 1C, a null morpho-duration distribution). Recent 886 EVOLUTION APRIL 2007 studies (Liow 2004, 2006) have concluded that crinoid and ostracode lineages in general show null morpho-duration distributions (Fig. 1C). Similarly, morphological distances from the centroid of morphospace of ammonoid survivors across the Permian-Triassic extinction are not significantly different from those of victims (McGowan, in press). These results are contrary to the long-held idea that geologically persistent or “living fossil” taxa have special or distinctive properties that enable them to remain unchanged whereas their relatives experience speciation and extinction (e.g., see Wills 2001). Previous attempts to investigate the relationship between morphological distribution and lineage duration used only crinoid genera and families (Liow 2004) and trachyleberidid ostracode genera (Liow 2006). Hence taxonomic coverage was very limited. To test the validity of either of the three morpho-duration distributions (Fig. 1) as a general evolutionary pattern, this paper uses multiple datasets to move toward a more representative sample of phylogenetically independent para(clades) across more branches of the metazoan tree of life. These datasets span a wide range of body plans, ecologies, geologic ranges, and lineage ages. Datasets where species are the units of study were also included in the current analysis, in contrast with previous studies where only genus and family data were available. A methodological limitation of previous attempts to investigate the relationship between morphological distribution and lineage duration (Liow 2004, 2006) is that a continuous variable (lineage duration) was arbitrarily divided into discrete categories (shorter and longer). To overcome this limitation, I developed a sequential rarefaction method to determine whether there are tendencies for (para)clades to show one of the three continuous morpho-duration distributions illustrated in Figure 1. In addition, the hypothesis that lineages with longer durations have morphologies that are closer than expected to the grand average of their inclusive group (i.e., a shorter relative morphological distance), is tested both when these lineages are considered collectively as a group (group analyses) and when they are considered individually (single analyses). This is because both individual lineages and groups of lineages with long durations could be unique or distinctive in comparison with their shorter duration counterparts. The datasets used in this study differ in the numbers of lineages, numbers and types of morphological characters represented, operational taxonomic units, intrinsic probabilities of geologic preservation, geologic age ranges, and lineage duration distributions. All of these factors may bias morpho-duration distributions but have never been analyzed within a comparative analytical framework. I also test whether lineages with longer durations tend to arise significantly earlier in the history of their inclusive group. Biases and possible drawbacks in using multiple published datasets are presented for transparency. I end by discussing numerous macroevolutionary implications of the results. Probabilistic relative morphological distance of lineages MORPHOLOGY AND LINEAGE DURATIONS A. Positive morphoduration distribution B. Negative morphoduration distribution C. Null morpho-duration distribution Lineage duration The three general relationships between the probabilistic relative morphological distances of lineages (from a grand morphological average) versus their lineage durations (i.e., morphoduration distributions). Panel A shows a positive morpho-duration distribution, panel B shows a negative morpho-duration distribution, and panel C shows a null morpho-duration distribution. Figure 1. Methods DATA Literature derived morphological character matrices were included in this study only when all of the following criteria were satisfied: (1) stratigraphic ranges were identified for each taxon (graphically, verbally, that is, with global or regional names of strata, or numerically); (2) stratigraphic ranges varied among taxa; (3) at least nine taxa, usable in the current analyses are represented, to allow a large enough sample to detect trends and to perform rarefaction analyses but not so restrictively large that too few datasets qualify; (4) taxa are of equivalent taxonomic ranks (exceptions are noted in Table 1); (5) fossil taxa were represented. Datasets with many extant taxa are excluded because of the issues of one-sided range-truncations (see Gilinsky 1988). However, a small number of datasets with partial extant representation were used to increase the sample size and overall taxonomic breadth for this study (Table 1). Wagner (2000) assembled a database of morphological character matrices. I searched among these matrices for those matching the criteria listed above. I supplemented Wagner’s collection by initially browsing journals that were represented in his database (Wagner 2000) and subsequently by systematically searching through those journals that preliminarily yielded more suitable datasets. These were Lethaia, Historical Biology, Journal of Paleontology, Paleobiology, and Systematic Biology (1996–2005). Other journals specializing in systematic studies yielded surprisingly few datasets that met the listed criteria. More datasets suitable for the current study were found using references cited in papers that met the listed criteria. Updates of phylogenetic hypotheses were made for a limited number of the datasets, but only the most recent paper(s) by the same author(s) discussing the same taxa is/are included here to avoid duplication. I also included Wagner’s datasets of Paleozoic gastropods (see http://pjw3.fmnh.org/EvolutionMatrices 2000.html) and used his phylogenetic hypotheses to subdivide some of his datasets (Table 1). New species character matrices that I coded from four extinct ostracode genera, namely Curfsina Deroo 1966, Opimocythere Hazel 1968, Phalcocythere Siddiqui 1971, and Schizoptocythere Siddiqui and Al-Furaih 1980 were also included in the analyses (see online Supplementary Materials, Appendices S1–S3 for character matrices, stratigraphic ranges, character descriptions, and references). In summary, of the 66 datasets (Table 1) that are used in the analyses, 38 were used in Wagner (2000). The remaining were datasets coded for this study (N = 4), Wagner’s own matrices of Paleozoic gastropods (N = 14), and datasets from other sources (N = 10). These datasets represent taxa from the Paleozoic (N = 26), the Mesozoic (N = 4), the Cenozoic (N = 20), both the Paleozoic and the Mesozoic (N = 4), and both the Mesozoic and the Cenozoic (N = 12). The data span mammals (N = 7), other vertebrates (N =7), trilobites (N = 4), other arthropods, including ostracodes, (N = 7), mollusks (N = 27), echinoderms (N = 8), brachiopods (N = 5), and cnidarians (N = 1) and hence are a broad representation of fossilizable animals across the Phanerozoic. DATA TREATMENT Stratigraphic ranges or geologic durations are explicitly equated to lineage durations. Henceforth I use these terms interchangeably. It is assumed that each included study involves closely related taxa that have similar preservation potentials such that even though stratigraphic ranges are underestimates of true durations, the rank order of the ranges reflects the rank order of the true durations (but see Discussion). EVOLUTION APRIL 2007 887 888 EVOLUTION APRIL 2007 21 22 19 20 17 18 15 16 13 14 11 12 10 9 7 8 4 5 6 3 2 1 Alvarez et al. 1998 Amati and Westrop 2004 Anderson and Roopnarine 2003 Angielczyk and Kurkin 2003 Bloch et al. 2001 Bodenbender and Fisher 2001 Brochu 1997 Brunet–Lecomte and Chaline 1990 Cairns 2001 Caron et al. 2004 2004 Damiani 2001 Dashzeveg and Meng 1998 Dewing 2004 Ebbestad and Budd 2003 Forey 1991 Froelich 2002 Adnet and Capetta 2001 Adrain and Westrop 2001 Adrain and Edgecomb 1997 Allmon 1996 Allmon 1996 Alroy 1995 Author some analyses (see text). Other vertebrates Mammals Brachiopods Trilobites Cnidarians Other arthropods Other vertebrates Mammals Other vertebrates Mammals Mammals Echinoderms Mammals Molluscs Brachiopods Trilobites Molluscs Molluscs Mammals Trilobites Trilobites Other vertebrates Group Sarcopterygii Equidae Strophemenata Burlingiidae Mastodonsauroidea Ctenodactyloidea Dendrophylliidae Nektaspida Crocodylia Terricola Plesiadapiformes Blastoidea Dicynodontia Corbulidae Athyridida Illaenidae Turritellidae Turritellidae Hipparionini Encrinurine Ptychaspididae Squaliformes AF F AF F AF AF F AF AF G AF AF AF F AF G F F SB SB F AF Domain G S S S G G G/SG G/S S S S G G G/S F/SB S G/SG S S S S G Unit 31 14 9 16 21 17 30 9 61 16 14 68 20 12 36 19 51 36 17 31 12 23 N 56 47 15 19 38 26 10 12 164 3 32 94 53 70 37 17 14 30 56 40 16 29 Nchar Scythian–Recent Eocene Ashgill–Llandovery Mid-upper Cambrian Cretaceous–Recent Cambrian– Ordovician Permian–Triassic Eocene–Miocene Cretaceous–Recent .5–0 MYA Paleocene–Eocene Llandeilo–Kazanian Kazanian–Anisian Cretaceous–Recent Ordovician–Jurassic Mid–late Ordovician Late Cretaceous–Recent Paleocene–Eocene Miocene–Pliocene Telychian–Ludfordian Sunwaptan–Ibexian Late Jurassic–Pleistocene Geologic range M T M M T T M M M/FW T T M T M M M M M T M M M Realm MY L L S MY MY MY MY MY MY S S S MY MY S MY MY MY S/L S MY DUR N N N Y Y Y N Y N N Y Y Y N Y N N N Y Y Y Y DUR-I continued Inferred durations only Teeth only [OL 4] Inferred durations only Teeth only Notes some notes mentioned in the text. In particular, parentheses indicate those datasets overlapping with other datasets (OL N = overlapping with dataset number N from column 1) that were removed for whether the durations (DUR) are measured in millions of years (MY), stages (S), or manually measured on range charts (L), and finally if inferred durations (DUR-I) were available. The last column records (N), the number of characters used in the analyses (Nchar), the geologic range represented in the studies, the biological realm in which the clades are found (M = marine, FW = freshwater, T = terrestrial), of the studies (where AF = above family, F = family, SB = subfamily, SG = genus, G = genus), the taxonomic unit whose characters were coded (as before, with S = species), the number of taxa involved Table 1. The references used in the analyses, the groups they represent (a general grouping followed by the latin name of the domain or the higher level grouping encompassing the domain), the domain LEE HSIANG LIOW 39 38 34 35 36 37 30 31 32 33 29 27 28 25 26 24 23 Continued. Gahn and Kammer 2002 Grande and Bemis 1998 Hopkins 2004 Jeffery and Emlet 2003 Jeffery 1998 Karasawa and Kato 2003 Leighton and Maples 2002 Michaux 1989 Monks 1999 Monks 2002 Monks and Owen 2000 Nutzel et al. 2000 O’Keefe 2004 Popov et al. 1999 Roopnarine 2001–2001 Roopnarine 2001–2002 Roopnarine 2001–2003 Author Table 1. Molluscs Molluscs Molluscs Other vertebrates Brachiopods Molluscs Molluscs Molluscs Molluscs Brachiopods Brachiopods Echinoderms Other arthropods Mammals Echinoderms Other vertebrates Echinoderms Group Chione Puberella Subulitoidea Sauropterygia Atrypida Chione Ancillinae Ancylocertina Hamitidae Orbirhynchia Productida Cyclaster Goneplacidae Rodentia Temnopleurids Amiidae Botryocrinidae G G AF AF AF G SB AF F G AF G F G AF F G Domain S S G G/S S S S S S S G S G S S S S Unit 13 17 11 12 25 16 20 25 23 16 14 10 15 9 16 22 10 N 13 20 16 88 27 20 36 26 30 22 24 22 45 30 38 46 14 Nchar Oligocene–Recent Oligocene–Recent Devonian–Triassic Jurassic Ordovician Oligocene–Recent Eocene–Recent Lower Albian–upper Albian Lower Albian–upper Turonian Albian–Campanian Givetian–Pennsylvanian Late Cretaceous–Paleogene Paleogene–Recent 38–15 MYA Eocene–Pliocene Cretaceous–Recent Mississippian Geologic range M M M M M M M M M M M M M T M M/FW M Realm MY MY MY MY L MY MY MY MY MY S S MY MY MY MY L DUR N N Y Y N N N Y Y Y N N N Y N N N DUR-I continued [OL 37] [OL 32] Teeth only Notes MORPHOLOGY AND LINEAGE DURATIONS EVOLUTION APRIL 2007 889 Continued. 890 EVOLUTION APRIL 2007 Wagner (2000) Wagner (2000) Wagner (2000) Wagner (2000) Wagner (2000) Wagner (2000) Wagner (2000) Yates and Warren (2002) Liow Liow Liow Liow 55 56 57 58 59 60 61 62 63 64 65 66 40 Roopnarine 2001–2004 41 Schneider 1995 42 Smith 1988 43 Smith and Arbizu 1987 44 Smith et al. 1995 45 Smith and Wright 1993 46 Tinn and Meidla 2004 47 Vermeij and Carlson 2000 48 Wagner 1999 49 Wagner 1997 50 Wagner 1997 51 Wagner 1997 52 Wagner 1997 53 Wagner 1997 54 Wagner (2000) Author Table 1. Cardiidae Molluscs Echinoderms Echinoderms Rapaninae Lophospiroida Rostrochoncha Ribeiriidae Technophoridae Bransoniidae Hippocardiidae Paleozoic gastropods Euomphaloid Pleurotomarioid Trochoid Murchisonioid Microdomatoid Trochonematoid Macluritoid Temnospondyli Molluscs Molluscs Molluscs Molluscs Molluscs Molluscs Molluscs Molluscs Ostracodes Ostracodes Ostracodes Ostracodes Curfsina Opimocythere Phalcocythere Schizoptocythere Beyrichiocopa Ostracodes Molluscs Molluscs Molluscs Molluscs Molluscs Molluscs Molluscs Other Vertebrates Ophiuroidea Echinoderms Echinoderms Agelacrinitinae Puberella Molluscs Group G G G G AF AF AF AF AF AF AF AF AF AF F F F F AF SB AF AF AF F AF SB G Domain S S S S S S S S S S S G S S S S S S S G/S S SB G G/SG G G S Unit 29 17 30 16 67 202 13 66 12 15 15 34 82 154 27 17 22 39 481 7 16 8 9 146 167 85 107 61 57 67 60 91 126 46 62 50 68 217 36 34 35 39 28 41 14 29 32 16 29 32 13 12 Mid Albian–Thanetian Upper Albian-mid Miocene Upper Maestrichtian—Oligocene Lower Santonian–mid Miocene Early Tremadoc–Eifelian Early Tremadoc–Eifelian Llanvirn–early Ludlow Early Arenig–Eifelian Early Caradoc–late Ludlow Early Caradoc–Late Ludlow Mid-Cambrian–Ashgill Carboniferous–Jurassic Cassinian–Pridoli Early Cambrian–Capitanian Early Cambrian–upper Caradoc Early-mid Cambrian–Ashgill Upper Arenig–upper Caradoc Llanvirn–Serpukhovian Early Cambrian–Givetian Eocene–Recent Early–Middle Ordovician (Permian) Triassic– Recent Jurassic–Recent Triassic–Recent Ordovician–Carboniferous Ordovician–Carboniferous Oligocene–Recent Nchar Geologic range 15 19 N M M M M M M M M M M M T M M M M M M M M M M M M M M M MY MY MY MY MY MY MY MY S MY MY MY S S S S S S MY/S MY S MY MY MY MY S MY N N N N N N N N N N N Y N N N N N N N N N Y Y Y N N N Families deleted [OL 55–61] [OL 50–53] [OL 38] Realm DUR DUR-I Notes LEE HSIANG LIOW MORPHOLOGY AND LINEAGE DURATIONS The data treatment here is similar to two previous analyses of lineage duration versus morphological distributions (Liow 2004, 2006), but with two crucial improvements. First, long-duration lineages are dynamically defined, not static subsets of the lineages in question. Second, long-duration lineages are compared with short-duration lineages in groups (group analyses) as well as individually (single analyses) (see below and Fig. 2). The relative morphological distance of each taxon in a given dataset is calculated as the sum of the distance of each of its character state from each corresponding average character state of the dataset. Both unweighted distances and weighted distances where each character contributes equally to the total distance of a taxon from the average of the given dataset were calculated. An average character state is calculated as either the modal or mean character state. The latter is reasonable for binary and ordered multistate characters, but less so for unordered multistate characters. Hence unordered multistate characters are converted into binary characters by coding the modal character state as “0” and all other character states as “1.” Numerical or ordered multistate characters having character states with a minimum of “6” are log transformed (new value = ln (old value+2)) so that they will not dominate the calculation of morphological distances in unweighted treatments. Some of the datasets included here were originally assembled for cladistic analyses. The exceptions are the four ostracode species datasets coded for this study and Wagner’s Paleozoic gastropod datasets used for analyses of morphospace occupation as well as for phylogenetic analyses. There may be a concern that cladistic datasets represent a biased sample of morphological characters, relative to phenetic morphological character datasets (e.g., Foote 1999; Liow 2006). However, the cladistic nature of the datasets does not affect my analyses for the following reasons. Cladistic datasets consist of an array of presumably evolutionarily significant pleisiomorphic and derived characters obtained from a sampling of the morphology of the organisms they describe. Cladistic data are hence valuable in this attempt to capture relative morphological distributions. In the current analyses, outgroup taxa used to polarize cladistic analyses are removed as they may only be relatively distantly related to the inclusive group and hence can skew the calculation of the grand empirical morphological average. In doing so, some characters in the ingroup taxa may reflect only one state such that they do not contribute to the calculated distance matrices. Likewise, when larger groups are parsed into smaller groups for further analyses, some characters become uniformly represented. These uniform characters are removed from the calculations because they will add to the counts of characters used (Table 1) without contributing to the distance measures described here. The purposeful exclusion of autapomorphies from cladistic datasets may bias results if some lineages have many of them, that is, if a lineage is actually very deviant morphologically from an average because of their large number of unique characters. Two factors counteract this potential bias: (1) the analyses of each dataset involve, in part, relative differences computed in Principal Component space (see later section) such that autapomorphies do not contribute to principal axes and hence also do not contribute to the results; (2) autapomorphies will only systematically bias results calculated using Euclidean type distance metrics, if they are distributed nonrandomly with respect to durations. However, there is no prior expectation that this biased distribution exists. Three types of stratigraphic ranges of taxa are reported in the literature (Table 1). First and most commonly, numerical values or stage names were given by the authors of the papers (MY, Table 1). Ranges from the prior were used directly as durations; the latter were converted to numerical values of the midpoints of the geologic stages using Gradstein et al. (2004). Second, where time intervals not conforming to internationally recognized names were reported, numbers sequentially assigned to reflect their chronological order were used to calculate relative durations (S, Table 1). Finally, if no stage names or durations are given, I measured the illustrated lengths of stratigraphic ranges if they were drawn to scale (L, Table 1). Where values were calculated, durations have a value of zero if a taxon is found only in one named stage (i.e., a singleton taxon). Because rank order statistics are used in this study, allowing singletons to have equal rank order durations is preferable to allowing singletons to have varying rank order durations stemming from differences in the lengths of named stages. These differences in stage length can artificially introduce certainty in duration variation of singletons. In cases where more than one of these types of stratigraphic ranges was available, the data are analyzed with each of them to check for possible differences in results. Inferred geologic range extensions based on phylogenetic inference are reported in some studies (e.g., Bloch et al. 2001; Bodenbender and Fisher 2001). Where such inferences were available (Table 1), I also analyzed these data with the inferred durations (see Lane et al. 2005 for a discussion of types and properties of various inferred durations). In group analyses, taxa with long durations are increasingly inclusive sequential groups (Fig. 2B). First, a long-duration group is simply the taxon with the longest duration (taxon A in Fig. 2B), then the two taxa with the longest durations (taxa A and B in Fig. 2B), then the three taxa with the longest durations (taxa A, B, and C in Fig. 2B) and so on, until half the taxa have been included in the long-duration group. Then I compare the mean relative morphological distance of each long-duration group with that of an equivalent number of randomly selected short-duration taxa, with replacement. This rarefaction is done because sample sizes are different for taxa with longer and shorter durations (see EVOLUTION APRIL 2007 891 LEE HSIANG LIOW A Morphological distance from the grand empirical average B.1 Morphological distance from the grand empirical average also Liow 2004, 2006). The rarefaction was repeated 500 times to determine the frequency with which a given long-duration group has a mean relative morphological distance that is less than the mean calculated for each randomly selected short-duration group. This bootstrap-generated frequency corresponds to the probability of obtaining a long-duration group with a relative morphological distance that is less than or equal to a random draw of an equivalent G number of morphologies of short-duration taxa. The procedure was then reversed such that the probability of obtaining a shortduration group that is more distant from the grand average than observed is tabulated. Combining both sets of probabilities and plotting them against their respective group durations results in a morpho-duration distribution plot (Fig. 1), for which a rank order correlation (Kendall’s tau) and corresponding P-value (p(g)) was C E F A B Lineage duration Group Analysis C.1 Single Analysis C C A A B B B.2 C.2 C C A A B B B.3 C.3 C C A A B B Lineage duration Figure 2. Panel A is a hypothetical plot showing the morphological distance of each taxon (black circles) from the grand empirical average plotted versus its lineage duration. The plots in panels B and C are replicas of panel A. Panel B illustrates a group analysis where sequentially larger groups of taxa (A, A + B, A + B + C, etc.) with longer durations are compared with rarified samples of the remaining taxa of shorter durations to the left of the circled taxa (Fig. 2B.1 through B.3). Panel C illustrates a single analysis where individual taxa (A, then B, then C, etc.) are compared with a randomly picked taxon from the remaining pool of taxa (excluding taxa with longer durations than the one being compared) with shorter durations (Fig. 2C.1 through C.3). Taxa G, E, and F in panel A have the same durations and their median probabilities of being more or less distant from the grand average morphology are used in subsequent analyses for both group and single analyses. 892 EVOLUTION APRIL 2007 MORPHOLOGY AND LINEAGE DURATIONS calculated to determine which of the scenarios illustrated in Figure 1 best describes the dataset in question (tau and p(g) are reported in online Supplementary Materials, Appendix S4). Various taxa with long durations may be individually morphologically less distant from the grand average than expected, when compared with their short-duration relatives. Single analyses were done by sequentially defined long-duration taxa (Fig. 2C). Each long-duration taxon was compared with a randomly selected short-duration taxon, that is, excluding taxa that have longer durations than the long-duration taxon in question. This was repeated 500 times. This bootstrap-generated frequency corresponds to the probability of obtaining a long-duration taxon with a relative morphological distance that is less than or equal to the random draws of morphologies of short-duration taxa. The procedure was then reversed such that the probability of obtaining a short-duration taxon that is more distant from the grand average than observed is tabulated. As described for group analyses, a rank order correlation (Kendall’s tau) and corresponding P-value (p(s)) were then calculated for the combined set of probabilities versus their respective durations. The sign and probability of the correlation were used to determine which of the scenarios illustrated in Figure 1 best describes the dataset in question (tau and p(s) are reported in the online Supplementary Materials, Appendix S4). In both group and single analyses, I repeat the procedures described above but replace relative morphological distances with principal component scores obtained from Principal Component Analysis of distance matrices obtained using the character matrices (Principal Coordinate Analyses [PCO] Gower 1966). This data reduction technique removes redundancy in the original morphological data. The number of scores used is adjusted to explain about 80% of the variance and varies from five to 20, depending on the size of the data matrix. This bootstrap-generated frequency corresponds to the probability of obtaining a long-duration group with a sum of principal component scores that is less than or equal to the same of a random draw of an equivalent number of morphologies of short-duration taxa. These probability values for group and single analyses are plotted against taxon durations and rank order correlations (Kendall’s tau) and their associated probabilities for group and single analyses (p(g, pco) and p(s, pco), respectively) are calculated (see online Supplementary Materials, Appendix S4). Taxa with the same calculated durations could have different bootstrapped probabilities of being morphologically distant from the grand average (e.g., taxa E, F, and G in Fig. 2A). I recorded the median bootstrapped probabilities of taxa with the same calculated duration. These are then used in performing rank order correlations that indicate whether long-duration taxa are less distant from the grand average morphology than by chance alone in both group and single analyses. The abbreviations p(g, m), p(g, m, pco), and p(s, m), p(s, m, pco) are used to indicate the probabilities from the rank order correlations of median bootstrapped probabilities of group analyses, group analyses using PCO values, single analyses, and single analyses using PCO values versus durations, respectively (online Supplementary Materials, Appendix S4). There are multiple ways of quantifying stratigraphic ranges and relative morphological distances, that is, using original and inferred durations, stratigraphic ranges measured in different ways, using the mode or mean character states as the average, or distances or principal coordinate scores. At the minimum, there are 8 combinatory ways of calculating morpho-duration distributions (the Brunet-Lecomte and Chaline 1990 dataset, see Table 1 and online Supplementary Materials, Appendix S4) and at maximum there are 48 ways (the Adrain and Edgecomb 1997 dataset, see Table 1 and online Supplementary Materials, Appendix S4). Correspondingly, significance tests of trends or mopho-duration distributions (Fig. 1) may differ among various data treatments within a dataset. I present all the results obtained (online Supplementary Materials, Appendix S4) using various data treatment variants but summarize whether the taxa represented in a given dataset show a positive, negative, or null morpho-duration distribution using the following criteria. If there is only one significant result in the possible data variants, then the dataset is assumed to demonstrate a null morpho-duration distribution (Fig. 1C). If opposing significant results are in a ratio of one to one, then the relationship is also taken to be nonsignificant. If opposing significant results are in a ratio of more than one to one, then the more commonly represented sign is accepted. For instance, if there are three significantly negative values and only one significantly positive value among the data variants of a given dataset, then this dataset is taken to show a negative morpho-duration distribution (Fig.1B). In addition, I consider the possibility that any conflict of the signs of significant correlation indicates a nonsignificant situation and refer to this as a conservative solution (see Results). Because significant cases are sometimes already removed in this method of summary, Bonferroni corrections that further overcorrect for significance are not used. The datasets used for these analyses differ in their focal taxonomic level. To test whether the difference in taxonomic scale affects conclusions drawn regarding morpho-duration distributions, I first enumerated Linnean taxonomic ranks of the taxa whose morphologies are coded (coded taxon value) as well as that of the domain of the study (domain taxon value), such that 0 = species, 1 = subgenus, 2 = genus, 3 = subfamily, 4 = family, and 5 = above family. I then calculated the taxonomic inclusiveness of each study as domain taxon value minus the coded taxon value. As an illustration, Jeffery and Emlet (2003) studied temnopleurid echinoids (domain taxon value = 5) and coded the morphology of temnopleurid species (coded taxa value = 0), hence the taxonomic inclusiveness is 5. Where there is a mixture of ranks of taxa whose morphologies are coded, I use the mean value of the coded taxon values (e.g., if both genera and subgenera were coded, then the EVOLUTION APRIL 2007 893 LEE HSIANG LIOW This table groups references listed in Table 1 according to whether they demonstrate a positive, NEGative, or null morphoduration distribution (see Fig. 1) in group analyses (GROUP). Results of single analyses (SINGLE) follow. In addition, (raw) mean and median durations (M.y.), standard deviation of durations, beta, alpha, and skew are listed. Durations and standard deviations thereof are calculated only for those datasets where stratigraphic ranges are reported in or convertible to millions of years. An asterisk implies that conservatively, the diagnosis would have reflected a null morpho-duration distribution. Table 2. Study GROUP Adrain and Edgecomb 1997 Allmon 1996 (Table 9) Alvarez et al. 1998 Anderson and Roopnarine 2003 Angielczky and Kurkin 2003 Bloch et al. 2001 Brochu 1997 Brunet-Lecomte and Chaline 1990 Cairns 2001 Damiani et al. 2001 Gahn and Kammer 2002 Jeffery 1998 Leighton and Maples 2002 Monks 2002 Monks and Owen 1999 Opimocythere (this study) Phalcocythere (this study) Popov et al. 1999 Roopnarine 2001 (set 1) Wagner 1997 Wagner 1999 Wagner 2000 (Euomphaloid) Wagner 2000 (Macluritoid) Wagner 2000 (Microdomatoid) Wagner 2000 (Mursonoid) Wagner 1997 (Ribeiriidae) Wagner 1997 (Technophoridae) Wagner 1997 (Bransoniidae) Wagner 1997 (Hippocardiidae) Wagner 2000 (Trochoids) Wagner 2000 (Trochonematoid) Schneider 1995 Adrian and Westrop 2001 Alroy 1995 Amati and Westrop 2004 Caron et al. 2004 Curfsina (this study) Dashzeveg and Meng 1998 Dewing 2004 Ebbestad and Budd 2003 Froelich 2002 Grande and Bemis 1998 Hopkins 2004 Jeffery and Emlet 2003 Karasawa and Kato 2003 Monks 1999 Nutzel et al. 2000 set-2 NEG NEG NEG∗ NEG NEG NEG NEG NEG NEG NEG NEG NEG NEG NEG NEG NEG NEG NEG NEG NEG NEG NEG NEG NEG NEG NEG NEG NEG NEG NEG NEG NEG∗ NS NS NS NS NS NS NS NS NS NS NS NS NS NS NS SINGLE Mean-Dur Med-Dur Sd-dur Beta Alpha Skew NEG NS POS∗ NEG NS NS NEG NEG NEG NS NS NS NS NEG NEG NS NS NS NS NEG NEG NS NEG NEG NS NEG NEG NS NEG NS NEG NS NS NS NS POS NS NS NS NS NEG POS POS NS NS NS NS NA 3.7 43.5 27.8 NA NA 2.9 .2 16.9 2.9 NA NA NA 1.4 2.3 5.3 3.1 NA 1.0 NA NA 5.2 2.1 1.9 4.1 NA NA NA NA 6.2 2.9 88.1 NA 1.6 1.9 22.7 3.6 2.0 NA NA .7 NA 2.2 6.3 13.6 .5 59.5 NA 3.2 33.5 14.8 NA NA 1.8 .1 5.9 .0 NA NA NA .0 .0 1.5 .6 NA .0 NA NA 2.6 .0 .0 .0 NA NA NA NA 6.1 .0 95.0 NA 1.0 .9 14.5 2.0 .0 NA NA .7 NA 1.5 4.0 .0 .0 62.0 NA 2.3 42.9 28.2 NA NA 3.8 .2 23.7 4.9 NA NA NA 2.3 3.2 8.6 5.4 NA 1.5 NA NA 6.4 3.0 2.8 6.9 NA NA NA NA 10.6 4.1 54.0 NA .9 2.1 34.6 5.8 3.2 NA NA .4 NA 1.8 4.4 17.0 1.4 57.4 .7 .6 1.0 1.0 1.7 1.1 1.3 .8 1.4 1.7 1.1 .7 1.2 1.6 1.4 1.6 1.7 .5 1.4 2.3 1.1 1.2 1.5 1.5 1.7 1.4 1.0 3.0 2.8 1.7 1.4 .6 1.8 .6 1.1 1.5 1.6 1.6 .6 .8 .6 2.0 .8 .7 1.3 2.8 1.0 1.7 6.0 44.1 27.5 .4 .5 2.3 .3 12.0 1.7 1.8 5.2 .6 .9 1.7 3.3 1.8 1.8 .7 .1 39.4 4.3 1.4 1.3 2.4 .4 1.0 .1 .1 3.6 2.1 143.8 .3 2.7 1.7 14.9 2.2 1.2 4.2 1.9 1.1 1.2 2.7 9.0 10.8 .2 61.6 1.5 .8 .3 .4 3.0 2.8 1.3 3.7 .6 1.5 1.5 .9 2.7 2.1 1.6 1.1 1.5 1.5 2.3 5.7 .3 1.0 1.7 1.8 1.3 3.2 2.0 7.3 7.4 1.1 1.4 .2 3.8 1.2 1.5 .5 1.3 1.8 1.0 1.4 1.9 1.8 1.2 .7 .6 4.7 .3 continued 894 EVOLUTION APRIL 2007 MORPHOLOGY AND LINEAGE DURATIONS Table 2. Continued. Study GROUP Schizoptocythere (this study) Smith 1988 Smith et al. 1995 Vermeij and Carlson 2000 Wagner 2000 (Paleozoic coiled gastropods) Wagner 2000 (Pleurotomarioid) Adnet and Capetta 2001 Allmon 1996 (Table 1) Bodenbender and Ficher 2001 Forey 1991 Michaux 1989 O’Keefe 2004 Roopnarine 2001 (set 2) Roopnarine 2001 (set 3) Roopnarine 2001 (set 4) Smith and Arbizu 1987 Smith and Wright 1993 Tinn and Meidla 2004 Yates and Warren 2002 NS NS NS NS NS NS POS POS POS POS POS POS POS POS POS POS POS POS POS SINGLE Mean-Dur Med-Dur Sd-dur Beta Alpha Skew NS NS NEG NS POS NS NS NS POS NS NS POS NS NS NS NS NS NS POS 2.7 3.2 59.6 11.4 3.9 4.7 64.3 7.7 NA 7.4 5.2 1.3 1.0 1.2 .9 NA 29.6 NA 8.7 .0 .0 33.5 10.0 .0 .0 62.3 .0 NA .0 4.2 .0 .0 .0 .0 NA 18.0 NA .0 6.0 10.1 70.2 12.3 6.7 7.5 45.2 16.0 NA 17.6 5.2 2.6 1.5 1.6 1.3 NA 33.7 NA 13.6 2.2 3.2 1.2 1.1 1.7 1.6 .7 2.1 1.5 2.4 1.0 2.0 1.4 1.3 1.4 1.4 1.1 .9 1.6 1.3 1.0 50.8 10.3 2.2 3.0 91.3 3.7 .6 3.1 5.2 .7 .7 1.0 .6 2.5 26.0 .8 5.1 1.8 2.0 .3 .6 1.3 1.2 .2 1.0 2.5 1.1 .9 2.5 2.3 2.0 2.5 1.3 .4 2.2 .9 coded taxon value = 1.5). I also tabulated the number of morphological characters used in my analyses, the number of taxa, and whether the group in question is from the aquatic or terrestrial realm. In addition, I calculated mean and median durations for taxa that have stratigraphic ranges reported in millions of years, the standard deviation of the durations, alpha (square of the mean divided by standard deviation of durations) which is a shape parameter, beta (standard deviation divided by square root of alpha) which is a scale parameter and skew (two divided by square root of alpha) (Wackerly et al. 2002). The latter three are descriptors of gamma distributions, which I assume are approximated by the duration distributions of the datasets because durations are always nonnegative and right-skewed. For completeness, I also reanalyzed the data used in Liow (2004, 2006) to compare previous results using this newly developed continuous method of comparing morphological distance versus durations. Results First, a few statements are made to make way for economy and clarity. Weighted and unweighted relative morphological distances do not provide different results in the morpho-duration distributions. Hence, I discuss only the results using weighted relative morphological distances. Similarly, using either grand empirical average morphologies of inclusive groups calculated as sums of mean or modal character states did not in general change resulting patterns of morpho-duration distributions. Likewise, the use of different approaches of quantifying durations did not consistently result in qualitatively different probabilities for the same datasets. Finally, the frequentist approach of determining which particular pattern of morpho-duration distribution best represents the situation in a given dataset (see Methods) removes inconsistencies (see online Supplementary Materials, Appendix S4 for detailed results). A majority of the 66 datasets show a significant negative morpho-duration distribution (Fig. 1B), but positive distributions (Fig. 1A) and null distributions are also represented (Fig. 1C, Tables 2 and 3). Because some datasets were subsets of others or have overlapping taxa (Table 1), I retabulated the number of cases of each of the above excluding the overlaps and found that the rank order of the number of studies of each distribution type was not altered (Table 3). Thus, when taxa are widely sampled, all three of the described scenarios of morpho-duration distributions are observed. In the majority of the cases, relative morphological distances are negatively distributed with respect to durations (a negative morpho-duration distribution). Even using a conservative approach, where a dataset is considered to have a nonsignificant relationship when the signs of the correlations disagree among treatments, datasets that show a negative morpho-duration distribution still predominate (Table 2). Notice that grand means of rank order correlations (tau) do not change in sign when nonsignificant results are used in their calculation (Table 3). In group analyses, datasets showing negative, positive, and null morpho-duration distributions neither have significantly different numbers of taxa represented nor different numbers of coded morphological characters (t-test, P > 0.05). EVOLUTION APRIL 2007 895 LEE HSIANG LIOW Table 3. This is a summary of the results from Table 2 and Online Appendix 4 for GROUP and SINGLE analyses. The first row in each block (positive, negative, and nonsignificant morpho-duration distributions) shows the percentages of datasets that display the respective morpho-duration distributions. Percentages in parentheses exclude potentially overlappping datasets (Table 1). Mean Tau is the grand mean rank order correlation followed by its standard deviation; mean P is the grand mean P value followed by its standard deviation, calculated from the significant cells of each dataset (online Supplementary Materials, Appendix S4). The numbers following in brackets are the same metrics averaged from all the cells in each dataset grouping. Morpho-duration distribution Positive Negative Nonsignificant Percentage Mean Tau Mean P Percentage Mean Tau Mean P Percentage Mean Tau Mean P Datasets demonstrating a positive morpho-duration distribution have marginally significantly greater coded taxon values (0.8) than either those demonstrating a negative one (0.3, t-test, P = 0.09) or a null one (0.3, t-test, P = 0.05). There is no significant difference in either their domain values or taxonomic inclusiveness (t-test, all P > 0.05). Datasets representing organisms from aquatic (marine and freshwater) or terrestrial environments are not differentially represented in the three patterns of morpho-duration distributions (chi-square test, all P > 0.05). In terms of taxonomic representation, there is a significant difference in terms of distribution of mammal representation (chisquare test, P = 0.03). Datasets demonstrating a negative morphoduration distribution are represented by three mammal studies out of a total of 32, no null cases out of a total of 21 and groups having a positive distribution are four out of a total of 13. Other common groups represented in the comparisons, mollusks, echinoderms, and all vertebrates considered together, show no significant differences in frequencies among the three patterns of morpho-duration distribution. Whether the datasets represent only extinct or both extinct and extant lineages, is also not a factor in their distribution among the three patterns of morpho-duration distributions (chi-square test, all P > 0.05). Datasets showing negative, positive, and null morphoduration distributions do not have significantly different mean or median durations, or descriptors of the duration distribution, including the shape parameter alpha, the scale parameter beta and skew (t-tests, all cases P 0.05). The former results are for groups of increasingly inclusive long-duration and short-duration taxa. In contrast, for comparisons of individual taxa with the remaining longer duration or shorter duration taxa in single analyses, most datasets demon- 896 EVOLUTION APRIL 2007 GROUP SINGLE 20 [18] .47 ± .05 [.34 ± .10] .01 ± .01 [.26 ± .13] 48 [50] −.49 ± .09 [−.31 ± .12 ] .01 ± .01 [.27 ± .13] 32 [32] −.08 ± .29 [−.04 ± .13] NA [.36 ± .08 ] 12 [10] .39 ± .10 [.24 ± .22] .02 ± .01 [.30 ± .28] 26 [27] −.46 ± .06 [−.30 ± .29 ] .01 ± .01 [.26 ± .26] 62 [63] −.17 ± .02 [−.01 ± .21] NA [.50 ± .28 ] strated the null morpho-duration distribution. In fewer of the cases, longer duration taxa are individually morphologically closer to the grand average than expected, demonstrating a negative morpho-duration distribution, and in rarely do they have a positive morpho-duration distribution (Table 3). Even after disregarding cases that may be nonindependent, because they stem from studies using overlapping taxa, the percentages of occurrence of each morpho-duration distribution remain similar (Table 3). Datasets consisting only of extinct taxa have lineage durations that are significantly positively correlated with age of the lineages in 16 out of 47 cases (Kendall’s rank test, P < 0.05). The 31 nonsignificant cases show positive correlation coefficients in all but six cases, each with very small negative coefficients (data not shown). Datasets including extant taxa have lineage durations that are significantly positively correlated with age of the lineages in 13 out of 19 cases (Kendall’s rank test, P < 0.05), with the remaining six being nonsignificant (data not shown). Analyses using crinoid genus data (Liow 2004) corroborate the prior results. The crinoid orders where discrete groups of longer duration genera were found to be morphologically less distant from an average than expected in the previous study demonstrated significant negative morpho-duration distributions in this study. In addition, individual compared longer duration taxa (single analyses) demonstrate mainly negative and null morphoduration distributions (Online Appendix 4). Group analyses using trachyleberidid ostracode data (Liow 2006) proved different in some cases in comparison to the conclusions drawn previously (Online Appendix 4). In particular, contemporaneous genera and genera in cohorts originating in the same geologic stage or time interval that were previously thought to show a positive relationship between morphological distance and duration show a negative morpho-duration distribution in this MORPHOLOGY AND LINEAGE DURATIONS new analysis using a dynamic definition of long-duration forms (Online Appendix 4). Discussion Much has been written about stasis at the species level from the view points of paleontology, population biology, genetics, and development (Van Valen 1982; Wake et al. 1983; Rutherford 2000; Merilä et al. 2001; Schwenk and Wagner 2001; Belade et al. 2002; Eldredge et al. 2005; Grether 2005). However, proposed mechanisms maintaining within-population or within-species phenotypic stability do not inform us of the morphological patterns of lineage durations among species and higher-than species lineages. Quantitative comparisons of persistent morphologies are not straightforward because lineages that have comparatively long durations are relatively rare (small sample sizes). The present study is a continued attempt to quantify the relationship between morphology and duration within a rigorous framework. Taxonomic and methodological limitations of two previous attempts (Liow 2004, 2006) have been alleviated. A newly developed sequential rarefaction approach allows durations to be treated as a continuum in analyses relating them to morphologies. Moreover, lineages are also treated as either individually having long durations, or persistent as groups. Lineages with long durations could conceivably be morphologically more distant from the grand average of their inclusive group than under random expectations. This scenario may indicate that being different confers a competitive edge (Fig. 1A, a positive morpho-duration distribution). Conversely, lineages with long durations could be morphologically closer to the grand average than expected (Fig. 1B, a negative morpho-duration distribution). This second scenario may indicate that being average confers flexibility and generality. What do we observe from studying a large suite of independently collected data representing diverse groups? When lineages with longer durations are considered collectively (group analyses), both of the described scenarios as well as the null situation were observed (Fig. 1). However, a greater number of cases display a negative morpho-duration distribution (Table 3), including the genera of crinoid orders reanalyzed using the data from Foote (1999) in Liow (2004), as well as the genera of a large family of ostracodes (using Liow 2006, see online Supplementary Materials, Appendix S4). In contrast, when these long-duration lineages are individually considered (single analyses), null morpho-duration distributions are predominant (Fig. 1C, Table 3). EVOLUTIONARY IMPLICATIONS Datasets showing group positive morpho-duration distributions are more likely to reflect the morphology of genera or families; those showing null or negative group distributions are more likely to reflect the morphology of species or subgenera. A positive morpho-duration distribution suggests that sufficiently distinct morphologies identified as higher level (para)clades that are morphologically deviant from equivalent groups, persist for longer periods of time. Perhaps distinct or deviant morphologies could invade new niches, that is, niches previously unused by related groups, thus encountering minimum competition, allowing for the production of descendents which perpetuate the lineage possessing those morphologies. However, positive morpho-duration distributions are less common than negative ones hence much of the remaining discussion will ruminate upon the latter distribution. When less inclusive lineages in the biological hierarchy (e.g., species or subgenera) are studied in group analyses, the morphoduration distributions observed are frequently negative. This suggests that being similar to morphological forms that have already proven effective for closely related lineages may help promote survivorship or persistence. This may also reflect an above-theindividual-level analogue of genetic compensation, where selection is expected to favor genetic changes that restore the ancestral phenotype, because any change in development caused by perturbations are likely to reduce fitness (Grether 2005). The observation that group analyses of taxa shows significant negative or positive morpho-duration distributions more frequently than individually considered taxa may reflect one of two things. The first less interesting possibility is that there is more statistical power in bigger numbers (i.e., comparing groups rather than individuals). The second possibility is that taxa with longer durations are begetting descendents that are not only morphologically similar to themselves (pulling the grand average morphological value closer to their own morphology) but that also tend to have long durations. This idea may hold for two reasons. First, higher-than-organismal-level properties are heritable (e.g., Jablonski and Hunt 2006). Second, there is at least a 10% probability of finding ancestors in the fossil record (Foote 1996). Even if direct ancestors are not present in the datasets analyzed, very closely related lineages may also produce the same patterns. A variation to this second possibility is that long-duration lineages do not necessarily give rise preferentially to long-duration lineages, but rather, are prolific. In their proliferation, they give rise to both longer and shorter duration descendent lineages, simply by chance. Both types of descendents contribute to the morphological averageness of the long-duration originator by pulling the grand group average closer to the morphology of the originator lineage. This is consistent with Wagner and Erwin’s (1995) finding that lineages that persist for long periods of time give rise to more descendents. It also agrees with William’s desperation hypothesis (1992, p. 132) which proposes that persistent forms often generate numerous ephemeral forms. On the other hand, the current discussion seems to contradict the finding that ancestral species are statistically more likely to become extinct before their coexisting descendents (Pearson 1998). However, rarer ancestral species that persist despite their descendents will have truly long EVOLUTION APRIL 2007 897 LEE HSIANG LIOW durations, and hence are more relevant to the discussions here regarding long-duration taxa in greater inclusive groups. Clearly, more work is required to understand the links between diversification history and morphology. Lineages with longer durations are significantly older. In other words, they occur earlier in their group’s history or have a greater geologic age, or time of first appearance. This pattern holds even when only extinct groups are examined such that age is not a constraint on observed durations. Lineages with longer durations, even though they tend to be old, are not necessarily morphologically similar to very basal forms (Liow 2004) or outgroups in a cladistic context. However they are often morphologically average when compared with lineages in the same inclusive clade through their collective history. These results suggest that “successful” lineages, that is, those that lasted, have benefited by “being there early” and “looking right.” In contrast, “unsuccessful” lineages, that is, those that did not last as long, suffered from some combination of “not being there early” and “not looking right.” That is to say, they appeared later and/or they are experimental forms or morphologically “deviant” forms (see also Williams 1992). This recalls niche preemption as discussed by McKinney (1998, p. 7) wherein the first taxon that occupies the empty “morphological niche” is not subsequently replaced but morphologically emulated. Vermeij (1991) and Rosenzweig and McCord (1991) discuss the idea of niche preemption in the fossil record in terms of invasions and mass extinctions. I suggest that analogous processes of preemption can occur within a clade during time intervals of background extinction, origination and migration. Morpho-duration distributions also have bearings on patterns of morphospace occupation (see Foote 1997; Roy and Foote 1997 for reviews). Temporal trajectories of morphospace distribution of individual lineages have received limited attention. Notably though, it has been shown that as richness increases over time, species are preferentially added to margins of empirical morphospace (Roy et al. 2001, references therein, also Ricklefs 2004). The patterns shown in this study constrains this process such that the addition of morphologically deviant taxa should not shift the grand empirical morphological average of the inclusive clade directionally over time. If long-duration lineages are preferential ancestors to many lineages in the inclusive clade, as suggested in the previous paragraphs, then their descendents, even when morphologically distant from their ultimate ancestor, should be preferentially added to random parts of the margins of empirical morphospace for the both patterns observed here and the findings of Roy et al. (2001) to hold. BIASES AND SOURCES OF ERROR The only efficient way to widely investigate the relationship between morphology and duration is to sample the published lit- 898 EVOLUTION APRIL 2007 erature. However, there are a number of biases and sources of error due to the usage of heterogeneous data collected for other purposes, some of which have already been mentioned. First, empirical values of grand average morphologies are used as reference points but are calculated from incompletely sampled datasets. Hence they may not represent the true average morphology of the (para)clade in question. The completeness of studies could not be ascertained in a straightforward manner from the original publications and hence were not considered here. However, if the morphological characters and the taxa are randomly chosen with respect to lineage durations, the sample of taxa should represent a spread of morphologies that allows a reliable estimate of an average morphology of the group for their known history. Moreover, the groups of datasets representing the three different morpho-duration distributions have undifferentiable distributions of the numbers of taxa and characters include in the analyses, indicating that these factors are not systematically causing bias in the results. Similarly, the characters coded may not adequately represent the whole-organism morphology of the taxa (Table 1 reports cases where it was obvious only a small subset of morphological characters were used). Also, only adult morphologies are reflected in the datasets used here. Second, the taxa analyzed may not actually belong together in a natural group due to incomplete knowledge of the (para)clade. This problem is certainly not unique to this study and the fact that the datasets originate from experts in their respective taxonomic groups gives confidence that best efforts were made to include only members that belong to the natural group in question. In a similar vein, the taxonomic ranks of the taxa analyzed in each dataset may not be equivalent. Although the identified cases are few (Table 1), there could be cases in which the lineages are not actually comparable even though they are hypothesized by the original authors to represent the same ranks. The ranks of stratigraphic ranges may not reflect the true ranks of lineage durations. Rates of preservation can vary such that taxa that in reality have their first or last appearances in poorly preserved intervals will tend not to be sampled in them. Thus they will have significantly shorter observed durations than those taxa that first or last appear in better preserved time intervals and that range through poorly preserved intervals. However, using inferred or raw durations provided largely similar results of morpho-duration distributions for datasets where such inferences were available (16 of the 66 datasets), hence preservation rates biases may not be large. One-sided range-truncations of extant taxa may similarly affect the true rank order of durations. However, those datasets with extant representation do not show a different distribution of results among the three scenarios of morpho-duration distributions. Factors that promote high rates of preservation in the fossil record, for example, wide geographic ranges, numerical abundance, may cause relative overestimations MORPHOLOGY AND LINEAGE DURATIONS of the durations of some taxa. However, it has been shown that even with sampling standardization that takes such high preservation rates into account, those taxa presumed to have long durations remained classified as long-duration taxa (Liow 2007). In addition, morphological factors could be correlated with traits that simultaneously influence preservation and lineage duration. Here it is explicitly assumed that the lineages within datasets are similar enough morphologically such that preservation rates are not likely to differ greatly. In addition, the type of depositional environment and the propensity for preservation as approximated by ecological realms (terrestrial or aquatic) and phylogenetic representation played no systematic part in generating the resulting morpho-duration patterns. Conclusions Many factors can influence the survivorship of individuals, populations and lineages during their lifetimes. These factors may interact in a complex fashion so that it is difficult to tease apart their individual contributions in holding populations and lineages in prolonged stasis, in causing change, or accelerating extinction. Despite this complexity, some factors have been clearly demonstrated to be important contributors to survivorship (Jablonski 2005) and others are beginning to be investigated as potential properties that may confer greater lineage persistence in geologic time. In particular, morphological distribution is predictably related to taxon duration. This relationship can be complicated by taxonomic ranks, which have previously not been explicitly considered in discussions of persistent lineages. Contrary to the common idea that lineages with very long durations are special or unique in some significant way, they often tend to be more average than expected when compared with their relatives. This suggests that deviations from locally optimal solutions, which evolutionary processes have already found are usually not good candidates for longer term survival. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I thank S. Lidgard, L. Van Valen, P. Wagner, D. Jablonski, P. Harnik, and C. Simpson for many helpful discussions on the topic of lineage persistence and morphology. A. McGowan, three anonymous reviewers, and the associate editor C. Janis made numerous suggestions that greatly improved this manuscript. N. C. Stenseth and the folks at CEES supported me with a conducive environment so this work could be completed. I also thank T. F. Hansen who pointed out William’s desperation hypothesis to me. LITERATURE CITED Adnet, S., and H. Cappetta. 2001. A palaeontological and phylogenetical analysis of squaliform sharks (Chondrichthyes : Squaliformes) based on dental characters. Lethaia 34:234–248. Adrain, J. M., and G. D. Edgecomb. 1997. Silurian encrinurine trilobites from the central Canadian Arctic. Palaeontol. Can. 14:1–109. Adrain, J. M., and S. R. Westrop. 2001. Stratigraphy, phylogeny, and species sampling in time and space. Pp. 29121 in J. M. Adrain, G. D. Edgecomb, and B. S. Lieberman, eds. Fossils, phylogeny, & form: an analytical approach. Kluwer/Plenum Press, New York. Allmon, W. D. 1996. Evolution and systematics of Cenozoic American Turritellidae (Gastropoda). 1. Paleocene and Eocene species related to “Turritella mortoni Conrad” and “Turritella humerosa Conrad” from the U.S. Gulf and Atlantic Coastal Plains. The Paleontological Research Institution, Ithaca, NY. Alroy, J. 1995. Continuous track analysis—a new phylogenetic and biogeographic method. Syst. Biol. 44:152–178. Alvarez, F., R. Jia-Yu, and A. J. Boucot. 1998. The classification of athyridid brachiopods. J. Paleontol. 72:827–855. Amati, L., and S. R. Westrop. 2004. A systematic revision of Thaleops (Trilobita: Illaenidae) with new species from the middle and late Ordovician of Oklahoma and New York. J. Syst. Palaeontol. 2:207–256. Anderson, L. C., and P. D. Roopnarine. 2003. Evolution and phylogenetic relationships of Neogene Corbulidae (Bivalvia; Myoidea) of tropical America. J. Paleontol. 77:1086–1102. Angielczyk, K. D., and A. A. Kurkin. 2003. Phylogenetic analysis of Russian Permian dicynodonts (Therapsida : Anomodontia): implications for Permian biostratigraphy and Pangaean biogeography. Zool. J. Linn. Soc. 139:157–212. Anstey, R. L. 1978. Taxonomic survivorship and morphologic complexity in Paleozoic Bryozoan genera. Paleobiology 4:407–418. Baumiller, T. K. 1993. Survivorship analysis of Paleozoic Crinoidea—effect of filter morphology on evolutionary rates. Paleobiology 19:304–321. Beldade, P., K. Koops, and P. M. Brakefield. 2002. Developmental constraints versus flexibility in morphological evolution. Nature 416:844–847. Bloch, J. I., D. C. Fisher, K. D. Rose, and P. D. Gingerich. 2001. Stratocladistic analysis of Paleocene Carpolestidae (Mammalia, Plesiadapiformes) with description of a new late Tiffanian genus. J. Vert. Paleont. 21:119– 131. Bodenbender, B. E., and D. C. Fisher. 2001. Stratocladistic analysis of blastoid phylogeny. J. Paleont. 75:351–369. Boucot, A. J. 1975. Evolution and extinction rate controls. Elsevier Scientific Publishing Company, Amsterdam, The Netherlands. Boyajian, G., and T. Lutz. 1992. Evolution of biological complexity and its relation to taxonomic longevity in the Ammonoidea. Geology 20:983– 986. Brochu, C. A. 1997. Morphology, fossils, divergence timing, and the phylogenetic relationships of Gavialis. Syst. Biol. 46:479–522. Brunet-Lecomte, P., and J. Chaline. 1991. Morphological evolution and phylogenetic-relationships of the European ground voles (Arvicolidae, Rodentia). Lethaia 24:47–53. Buzas, M. A., and J. J. Culver. 1984. Species duration and evolution: benthic Foraminifera on Atlantic continental margin of North America. Science 225:829–830. Cairns, S. D. 2001. A generic revision and phylogenetic analysis of the Dendrophylliidae (Cnidaria: Scleractinia). Smithson. Contrib. Zool. 615:1– 88. Caron, J. B., D. M. Rudkin, and S. Milliken. 2004. A new Late Silurian (Pridolian) naraoiid (Euarthropoda : Nektaspida) from the Bertie Formation of southern Ontario, Canada-Delayed fallout from the Cambrian explosion. J. Paleontol. 78:1138–1145. Chen, Z.-Q., K. Kaihob, and A. D. George. 2005. Survival strategies of brachiopod faunas from the end-Permian mass extinction. Palaeogeog. Palaeoclimat. Palaeoecol. 224:232–269. Damiani, R. J. 2001. A systematic revision and phylogenetic analysis of Triassic mastodonsauroids (Temnospondyli : Stereospondyli). Zool. J. Linn. Soc. 133:379–482. EVOLUTION APRIL 2007 899 LEE HSIANG LIOW Dashzeveg, D., and J. Meng. 1998. New Eocene ctenodactyloid rodents from the Eastern Gobi Desert of Mongolia and a phylogenetic analysis of ctenodactyloids based on dental features. Am. Mus. Novit. 3246:1–20. Dewing, K. 2004. Shell structure and its bearing on the phylogeny of Late Ordovicianarly Silurian strophomenoid brachiopods from Anticosti Island, Quebec. J. Paleontol. 78:275–286. Ebbestad, J. O. R., and G. E. Budd. 2002. Burlingiid trilobites from Norway, with a discussion of their affinities and relationships. Palaeontology 45:1171–1195. Eldredge, N., J. N. Thompson, P. M. Brakefield, S. Gavrilets, D. Jablonski, J. B. C. Jackson, R. E. Lenski, B. S. Lieberman, M. A. McPeek, and W. Miller. 2005. The dynamics of evolutionary stasis. Paleobiology (Suppl.) 31:113–145. Flessa, K. W., K. V. Powers, and J. L. Cisne. 1975. Specialization and evolutionary longevity in the Arthropoda. Paleobiology 1:71–91. Foote, M. 1996. On the probability of ancestors in the fossil record. Paleobiology 22:141–151. ———. 1997. The evolution of morphological diversity. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 28:129–152. ———. 1999. Morphological diversity in the evolutionary radiation of Paleozoic and post-Paleozoic crinoids. Paleobiology 25:1–115. Forey, P. L. 1991. Latimeria chalumnae and its pedigree. Environ. Biol. Fishes 32:75–97. Froehlich, D. J. 2002. Quo vadis eohippus? The systematics and taxonomy of the early Eocene equids (Perissodactyla). Zool. J. Linn. Soc. 134:141– 256. Gahn, F. J., and T. W. Kammer. 2002. The cladid crinoid Barycrinus from the Burlington Limestone (early Osagean) and the phylogenetics of Mississippian botryocrinids. J. Paleontol. 76:123–133. Gilinsky, N. L. 1988. Survivorship in the Bivalvia: comparing living and extinct genera and families. Paleobiology 14:370–386. Gower, J. C. 1966. Some distance properties of latent root and vector methods used in multivariate analysis. Biometrika 53:325–338. Gradstein, F. M., J. G. Ogg, and A. G. Smith. 2004. A geologic time scale 2004. Cambridge Univ. Press, Cambridge, U.K., New York Grande, L., and W. E. Bemis. 1998. A comprehensive phylogenetic study of amiid fishes (Amiidae) based on comparative skeletal anatomy. An empirical search for interconnected patterns of natural history. J. Vertebr. Paleontol. 18:1–690. Grether, G. F. 2005. Environmental change, phenotypic plasticity, and genetic compensation. Am. Nat. 166:115–123. Hansen, T. A. 1978. Larval dispersal and species longevity in lower Tertiary gastropods. Science 199:885–887. Hopkins, S. S. B. 2004. Phylogeny and biogeography of the genus Ansomys Qiu, 1987 (Mammalia : Rodentia : Aplodontidae) and description of a new species from the Barstovian (Mid-Miocene) of Montana. J. Paleontol. 78:731–740. Jablonski, D. 1986a. Background and mass extinctions: the alternation of macroevolutionary regimes. Science 231:129–133. ———. 1986b. Larval ecology and macroevolution in marine invertebrates. Bull. Mar. Sci. 39:565–587. ———. 1994. Extinctions in the fossil record. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. 334:11–16. ———. 2005. Mass extinctions and macroevolution. Paleobiology 31:192– 210. Jablonski, D., and D. M. Raup. 1995. Selectivity of end-Cretaceous marine bivalve extinctions. Science 268:389–391. Jablonski, D., and G. Hunt. 2006. Larval ecology, geographic range, and species survivorship in Cretaceous mollusks: organismic versus specieslevel explanations. Am. Nat. 168:556–564. Jackson, J. B. C. 1974. Biogeographic consequences of eurytopy and stenotopy 900 EVOLUTION APRIL 2007 among marine bivalves and their evolutionary significance. Am. Nat. 108:541–560. Jeffery, C. H. 1998. Carrying on regardless: the echinoid genus Cyclaster at the Cretaceous-Tertiary boundary. Lethaia 31:149–157. Jeffery, C. H., and R. B. Emlet. 2003. Macroevolutionary consequences of developmental mode in temnopleurid echinoids from the Tertiary of southern Australia. Evolution 57:1031–1048. Jernvall, J., and M. Fortelius. 2004. Maintenance of trophic structure in fossil mammal communities: site occupancy and taxon resilience. Am. Nat. 164:614–624. Kammer, T. W., T. K. Baumiller, and W. I. Ausich. 1997. Species longevity as a function of niche breadth: evidence from fossil crinoids. Geology 25:219–222. ———. 1998. Evolutionary significance of differential species longevity in Osagean-Meramecian (Mississippian) crinoid clades. Paleobiology 24:155–176. Karasawa, H., and H. Kata. 2003. The family Goneplacidae MacLeay, 1838 (Crustacea: Decapoda: Brachyura): systematics, phylogeny, and fossil records. Paleontol. Res. 7:129–151. Koehl, M. A. R. 1996. When does morphology matter? Annu. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 27:501–542. Lane, A., C. M. Janis, and J. J. Sepkoski. 2005. Estimating paleodiversities: a test of the taxic and phylogenetic methods. Paleobiology 31:21–34. Leighton, L. R., and C. G. Maples. 2002. Evaluating internal versus external characters: Phylogenetic analyses of the Echinoconchidae, Buxtoniinae, and Juresaniinae (phylum Brachiopoda). J. Paleontol. 76:659–671. Levinton, J. S., and L. Ginzburg. 1984. Repeatability of taxon longevity in successive Foraminifera radiations and a theory of random appearance and extinction. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 81:5478–5481. Liow, L. H. 2004. A test of Simpson’s “Rule of the survival of the relatively unspecialized” using fossil crinoids. Am. Nat. 164:431–443. ———. 2006. Do deviants live longer? Morphology and longevity in trachyleberidid ostracodes. Paleobiology 32:55–69. ———. 2007. Does versatility as measured by geographic range, bathymetric range and morphological variability contribute to taxon longevity? Glob Ecol Biogeogr 16:117–128. McGowan, A. J. Does shape matter? Morphological diversity and differential survivorship among Triassic ammonoid genera. Hist. Biol. In press. McKinney, M. L. 1998. Biodiversity dynamics: niche preemption and saturation in diversity equilibria. Pp. 1–15 in M. L. McKinney and J. A. Drake, eds. Biodiversity dynamics: turnover of populations, taxa, and communities. Columbia Univ. Press, New York. Merilä, J., B. C. Sheldon, and L. E. B. Kruuk. 2001. Explaining stasis: microevolutionary studies in natural populations. Genetica 112–113:199– 222. Michaux, B. 1989. Cladograms can reconstruct phylogenies—an example from the fossil record. Alcheringa 13:21–36. Monks, N. 1999. Cladistic analysis of Albian heteromorph ammonites. Palaeontology 42:907–925. Monks, N. 2002. Cladistic analysis of a problematic ammonite group: The Hamitidae (Cretaceous, Albian-Turonian) and proposals for new cladistic terms. Palaeontology 45:689–707. Monks, N., and E. Owen. 2000. Phylogeny of Orbirhynchia pettitt, 1954 (Brachiopoda : Rhynchonellida). Palaeontology 43:871–880. Nutzel, A., D. H. Erwin, and R. H. Mapes. 2000. Identity and phylogeny of the late Paleozoic Subulitoidea (Gastropoda). J. Paleontol. 74:575–598. Oji, T. 1996. Is predation intensity reduced with increasing depth? Evidence from the west Atlantic stalked crinoid Endoxocrinus parrae (Gervais) and implications for the Mesozoic marine revolution. Paleobiology 22:339–351. O’Keefe, F. R. 2004. Preliminary description and phylogenetic position of a MORPHOLOGY AND LINEAGE DURATIONS new Plesiosaur (Reptilia : Sauropterygia) from the Toarcian of Holzmaden, Germany. J. Paleontol. 78:973–988. Pearson, P. N. 1998. Speciation and extinction asymmetries in paleontological phylogenies: evidence for evolutionary progress? Paleobiology 24:305– 335. Popov, L. E., I. F. Nikitin, and E. V. Sokiran. 1999. The earliest atrypides and athyridides (brachiopoda) from the Ordovician of Kazakhstan. Palaeontology 42:625–661. Raup, D. M. 1985. Mathematical models of cladogenesis. Paleobiology 11:42– 52. Ricklefs, R. E. 2004. Cladogenesis and morphological diversification in passerine birds. Nature 430:338–341. Roopnarine, P. D. 2001. A history of diversification, extinction, and invasion in tropical America as derived from species-level phylogenies of chionine genera (Family Veneridae) (Reprinted). J. Paleontol. 75:644–657. Roy, K., and M. Foote. 1997. Morphological approaches to measuring biodiversity. TREE 12:277–281. Roy, K., D. P. Balch, and M. E. Hellberg. 2001. Spatial patterns of morphological diversity across the Indo-Pacific: analyses using strombid gastropods. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B. 268:2503–2508. Rosenzweig, M. L., and R. D. McCord. 1991. Incumbent replacement— evidence for long-term evolutionary progress. Paleobiology 17:202–213. Rutherford, S. L. 2000. From genotype to phenotype: buffering mechanisms and the storage of genetic information. BioEssays 22:1095–1105. Schopf, J. W. 1994. Disparate rates, differing fates: tempo and mode of evolution changed from the Precambrian to the Phanerozoic. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 91:6735–6742. Schneider, J. A. 1995. Phylogeny of the Cardiidae (Mollusca, Bivalvia): Protocardiinae, Laevicardiinae, Lahilliinae, Tulongocardiinae subfam. n. and Pleuriocardiinae subfam. n. Zool. Scr. 24:321–346. Schwenk, K., and G. P. Wagner. 2001. Function and the evolution of phenotypic stability: connecting pattern to process. Am. Zool. 41:552–563. Simpson, G. G. 1944. Tempo and mode in evolution. Columbia Univ. Press, New York. ———. 1953. The major features of evolution. Columbia Univ. Press, New York. Schneider, J. A. 1995. Phylogeny of the Cardiidae (Mollusca, Bivalvia): Protocardiinae, Laevicardiinae, Lahilliinae, Tulongocardiinae subfam. n. and Pleuriocardiinae subfam. n. Zool. Scr. 24:321–346. Smith, A. B. 1988. Patterns of diversification and extinction in Early Paleozoic echinoderms. Palaeontology 31:799–828. Smith, A. B., and M. A. Arbizu. 1987. Inverse larval development in a Devonian Edrioasteroid from Spain and the phylogeny of Agelacrinitinae. Lethaia 20:49–62. Smith, A. B., G. L. J. Paterson, and B. Lafay. 1995. Ophiuroid phylogeny and higher taxonomyorphological, molecular and paleontological perspectives. Zool. J. Linn. Soc. 114:213–243. Smith, A. B., and C. W. Wright. 1993. British Cretaceous echinoids. Part 3, Stirodonta 2 (Hemicidaroida, Arbacioida and Phymosomatoida, Part 1). Monogr. Palaeontol. Soc. 593:199–267. Stanley, S. M. 1979. Macroevlution. The John Hopkins Univ. Press, Baltimore and London. Tinn, O., and T. Meidla. 2004. Phylogenetic relationships of Early-middle Ordovician ostracods of Baltoscandia. Palaeontology 47:199–221. Van Valen, L. 1982. Integration of species: stasis and biogeography. Evol. Theory 6:99–112. Vermeij, G. J. 1991. Anatomy of an invasion —the trans-Arctic interchange. Paleobiology 17:281–307. Vermeij, G. J., and S. J. Carlson. 2000. The muricid gastropod subfamily Rapaninae: phylogeny and ecological history. Paleobiology 26:19–46. Wackerly, D. D., W. Mendenhall III, and R. L. Scheaffer. 2002. Mathematical statistics with applications. Duxbury. Wagner, P. J. 1997. Patterns of morphologic diversification among the Rostroconchia. Paleobiology 23:115–150. ———. 1999. The utility of fossil data in phylogenetic analyses: a likelihood example using Ordovician-Silurian species of the Lophospiridae (Gastropoda : Murchisoniina). Am. Malacol. Bull. 15:1–31. ———. 2000. Exhaustion of morphologic character states among fossil taxa. Evolution 54:365–386. Available from http://pjw3.fmnh. org/EvolutionMatrices˙2000.html. Accessed jan. 2006. Wagner, P. J., and D. H. Erwin. 1995. Phylogenetic patterns as tests of speciation models. Pp. 87–122 in D. H. Erwin and R. L. Anstey, eds. New Approaches to Speciation in the Fossil Record. Columbia Univ. Press, New York. Wainwright, P. C., and S. M. Reilly. 1994. Ecological morphology: integrative organismal biology. Univ. Chicago Press, Chicago. Wake, D. B., G. Roth, and M. H. Wake. 1983. On the problem of stasis in organismal evolution. J. Theo. Biol. 101:211–224. Ward, P. D., and P. W. Signor. 1983. Evolutionary tempo in Jurassic and Cretaceous ammonites. Paleobiology 9:183–198. Williams, G. C. 1992. Natural selection: domains, levels and challenges. Oxford Univ. Press, New York and Oxford. Wills, M. A. 2001. Morphological disparity : a Primer. Pp. 55–144 in J. M. Adrain, G. D. Edgecombe and B. S. Lieberman, eds. Fossils, phylogeny, and form: an analytical approach. Kluwer Academic and Plenum Publishers, New York. Yates, A. M., and A. A. Warren. 2000. The phylogeny of the ’higher’ temnospondyls (Vertebrata : Choanata) and its implications for the monophyly and origins of the Stereospondyli. Zool. J. Linn. Soc. 128:77–121. Associate Editor: C. Janis Supplementary Material The following supplementary material is available for this article: Appendices S1–S4. This material is available as part of the online article from: http://www.blackwell-synergy.com/doi/abs/10.1111/j.1558-5646.2007.00077.x (This link will take you to the article abstract). Please note: Blackwell Publishing is not responsible for the content or functionality of any supplementary materials supplied by the authors. Any queries (other than missing material) should be directed to the corresponding author for the article. EVOLUTION APRIL 2007 901