Viral Delivery of Recombinant Growth Hormone to Rescue Effects of Chronic Stress on Hippocampal Learning by Christopher M. Saenz B.S. Biology Hunter College, CUNY 2007 SUBMITTED TO THE DEPARTMENT OF THE BRAIN AND COGNITIVE SCIENCES IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF MASTER OF SCIENCE IN NEUROSCIENCE AT THE MASSACHUSETTS INSTITUTE OF TECHNOLOGY February 2012 MASSACHUSE~T NrrJr ACUSETTS INSTriUTE OF TECHNOLOGY FEB 21 2012 © 2012 Christopher M. Saenz. All rights reserved. The author hereby grants to MIT the permission to reproduce and to distribute publicly paper and electronic copies of this thesis document in whole or in part in any medium noYw nown or hereafter created. ARCHrES Signature of Author: Departme if6B in'and Cognitive Sciences January 31, 2012 Al Certified by: JI), Ki A. Goosens Assistant Professor, Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences 'W Accepted by: L/ Earl K. Miller Picower Professor of Neuroscience Chairman, Committee for Graduate Education THIS PAGE HAS INTENTIONALLY BEEN LEFT BLANK Viral Delivery of Recombinant Growth Hormone to Rescue Effects of Chronic Stress on Hippocampal Learning by Christopher M Saenz Submitted to the Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences on January 31, 2012 in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Master of Science in Neuroscience ABSTRACT Chronic stress has been linked to variation in gene regulation in the hippocampus (HIP) among other areas. These lead to cytoskeletal and volumetric rearrangements in various nuclei of the central nervous system and are thought to contribute to several stress-sensitive disorders. One such gene that has been shown to be downregulated in HIP in response to stress is somatotropin, colloquially known as growth hormone (GH). These experiments were conducted to develop a novel assay for examination of working memory in rats and explore the nature of stress-induced impairment of hippocampal function and determine whether infusion of a modified herpes simplex virus (HSV) carrying the recombinant rodent growth hormone (GH) would be sufficient to restore normal hippocampal function. After 21 days of chronic immobilization stress (CIS), animals received bilateral infusions into the dorsal HIP of 2pl HSV carrying either GH with green florescent protein (GFP) or GFP only. On the second day following the infusion, the animals received trace conditioning, a HIP-dependent task, with five tone-shock pairings of a 16 second tone followed by a 30 second trace interval terminating with a 1 second 0.85 milliamp footshock. An inter-trial interval of 3 minutes was used to separate the tone-shock pairings. The following day the animals were tested for fear to the context and for fear to the tone in a novel context, measured by amount of time the animal spent freezing. Using this criterion, animals that had undergone stress that received the control vector were less likely to freeze when presented with the tone, indicating an impairment of hippocampal function. Viral-mediated overexpression of GH in the dorsal HIP was able to reverse the CIS-related impairment in hippocampal function. ELISA was used to verify the expression of GH from the infused vector. These experiments may yield future directions of investigation for stress-based disorders. Thesis Supervisor: Ki Ann Goosens Title: Assistant Professor Acknowledgements "there is a loneliness in this world so great that you can see it in the slow movement of the hands of a clock. people so tired mutilated either by love or no love. people just are not good to each other one on one. the rich are not good to the rich the poor are not good to the poor. we are afraid. our educational system tells us that we can all be big-ass winners. it hasn't told us about the gutters or the suicides. or the terror of one person aching in one place alone untouched unspoken to watering a plant." - Charles Bukowski excerpt from "The Crunch" from Love is a dog from hell I would also like to take this time to acknowledge the lives of the animals used in these experiments. Table of Contents Introduction The hippocampus and trace conditioning The hippocampus and context conditioning The role of growth hormone in the hippocampus 6 7 10 11 Aims 12 Experiment 1: Exploration of trace footshock conditioning Materials and Methods Results 14 14 15 Experiment 2: Investigation of effects of chronic immobilization stress on trace and 17 context conditioning 17 Materials and Methods 19 Results Experiment 3: Determination of level of functional output of the virus delivering 22 recombinant growth hormone to hippocampus 22 Materials and Methods 23 Results Discussion 24 Figures 28 References 37 Introduction Chronic stress leads to changes in the brain, including a region called the hippocampus (HIP). These changes include atrophy of the dendritic arbors of HIP neurons, reduction of HIP volume, and loss of plasticity (McEwen, 1994; 1999; Sapolsky, 2000; Vyas et al., 2002; Donohue et al., 2006). Cognitive changes, seen in humans as well as rodents, include deficits in HIP-dependent processes such as working memory, which can be measured, in one way, by trace conditioning(Swaab et al., 2005; Hales and Brewer, 2010; van Vugt et al., 2010). Although there are likely many mechanisms leading to these changes, variation in expression of trophic factors, including growth hormone (GH), has been shown to accompany chronic stress (Donahue et al., 2006). Additionally, GH has been shown to be vital to longterm potentiation (LTP) in the HIP, a process of synaptic strengthening, (Zearfoss et al., 2008), and leads to variation in hippocampal gene expression through the JAKSTAT and MEK-ERK pathways, two pathways shown to be important for learning and memory (Rosenfeld and Hwa, 2009). Based on many studies linking stress to mental disorders(Esch et al., 2002; Alfonso et al., 2005; Swaab et al., 2005; Bale, 2006), study of these phenomena may provide important novel insight into the etiologies of several stress-linked disorders such as post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), major depressive disorder (MDD) and anxiety disorders (AD) (Sapolsky, 2000; Gilbertson et al., 2002; Parker and Schatzberg, 2003; Alfonso et al., 2005; Swaab et al., 2005; Bale, 2006; Spijker and van Rossum, 2009). The HIP has been shown to be vital to many aspects of learning and memory including spatial memory, short-term memory and consolidation from short to long- term memory(Best et al., 2001; Hales and Brewer, 2010; van Vugt et al., 2010; Epsztein et al., 2011; Fujisawa and Buzsaki, 2011; Vann and Albasser, 2011). Another function of the HIP is recollection of the sequence in which events occurred(Lee and Wilson, 2002). There are cells that fire in HIP in response to locations in space, termed place cells(Best et al., 2001; Brun, 2002; McNaughton et al., 2006; Moser et al., 2008; Epsztein et al., 2011); firing in individual cells is linked to specific locations in space, which are stable over time. These locations are termed the "place field" of that place cell. This phenomenon has been shown to be a mechanism by which an organism can retain memory of a place(Best et al., 2001; Frankland et al., 2004; Esclassan et al., 2009; Acheson et al., 2011). Chronic stress leads to deterioration of this capability and contributing factors could be the aforementioned HIP volumetric and cellular atrophy(Luine et al., 1994). In this series of studies, I explored a behavioral protocol by which these changes can be assayed and used them to assess a method of amelioration of such changes. The hippocampus and trace conditioning Pavlovian conditioning pairs a neutral conditional stimulus (CS) with an unconditional stimulus (US) to elicit a conditional response (CR). For example, some Pavlovian learning tasks involve pairing of a tone CS with a reward as the US; the CR for this task is traditionally a measure of the animals prediction of reward such as salivation or a nose poke into a reward port (Fanselow and Poulos, 2005; Delamater, 2012). During a different type of Pavlovian learning, auditory fear conditioning, an auditory CS is paired with an aversive US such as a footshock. During this training, the animal learns to associate at least two stimuli with the occurrence of the footshock. First, he learns that the context, the environment in which the animal is conditioned, including the shape of the chamber, sights, and smells, is a predictor of the aversive stimulus. Second, he learns that the auditory cue predicts the occurrence of footshock(Otto and Poon, 2006). Several structures, including the amygdala, prelimbic cortex and anterior cingulate cortex, form the neural substrate for Pavlovian conditioning(Frankland et al., 2004; Dityatev and Bolshakov, 2005; Bangasser, 2006; Krasne, 2011). Auditory delay conditioning, in which the CS and US co-terminate, does not require the hippocampus, though trace conditioning does(Bangasser, 2006; Esclassan et al., 2009). In this variation of fear conditioning, the aversive US follows the termination of the CS after a temporal delay. Unlike auditory delay conditioning, both context conditioning and auditory trace conditioning require the HIP to establish the connection between the CS and US(Bangasser, 2006; Esclassan et al., 2009; Pang et al., 2010; Moita, 2011). Working memory, the ability to retain information in the short term for mental manipulation, has been shown to be one of the fundamental roles for HIP. It has long been speculated that the HIP plays a time-keeping role in memory in several regards. In human fMRI studies of conditional fear, the HIP was shown to be a predictor of the US with level of increase in HIP activity proportionate to accuracy of prediction(Hales and Brewer, 2010). Human electrocorticograph studies corroborate this with increase in the power of gamma oscillations in the hippocampus during working memory tasks(van Vugt et al., 2010). In rats, increases in power of gamma oscillations and coherence of gamma oscillations between the CA1 and CA3 subfields of the HIP were shown to be fundamental to a Tmaze working memory task (Montgomery and Buzsaki, 2007). These oscillations are the sum activity of local networks of neurons and are a mechanism by which the actions of many neurons are coordinated. Coordinated activity between the HIP and PFC has been shown to be a fundamental component of working memory, while coordinated activity between HIP and the amygdala is a necessary component of delay conditioning (Bauer et al., 2007; Hales and Brewer, 2010). Trace conditioning studies in rats show a strong increase in neuronal excitability and an increase in spine density in the CA1 subfield of the dorsal HIP(Moyer et al., 1996; Leuner et al., 2003). In rabbits trace conditioning was found to have an effect on CA1 excitability as well(Weible et al., 2006). Furthermore, after hippocampectomy, rabbits were unable to learn CS-US pairings under a trace conditioning paradigm(Moyer et al., 1990). Additionally, training during ripple events, periods of fast spiking activity in relation to the oscillatory activity, in the hippocampus enhanced learning for this task(Nokia et al., 2010). Stimulation of the hypothalamus, leading to release of GH, following trace eye-blink conditioning training led to rapid training on the task and coincided with increased hippocampal activity(Hyvarinen et al., 2006). This last study is particularly of note to this study since GH secretion is regulated by the hypothalamus and overexpression of GH in HIP may mimic this rapid training effect(Muller and Locatelli, 1999). The hippocampus and context conditioning A subset of cells in the hippocampus fire action potentials in response to locations in space. These cells are known as place cells and their fields of activity are known as place fields(Moser et al., 2008). These fields are typically stable for each cell and remain stable day to day. Activity in these cells has been shown to be shifted in response to context conditioning, namely, remapping of the place fields of subsets of cells, presumably as a mechanism by which this structure can discern between different states of an environment(Moita et al., 2004). Testing animals to retention of the context in which they were fear conditioned is how this study assayed HIP spatial memory. Animals that have been exposed to a context and allowed to explore before fear conditioning have been shown to fear that environment on subsequent exposure(Frankland et al., 2004). Electrolytic lesions impair recall as assayed by freezing to fear conditioning context(McNish et al., 1997; Gewirtz et al., 2000). Chemical lesioning studies have shown the HIP to be vital to conditioning to a context and full hippocampectomy completely impairs contextual conditioning(Maren et al., 1997; McEchron et al., 1998; Esclassan et al., 2009; Acheson et al., 2011). Several immediate-early genes including NGF1-B, Arc, and cFos,genes known to be expressed as part of LTP, have also been shown to be upregulated in the HIP of animals that recently experienced fear conditioning(Hertzen and Giese, 2005; Huff et al., 2006; Lonergan et al., 2010; Czerniawski et al., 2011). Scopolamine inactivation of muscarinic acetylcholine receptors systemically administered disrupted context but not tone conditioning suggesting a strong cholinergic component to context conditioning(Anagnostaras et al., 1995). These effects are not due to an increase in motor activity but instead a conditioning insufficiency(Maren et al., 1998). Functionality of the dorsal HIP (dHIP) differs from the ventral HIP (vHIP). dHIP has been shown to be more important to spatial memory and vHIP function seems to be more related to emotional regulation. Lesions of vHIP affect fear expression and anxiety, without effects on non-emotional spatial memory (Bannerman et al., 2003; Trivedi, 2004; Weible et al., 2006). The role of growth hormone in the hippocampus As mentioned above, systemic GH secretion is regulated by the hypothalamus. This release is partially regulated by HIP projections to the hypothalamus(Dedovic et al., 2009). This occurs in a manner such that GH secretion leads to subsequent release of somatostatin, the inhibitor of GH function, as a selfattenuating process(Muller and Locatelli, 1999). Hippocampal neurons in adult animals but not juveniles have been shown to synthesize GH (Donahue et al., 2006). Furthermore, GH levels in whole hippocampi (dorsal and ventral combined) have been found to be elevated in response to an acute stressor(Donahue et al., 2006), while other results suggest a decrease in GH expression in the dHIP after chronic stress (Goosens lab, unpublished data). We are interested in GH because several groups have found it to facilitate synaptic transmission (Mahmoud and Grover, 2006) and promote memory formation (Donahue et al., 2002; 2006; Zearfoss et al., 2008). A role for GH in declarative memory was also found in humans, by exploiting the negative feedback mechanism of release of GH to inhibit production of GH during the trial; a study in which inhibition of GH release led to impairment of ability to recall previously learned word-pairs, a measure of memory(Hallschmid et al., 2011). The second experiment aims to further explore roles for GH in learning and memory by examining effects of overexpression of GH in the stresscompromised brain. Aims This project is divided into three components: 1) exploring auditory trace fear conditioning as a means to assay working and long-term memory; 2) determining whether growth hormone overexpression can repair stress induced deficits in these forms of memory as well as spatial memory; 3) assaying the expression levels of transgenes from vectors developed for this set of experiments using enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay (ELISA). Trace conditioning has been studied in rats before but not extensively. The studies that have been conducted typically consist of eye-blink conditioning during which the CS is a tone or light, the US is a puff of air and the CR is the blinking response. The time scale of the trace interval is approximately 500 ms. To optimize the trace-conditioning paradigm used in the second set of experiments, several key aspects of trace conditioning were investigated. The trace length was varied to develop an understanding of the innate ability of rats to establish the association across a time interval. Shock intensity and length were also varied to determine which US was strong enough to induce a strong relationship with the CS but not so strong that animals would freeze to any new environment on testing day, a generalized fear to novel contexts. These experiments in and of themselves are interesting in that the animals were able to develop the association between the CS and US when the two stimuli were separated by a trace length of 50 s. To explore the manner in which CIS alters hippocampal function, viralmediated gene transfer was used to overexpress rGH and a green florescent reporter (GFP), or GFP only, in the dorsal hippocampus of rats following either CIS or handling (no stress, NS). During this task, the animals were presented with conditional stimuli (CS) separated by trace periods from the unconditional stimuli (US) (Fig. 1). All animals were able to learn the task at the same rate, however, animals that had undergone CIS were less likely to freeze to the tone the next day during retesting, indicating either a deficit in consolidation or recall. Essentially, CIS animals show deficit in ability to link the CS and US and, additionally, HSV-rGH administration into the dorsal hippocampus reversed this effect. ELISA is a popular method to determine the concentration of a substance in a solution. A vital question after the development of the HSV-rGH vector was to determine if the transgene was sufficient to induce over-expression of growth hormone in the HIP. By using this method, it was determined that in HIP infected with this vector, growth hormone was elevated compared to the unstressed controls. I hypothesize there is an optimum trace and shock duration for trace conditioning in rats, CIS impairs ability to pair the CS to the US as assayed by freezing during the trace interval and the rGH-HSV vector will prove efficacious as both a delivery mechanism and restorative to the HIP after CIS. Experiment 1: Exploration of trace footshock conditioning Materials and Methods Animal subjects This set of experiments used two cohorts of animals, each consisting of male rats (250-275g;Taconic). Animals were single-housed in under a 12-hour on, 12hour off light cycle and given ad libitum access to both standard rodent chow and water. The first cohort consisted of 16 rats divided into four groups of four animals that underwent conditioning using 2 different trace intervals and 2 different shock durations. These animals were handled for 2 days prior to the behavior assays described below. The second cohort of animals consisted of 18 animals divided into 3 groups of 6 to investigate three different shock intensities. These animals were also handled for 2 days prior to the behavior experiments. BehaviorAssays. Animals in the first experiment, exploring trace and shock duration, were divided into four groups: 35 s trace with a 1 s shock, 35 s trace with a 2 s shock, 50 s trace with a 1 s shock, 50 s trace with a 2 s shock. These animals underwent a training protocol that consisted of 3 minutes habituation to the chamber and 4 tone shock pairings of a 16s 2kHz tone (CS) followed by the trace interval corresponding to the subject grouping and followed by a 0.5SmA scrambled footshock. 24 hours later animals were presented with a series of 15 tones at 2 10s intervals to test for tone-elicited freezing during the tone, trace interval, and interstimulus interval (ISI) periods in a novel context. All freezing was calculated by VideoFreeze (Med Associates, Inc. St Albans, VT). Statistics were calculated in Prism 4.0 (GraphPad, Inc. La Jolla, CA). The protocol used for the second experiment consisted of 5 tone shock pairings with 16s 2kHz tone CS, a 20s trace interval and a US of 0.40, 0.60, or 0.85 mA scrambled footshocks. These animals were also tested 24 hours later for retention of the training by presenting the 16s CS tone 15 times at 180s intervals in a novel context. A shorter ISI was used in this experiment because the shorter trace represented a smaller part of the ISI compared to the 50s trace for the previous cohort. Analysis was conducted as above. Results Shock duration was found to affect conditioning though trace interval was not (during TRACE: repeated measures ANOVA; effect of trace interval length, F(1,5) = 1.076, p = 0.3824; effect of shock duration, F(1,5) = 2.385, p = 0.0487; interaction of trace length x shock duration, F(1,5) = 0.466, p = 0.7998) (during TONE: repeated measures ANOVA; effect of trace interval length, F(1,5) = 0.595, p = 0.7037; effect of shock duration, F(1,5) = 1.221, p = 0.3104; interaction of trace length x shock duration, F(1,5) = 1.113, p = 0.3631) (during ISI: repeated measures ANOVA; effect of trace interval length, F(1,5) = 2.359, p = 0.0508; effect of shock duration, F(1,5) 1.954, p = 0.0987; interaction of trace length x shock duration, F(1,5) = 1.358, p = 0.2530) (Fig. 2). Freezing for all groups except the 35 s trace + 1 s tone group reached maximum freezing following the first tone shock pairing during both the tone and trace epochs. = During testing, all animals froze to the CS after the first tone presentation and no significant differences were seen in the freezing to the tone (p=ns), during the trace (p=ns) or during the ISI (p=ns) by repeated measures ANOVA (during TRACE: repeated measures ANOVA; effect of trace interval length, F(1,15) = 1.340 p = 01837; effect of shock duration, F(1,15) = 0.987, p = 0.4711; interaction of trace length x shock duration, F(1,15) = 2.674, p = 0.6900) (Fig. 3, top; only trace interval data shown). However, during the period prior to onset of the first tone, 3 of the groups were freezing at levels above 25%, a sign of generalized fear. The exception to this was a trend of the 35s trace + is shock group which froze approximately 20% of the time during the 3 minutes prior to onset of the first tone (ANOVA; effect of trace interval length, F(1,1) = 0.018 p = 0.8950; effect of shock duration, F(1,1) 5.712, p = 0.0341; interaction of trace length x shock duration, F(1,1) = 1.305, p = = 2.755) (Fig. 3, bottom). Analysis of the different shock intensities showed that there is a difference in freezing during the trace interval though not in response to the tone but post-hoc TMP showed no differences (during TONE: repeated measures ANOVA; effect of shock intensity, F(2,8) = 11.54, p = 0.7563; interaction of shock intensity x time, F(2, 8) = 3.383, p = 0.0862) (during TRACE: repeated measures ANOVA; effect of shock intensity, F(2,8) = 4.563, p = 0.0283; interaction of shock intensity x time, F(2,8) = 3.061, p = 0.0065), when the animals were presented with the tone the animals in the 0.85 mA shock group froze more than the other groups (during TONE: repeated measures ANOVA; effect of shock intensity, F(2,15) = 4.426, p = 0.02 19; interaction of shock intensity x time, F(2,15) = 7.454, p = 0.0104)(Fig. 4). Additionally, during the trace interval, a better predictor of the animal's ability to associate the CS and US, the 0.85 mA group froze more as well (during TRACE: repeated measures ANOVA; effect of shock intensity, F(2,15) = 14.65, p = 0.0 003; interaction of shock intensity x time, F(2,15) = 2.405, p = 0.0155). Experiment 2: Investigation of the effects of CIS on trace and context conditioning Materials and Methods Virus generation The gene for somatotropin was cloned into pHSVgfp, a backbone plasmid, to become the HSV-rGH; the control virus was generated using solely the backbone to express GFP, to become the HSV-GFP. Viral plasmids were transfected into 2-2 cells via Lipofectamine (Invitrogen, Inc, Grand Island, NY), which were then superinfected with Sdl1.2 replication deficient HSV1 virus. The 2-2 cells contain the portion of the IE2 gene deleted from the 5d1.2 that allows the packaging of the virus backbone material into functional vectors. After three rounds of amplification, the viruses were purified by centrifugation across a series of 10%, 30% and 60% sucrose gradients. Final concentrations of the viruses were 9 x 109 particle forming units for the rGH-HSV and 4 x 108 for the GFP-HSV. Helper virus concentrations were 1.7 x 1010 and 1.2 x 109 respectively(Lim and Neve, 2001). Animal Subjects Animals were 20 male Long-Evans rats acquired at 250-275 grams from Taconic for the first cohort of 12 animals. The second cohort consisted of 14 animals from Charles River Laboratories. Animals were single-housed in under a 12hour on, 12-hour off light cycle. The CIS group underwent daily immobilization stress in decapitation cones 4 hours/day for 21 days prior to surgery while the NS group was handled the 2 days prior to surgery. Infusion On the day following the last bout of CIS, all animals were anesthetized with Nembutal (0.65mg/kg, intraperitoneally) or isoflurane at a concentration of 3-5% in oxygen and bilaterally infused to the coordinates of AP -3.30mm, ML ± 2.00mm DV 3.20mm relative to bregma which targeted the dorsal hippocampus (see Fig 5). Animals were administered ketoprofen (1mg, s.c.) for swelling and pain at the time of surgery and 24 hours following surgery. These experiments used 24 animals divided into four groups: non-stressed with the control GFP vector (NS + GFP, n=7), non-stressed with the GH vector (NS + rGH, n=4), chronic immobilization stressed with the control GFP vector (CIS + GFP, n=7), and chronic immobilization stressed with the growth hormone vector (CIS + rGH, n=6). Following the last day of stress, the animals were anesthetized with isoflurane and 2 pd of at least 108 infectious units/mL of either rGH-HSV or control GFP-HSV was infused over the course of 20 minutes to maximize penetrance through the extracullular matrix. Behavioral analysis After 72 hours the animals were trace conditioned with 5 tone shock pairings. A CS of 15 seconds of 2 kHz tone, a trace of 35 seconds and US consisting of a 1 second foot shock at 0.85 mA were the parameters for training. The animals were tested the next day in the same context in the absence of the CS to assay contextual fear and, four hours later tested in a novel context, consisting of white plastic inserts in the testing chamber and a different scent cue, for retention of freezing to the tone, a conditioned response (CR). Video was taken during trace conditioning and the tone recall for analysis by VideoFreeze software (Med Associates; Georgia, VT). Results During conditioning, no clear differences were seen between the treatment groups. All animals were able to associate the US with the CS even across the 35s trace interval. No differences were seen in freezing during the trace, tone or interstimulus intervals (during TRACE: repeated measures ANOVA; effect of stress F(1,4) = 0.442, p = 0.775; effect of vector, F(1,4) = 0.675, p = 0.6150; interaction of stress x vector, F(1,4) = 0.838, p = 0.5125) (during TONE repeated measures ANOVA; effect of stress F(1,4) = 1.413, p = 0.2555; effect of vector, F(1,4) = 1.164, p = 0.3477; interaction of stress x vector, F(1,4) = 1.135, p = 0.3604) (during ISI: repeated measures ANOVA; effect of stress F(1,4) = 0.808, p = 0.5305; effect of vector, F(1,4) = 0.859, p = 0.5005; interaction of stress x vector, F(1,4) = 0.775, p = 0.5508)) (Fig. 6). This is in direct contrast to what was originally hypothesized and will be addressed in the discussion section. On testing day, CIS + GFP animals were impaired in this form of learning (during TRACE: repeated measures ANOVA; effect of stress F(1,9) = 2.365, p = 0.0153; effect of vector, F(1,9) = 1.681, p = 0.0153; interaction of stress x vector, F(1,9) = 1.153, p = 0.3286) and froze less during the trace period and this effect was not rescued by rGH (Fig. 7, Top). This effect was verging on significance though did not make the cutoff for freezing during the trace period though the Tukey's multiple comparison that was conducted did show a difference between CIS + GFP and NS +GFP. Additionally, the animals in the NS + rGH group also were impaired in this form of learning since the repeated measures ANOVA showed this group was not significantly different from any other group. Interestingly, the fact that the animals in the NS + rGH group also seem to show deficit during testing is suggestive of an optimum rGH level in the hippocampus above which connectivity suffers. Strangely, the animals in the NS + rGH group do freeze during the tone epoch(during TONE repeated measures ANOVA; effect of stress F(1,9) = 2.169, p = 0.0264; effect of vector, F(1,9) = 0.716, p = 0.6938; interaction of stress x vector, F(1,9) = 0.726, p = 0.6848) however it seems that once the tone ceases, they do not recall the trace interval indicating an element of generalization (Fig. 7, Bottom). During the first three minutes of the first 4 5-minute ISI differences in freezing were seen in response to the CS (during ISI: repeated measures ANOVA; effect of stress F(1,11) = 1.011, p = 0.4376; effect of vector, F(1,11) = 1.036, p = 0.4158; interaction of stress x vector, F(1,11) = 1.020, p = 0.4296). CIS did impair learning of this task; freezing in the CIS + GFP group froze less than NS + GFP (Tukey's multiple comparison (TMP); p 0.001). CIS + rGH was found to be elevated compared to CIS + GFP (TMP; ps 0.05) though was still found to be different from NS + GFP (TMP; p9 0.05), thus GH was able to partially restore this decrease in learning for this task. The NS + rGH group was found to be different from the NS + GFP (TMP; ps p.001) and not significantly different from CIS + rGH or CIS + GFP (TMP; p=n.s.). This is an interesting effect indicating that aberrant GH signaling in the NS brain may actually impair learning of this task. Figure 8 shows the data for the first eight minutes of context testing. CIS impaired performance for memory to the context; the CIS + GFP group froze less than the NS + GFP group (repeated measures ANOVA; effect of stress F(1,15) = 1.323, p = 0.1872; effect of vector, F(1,15) = 0.599, p = 0.8748; interaction of stress x vector, F(1,15) = 0.462, p = 0.9577; TMP, ps0.001). GH was unable to reverse this effect; the CIS + rGH group displayed the same amount of CR to the context as CIS + GFP (TMP, p=n.s.). Similar to the tone testing, GH administration to healthy animals impaired memory; the NS + CIS group responded less than the NS + GFP group (TMP p 0.001) though this impairment was not as robust as CIS impairment. The NS + rGH group responded more robustly than the CIS + GFP group (TMP 0.001). When the freezing across the eight minutes was averaged, the visualization became clearer and subsequent analysis confirmed the previously seen statistics ( ANOVA; effect of stress F(1,1) = 52.826, p < 0.001; effect of vector, F(1,1) = 14.622, p = 0.0.002; interaction of stress x vector, F(1,1) = 4.787, p = 0.0.0293). Post-hoc analysis via TMP re-iterated the effect of CIS on context retention; CIS + GFP froze less than NS + GFP (TMP, ps 0.001). GH was not able to reverse this effect; CIS + rGH was not found to be different from CIS + GFP (TMP, p=n.s.). Again, GH administration in HIP impeded learning in the healthy animal; NS + rGH froze less than NS + GFP (TMP, ps 0.05) and this impairment was found to be as severe as CIS(TMP vs CIS + GFP, p= n.s.). These data indicate an ability for GH to restore function for some aspects of trace conditioning but not context conditioning. Experiment 3: Determination of level of functional output of the virus delivering recombinant growth hormone gene to hippocampus Materials and Methods Animal subjects Animals were 8 male Long-Evans rats acquired at 250-275 grams from Taconic. Animals were single-housed in under a 12-hour on, 12-hour off light cycle. The CIS group underwent daily immobilization stress in Decapicones 4 hours/day for 21 days prior to surgery while the US group was handled the 2 days prior to surgery. Infusion. On the day following the last bout of CIS animals were anesthetized by Nembutal (0.65mg/kg, intraperitoneally) and bilaterally infused to the coordinates of AP -33.0, ML ± 20.0 DV -32.0. Animals were administered ketoprofen (1mg, s.c.) for swelling and pain at the time of surgery and 24 hours following surgery. These experiments used 8 animals divided into four groups: NS + GFP (n=2), NS + rGH (n=2), CIS + GFP (n=2), and CIS + rGH (n=2). Chronic immobilization stress consisted of 4 hours of immobilization every day for 21 days. Following the last day of stress, the animals were anesthetized with isoflurane and 2 d of at least 108 infectious units/mL of either rGH-HSV or control GFP-HSV were infused over the course of 20 minutes to maximize penetrance through the extracellular matrix. ELISA On the third day after surgery, animals were sacrificed. Whole hippocampi were dissected after sacrifice under deep isoflurane anesthesia. Tissue was homogenized in ice cold homogenization buffer at 10 times the volume of the tissue using a VWR rotating tissue homogenizer. Homogenization buffer consisted of PBS containing one tablet each of PhosSTOP Phosphotase Inhibitor (Roche; Basel, Switzerland) and cOmplete Mini, EDTA-free Protease Inhibitor Cocktail (Roche; Basel, Switzerland) per 10mL of solution as indicated. Following homogenization, the homogenate was centrifuged in a tabletop centrifuge for 30 minutes at 14000 rpm and the supernatant was decanted. The assay was conducted using a Millipore rodent growth hormone ELISA kit on the decanted supernatant, pooled by group and imaged on a Bio-Rad iMark microplate absorbance reader (Bio-Rad Hercules, CA). Results The ELISA was conducted in duplicate, pooled by treatment group (Fig 6). However, in the absence of treatment group replicates, only trends can be discussed. The NS+ GFP group showed discernible expression of rGH. The CIS + rGH seemed to show a distinct increase over CIS + GFP as to be expected but surprisingly, also a higher level than NS + rGH. Discussion The data for the analysis of trace and shock length in Experiment 1 were not found to be statistically significantly conclusive, however, certain trends stand out. Firstly, though animals are able to make the association between the CS and US for all of the trace and shock lengths, all groups except the 35s trace + is shock group tend to generalize their responses and, when presented with the novel testing context, freeze aberrantly. Using the shorter 35s trace with the largest shock intensity, produced robust learning of the task while minimizing the risk of having the animals generalize and present with the CR during the preliminary epoch of the testing session. Additionally, though the animals in the 0.85mA group froze when presented with the CS, they did not reach a ceiling effect in which all the animals froze all the time as a response. Such an effect would be an indicator of overtraining, leaving little room for meaningful interpretation of data in later experiments. Though the rGH was sufficient to recover some aspects of working memory, the recovery was not complete. CIS does seem to negatively impact retention of the CS-US pairing as initially hypothesized and rGH is able to lead to recovery of the CR during ISIs during tone testing. The hippocampus is a large structure; perhaps further restoration of this structure could lead to reduction in trace conditioning deficit seen after CIS. That the rGH-HSV administration was able to restore some ability to associate during tone testing but not context testing is at odds with current literature which states that both are the domain of the dorsal hippocampus (dHIP) (Esclassan et al., 2009). However, the ventral HIP (vHIP) has been shown to be important for regulating the HPA axis(Radley and Sawchenko, 2011) and maybe restoration of the dorsal dHIP without restoration of the vHIP attenuates overall mitigation of deleterious stress effects. Tough the dorsal HIP is partially restored, control of further release of stress hormones by ventral HIP is not(HERMAN et al., 2005). Additionally, the amygdala (AMY) forms the basis for associative memory and it has been shown that trace conditioning does not require the hippocampus during the tone test but is a vital part of association to the context(Raybuck and Lattal, 2011). Further exploration with GH may further restore trace-conditioning learning in a dose dependent manner though. Two injection sites per hemisphere would cover the bulk of the dorsal hippocampus and may further ameliorate effects of CIS on dHIP-dependent tasks. Alternatively, a smaller virus particle capable of more fully penetrating the extracellular matrix such as AAV could be used to further distribute the transgene (Davidson and Breakefield, 2003). One of the more interesting aspects of this second experiment was the deterioration of the ability of the NS + rGH group to pair the tone or context with the US. The NS + rGH group froze more less than the NS + GFP group but more than either of the CIS groups. Since GH is upregulated in response to memory formation and has been found to be present in levels low enough that they cannot be detected in the HIP of a rat that has not undergone conditioning, it may be that excess rGH signaling in the dorsal HIP may lead to an occlusion of plasticity(Donahue et al., 2002). Occlusion has been shown to occur by a group that tested mice with a constitutively active CAMKII gene product, an initiator of LTP, on a spatial memory task(Bejar et al., 2002). Mice with the mutant CAMKII had higher escape latencies than their wild type litter mates indicating a necessity of control over induction of LTP. Growth hormone has been shown to be involved in long term plasticity(Zearfoss et al., 2008); over-expression in HIP that is not compromised by CIS may block this time sensitive rGH function. The ELISA data does show that rGH-HSV administration is able to lead to overexpression of rGH. The antibody adhered to the ELISA plate was able to bind the polypeptide thus the epitope the antibody is attracted to is present in the gene product. The CIS + rGH group showed a robust increase in growth hormone level compared to the NS + GFP and CIS + GFP groups. This may be due to a synergistic effect of the transgene and endogenous restorative mechanisms. As stated above, GH is upregulated in response to HIP-dependent learning, these animals underwent no experiment that would induct GH expression and that may explain the lack of signal in the NS + GFP group(Donahue et al., 2002). In sum, rats are able to establish meaningful connections between the CS and US over a 35s trace interval though extending beyond that may lead to generalized expression of fear. Additionally, long shock durations seem to lead to an increase in freezing. Saliency of the US is important to develop a strong link to the CS. CIS has been shown to alter freezing in response to the training context though, under these criteria, did not alter freezing to the tone or during the trace. CIS did decrease freezing during the ISI but that may be due to an increase in anxiety though not fear. Growth hormone over-expression was able to attenuate though not completely reverse that CIS-induced decrease in freezing during the ISI. ELISA confirmed that over-expression produced functional hormone. In the future, GH and its related pathways are a promising avenue of exploration for treatment of psychiatric diseases that coincide with chronic stress and its corresponding alteration of the hippocampus. Figures Delay Conditioning cs m M I Trace Conditioning us~~ u Figure 1 During delay conditioning, the cessation of the CS and US coincide. For the trace conditioning paradigm the US follows CS termination by a trace period. Freezing during ISIs -4- 35s Trace -e- 35s Trace - 50s Trace S50s Trace Is Shock 29 Shock Ia Shock 2s Shock 4 q 4'.. Freezing during tones -a- 35s Trace Ia Shock - 3S Trace 2s Shock 50 e-Trace 1a Shock - 50s Trace 2s Shock Freoaing during trace intervals 36s Trace ia Shock -.- 36s Trace 2s Shock -_-50s Trace is Shock - -+- 5s Trace 2s Shock Figure 2 Freezing during 3 different time periods during fear conditioning for analysis of trace length and shock length. Top: Freezing during the 3 min ISI. Middle: Freezing during the 16s CS presentation. Freezing during trace interval 100-35s Trace is Shock -7-- 35s Trace 2s Shock 50s Trace 2s Shock 50s Trace 2s Shock Freezing prior to first testing tone ~25100 Figure 3 Top: Freezing during the trace intervals for each group for analysis of effects of trace and shock duration on conditioning.. Bottom: Freezing during the initial introduction to the novel context in which the CS is presented for testing. Freezing to ton* duding conditioning -6-0.85 mA mA mA 74-.-0.60 I-6-0.40 LP Freezing to tonm during testing -- 0.85 mA -*- 0.60 mA -- 0.40 mA -a- 0.85 mA -- 0.85 mA -e-0.60 mA -*-0.60 mA -'--0.40 mA -- so es. 2a 26 400 Freezing to trace during conditioning Free Ing to trace during testing 0.40 mA Figure 4 For analysis of three different shock intensities, during conditioning no significant difference was seen in response to the tone however during the trace interval a significant difference was seen between the groups. For testing each block represents responses for three traces or tones. There was no sign of elevated freezing during the initial exposure to the novel context in which testing occurred. Upon presentation with the CS, the 0.85 mA shock group displayed the greatest level of the CR which was sustained almost to the end of the 15 tones. During the trace interval this group also displayed significantly more freezing as well. Figure 5 A representative Infection In dHIP at SX magnification 4 days after Infusion with 2 pl of the control HSV-GFP. The cell bodies of pyramidal cells In the CA1 layer are visible. Freaing during te Inter-stimulus Intervals during conditioning -.- NS + S0109 -- NS + rGH St + Solos -+- St + rGH Freezing to the tone during condl~onlng -- NS + S0 109 NS + rGH -9-St + S0109 -e- + St + rGH / ~/~b/ Freezing during the trace Intrval during conditioning 10- NSo+- 109 -- NS + rGH -*-CIS + 50109 -CIS+ rGH Figure 6 No differences were seen during conditioning between groups during exploration of interaction between CIS and rGH. For all three epochs assayed during fear conditioning, there was no significant difference in the rates of learning for these four groups. Freezing during the trace period -a- CIS + GFP -.- CIS + rGH -M-NS + GFP - NS + rGH Freezing to the tone -w- CIS + GFP -,-CIS+ rGH - -NS + GFP NS + rGH 10 I 10% ri % Freezing by mbtmt during first four Me~ - -*-CIS+GFP --- CIS+ rGH nI: 90 -.- NS + GFP NS + rGH 300 20 .b 5 Figure 7 After conditioning the animals in Experiment 2 were tested for freezing to the CS in a novel environment Neither freezing to the tone nor the trace was found to be significantly different. However, when examining the ISI by minute, the CIS+GFP group froze significantly less than other groups. Context Testing -w- CIS + GFP -*-CIS + rGH -- NS + GFP -+-NS + rGH Freezing to the Context on Testing Day s9, CIS+ GFPCI;rGH NS +GFP NS+ rGH Figure 8 Top: Freezing when rats were re-exposed to the context in which they were conditioned, in 30s bins for 8 minutes. Via repeated measures ANOVA the groups were found to be different (and by Tukey's multiple comparison test, all groups were shown to be different from one another save the two CIS groups. Bottom: The average freezing across that 8 minutes was also found to be significantly different for all groups except the two CIS groups. Pooled Growth Hormone Extracted from dHIP U 250 CIS + GFP 2CIS + rGH NS + GFP 1NS + rGH CI8 + OFPCIB + rH NI + QFP NO6 r0H Figure 9. A comparison of the GH levels In the hippocampi of each group. t = No bar Is shown for the NS + GFP group because no growth hormone was detectable in this sample. References. Acheson, D.T., Gresack, J.E., and Risbrough, V.B. (2011). Hippocampal dysfunction effects on context memory: Possible etiology for posttraumatic stress disorder. Neuropharmacology 1-12. Alfonso, J., Frasch, A.C., and Flugge, G.(2005). Chronic Stress, Depression and Antidepressants: Effects on Gene Transcription in the Hippocampus. Reviews in the Neurosciences 16, 43-56. Anagnostaras, S.G., Maren, S., and Fanselow, M.S. (1995). Scopolamine selectively disrupts the acquisition of contextual fear conditioning in rats. Neurobiol Learn Mem 64, 191-194. Bale, T.L. (2006). Stress sensitivity and the development of affective disorders. Hormones and Behavior 50, 529-533. Bangasser, D.A. (2006). Trace Conditioning and the Hippocampus: The Importance of Contiguity. Journal of Neuroscience 26, 8702-8706. Bannerman, D.M., Grubb, M., Deacon, R.M.J., Yee, B.K., Feldon, J., and Rawlins, J.N.P. (2003). Ventral hippocampal lesions affect anxiety but not spatial learning. Behav Brain Res 139, 197-213. Bauer, E.P., Paz, R., and Pare, D. (2007). Gamma oscillations coordinate amygdalorhinal interactions during learning. Journal of Neuroscience 27, 9369-9379. Bejar, R., Yasuda, R., Krugers, H., Hood, K., and Mayford, M. (2002). Transgenic calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II activation: dose-dependent effects on synaptic plasticity, learning, and memory. Journal of Neuroscience 22, 5719-5726. Best, P.J., White, A.M., and Minai, A.(2001). Spatial processing in the brain: the activity of hippocampal place cells. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 24, 459-486. Brun, V.H. (2002). Place Cells and Place Recognition Maintained by Direct Entorhinal-Hippocampal Circuitry. Science 296, 2243-2246. Czerniawski, J., Ree, F., Chia, C., Ramamoorthi, K., Kumata, Y., and Otto, T.A. (2011). The importance of having Arc: expression of the immediate-early gene Arc is required for hippocampus-dependent fear conditioning and blocked by NMDA receptor antagonism. Journal of Neuroscience 31, 11200-11207. Davidson, B.L., and Breakefield, X.O. (2003). Viral vectors for gene delivery to the nervous system. Nat Rev Neurosci 4, 353-364. Dedovic, K., Duchesne, A., Andrews, J., Engert, V., and Pruessner, J.C. (2009). The brain and the stress axis: the neural correlates of cortisol regulation in response to stress. Neuroimage 47, 864-871. Delamater, A.R. (2012). On the nature of CS and US representations in Pavlovian learning. Learn Behav 40, 1-23. Dityatev, A.E., and Bolshakov, V.Y. (2005). Amygdala, long-term potentiation, and fear conditioning. Neuroscientist 11, 75-88. Donahue, C.P., Jensen, R.V., Ochiishi, T., Eisenstein, I.,Zhao, M., Shors, T., and Kosik, K.S. (2002). Transcriptional profiling reveals regulated genes in the hippocampus during memory formation. Hippocampus 12, 821-833. Donahue, C.P., Kosik, K.S., and Shors, T.J. (2006). Growth hormone is produced within the hippocampus where it responds to age, sex, and stress. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 103, 6031-6036. Donohue, H.S., Gabbott, P.L.A., Davies, H.A., Rodriguez, J.J., Cordero, M.I., Sandi, C., Medvedev, N.I., Popov, V.I., Colyer, F.M., Peddie, C.J., et al. (2006). Chronic restraint stress induces changes in synapse morphology in stratum lacunosum-moleculare CA1 rat hippocampus: a stereological and three-dimensional ultrastructural study. Neuroscience 140, 597-606. Epsztein, J., Brecht, M., and Lee, A.K. (2011). Intracellular determinants of hippocampal CA1 place and silent cell activity in a novel environment. Neuron 70, 109-120. Esch, T., Stefano, G., Fricchione, G., and Benson, H. (2002). The role of stress in neurodegenerative diseases and mental disorders. Neuroendocrinol Lett 23, 199208. Esclassan, F., Coutureau, E., Di Scala, G., and Marchand, A.R. (2009). Differential contribution of dorsal and ventral hippocampus to trace and delay fear conditioning. Hippocampus 19, 33-44. Fanselow, M.S., and Poulos, A.M. (2005). The neuroscience of mammalian associative learning. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 56, 207-234. Frankland, P.W., Josselyn, S.A., Anagnostaras, S.G., Kogan, J.H., Takahashi, E., and Silva, A.J. (2004). Consolidation of CS and US representations in associative fear conditioning. Hippocampus 14, 557-569. Fujisawa, S., and Buzsiki, G.(2011). A 4 Hz oscillation adaptively synchronizes prefrontal, VTA, and hippocampal activities. Neuron 72, 153-165. Gewirtz, J.C., McNish, K.A., and Davis, M. (2000). Is the hippocampus necessary for contextual fear conditioning? Behav Brain Res 110, 83-95. Gilbertson, M.W., Shenton, M.E., Ciszewski, A., Kasai, K., Lasko, N.B., Orr, S.P., and Pitman, R.K. (2002). Smaller hippocampal volume predicts pathologic vulnerability to psychological trauma. Nature Publishing Group 5, 1242-1247. Hales, J.B., and Brewer, J.B. (2010). Activity in the hippocampus and neocortical working memory regions predicts successful associative memory for temporally discontiguous events. Neuropsychologia 48, 3351-3359. Hallschmid, M., Wilhelm, I.,Michel, C., Perras, B., and Born, J.(2011). A Role for Central Nervous Growth Hormone-Releasing Hormone Signaling in the Consolidation of Declarative Memories. PLoS ONE 6, e23435. HERMAN, J., OSTRANDER, M., MUELLER, N., and FIGUEIREDO, H. (2005). Limbic system mechanisms of stress regulation: Hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenocortical axis. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 29, 1201-1213. Hertzen, von, L.S.J., and Giese, K.P. (2005). Memory reconsolidation engages only a subset of immediate-early genes induced during consolidation. Journal of Neuroscience 25, 193 5-1942. Huff, N.C., Frank, M., Wright-Hardesty, K., Sprunger, D., Matus-Amat, P., Higgins, E., and Rudy, J.W. (2006). Amygdala regulation of immediate-early gene expression in the hippocampus induced by contextual fear conditioning. Journal of Neuroscience 26, 1616-1623. Hyv~rinen, E., Korhonen, T., and Arikoski, J.(2006). The effect of rewarding hypothalamic stimulation on behavioral and neural hippocampal responses during trace eyeblink conditioning in rabbit (Oryctolagus cuniculus). Behav Brain Res 167, 141-149. Krasne, F.B. (2011). Design of a neurally plausible model of fear learning. 1-23. Lee, A.K., and Wilson, M.A. (2002). Memory of sequential experience in the hippocampus during slow wave sleep. Neuron 36, 1183-1194. Leuner, B., Falduto, J., and Shors, T.J. (2003). Associative memory formation increases the observation of dendritic spines in the hippocampus. Journal of Neuroscience 23, 659-665. Lim, F., and Neve, R.L. (2001). Generation of high-titer defective HSV-1 vectors. Curr Protoc Neurosci Chapter4, Unit 4.13. Lonergan, M.E., Gafford, G.M., Jarome, T.J., and Helmstetter, F.J. (2010). Timedependent expression of Arc and zif268 after acquisition of fear conditioning. Neural Plasticity 2010, 139891. Luine, V., Villegas, M., Martinez, C., and Mcewen, B.S. (1994). Repeated stress causes reversible impairments of spatial memory performance. Brain Res 639, 167-170. Mahmoud, G.S., and Grover, L.M. (2006). Growth hormone enhances excitatory synaptic transmission in area CA1 of rat hippocampus. I Neurophysiol 95, 29622974. Maren, S., Aharonov, G., and Fanselow, M.S. (1997). Neurotoxic lesions of the dorsal hippocampus and Pavlovian fear conditioning in rats. Behav Brain Res 88, 261-274. Maren, S., Anagnostaras, S.G., and Fanselow, M.S. (1998). The startled seahorse: is the hippocampus necessary for contextual fear conditioning? Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed.) 2, 39-42. McEchron, M.D., Bouwmeester, H., Tseng, W., Weiss, C., and Disterhoft, J.F. (1998). Hippocampectomy disrupts auditory trace fear conditioning and contextual fear conditioning in the rat. Hippocampus 8, 638-646. McEwen, B. (1994). Corticosteroids and hippocampal plasticity. Ann N YAcad Sci 746, 134. McEwen, B.S. (1999). STRESS AND HIPPOCAMPAL PLASTICITY. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 22, 105-122. McNaughton, B.L., Battaglia, F.P., Jensen, 0., Moser, E.I., and Moser, M.-B. (2006). Path integration and the neural basis of the 'cognitive map'. Nat Rev Neurosci 7, 663-678. McNish, K.A., Gewirtz, J.C., and Davis, M. (1997). Evidence of contextual fear after lesions of the hippocampus: a disruption of freezing but not fear-potentiated startle. JNeurosci 17, 9353-9360. Moita, M.A. (2011). Time determines the neural circuit underlying associative fear learning. 1-9. Moita, M.A.P., Rosis, S., Zhou, Y., Ledoux, J.E., and Blair, H.T. (2004). Putting fear in its place: remapping of hippocampal place cells during fear conditioning. Journal of Neuroscience 24, 7015-7023. Montgomery, S.M., and Buzsaki, G.(2007). Gamma oscillations dynamically couple hippocampal CA3 and CA1 regions during memory task performance. Proc Nati Acad Sci USA 104, 14495-14500. Moser, E.I., Kropff, E., and Moser, M.-B. (2008). Place Cells, Grid Cells, and the Brain's Spatial Representation System. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 31, 69-89. Moyer, J.R., Deyo, R.A., and Disterhoft, J.F. (1990). Hippocampectomy disrupts trace eye-blink conditioning in rabbits. Behav. Neurosci. 104, 243-252. Moyer, J.R., Thompson, L.T., and Disterhoft, J.F. (1996). Trace eyeblink conditioning increases CA1 excitability in a transient and learning-specific manner. JNeurosci 16, 5536-5546. Muller, E., and Locatelli, V. (1999). Neuroendocrine control of growth hormone secretion. Physiological Reviews. Nokia, M.S., Penttonen, M., and Wikgren, J.(2010). Hippocampal ripple-contingent training accelerates trace eyeblink conditioning and retards extinction in rabbits. Journal of Neuroscience 30, 11486-11492. Otto, T., and Poon, P. (2006). Dorsal hippocampal contributions to unimodal contextual conditioning. Journal of Neuroscience 26, 6603-6609. Pang, M.-H., Kim, N.-S., Kim, I.-H., Kim, H., Kim, H.-T., and Choi, J.-S. (2010). Cholinergic transmission in the dorsal hippocampus modulates trace but not delay fear conditioning. Neurobiol Learn Mem 94, 206-213. Parker, K., and Schatzberg, A. (2003). Neuroendocrine aspects of hypercortisolism in major depression. Hormones and Behavior. Radley, J.J., and Sawchenko, P.E. (2011). A Common Substrate for Prefrontal and Hippocampal Inhibition of the Neuroendocrine Stress Response. Journal of Neuroscience 31, 9683-9695. Raybuck, J.D., and Lattal, K.M. (2011). Double Dissociation of Amygdala and Hippocampal Contributions to Trace and Delay Fear Conditioning. PLoS ONE 6, e15982. Rosenfeld, R.G., and Hwa, V.(2009). The growth hormone cascade and its role in mammalian growth. Horm. Res. 71 Supp12, 3 6-40. Sapolsky, R.M. (2000). Glucocorticoids and hippocampal atrophy in neuropsychiatric disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry 57, 925-935. Spijker, A.T., and van Rossum, E.F.C. (2009). Glucocorticoid receptor polymorphisms in major depression. Focus on glucocorticoid sensitivity and neurocognitive functioning. Ann NYAcad Sci 1179, 199-215. Swaab, D., Bao, A., and Lucassen, P. (2005). The stress system in the human brain in depression and neurodegeneration. Ageing Res Rev 4, 141-194. Trivedi, M. (2004). Lesions of the ventral hippocampus, but not the dorsal hippocampus, impair conditioned fear expression and inhibitory avoidance on the elevated T-maze. Neurobiol Learn Mem 81, 172-184. van Vugt, M.K., Schulze-Bonhage, A., Litt, B., Brandt, A., and Kahana, M.J. (2010). Hippocampal gamma oscillations increase with memory load. Journal of Neuroscience 30, 2694-2699. Vann, S.D., and Albasser, M.M. (2011). Hippocampus and neocortex: recognition and spatial memory. Curr Opin Neurobiol. Vyas, A., Mitra, R., Shankaranarayana Rao, B.S., and Chattarji, S. (2002). Chronic stress induces contrasting patterns of dendritic remodeling in hippocampal and amygdaloid neurons. J Neurosci 22, 6810-6818. Weible, A.P., O'Reilly, J.-A., Weiss, C., and Disterhoft, J.F. (2006). Comparisons of dorsal and ventral hippocampus cornu ammonis region 1 pyramidal neuron activity during trace eye-blink conditioning in the rabbit. Neuroscience 141, 1123-1137. Zearfoss, N.R., Alarcon, J.M., Trifilieff, P., Kandel, E., and Richter, J.D. (2008). A Molecular Circuit Composed of CPEB-1 and c-Jun Controls Growth HormoneMediated Synaptic Plasticity in the Mouse Hippocampus. Journal of Neuroscience 28, 8502-8509.