Behavioural and heart rate responses to predation

advertisement
Color profile: Generic CMYK printer profile
Composite Default screen
788
Behavioural and heart rate responses to predation
risk in wild and domesticated Atlantic salmon
Jörgen I. Johnsson, Johan Höjesjö, and Ian A. Fleming
Abstract: Artificial culture may relax the selective pressures from predators, thereby altering behavioural and heart rate
responses to predation risk. Culture may also impose new selection that adapts fish to confinement. Predictions from
these hypotheses were tested by comparing seventh-generation farm Atlantic salmon (Salmo salar) with wild Atlantic
salmon from the principal founder population of the farm strain. Wild age 1+ salmon had higher standard heart rates
and showed a more pronounced flight and heart rate response to a simulated predator attack than did farmed salmon.
However, wild fish were closer to the model predator at attack, which may have accentuated these differences. Both
strains habituated to the threat, showing less response to the second attack than to the first. In contrast with age 1+
fish, wild age 2+ salmon had lower standard heart rates than farmed fish. Moreover, in age 2+ salmon, domestication
effects were less clear and the response to predation threat considerably weaker, suggesting that environmental effects
of culture override genetic effects as time in captivity increases. Domestication selection may thus alter reaction norms
of farmed animals over environmental gradients and time. This should be considered when attempting to predict the
consequences of release or escape of domesticated animals in the wild.
Résumé : L’élevage en milieu artificiel diminue les pressions des prédateurs et change ainsi les réactions comportementales et physiologiques (rythme cardiaque) aux risques de prédation. L’élevage peut aussi imposer un nouveau type de sélection qui favorise l’adaptation des poissons à la restriction d’espace. La comparaison de Saumons de l’Atlantique (Salmo
salar) élevés depuis 7 générations en pisciculture et de saumons sauvages provenant de la population souche principale de
la pisciculture a permis de vérifier certaines prédictions générées à partir de ces hypothèses. Les saumons sauvages d’âge
1+ ont des rythmes cardiaques standard plus élevés et exhibent des réactions plus fortes de fuite et de modification du
rythme cardiaque à l’attaque simulée d’un prédateur que les saumons de pisciculture. Cependant, les poissons sauvages
étaient plus près du modèle de prédateur lors de l’attaque, ce qui peut avoir accentué ces différences. Les deux groupes
développent une habituation aux attaques et réagissent moins fortement à une seconde attaque qu’à la première. Contrairement aux poissons d’âge 1+, les saumons sauvages d’âge 2+ ont des rythmes cardiaques standard plus bas que les saumons de pisciculture. De plus, chez les saumon d’âge 2+, les effets de la domestication sont moins apparents et les
réactions à la menace de prédation sont considérablement diminuées, ce qui laisse croire que les effets environnementaux
de l’élevage masquent les effets génétiques à mesure que la durée de la captivité augmente. La sélection opérée par la domestication peut ainsi modifier les normes des réactions des animaux élevés en milieu artificiel sur une gamme de gradients environnementaux et temporels. Ce sont des facteurs dont on doit tenir compte lorsqu’on cherche à évaluer l’impact
de la libération ou de la fuite d’animaux domestiqués dans la nature.
[Traduit par la Rédaction]
Johnsson et al.
794
Introduction
When animal populations are domesticated, their environment changes dramatically. Predictability and homogeneity
replace spatial and temporal unpredictability and variation,
and the selective pressure from natural predators is removed
Received October 25, 2000. Accepted January 26, 2001.
Published on the NRC Research Press Web site on March 19,
2001.
J16036
J.I. Johnsson1 and J. Höjesjö. Department of Zoology,
Göteborg University, Medicinaregatan 18, Box 463,
SE-405 30 Göteborg, Sweden.
I.A. Fleming.2 Norwegian Institute for Nature Research,
Trondheim, Norway.
1
Corresponding author (e-mail: jorgen.johnsson@zool.gu.se).
Present address: Coastal Oregon Marine Experimental Station
and Department of Fisheries and Wildlife, Oregon State
University, Newport, OR 97365, U.S.A.
2
Can. J. Fish. Aquat. Sci. 58: 788–794 (2001)
J:\cjfas\cjfas58\cjfas-04\F01-025.vp
Friday, March 16, 2001 10:24:50 AM
(Kohane and Parsons 1989). A straightforward prediction
from this scenario is that domestication should reduce
behavioural responses to predation risk. This prediction has
been supported by various experimental studies on
salmonids including rainbow/steelhead trout (Oncorhynchus
mykiss) (Johnsson and Abrahams 1991; Berejikian 1995),
brown trout (Salmo trutta) (Johnsson et al. 1996), and Atlantic salmon (Salmo salar) (Einum and Fleming 1997; Fleming and Einum 1997).
Predation risk can induce cardioventilatory responses. In
threatened mammals, bradycardia (decreasing heart rate) is
generally associated with freezing or hiding and tachycardia
(increasing heart rate) with flight or defence (Moen et al.
1978; Smith et al. 1981; Espmark and Langvatn 1985).
There is, however, little information on heart rate effects of
threat in aquatic animals and even less on integrated behavioural and heart rate responses. Metcalfe et al. (1987) found
that exposure to a model trout predator increased opercular
beat rate (which is positively correlated with heart rate) in
juvenile Atlantic salmon. In rainbow trout, simulated attacks
DOI: 10.1139/cjfas-58-4-788
© 2001 NRC Canada
Color profile: Generic CMYK printer profile
Composite Default screen
Johnsson et al.
by a heron model induced both tachycardia and flight
(Höjesjö et al. 1999).
The aim of this study was to investigate how artificial selection affects behavioural and heart rate responses to predation threat in salmon. Such knowledge may help to predict
the performance and impact of released and escaped farm
fish in the wild (Jonsson 1997). Using a method where biopotentials are recorded from the water by external electrodes
(Goodman and Weinberger 1971), we simultaneously measured heart rate and behaviour of free-swimming farm and
wild Atlantic salmon. Furthermore, by comparing seventhgeneration farm salmon with wild fish reared under similar
environmental conditions and derived from the principal
founder population of the farm strain, we studied the effects
of domestication selection on heart rate and behaviour. Two
experiments were conducted on age 1+ fish (1997) and age
2+ fish (1998), respectively, addressing two specific questions. (i) Has domestication selection reduced the behavioural and heart rate response to predation risk in salmon?
(ii) Has domestication selection in the confined hatchery environment reduced the standard heart rate of farm salmon?
Methods
Experimental fish
The farm salmon originated in Norway’s national breeding
programme at Sunndalsøra (AquaGen) and were taken as eyed
eggs in 1996 to the Norwegian Institute for Nature Research Station at Ims, southwest Norway. These fish constituted the seventh
generation of the first brood line (“population 1” sensu Gjedrem et
al. 1991) that has undergone intentional selection such that it is
now derived principally, if not solely, from the river Namsen, midNorway (Gjedrem et al. 1991; Fleming and Einum 1997). Simultaneously, eyed eggs of wild salmon from the River Namsen were
taken to the Norwegian Institute for Nature Research Station. The
juveniles were initially reared in separate 1-m2 tanks, two tanks per
population, with rotation among the tanks approximately once a
month. After 3 months, the juveniles were transferred to 2-m2
tanks (approximately 2000 fish per tank), two tanks per population,
with each tank containing equal numbers of fish from the 1-m2
duplicate tanks. Rotation among tanks occurred after 2 months,
and 2 months subsequent, 100 parr from each 2-m2 tank were
anaesthetised using chlorobutanol, tagged with passive integrated
transponders, and placed into a single, common 7-m2 tank. Thus,
by rearing the juveniles under similar environmental conditions, including common densities and rotation among tanks, we attempted
to reduce possible rearing tank effects (Fleming and Einum 1997).
Age 1+ and 2+ fish were subsequently used in the experiments.
Heart rate monitoring
We monitored action potentials generated by the heart musculature using stainless steel electrodes, a method first introduced by
Goodman and Weinberger (1971) and later used in various studies
(e.g., Borate et al. 1993). A bar-shaped electrode was placed on the
bottom of an aquarium and another electrode, constructed as a cage
to match the aquarium dimensions, was positioned over the bar,
thus enclosing the fish. The signals were amplified using a Grass
EEG amplifier (model 7p511). In experiment I (age 1+ fish, 1997),
the signals were filtered with a Rockland series 2000 filter used as
a bandpass filter, allowing the electrocardiogram signal to be separated from the total muscular activity. The resulting electrocardiogram complex was simultaneously observed on an oscilloscope
(Siemens, Sieskop 4M) and recorded on a Teac tape recorder
(model R-70). In experiment II (age 2+ fish, 1998), the signals
789
were filtered and analysed electronically using LabView 5.0 software (National Instruments). The use of external electrodes avoided
the stress caused by surgery and handling (Laitinen and Valtonen
1994).
Experimental procedure
Eight to 10 experimental fish, half from the Namsen strain and
half from the AquaGen strain, were transferred from holding tanks
to experimental aquaria: 31 L for age 1+ fish and 50 L for age 2+
fish. The aquarium sizes were chosen to allow the fish maximal
swimming space while ensuring stable and readable signals from
the electrodes. However, when fish were very mobile, signals became weaker due to disturbance from general muscular activity.
Fish length to tank volume ratios were kept similar for the two
age-classes (0.59 cm/L for age 1+ fish and 0.63 cm/L for age 2+
fish). There was no indication that this ratio was important within
the two age-classes, as neither behaviour nor heart rate was related
to fish size (P > 0.1).
During the two consecutive days following 48 h of acclimatisation, the behaviour of each fish was monitored twice daily,
starting at 09:00 and 13:00, respectively, using a VHS video camera in front of the aquarium. Heart rate was recorded simultaneously. Thus, each fish was studied during four 15-min trials. In
all trials, the general activity was monitored for 3 min, whereupon
each fish received four pellets. In addition, during trials II and III,
each fish was subjected to a simulated predator attack at the time
of feeding using a plastic heron model. The attack was simulated
by releasing the heron head, which fell rapidly through a slit in the
aquarium. The bill of the heron hit the bottom of the aquarium,
whereupon it was immediately withdrawn. To avoid injury to the
fish, the bill was padded with soft plastic tubing. Similar methods
have previously been shown to elicit predator avoidance behaviour
in rainbow trout (Jönsson et al. 1996; Höjesjö et al. 1999). The
response to the predator attack was then monitored for 12 min.
After the last trial, the fish were killed using an overdose of 2phenoxyethanol, whereupon final weight and length were measured
and sex and maturation determined by gonadal inspection. This
procedure was repeated four times, yielding 36 replicates for age
1+ fish (1997) (18 Namsen, 18 AquaGen salmon) and 37 replicates
for age 2+ fish (1998) (19 Namsen, 18 AquaGen salmon). Mean
(range) water temperatures during the experiments were 22.8°C
(21.6–23.3°C) in 1997 and 14.5°C (14.0–15.3°C) in 1998.
Data analysis
The activity level of each fish was measured and transformed to
an activity index where “0” represents a fish that is motionless, “1”
a fish that is moving fins or body without moving forward, and “2”
a fish that is swimming. Vertical position was determined by relating the position (i.e., lateral line) of the fish to two horizontal bars
dividing the aquarium into three parts where “1” represents the
lower third of the aquarium, “2” the middle third of the aquarium,
and “3” the upper third of the aquarium. The behavioural and heart
rate data were divided into 10 time periods for each 15-min trial,
where period 1 = 180–0 s before food/predator attack, 2 = 0–15 s
after food/predator attack, and 3 = 15–30 s, 4 = 30–45 s, 5 = 45–
60 s, 6 = 60–120 s, 7 = 120–180 s, 8 = 180 – 360 s, 9 = 360 –
540 s, and 10 = 540 – 720 s after the attack. Thus, the data for periods 2–5 that immediately followed the predator attack were integrated over the shortest time (15 s) to provide more detail. An
initial analysis where period 1 was broken into 1-min intervals revealed no significant variation in heart rate with time prior to
food/predator attack, indicating that it was relatively stable. The
average of these first time periods from each of the four trials,
when the fish were considered to be undisturbed, was used for analysing standard activity, vertical position, and heart rate.
© 2001 NRC Canada
J:\cjfas\cjfas58\cjfas-04\F01-025.vp
Friday, March 16, 2001 10:24:50 AM
Color profile: Generic CMYK printer profile
Composite Default screen
790
Can. J. Fish. Aquat. Sci. Vol. 58, 2001
Table 1. Mean weights and lengths (±SE) of wild Namsen and farm AquaGen Atlantic salmon
used in the two experiments.
Wild
Males
Age 1+ salmon
n
Weight (g)
Length (cm)
Age 2+ salmon
n
Weight (g)
Length (cm)
Farm
Females
Males
Females
13
83.7 (3.7)
18.0 (0.2)
5
84.1 (6.0)
18.2 (0.3)
15
81.8 (3.3)
18.5 (0.2)
3
87.9 (22.4)
18.9 (2.0)
12
283.9 (10.2)
30.5 (0.4)
7
313.3 (28.1)
31.3 (0.9)
11
337.6 (20.9)
32.3 (0.8)
6
326.9 (29.3)
32.2 (1.1)
The time elapsed from the moment (video frame) before the bill
of the heron hit the water surface until the fish started to flee (burst
swimming) was used to estimate the reaction time to the predator
attack. The time elapsed from the start of this escape reaction until
the fish coasted (ceased active swimming) was used to estimate the
duration of the flight reaction. The number (proportion) of fish
showing a flight reaction was also calculated. The distance from
the eye of the fish to the place where the heron bill hit the water
surface, measured immediately before the heron attack, was used
to estimate the proximity to the predator at attack. The difference
in performance between period 1 (before attack) and period 2 (0–
15 s after attack, for activity) or period 5 (45–60 s after attack, for
vertical position) was used to measure the response to the predator.
In order to avoid any observational bias, all behavioural analyses
were carried out without knowing the origin of the fish.
The normally distributed heart rate data were analysed using a t
test to examine mean differences in standard heart rate between
strains. Strain effects were also analysed using analysis of variance
for repeated measurements to include variation among the 10 time
periods. The behavioural data did not conform to a normal distribution and were therefore analysed using nonparametric statistical
methods. To simplify presentation, figures only present data from
the most interesting trials (trial II (first predator attack) and trial III
(second predator attack)), and detailed statistics are only presented
for P < 0.5. The alpha value was set to 0.05 for all tests.
Results
Preliminary analyses revealed no significant effects of size
(Table 1), sex, or maturation.
Experiment I on age 1+ salmon
Activity and position
Standard swimming activity was higher in wild than in
farm salmon (Mann–Whitney U test; U = 92, P = 0.03).
Moreover, both predator attacks induced a significant increase
in activity (Wilcoxon signed rank test; first attack: z = –3.82,
P < 0.001; second attack: z = –2.19, P = 0.03) (Figs. 1a and
1b), but the response of wild and farm salmon did not differ
in this respect.
Fish generally occupied the lower third of the water
column. The first predator attack induced a lower vertical
position (Wilcoxon signed rank test; z = –2.52, P = 0.01) independent of strain. No other treatment effects were found.
Escape reaction
More wild fish (100%) than farm fish (72%) reacted with
a flight to the first simulated heron attack (Fishers exact test;
P = 0.04) but not to the second attack (67% fled in both
strains). There was no significant difference in reaction time
between wild and farm fish either for the first attack
(mean ± SE = 0.16 ± 0.11 versus 0.62 ± 0.56 s, respectively)
or for the second attack (0.40 ± 0.23 versus 0.81 ± 0.52 s,
respectively). Flight duration was significantly longer in
wild fish in response to the first attack but not to the second
where flight duration was reduced independent of strain
(Fig. 2). Flight duration was negatively correlated with the
distance to the predator at attack (first attack: rs = –0.41, P =
0.02; second attack: rs = –0.42, P = 0.01). Wild fish were
closer to the predator than farm fish at the first attack
(Mann–Whitney U test; U = 94.5, P = 0.03) but not at the
second attack.
Heart rate
Standard heart rate was higher in wild fish than in farm
fish (t = –2.18, P = 0.04) (Figs.1c and 1d). Following the
first predator attack, heart rate varied significantly with time
(F9,288 = 4.50, P < 0.0001). Moreover, a significant interaction between strain and time period (F9,288 = 2.18, P = 0.02)
indicated that wild salmon responded stronger than farm fish
to the first predator attack. The heart rate of the wild fish
first decreased (bradycardia) and then increased (tachycardia), remaining high until the end of the observation period.
Farm fish had a weaker response, with heart rate returning to
preattack levels by the end of the last observation period
(Fig. 1c). There was no significant variation in heart rate following the second predator attack (Fig. 1d).
In both strains, heart rate measured 540–720 s after the
first attack was positively correlated with flight duration
(rs= 0.35, P = 0.04), suggesting that heart rate and escape
responses were linked.
Experiment II on age 2+ salmon
Activity and position
Neither standard swimming activity nor the predatorinduced increase in swimming activity (Wilcoxon signed
rank test; first attack: z = –3.21, P = 0.001; second attack:
z = –2.18, P = 0.03) differed significantly between wild and
farm salmon (Figs.1e and 1f).
Fish generally occupied the lower two thirds of the water
column (mean position index = 1.7 ± 0.1), and the first predator attack induced a lower vertical position (Wilcoxon
signed rank test; z = –3.20, P = 0.001) independent of strain.
No other significant treatment effects were found.
© 2001 NRC Canada
J:\cjfas\cjfas58\cjfas-04\F01-025.vp
Friday, March 16, 2001 10:24:51 AM
Color profile: Generic CMYK printer profile
Composite Default screen
Johnsson et al.
791
Fig. 1. Mean swimming activity index and heart rate measured in two experiments on (a–d) age 1+ and (e–h) age 2+ Atlantic salmon.
The response to two simulated heron attacks separated by 18 h (denoted by the arrow) was measured over 10 time periods. Error bars
denote the standard error of the mean. Wild fish are represented by solid circles and farm fish by open circles.
Escape reaction
Less than half of the wild (42%) and farm fish (44%) reacted with a flight to the first simulated heron attack. The
second attack induced fewer flight responses both by the
wild (26%) and the farm fish (22%). Neither reaction time
(first attack: 0.09 ± 0.02 versus 0.11 ± 0.03 s; second attack:
0.16 ± 0.06 versus 0.24 ± 0.11 s, respectively) nor flight duration (Fig. 2) differed significantly between wild and farm
© 2001 NRC Canada
J:\cjfas\cjfas58\cjfas-04\F01-025.vp
Friday, March 16, 2001 10:24:52 AM
Color profile: Generic CMYK printer profile
Composite Default screen
792
Fig. 2. Flight duration in response to two simulated heron attacks
in wild and farm Atlantic salmon of two age-classes. Note the difference in the scale of the vertical axis between the age 1+ and
2+ panels. Error bars denote the standard error of the mean and
asterisks denote significant differences (P < 0.05) between strains
(Mann–Whitney U test) or trials (Wilcoxon signed rank test).
fish. Again, flight duration was negatively correlated with
the distance to the predator at attack (first attack: rs = –0.49,
P = 0.003; second attack: rs = –0.45, P = 0.007). However,
there were no significant differences in the distance to the
predator between wild and farm fish.
Heart rate
In contrast with age 1+ fish, standard heart rate (t = 2.50,
P = 0.02) of age 2+ farm fish was higher than that of wild
fish. The same difference was found during trial II (F1,24 =
5.39, P = 0.03) (Fig. 1g). In both trials II and III, mean heart
rate varied significantly among time periods (P < 0.02 in
both cases) (Figs. 1g and 1h), but this effect was independent of strain. No other significant differences or correlations
were found.
Comparing performance of 1+ and 2+ age-classes
Table 2 compares the performance of the two age-classes.
The flight response to the predator was much more pronounced in age 1+ than in age 2+ fish (also see Fig. 2). Standard activity levels did not differ between year-classes,
whereas vertical position was higher in age 2+ salmon (Table 2). In contrast, heart rate was higher in age 1+ salmon.
Discussion
The behaviour of age 1+ wild and farm salmon in this
study supports previous findings that cultured fish show a reduced behavioural response towards predators (Johnsson and
Abrahams 1991; Johnsson et al. 1996; Einum and Fleming
Can. J. Fish. Aquat. Sci. Vol. 58, 2001
1997). The farm salmon in the present study showed a
reduced flight reaction to the first aerial predation threat
compared with wild fish from the progenitor population. We
compared cultured fish with wild fish derived from the principal founding population of the cultured strain and reared
from eggs under similar environmental conditions (also see
Fleming and Einum 1997). Thus, the differences observed
are likely attributable directly to the effects of domestication. While maternal influences (e.g., differences in egg
size) are possible, these are likely to be confined to the earliest life stages (i.e., the first few months following hatching;
Swain and Lindsey 1986; Einum and Fleming 2000) and are
unlikely to have played an important role in the results. Theoretically, the decision whether to flee from a predator or
not should be guided by the trade-off between the risk of
capture if remaining and the loss of foraging opportunities if
fleeing (Ydenberg and Dill 1986; Endler 1991). Selection in
the predator-free culture environment may have altered this
trade-off decision, with farm salmon being less inclined to
flee for a given level of threat. Alternatively, the ability of
farm salmon to detect or evaluate a predator threat may be
diminished. Either way, this will likely reduce their probability of surviving an encounter with a real predator (Webb
1984; Berejikian 1995).
It should be pointed out, however, that the stronger antipredator response in wild salmon could be due partly to the
fact that they were closer to the predator model at attack
than farmed fish. The flight reaction of both strains was
stronger the closer the predator was at attack, supporting the
economic model proposed by Ydenberg and Dill (1986) that
predicts an inverse relationship between flight probability
and the distance of the predator. Both strains habituated rapidly to the model heron, being less likely to flee and reducing their flight duration during the second attack, indicating
that they could rapidly discount the danger from the repeated, simulated predation threat. The cost of being killed
by a predator is considerably greater than that of lost foraging opportunities (the death versus dinner dilemma). Consistently, individuals are often initially cautious of all
approaching large animals (or models) and then gradually
learn which are really dangerous (see review by Smith
1997). Our results suggest that this ability has not been lost
in AquaGen farm salmon after seven generations of domestication. In agreement with previous studies on rainbow trout
(Jönsson et al. 1996), both farm and wild salmon lowered
their position in the water column following the first model
heron attack. This should increase survival probabilities in
the wild, as the capture success of live herons declines with
increasing water depth (Kramer et al. 1983).
The stronger behavioural response to the first predation
threat in age 1+ wild salmon was accompanied by a more
pronounced heart rate response. Moreover, heart rate 9–
12 min after the attack was positively correlated with flight
duration, and, in contrast with the farm fish, the heart rate of
wild fish did not quickly return to pre-attack levels. Clearly,
behavioural and heart rate fright responses were closely
linked. Thus, it seems that the wild salmon were more
“stressed” by the predator attack. This may be adaptive if a
high heart rate supplies the body with oxygen to prepare for
flight, should the predator appear again (Höjesjö et al. 1999).
Domestication had no significant effect on antipredator
© 2001 NRC Canada
J:\cjfas\cjfas58\cjfas-04\F01-025.vp
Friday, March 16, 2001 10:24:52 AM
Color profile: Generic CMYK printer profile
Composite Default screen
Johnsson et al.
793
Table 2. Summary of behavioural and heart rate differences between wild (W) and farm (F) Atlantic salmon and
between age-classes.
Variable
Age 1+
salmon
Age 2+
salmon
Number (proportion) of fish
fleeing after first attack*
W
>
F
>
=
W
=
F
Flight duration after first attack**
W
>
F
>
W
=
F
W
>
F
=
W
=
F
<
W
>
F
>
Standard activity**
Standard vertical position**
Standard heart rate***
>
=
<
>
W
=
F
W
=
F
W
<
F
Note: The symbols > and < denote directions of significant
differences (P < 0.05) between strains (vertical comparisons) or
age-classes (horizontal comparisons). Statistical tests used: *Fishers
exact test, **Mann–Whitney U test, and *** Student’s t test.
behaviour in age 2+ fish. It is unlikely that the difference in
water temperature between the experiments was responsible
for this discrepancy. A clear difference in antipredator behaviour occurred with age 0+ fish from the same two populations at around 15°C (Fleming and Einum 1997). Instead,
environmental effects of culture could override genetic effects as time in captivity increases. For example, sensitivity
to disturbances such as overhead movements and netting
may decrease with time spent in captivity, reducing general
vigilance as well as masking genetic differences in behaviour. The general reduction in antipredator response in age
2+ salmon may also be due to their larger size and older age,
as larger prey are more likely to survive a predator attack
(Godin 1997). Given the linkage between heart rate and behaviour in age 1+ fish, it is perhaps not surprising that wild
and farm age 2+ salmon showed a much less pronounced
heart rate response to the predator attack in association with
the reduced behavioural response.
The prediction that fish confined under farm conditions
for multiple generations would adapt physiologically and
would show lower standard heart rates than wild salmon was
supported in age 1+ fish. The increased heart rate in the wild
salmon may have resulted from their higher general activity,
suggesting higher energy expenditure. Thus, if these differences occur in the hatchery as well, they may help explain
the faster hatchery growth in domesticated salmon compared
with wild-type salmon (Einum and Fleming 1997; Fleming
and Einum 1997). The prediction was not supported in age
2+ fish because farm salmon had higher standard heart rates
than the wild salmon. As activity levels did not differ between age 2+ farm and wild salmon, the domesticated strain
seemed to maintain a higher heart rate for a given activity
level. We have no good explanation for this discrepancy be-
tween age 1+ and 2+ fish. Clearly, more controlled studies
are needed to elucidate how domestication and the culture
environment alter basic physiological adaptations in fish.
In summary, age 1+ salmon showed behavioural and heart
rate responses to predation risk that were integrated and reduced by artificial culture. Given that the strains had a common origin seven generations back, the differences found in
age 1+ fish likely reflect evolutionary changes due to domestication selection. In age 2+ salmon, antipredator responses
were considerably reduced and genetic domestication effects
were less clear, suggesting that environmental effects of artificial rearing override genetic effects as time in captivity increases. This should be taken into account when wild-type
stocks are maintained in hatcheries or when attempting to
predict the consequences of release or escape of domesticated animals in the wild (Snyder et al. 1996; Jonsson 1997;
Fleming et al. 2000).
Acknowledgments
This project was supported by European Commission contract FAIR CT 97 3498. J.H. was supported by grants from
the Helge Ax:son Johnson Foundation. The Nordic Academy
for Advanced Study supported a sabbatical visit by I.A.F. to
the Department of Zoology, Göteborg University, in 1998 to
work partly on this project. We thank the staff of the Norwegian Institute for Nature Research Station at Ims and
Rickard Johansson for excellent assistance and AquaGen
and the Skorstad rearing station for supplying the eggs for
the experiments. Emma Sernland and Uno Unger assisted
with video analyses, and Jordi Altimiras and Michael
Axelsson provided helpful advice on the Labview analyses.
Two anonymous referees provided helpful comments that
improved the manuscript.
References
Berejikian, B.A. 1995. The effects of hatchery and wild ancestry and
experience on the relative ability of steelhead trout fry (Oncorhynchus mykiss) to avoid a benthic predator. Can. J. Fish. Aquat.
Sci. 52: 2476–2482.
Borate, K., Jensen, F.B, and Andersen, B.B. 1993. Cardiac activity,
ventilation rate and acid–base regulation in rainbow trout exposed to hypoxia and combined hypoxia and hypercapnia. Fish
Physiol. Biochem. 12: 101–110.
Einum, S., and Fleming, I.A. 1997. Genetic divergence and interactions in the wild among native, farmed and hybrid Atlantic salmon.
J. Fish Biol. 50: 634–651.
Einum, S., and Fleming, I.A. 2000. Selection against late emergence and small offspring in Atlantic salmon (Salmo salar).
Evolution, 54: 628–639.
Endler, J.A. 1991. Interactions between predator and prey. In Behavioural ecology: an evolutionary approach. Edited by J.R. Krebs
and N.B. Davies. Blackwell Scientific Publications, Oxford, U.K.
pp. 169–196.
Espmark, Y., and Langvatn, R. 1985. Development and habituation of
cardiac and behavioral responses in young red deer calves (Cervus
elaphus) exposed to alarm stimuli. J. Mammal. 66: 702–711.
Fleming, I.A., and Einum, S. 1997. Experimental tests of genetic
divergence of farmed from wild Atlantic salmon due to domestication. ICES J. Mar. Sci. 54: 1051–1063.
© 2001 NRC Canada
J:\cjfas\cjfas58\cjfas-04\F01-025.vp
Friday, March 16, 2001 10:24:53 AM
Color profile: Generic CMYK printer profile
Composite Default screen
794
Fleming, I.A., Hindar, K., Mjølnerød, I.B., Jonsson, B., Balstad, T.,
and Lamberg, A. 2000. Lifetime success and interactions of
farm salmon invading a native population. Proc. R. Soc. Lond.
Ser. B, Biol. Sci. 267: 1517–1523.
Gjedrem, T., Gjøen, H.M., and Gjerde, B. 1991. Genetic origin of
Norwegian farmed Atlantic salmon. Aquaculture, 98: 41–50.
Godin, J.-G.J. 1997. Evading predators. In Behavioural ecology of
teleost fishes. Edited by J.-G.J. Godin. Oxford University Press,
New York. pp. 191–236.
Goodman, D.A., and Weinberger, N.M. 1971. Submerged electrodes in an aquarium: validation of a technique for remote sensing of a behavior. Behav. Res. Method. Instrum. 3: 281–286.
Höjesjö, J., Johnsson, J.I., and Axelsson, M. 1999. Behavioural and
heart rate responses to food limitation and predation risk: an experimental study on rainbow trout. J. Fish Biol. 55: 1009–1019.
Johnsson, J.I., and Abrahams, M.V. 1991. Interbreeding with domestic strain increases foraging under threat of predation in juvenile steelhead trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss): an experimental
study. Can. J. Fish. Aquat. Sci. 48: 243–247.
Johnsson, J.I., Petersson, E., Jönsson, E., Björnsson, B.T., and Järvi,
T. 1996. Domestication and growth hormone alter antipredator
behaviour and growth patterns in juvenile brown trout, Salmo
trutta. Can. J. Fish. Aquat. Sci. 53: 1546–1554.
Jonsson, B. 1997. A review of ecological and behavioural interactions between cultured and wild Atlantic salmon. ICES J. Mar.
Sci. 54: 1031–1039.
Jönsson, E., Johnsson, J.I., and Björnsson, B.T. 1996. Growth hormone increases predation exposure of rainbow trout. Proc. R.
Soc. Lond. Ser. B, Biol. Sci. 263: 647–651.
Kohane, M.J., and Parsons, P.A. 1989. Domestication. Evolutionary change under stress. Evol. Biol. 23: 31–48.
Can. J. Fish. Aquat. Sci. Vol. 58, 2001
Kramer, D.L., Manley, D., and Bourgeois, R. 1983. The effect of
respiratory mode and oxygen concentration on the risk of aerial
predation in fishes. Can. J. Zool. 61: 653–665.
Laitinen, M., and Valtonen, T. 1994. Cardiovascular, ventilatory
and total activity responses of brown trout to handling stress. J.
Fish Biol. 45: 933–942.
Metcalfe, N.B., Huntingford, F.A., and Thorpe, J.E. 1987. The influence of predation risk on the feeding motivation and foraging
strategy of juvenile Atlantic salmon. Anim. Behav. 35: 901–911.
Moen, A.N., Dellfera, A.L., Hiller, A.L., and Buxton, B.A. 1978.
Heart rates of white-tailed deer fawns in response to recorded
wolf howls. Can. J. Zool. 56: 107–1210.
Smith, E.N., Johnson, C., and Martin, K.J. 1981. Fear bradycardia
in captive eastern chipmunk, Tamias striatus. Comp. Biochem.
Physiol. A, Comp. Physiol. 70: 529–532.
Smith, R.J.F. 1997. Avoiding and deterring predators. In Behavioural ecology of teleost fishes. Edited by J.-G.J. Godin. Oxford
University Press, New York. pp. 163–190.
Snyder, N.F.R., Derrickson, S.R., Beissinger, S.R., Wiley, J.W.,
Smith, T.B., Toone, W.D., and Miller, B. 1996. Limitations of
captive breeding in endangered species recovery. Conserv. Biol.
10: 338–348.
Swain, D.P, and Lindsey, C.C. 1986. Meristic variation in a clone
of the cyprinodont fish Rivulus marmoratus related to temperature history of the parents and of the embryos. Can. J. Zool. 64:
1444–1459.
Webb, P.W. 1984. Body and fin form and strike tactics of four teleost predators attacking fathead minnow (Pimephales promelas)
prey. Can. J. Fish. Aquat. Sci. 41: 157–165.
Ydenberg, R.C., and Dill, L.M. 1986. The economics of fleeing
from predators. Adv. Stud. Behav. 16: 229–249.
© 2001 NRC Canada
J:\cjfas\cjfas58\cjfas-04\F01-025.vp
Friday, March 16, 2001 10:24:53 AM
Download