AN ABSTRACT OF TW TILiS] OF Redacted for privacy An tnvestigation was made of the influence that agonistic behavior ma.y have in regulating population densities of the Nontane vole, Microtus montanus montanus (Peale) conducted on the The study was . A. Gaary ranch, Kiamath County, Oregon, from The objectives were June, ]$63, to March, 1965. (i) to determine if cert.ain measures of agonia tic behavior influence density, reproduction, sex and gs composition, and survival of voles from parent populations of different densities; and (2) to investigate the role that agonistie behavior and population density play in the dispersal movement of voles, Seven one-quarter acre enclosures, containing high numbers arising £z'c*u different initial parenb stocks, and one field plot, with a low density population, were live trapped periodically to determine their characteristics. fi'e of seven enclosures Mice were able to disperse from Observations of agonis tic behavior were made in controlled behavior trials of mica from these populations. aperent in the vol& po1atiors Aonj,s tic bezaior was rnor Diaperal that reaehed the higher densiti veent waa mtgg*sted te reduce 1ntr-3pocii1c srif by t.eporari1y loweriri th ffrst and This was nt yei high density pr11ation5. c denatties of .) rvidrt in rAii third year high density popilations. Adult iiaie vlea of he 1w dn3ity popalation utiuizod tore threat behavior than contac fighting behavior. Agonts tic behavior was ioz'e pronounced in the high denaity uoilatios containing a hither nwber of adults. f tales A larger ratio -.o iaies was augostd 'o incr,ase fighting behavior during high densities Increased intraspscific strife wa indicated tc have ru1ted fIh1 uiions exhibiting hiher roduc4ve p-irforitance fcr in lower sverae. ?rena..l :irr ortality, IhiCh be inducd by tntraisoific strife, held dom th lvL of a md.3d lation t11 t) i o decraa b a ..s s.içted to j'ak density uvcmil this study4 as d3nsitL3 increased survival kgonistic behavior was :rs factor which lirtited survival when pop-. ulat.trris :ore a. itor dcriitte. xplora cry hhavior corbined with fight.iri indicat.d to dispersi-n which increases with )O)ULIticfl dCTiiO5.. Yjee of 5eC)O oxrinccd r-re fighting bofor first yiar behavior was pit1ationa and andtn h1rd 7'D po1lat.i3ns disersd han rice of Non-dtspersal iales plyd 're agonistic behavior than dispersal nL&1e8 of the 1963 populations. This study indicates that agonistic behavior, which influences dispersal movement, survival, and, to a lesser extent, reproduction is a factor in the regulation of high density vole populations. Redacted for privacy In Charge of' Major Redacted for privacy Redacted for privacy Dean of Gaa School Date thesis is resented Typed by Electra Whetstone / / 3 AGONISTIC EHAVIOR IN !4ONANE VOLES, MIcROUS 1ONTAUS, FR DIFFE2.E DENS TRIES aubinitted to OREGON STATE UNIVERSiTY in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE June l6S The author wishes to express his appreciation to the following persons who made this thesis possible: Sincere thanks are given to Mr. Edward L.Hansen, Instructor, Department of Fisheries and Wildlife, Oregon State University, for his assistance, guidance, and supervision throughout the study. The author is very greatl'ul to Mr. Howard Wight, Associate Professor, and Mr. Lee Kuhn, Associate Professor, Department of Fisheries and Wildlife, Oregon State University, for their helpful criticism in the preparation of this thesis. Thanks are extended to Mr. Jerome Smith, graduate research assistant, for his and laboratory work. assistance in field The office and laboratory facilities provided by Mr. Gene Gross, supervisor of the Kiamath Branch Experiment Station, are appreciated. Mr. Edward Geary provided the land upon which the enclosures were built and thus mede this study possible. A final thank you is due my wife, Charlotte, for her help in tyLng the rough draft of this thesis. TABLE OF COTE5 Page INTRO1JcTION.. ... ....... .. .. ................ ....... 3. . . . . . . a * a a a a * a a 6 . . .. 6 . . , . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7 Contact Behavior.... .n...........e.......t..a...e.,.....,ea 7 Agosic Behavior. . . . . . . . . . . . . . 8 Disposition. . * . . * . * . . . . . . . . . . . . . * a a * Maintenance. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . * Expiox'atory l3ehavior. . . . * . Attao and Retaliation. . . . . . . Threat. . * . . . . . . * . . .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . * . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 9 . * . . . . . . * . . . . . . . . . . * . . . * . . . . . . . . . . . . . . * . . . . . . . . . . . . 12 Retreat and Avoidance...,.. ... . . , . . . . . . . . . . * . . . . . . . . . . ... ....... ......... ...... 12 Submissive Behavior........................................ 12 .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . * * . . . . . . . lb Territoriality..............-................................. 15 Displacement Activity. . . . * . . . . . . . * . . . 15 16 Study Area.... ..... ............,.,...........,.......,...... 16 Eno1oeureaandTheirPopu1ations............................ 16 Caledonia-6 Field Plot...................................,... 19 Dispersal Ramps.............................................. 19 Live Trapping. *a#*****t***Ø*s**ø**..****a**eS****a*Ø 20 acteristica... .... ......... 22 Determination of Population C]Behavior Studies. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . * . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . * . . . . . . . . . 23 TABLE OF COEWS - CONTINUED RESULTS B a . . a . . . . . . Behavior Characteristics. B * a B a a a a a a . . a . a a a . . . * . a a a a a a a a a a a 2 28 Agonistic Behavior Patterns of the 28 Non-dispersal Nice Coared to Dispersal NiC6...a.a.asaa.* 314 Solitary Behavior Patterns a a. a a 38 B B S B a a B 140 Trapping Periods..,....BB.....B...B..B......B......,..... 140 Population 1)SflSiti$B.Ba.*..B.....B.a...*a..WB*.B...a.aa...B 142 Sex and Ago COmpOsitiofl.... a. a. a.. .. *..,a.. ..a a.., a. * 51 Reproduction.B..aa....a..a...e..B..B...B.s...*.a.B....*-..a. 55 General Survjval....................B...B.,................ 62 Dispersal Rflfl1$a.....a...a*a..I..aa..aaB..B..aaea..a**aB*.s 71 Wounding. B S B a 77 Population Characteristics B * a a B B S a a S * B B * DISCUSSION..... ala .aa..a. a. a a a s a a a a * a a a a a a a a a , . B a a B B a a B B B * S B B S a B B S B * a S B B B * B B Ô S * B S * a S a a B B S B B S B 83 aas I,. B* *0** 5*5*0 SB .BaB*,BB. 5 83 ... 86 AgonisticBehaviorVersusRsproduction....................... 87 AgonisticBehaviorVersusGeneralSurvival................... 89 Agonistic Behavior Versus Dispersa1Movement................. 90 ONGI.tJSIONS. a S. BaBB. Ba..... *ta*s * a BS** a.a..,S. a... Ba BBBa B * Baa 93 Agonistic Behavior Versus Population Dens3.ties., Agonistic Behavior Versus Sex and Age Composition. a *a**a* SB ** BISLIOGRAPHY.... as...,.. a.. ...a.aea SaBB*e a. aaa*ØS.S.sBaa. 95 LIST OF FIGURES Page Figure vole approaching a subordinate vole............ 1. Dominant 2. Retaliation of a subordinate vole (left) from a dominant vole (right). . ..... ... ... ...s .. i .. .... 3. b. Two voles pushing and boxing .... 11 . . . .. . . . . . .. . . . . . . .. 13 each other with their forepaws. .. . .. . . .. . . . . . . . .. . . . * . . iunway of a dispersal ran leaving an enclosure. Note the wood box at the end of the ra containing a treadle which drops a mouse into the capture bucket............. 5. 6. 10 21 Top view of a capture bucket, containing a mouse, which has been removed from beneath the treadle box........... 21 The dual couartment cage was divided by a partition containing a hole and a sliding door through which either mouse was able to cross to the opponents aide ciuring a trial.......................................... 2 7. Headwouudsofanadultvole............................ 79 8. Rump wounds of an adult vole..........................,. 80 LIST OF TABLES Table Page I. II. STOCKfl HISTORY AND POPULATION PHASE SEQUENCE FORENCLOSURES iTO 7....,......................... 18 NUMBER TRIAL NICE AND DUAL BEAVIOR TRIAlS AND AVERAGES OF FIGHPING TDE, AGGRESSIVE AND DEFENSIVE FIGIff 1MG, AND DOMINANCE AND SUBORDINANCE FOR THE POPULATIONS REPRESEING III. IV. THE FIRST, SECOND, AND THIRD HIGH DENSITY YEAR PHASESFOR 1963 AND 196b. . . . * . . . . . . . .. . . . . . . . 29 AVERAGE JYJAL TRIAL BEHAVIOR CHARACERISTIC5 OF A]JLT MALE MICE FROM ENCLOSURES 1 TO 7 FROM JUNE THROUGH OCL'OBER, 1963. . . . . . . . . . . . * . . . . . . 31 AVERAGE BEHAV10T? CHARACrEBISTICS OF AIULT MALE MICE FROM ENCLOSURES 1 TO 7 AND THE QALEDONIA-6 PLCIP FROM JUNE TO DECEMBER, 196b.................. V. AVERAGE AGONIST IC BEHAVIOR CHARACTERISTICS FOR ArVLT MALE NON-DISPERSAL AND DISPERSAL MICE FROM ENCLOSURES 1, 2, 5, 3, AND 6 FROM JUNE THR.cJGH OCTOBER, 1963. ........ . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . VI. 35 AVERAGE AGONISTIC BEHAVIOR CHARACTERISTICS FOR AIIJLT MALE NON-DISPERSAL AND DISPERSAL MICE FROM ENCLOSURES 1, 2, 5, 3, AND 6 FROM JUNE THROUGH DECIBER, 196b. . . . . . . . . . . . . * . . . . . . . . . . VII. 32 37 EXPLORATORY, ACTIVE, INACTIVE, AND WASHING AND GROOMING SOLITARY BEHAVIOR CHARACTERISTICS FOR DISPERSAL MICE CAFflTItED IN RAMPS 1, 2, 5, 3, AND 6 AND FOR NON-DISPERSAL MICE FROM TEZE ENCLOSURES ILIRING JUNE THROUGH OCTOBER, 1963....... VIII. EXPLORATORY, ACTIVE, INACTIVE, AND WASHING AND GROOMING SOLITARY BEHAVIOR CHARACTERISTICS OF DISPERSAL NICE FROM RAMPS 1, 2, 5, 3, AND 6 THROUGHJUNETODECEMBER,19& ............. IX. 39 ......., hi. TRAP PERIOD DESIGNATIONS FOR 1 14, I 2d, AND II 3d FROM JUNE, 1963, THROUGH JANUARY, 196b....... li.3 LIST OF TABLES CONTINUEI) Page Table X TRAP PERIOD DESIGNATION FOR II 34, II 2d, II Sa, AND LI 7 FRC*I MAY THROUGH DECEMBER, 1964. . * . XI. . . . a a a a a * * a. a a a a a a a a TRAP PERIOD DESIGNATION FOR III 3d. a * a a a a a a a a a a a a 414 III 14 In 6d, AND THE CALED0Nm.-6 r'iør Fac x THROUGHDECI}i3]t, 19614. a a XII. a a a a a a a a . a a a a a a a a a a a a a a a a 145 TOTAL 141CR CAPIVRED, POPULATION EST4ATE, DENSiTY PER AE, AND THE NUMBER DEAD IN XIII. XIV. XV. XVI TRAPS FOR ENCLOSURES 1, 2, 5, 7, 3, 14, AND 6 FROM JUNE, 1963, TO JANUARY, 19614...........,.... 46 TOTAL MICE CAF'rURED, POPULATION ESTmATE, DENSITY PEL. ACRE, AND THE NUMBER DEAD IN TRAPS FOR ENCLOSURES 1 THROUGH 7 AND FOR CALEDONIA-6 PLOT FROM MAY THROUGH DECEMBER, 1964.....................,....,.......... 48 PERCENTAGE OF AWLTS, NUMBER OF MALES AND FEMALES, THEIR KEANS, AND THE SEX RATIOS JUNE, 1963, FOR I 14, I 2d, AND II 3d, FR( TOJANUARY, 196Li..............,.....,..........,.,.. 52 PERCENTAGES OF AIXJLTS, NUMBER OF MALES AND FEMALES, THEIR MEANS, AND THE SEX RATIOS FOR ENCLOSURES 1 TO 7 AND FOR CALEDONIA.-6 PLOT FROM MAY TO DECEMBER, 1964.................... 53 NUMBER OF SUB-A1TLT AND AJJLT FEMALES AND PERCEAOES OF THESE FEMALES PERFORATE, PREX3NANT, OR WITH MAMMARY GLANDS LARGE OR LACTATING, AND THEIR MEANS FOR ENCLOSURES 1 THROUGH 7 FROM JULY TO NOWi}1BER OR DECEMBER, 1963..................,........,......... XVII. 56 NUMBER OF SUB-A1JLT AND AThJLT FEMALES AND PERCENTAGES OF THESE FEMALES PERFORATE, PREGNANr, OR WITH MAMMARY GLANDS LARGE OR LACTATING AND THEIR MEANS FOR ENCLOSURES 3. THROUGH. 7 AND THE C.kLEDONIA-6 PLOT FROM MAYTO J3ECEMBER, 1964........... ...a.a*.a.aa..,.. 59 Table XVIII. SURVIVAL RATES FOR AIXILTS, SUB-A1JLTS, AND JUVENILES C(1'1BINED FOR I Id, I 2d, AND II 3d FR 1963.. ...,., Xii. JUNE THR.OUGH DEC]NB, . os. ......a...s.a...4o .....** 63 SURVIVAL RATES OF ALiJLTS, SUB-.AIJJLTS, AND JUVENILES IN II ld FRC MAY TO DEC11B]Ft, 19614..................................... XX. SURVIVAL RATES OF ALULTS, SUB-LWLTS, AND UVENDJES IN II 2d FRC MAY TO ........ .. . DECF'IBER,l9614.. ... . . . .... .. .... XXI. . . . . . , . . . * . . . . . . . 68 SURVIVAL RATES OF A]IJLTS, SUB-AII]LTS, AND JUVENILES IN III 6d FROM NAY TO DECEMBER., 196!i.........,...................,....... XXV. 67 SURVIVAL OF AWLTS, SUB-AWLTS, AND JUVENILES IN II Sd FROM NAY TO DECEMBER, 19614............,........................ XXIV. 66 SURVIVAL RATES OF AILtLTS, SiJB-AIULTS, AND JUVENILES IN III b FROM MAY TO DECENBER, 196b. . . . * . . . . . . . * . . . . . . . * . . . . . . . XXIII. 65 SURVIVAL RATES OF AWLTS, SUB-AWLTS, AND JUVENILES IN III 3d FR( MAY TO DECMBaR, 196b. .. .. .. . .. . .... .... ..... ......... .... ... .... XXII. 6b 69 SURVIVAL RATES OF AIJLTS, STJB-AWLTS, AND JUVENILES IN II 7 FROM MAY TO DECE}IBER., XXVI. .. ... ...,..... ........ ..... . ..... . ........ 70 TOTAL NICE GAUGI, CAP2URES PER RAMP DAY, POPULATIOI ESTATE, AN]) }RCENT SCARS AN]) WWNDS OF NICE AFflJRED IN TI DISPERSAL RAMPS FROM JULY 2, 1963, TO NOVEMBER 15, 1963.. ...... ...... ..... ... ..... ,.......... .... 72 LIST OF TABLES - CONTINUED Table XXVII. Page T(YLAL NICE CAUGI, CAPTURES PER RAMP DA.Y, POPULATION ESTfl4ATE, AND PEECXL' SCARS AND WOUNDS OF MICE CAPTURED IN TH DISPERSAL RAMPS FROM JUNE 2)4, 196)4, TO DECE'iBER. 9, 19613. ........... XXVIII. 7!i MONTHLY PERCENTAGES AND MEAN ANNUAL PERCENTAGES OF WOUNDS AND SCARS FOUND ON MICE CAPTURED, AND TtLi TOTAL CAPTURE FOR EN(1OSUES 1 THROUGH 7 FROM JUNE, 1963, TO JANUAt.Y, 196)4... .......................... ..,.....,. XXIX. 81 MONTHLY PERCENTAGES AND MEAN ANNUAL PERCENTAGES OF WCIINDS AND SCARS FOUND ON MICE CAPTURED, AND TILE TOTAL CAPJ1URE FOR ENCLOSURES 1 THROUGH 7 AND CALEDONIA-6 PLOT FROM MAY TO DECE2!IBER, 196)4 .................... , , * * . . .......... XXX. . . . . . . . . . . 82 AVERAGE FIGIfl]3 TflIES AND PEAK DENSITY ESTATES- FOR MICE OF ENCLOSURES 1 THROUGH 7 tJRING 1963 AND 196)4 ................ .....,......... 8)4 AGRONISTIC BkVIOR IN MONL4JE VOLES, HIGROTUS NONTLNUS, FRC* DIFFERENT PARENT POPULA!ION DFSITI5 This report presents the results of a field and laboratory investigation of agonietic behavioral patterns observed in populations of the I1ontane vole, Microtus montarius montanus (Peale), derived from parent populations of different densities * The purpose of this study was to determine the possible influence of agonistic behavior in the regulation of meadow mouse densities The field work for this study was conducted on the E. A. Geary Rench, flamath County, Oregon, from June, 1963, to March, 1965. Th objetiee were: (1) to determine if certain measures of, agonistjc behavior influence density, reproduction, sex and age composition, ar$ survival of voles from parent populations of different densities; and (2) to investigate the role that agonistic behavior and population density play in the dispersal movement of voles. In April, 1958, a research project, entitled The life history of the meadow mouse, Microtus montanus, in Oregon, with particular reference to factors influencing seasonal and annual population trends," was started by the Oregon Agriculture Experiment Station under the direction of the Department of Fisheries and Wildlife. Since October, 1960, the program has received additional support from the United States Public Health Service (Research Grant 7758). report is the fourth graduate study resulting from this project. This L The history of Miorotine fluctuations and irruptions has been recorded in Europe and Asia for centuries (Elton, l9I2). Such plagues and economic losses have exotted the interests of biologists to the point that an extensive amount of literature has accumulated on this subject (Elton, 11.2). Periodic fluctuations of small mammal populations have not been satisfactorily explained. Research work in the l93Os and l91O's emphasized mortality factors such as prodation,parasitism and disease, climatic conditions, and reduced food supplies (Hamilton, 1937). Chitty1 s (1952; 1955) studies of the vole, Nicrotus agreatis, revealed that fluctuations occurred every three or four years and tended to be synchronized. None of the above mortality factors appeared to be important because they were not synchronized with the cycle (Chitty, 1955). Chitty (1952) suggested that intraspecific strife is of primary importance in determining the size of natural populations of the vole. He inferred that high juvenile mortality was a symptom of severe intraspecific strife, and that this strife probably affected the hormone balance of females so that their young suffered defects which reduced their survival rate and impaired their ability to produce a normal generation. He hypothesized that the condition of one generation affects the breeding of the next two (Cliitty, 1952). Clarke (1955), working with }ttcrotua agrestis, used outdoor cages to study the influence of density on the reproduction and survival of two experimental vole populations. Fighting and chasing were observed in both populations, but aggressive activity and wounding 3 were more frequent in the cage with the highest population. limited cluded that the strife He con- population growth in both populations but was mOre apparent in the higher density population. Louch (19S6) used indoor pens in studying populations of Microtus psylvaniotzs penneylvanicus (Ord) to test the hypothesis that high population density is a stress factor which may limit population growth. He found that reproduction did not change with an increased population density, but that litter and adult surviv-al decreased &t high density levels. The increased litter iortality was apparently due to a disturbance of the lactating females from over crowded population conditions. The high adult mortality rates were attributed to an increased incidence of infectious disease at higher densities. Frank (l9?), studying the behavior of Microtus arvalis (Pallas) in outdoor pens, concluded that social behavior was important in regulating population growth. He emphasized that population increases result from the reproductive potential, the carrying capacity, and the condensation potential. The condensation potential mechanism can be said to be in effect when the home range is reduced, when social comnunities of females and families develop, and excess males are eliminated. The mechanism permits a cyclic species to live at an uncommonly high density. Frank further states, a rapid population increase cannot be regulated by normal mortality and dispersal, and when a supportable density is exceeded, a population crash occurs. "$hock-disease," a condition associated with physiological derangements involving low blood-sugar and glycogen levels, was Frank's explanation for a population crash. Chitty (19S5) suisnarized that it populations are not equally resistant to mortality forces at various levels of abundance then "the abundance of certain species may be limited primarily through a change in susceptibility, and the direct causes of death may vary according to local circumstances." He further stated that "the peculiarities of a cycle in numbers cannot be attributed to depletion of the food supply." Chitty (l97) found that descendants of voles which had been cravded had enlarged spleens even after a generation of low density, and this pathological condition which terminates a period of abundance probably Is controlled by a hereditary factor. Chitty (1960) proposed the general hypothesis that "all species are capable of regulating their own population densities without destroying the renewable resources of their enviroument or requiring enemies or bad weather to keep them train doing so." The effects of independent events, such as weather, became more severe as numbers rise and the quality of' the population falls. The offspring of a population are lost except those that replace the older animals. Besides the mortality factors conmon to a par- ticular population there is a mechanism that eliminates the remaining surplus. Chitty's polymorphic-behavior hypothesIs is the most recent attempt to explain this mechanism (Krebs, 1963). Chitty has proposed (Krebe, 1963) that populations change in quality during changes in abundance. He suggested that the mechanism was mutual antagonism associated with high breeding densIties which brings about a change in the properties of' the population through selection. Chitty and Phippa have shown that voles born during a phase of rapid population (1961) increase have higher average adult body weights than those born in years of limited population growth. They are examining the hypothesis that a decline in numbers is a necessary consequence of the selective action of hostility against the genotypes present during the phase of rapid increase. The evidence that tests Ghitty'a genetic chanism is limited. Aggression seems to be dependent upon genetics (ivia, 1962). Aggressive behavior patterns within individuals vary, and this "variation is likely to be influenced genetically, but it is also related to individual experience" (Barnett, 1957). Smith (196b) suggests that voles, Microtus montanus montanus (Peale), from a higher density parent population are selected for aggressiveness, and this aggressiveness probably causes a disrupted social structure. Intra- specific strife causes disrupted social structures characterized by prenatal mortality, poor nesting care, poor juvenile survival, and cannabilism (Calhoun, 1962 Clarke, 1955). Dispersal movement by voles is important in providing a stable social structure, but as the density of a population increases, the influence of dispersion on the social structure decreases (Smith, 196)4). Smith (l96Li) found that where an enclosed high density parent population was not allowed to disperse, the prenatal mortality and the amount o± wounding, caused by aggression, was greater than if dispersion was allowed or where voles were from a low density parent pop- ulation. A study of behavioral changes occurring in vole pojilationa may reveal a clearer explanation of the regulation of population densities. The study of animal behavior is of the activity for the whole organism or groups of organisms. Animal behavior can be divided into solitary behavior (which includes the activities of a single animal), arid social behavior (which includes all behavior patterns involving two or more interacting animals). The aspects of behavior that are important to this study are reviewed. Disposition Voles seem to possess individuality in dispositions and habits. Some voles are timid and nervous, while others are gentle (Bailey, i92l). Voles living in an uncomfortable envirorent are quarrelsome and often fight to the death. DiZerences in behavior are noted early when the young are beginning to run about, and Bailey (l921) implies that these differences seem to be inherited characteristics. The young are mild mannered until the second or third day after they open their eyes when they begin to take on the aggressive nature of the adults (Bailey, l92l). (aintenance Washing and grooming activities are common in conflict situations. Washing begins with rairld motions of the paws moving together under the mouth. Then the paws are raised to the cheeks and drawn down to the tip of the nose. Foflowing this procedure are longer strokes of the L 7 paws that reach to the ears and are accompanied with a bobbing motion of the head. Next th flanks and back are combed with the forepaws while mouthing the fur with a bobbing head motion. Scratching with the hind feet occurs often during the cleaning procedure of the entire body (Clarke, 1955). ploratory Behavior The exploratory behavior of a vole serves to acquaint it with the habitat in which it lives. When a mouse is placed into a strange area, it begins to explore its surroundings. Initially the body is tense and extended, and this Is termed the "elongate posture" (Eisenberg, 1962). The mouse moves in short runs, but soon discards Noseing the elongate posture and moves by means of a slower walk. is a very important part of a voles investigation, and during these movements a mouse often rears up on his hind legs, nods its head, and tests the air with its nose. When a new object is encountered, the exploratory actIvity pattern is prolonged (Shillito, 1963). Exploratory behavior combined With fighting and conflict tends to promote dispersion and movement in Norway rate (Barnett, 1957). Contact Behavior When two mice encounter one another, the contact may be made in a nose to nose or nasa-anal fashion (Eissnberg, 1962). Often among adult males, fighting is initiated immediately upon contact, and naso-nasal. contact is not experienced. The distinction between sexes by mice seems to be made rapidi(Eiaenberg, 1962), Agonistic Behavior Agonistic behavior, as defined by Scott (1962), is the involv-. merit of fighting, escapement, defensive posture, and passive behavior patterns Any sort of adaptation which is connected with a contest or conflict between two animals may be included in this term. A summary (Clark, 1962) of literature concerning Lighting and hostility in animals emphasizes the following characteristic principles of agonistic behavior. 1. Fighting is affected by learning. Success in fighting re-inforces aggressive behavior. 2. Social structure of populations limits the destructive levels of aggression. Dominance may be formed by older animals over younger ones without serious fighting. established the relationships peaceful manner. After a dominance order is of animals can be stabilized in a Threats by dominance are reacted to in a submissive way by lower hierarchy animals. 3. The territoriality phenomenon is important in reducing lethal combat, but, on the other hand, a violation of territory rights can cause fighting. b Animals disorganized in their social or geographic factors may experience an increased incidence of destructive, agonistic behavior. Scott (1962) states that deer mice fight only when two conditions are present. First, when there is a disorganized pop- ulation and strangers are forcibly brought together, and second, when the mice cannot possibly escape. 5. Fighting behavior is brought about by an animal's response to edernal stimulation. Males more frequently than females inflict wounds on other voles, usually other adult males (Bailey, 192b; Frank, 1957). Males will not tolerate each other when they are in the presence of females (Carter, 1963). Some males are more successful in Lights than others, and they come to be the dc*rth2auts of the population. A female in advanced pregnancy or with a litter will attack any vole that comes near her nest. A dominant, male or female, does not necessarily retain its position within the group (Clarke, 1955). Attack and Retaliation The dominant vole may make quick, agitated, jerky rune with brief pauses before attacking a subordinate (Figure 1). When moving in this manner, the body of the dominant is held high off the substrate and has a sleek appearance. Before the attack, the dominant will erect his hair and hunch up his back which makes him look larger. The dominant chases and attacks the subordinate, and fighting is the result. A subordinate may retaliate against the dominant before he is caught by turning and Lacing his pursuer. A retaliating subordinate may make a lunge with incisors bared at the dominant vole, or he may squat on his hind quarters and squeal loudly at the approach of the aggressor (Figure 2). A subordinate's retaliation often causes a dominant to retire. After a dominant retreats, he may reapproach the 8ubordinate, and the retaliation process Will be repeated. Often 10 Figure 1. Dominant vole approaching a subordinate vole. ii Figure 2. Retaliation of a subordinate vole (left) from a dominant vole (right). 12 this return of the dominant is vigorous enough to cause the sub ordinate to run (Clarke, 1955). In encounters of equally dominant voles, both mice usually stand or squat on their haunches. They push and box at each other with their forepaws as shown in Figure 3, and they may commence to wrestle. boxing and. wrestling occurs in both young and adult voles (Clarke, 19%). Threat The threat pattern is evident when a mouse remains on all fours with his hair erected, his body hunched up, and, sometimes, his incisors exposed. The head may dart at the opposite mouse, and this threat pattern terminates in a Light, chaèe, or when one mouse moves away (Clarke, 19%). Retreat and Avoidance When a mouse ía defeated he usually flees from the winners 'vicinity. If chased by the dominant, the subordinate may avoid contact by leaping away (Eiaenberg, 1962). Submissive Behavior A subordinate vole will tolerate the aggressive approach of a dominant. behavior". Clarke (19%) refers to submissive behavior as "appeasing in usin' bmiasive behavior an animal doesn't the aggressor the stimuli which releases an attack. vovide This behavior is shown when a subordinate turns over on his back in front of the dominant, and a would be attacker ceases Another posture position 13 4 Figure 3. Two voles pushing and bod.ng each other with their forepaws. I' that prevents art attack is a "freeze" sttion by the subordinate in such a manner that the pale skin of the ock and bee of the ears is visible. oceur Again, no attack The tail ts usually snarply erected. even though the dominant tend near the subordinate (Clarke, 1955). L'jp].acement .ctivity Voles exhicit soue behavior activities which seex to be inappropz'ite, activity. ndlar& (195) suggests that they are dieplaceaient cisplaeement activity (Tinbergen, 1959) is the behavior expressed when the activated drive of n aniial is denieci discharge through the noral conswnmatory acts. Two drives, such as the flight drive and the aggressive drive, may conflict with each other and activate a pattern of behavior that is in?proriate to the ituation Ixnmeuiately after uciinant retir .s from retaliating subordinate he niay eomnence in a rapid digir.g ..ctiyity. This inappropriate activity is aued w'iefl a highly activated agressive drive is thwartec by an activation of the flight drive (C1rke, 1955). a ubordiriata rtay exhibit which ia again inappropriate. acitivatiort of the A±ter r'id waehng and gr oing ctivity This activity is caused by s.ight ggreasive drive 'when the fligtt orive is activte (Clarke, 195). A?he jerky rAoveent exhibited by attacking males indicate3 cnf4ct between the aggressive fljg drjves. i'hen zjnate vole enters a aubordinate's cage, he may express the 2yements of ttridgeting, marking tilde, waltzing, and dancthg (aarke, l95), This behavior seems to indicate an activation of the flight drive as the aggressive vole enters the opponents cage. Territoriality Barnett (19S7) states that flight usually depends on two conditions: "being in a familiar place, and encountering a agrariger." The location of an encounter will influence the dominance Or sub- ordinance of the individuals concerned. Experiemnts have shown that the familiarization of a compartment and the inpregnation of scent in the nest is a definite influence on the establishment of territory behavior (Eisenberg, 1962). The wounds on the hind q ters of subordinates are the result of being chased, and wounds may be present in other areas of the body it subordinates have been caught. Dominant voles have wounds on their headi and necks which are the results of contact with retaliating subordinates or other dominants. Therefore, some scars on the head designate dominants, and scars on the hind quarters signify sub- ordinance (Clarke, 19%). It is apparent from this review of literature that the mechanisms for the regulation of Z'!icrotine populations are not completely understood. This study was established to provide an indication of the role that agonietic behavior may play in the regulation of vole densities. Study Area A one-fifth acre field plot and seven one-quarter acre enclosure plots are located in the Caledonia-6 pasture on the E. A. (}eary Ranch, eight miles northwest of IClamath Falls, Oregon. The Caledonia area has been reclaimed from Upper Klamath Lake and is composed of mucktype soil, rich in organic material, from one to several feet in depth and overlying a clay substrate. The water table during the wet months normally rises to within a few inches of ground level, and recedes during the dry months to eighteen inches or more below the surface. The vegetation within the study area consists mainly of meadow foxtil Alopecurus pratensis (L.), bent grass 'oati pains tris (Hudson), and blue grass Poa pratensia (L.). The climatic conditions are characterized by warm suxxmers and cool winters. Annual precipitation averages about lh inches, half in the form of snow. During the dry season, June, July, and August, the mean monthly temperature is approxtmately 68° F. The coldest month, January, averages 30° F (U. S. Weather Bureau, 1963). Enclosure and Thei Populations The seven one-quarter acre, Umouse proof" enclosures used in this project were constructed in the spring of of an enclosure (1Oli feet 1961. The construction by 1OL feet) has been explained in detail by Carter (1963) and Smith (19614). 17 Throughout the study, the enclosures contained mice that population phases. Phases originated from different high density refers to the number of years that the populations have had high densities. Four enclosures (1, 2, 5, and 7) were stocked in May, 1963, mith voles from a low, 1962, parent population density (75 mice per acre) that existed in the Caledonia-3 pasture. three enclosures (3, Lj, and 6) In May, 1963, contained voles from a high, 1962, parent population density (.76 - 792 mice per acre) that originated The sequence of the stocking from initial stocking in May, 1962. and population phases for the seven enclosures is shown in Table I. By fall o± 1961i., Enclosures 1, 2, 5, and 7 contained populations in nclosures 3, b, and 6 had their second year of high density, and populations in and 7 their third year of high density. Enclosures 1, 2, are to be designated as I 1, I 2, 1 when referring to 1963 5, and I 7, respectively, first year high density enclosures and are to be designated as II 1, II 2, II referring 5, 5, and II 7, respectively, when to 196L second year high densities. 6 are to be designated as II 3, II b, and Enclosures 3, b, and II 6, respectively, when referring to 1963 second year high density enclosures and are to be designated as III 3, III L, III 6, respectively, when referring to 196b third year high densities. From the arrangement of population phases the behavior and population characteristics of each generation phase could be compared to one another to provide an insight as to whether or not parent population density has an influence on ensuing generations with TABLE I. STOCKING HISTORY AND POPULLTION PHASE SEQUENG FOR ENCLOSURES 1 TO 7. flA.TE ENCLOSURES 3, b, and 6 Sjring 1962 Stock-mice from Caledonia Low 1961 parent population Fall 1962 Phase I First year high density populations ENCLOSURES 1, 2, 5, axd 7 Stock-mice from Caledonia I4ow 1962 parent population Project Beginning Fall 1963 Phase II Second year high density populations Phase I First year high density populations Fall 196h Phase III Third year high density populations Phase II Second year high density populations respect to agoriietic behavior. The enclosures were irrigated during the dry months of 1963 and In the 196b to provide the moat favorable vegetative cover possible. fall of both years, feed oat8 (10 pounds per enclosure per week) and bay were placed mithin the enclosures. Food and neettng material seemed to be adequate, even though natural foods and cover bad been depleted when high population densities occurred. Caledonia-6 Field Plot In July, l96i, a field grid was set up on the Caledonia-6 pasture adjacent to the seven enclosures. The objective was to gain additional information on behavior characteristics of voles living in low populated areas. The plot (60 feet by lLiQ feet) consisted of three rows of seven trap stations. from July to November. This plot was trapped once a month The vegetation was predominantly bent grass, blue grass, and meadow foxtail, but because o± constant cattle usage, the grass cover was usually one to two inches high. Prior to June, 1963, a dispersal ramp was attached to Enclosures 1, 2, 3, , and 6. The letter 1*dff added to the enclosure population designation indicates a dispersal ramp for the enclosure (I Id, I 2d, II 3d, I 5d, and II 64). Smith (l961) baa described the construction and dimensions of these dispersal ramps. Each dispersal ramp contained an open, flat runway which presented no over head cover and was considered to be an adverse condition to mice moving out of the 20 enclosures (:Figure b). It seems probable that mice exhibiting dispersal characteristics would comprise the bulk of voles caught in the capture bucket at the far end of each ramp (Figure ). Steel metal doors between the enclosures and ramps were opened for an average of 1? days a month for July, August, September, and October, and opened for three days in November, 1963. In 196b they were opened during the period June 23 to December 9 for an average of six days a month. The ramps were never opened during adverse weather which may disrupt the pattern of dispersal movement. Mcs raught in the ramps were placed in captivity after pertinent data was recorded. Live Trapping Live traps wore utilized to catch mice for observation of behavior and to study the characteristics of the populations. Grids were set up and trapped intensively in the above seven enclosures and Caledonia-6 plot. The grids were trapped during periods of from three to five days using Longworth and Sherman live traps. The number of traps used in an enclosure for any one trap period ranged from to 106 depending upon the population density. 3 Traps were baited with feed oats, set in the evening, and checked the following morning. Location of capture, sex, age, reproductive status, weight, notes on wounding, and signs of disease or ectoparasites were recorded for each mouse captured. Mice were marked with numbered monel metal tags and toe clipped (Carter, 1963). 21 Figure b. Runway of a dispersal ramp leaving an enclosure. Note the wood box at the end of the ramp containing a treadle which drops a mouse into the capture bucket. V Figure 5. Top view of a capture bucket, containing a mouse, which has been removed from beneath the treadle box. 22 Determination of Population Characteristics The data gained £rc live trapping was the basis for calculations of population densities, reproductive status, sex and age ratios, ew''vival rates, and the amount of wouxiding. The population statistics were later correlated with behavior statistics. The population density for each population trap period was calculated by the mark and recapture method using the Ltncoln index. The last day for each trap period was considered the: recapture day. Confidence limits were established at the five percent significance level (Davis, 1962). Reproduction for the population8 was based on the percentage of sub-adult and adult females having: (1) a perforate vaginal orifice (a sign of breeding condition), (2) mammary glands large or lactating, and (3) palpable pregnancies. Smith (19614) describes in more detail the criteria for estimating reproductive status. Sex was determined by noting the distance between the anus and penis or clitoris. This distance is greater in males than females. Voles were classed as juveniles, sub-adults, or adults. Their age classes were determined by size (weight)and pelage (1eke and Kinney, l9S6; Hothnann, l98). Minimum survival rates were calculated frcmi enclosure recapture data kept on each vole. These rates show the percentage of voles recaptured for each oóboz't (mice captured for the first time during any particular trap period). Survival rates were calculated only for voles live trapped and excludes voles caught in dispersal zampe 23 or found dead in traps. To obtain a general indication of survival among enclosure populations for 196b, the total percent of surviving voles captured in each month from August through November or December was averaged, and a mean of these monthly averages was calculated for each of the seven enclosures. Tne percentages of voles with scars or wounds were obtained from the trapping data. Since all voles are susceptable to wounding, even though adult males exhibit a greater proportion of the wounds, the percentages of wounds and scars for each population were based on the total mice captured and are presented as an indication of the amount of conflict among voles in each population. Behavior Studies A means to obtain a quantitative measurement of the social interactions was devised and used. The objective was to determine, quantitatively, behavioral patterns that occurred in each population so that they could be compared to the population characteristics already mentioned. Observations of behavior were conducted under laboratory conditions. To measure agonis tic behavior (fighting activities), five, one-quarter inch mesh hardware cloth cages (12 inches wide by 18½ inches long by nine inches high) were used to conduct behavior trials. These chages held two adult males captured from the populations. Figure 6 shows that these cages contained a hardboard partition to create two equal sized compartments, and that a sliding door could 2L Figure 6. The dual compartment cage was divided by a partition containing a hole and a sliding door through which either mouse was able to cross to the opponents side during a trial. 2 be removed to provide an opening between the two compartments. The cages, when in use, always contained alfalfa, potato, and oats. Adult mice were obtained from the enclosures and the field plot for observation in the dual compartment cage. Dual trials involved combinations of non-dispersal and dispersal adult males from the different population phases. Behavior data of all adult males representing a particular population were grouped together to provide an indication as to the agonistic behavior patterns o± that population. Also, data for non-dispersal males and dispersal males were separated for each enclosure population to show possible behavior differences between these two types of voles. After mice were brought into the laboratory for observation, one of the individuals in each trial was fur clipped on the back for identification. The mice were left in their compartments over night so that they would become accustomed to their own territory before they were observed. The trial began when each mouse was provided access to the opponents compartment by the removal of the partition door. All trials lasted ten minutes, and at each ten second. inverval the predominant behavior pattern of each mouse was recorded. at the end of each trial there was an accumulation of' 60 activity patterns for each mouse on trial. Thus, recorded A particular behavior pattern was represented by the number of recorded ten second invervala for that pattern. Fighting time was measured by using a stop watch to obtain the number of seconds of actual fighting. All observations were made from behind a canvas blind during the mid-day hours. The 26 presence of the observer did not seem to disturb the mice. Pertinent behavior activities summed up at the completion of each trial were as follows: 1. The total seconds of fighting 2. The total aggressive and total defensive fighting behavior 3. The total threat and total retreat behavior i. The total exploratory behavior S. The total active and total inactive behavior 6. The total washing arid grooming behavior Active or inactive behavior was indicated when movement or no movement occurred. In a trial where conflict occurred, each mouse was recorded as a dominant or subordinate if possible. This decision was based on avoidance behavior, aggressive approaches, defensive and aggressive Lighting postures, displacement activity patterns, and wounding. The voles were returned to their respective trap station areas after undergoing one trial per day £ or one or two days. Observations were made on solitary voles placed in an arena. It was expected that solitary behavior studies would disclose any variation in behavior and movement patterns that may exist among voles. All sex and age groups of voles were obtained from the dispersal ramps and populations to observe their behavior under solitary triaL ccrjtiona. The arena (36 inches wide, 21 inches deep, and 22 inches high) had three blank plywood walls and a glass front. The floor, covered with wood shavings, contained alfalfa arid sliced potate during each trial. A trial consisted of placing a 27 mouse in the arena, waiting quietly for two minutes, and then observing and recording the activity as in the dual trials, The patterns recorded were exploratory behavior, active behavior, inactiTe behavior, and washing and grocing behavior. After a behavior trial, the mouse, except for a dispersal mouse, was returned to his original point of capture. RESULTS The behavioral and pomlation characteristics for Znclosuree 1 to 7 and the Caledonia-6 plot obtained during the trapping months of 1963 arid l961 are presented here. To aid in comparing enclosures with populations stocked at the same time, Enclosures 1, 2, S, and 7 have been grouped separately from Enclosures 3, b, and 6 in moat of the following tables. Behavioral Characteristics Agonistic Behavior Patterns of the Populations The averages of fighting time, aggressive and defensive fighting, and dominance and subordinance f or the populations having either first year high densities, second year high densities, or third year high densities are compared in Table XI. Fighting time for adult male mice of the first year high density populations, l7. øeconda, was longer than the fighting time for adult male mice of the second and third year high density populations with 10.3 and 9.8 seconds, respectively. The aggressive fighting of mice was similar among the three phase years, but the third year high density populations did shcmr a slight increase of aggressive fighting (2.2) from the previous year's aggressive fighting (l.). Nore mice displayed dominance behavior in l96L than 1963. Dominance was identified in 314 percent of the trials for mice from the third year phase populations. 14, populations phase year second for averages or total over-all The 2 activity indicated displaying trial in interra second ten of number Indicates 1 23 29 314 23 28 23 10.3 9.8 1.9 22 2.3 1.6 76 89 112 128 19614 6d III and Ill 3d, III 19&) 7 II and Sd, II 2d, II ld, II 2 1.7 10.3 1814 129 Ccmbiaed 14, 17 15 21 214 2.14 1.5 1.8 1.5 10.3 72 53 1963 6d II and II 3d, II Subordinance % Dominance % Fighting1 Defensive Trials Number 93 Fighting Aggressive Seconds in Time Fighting 17.5 66 1963 5d I 7 I and Mice Number Patterns Behavior 2d, I Id, I 19614. AND 1963 FOR PHASES YEA.R DENSITI IIIGH TB1BD AND SECOND, FST, THE REPRF3EW1ING POPULATIONS TIlE FOR SUBOROINANCE AND IXINANCE AND FIGHflNG, DEFENSIVE AND AGGRESSIVE TD&E, F]OIftINO OF AVERAGES AND TRIAlS BEHA.VIOR WAL AND MICE TRIAL NUMBER II. TABLE The average dual trial behavior characteristics of adult male mice from Enclosures 1 to 7 from June to October, 1963, are presented in Table III. The fighting time average for I 7 mice (33.5 seconds) was the longest fighting time average for enclosure mice. I 54, II b, and I 24 mice bad fighting time averages of 15.9, 13.7, and 11.7 seconds, respectively. These averages were similar to the over-all, 1963 fighting time average of 1)4.2 seconds. II 64, I 14, and IX 3d mice had the shorter fighting time averages of 10.b, 10.3, and 9.2 seconds, respectively. I Sd and I? mice exhibited more fighting time than the other enclosure mice. 4ice from 17 and II)4 (no dispersal ramps) fought more per trial than mice of their respective year phase. from enclosures with dispersal ramps. I 5d, having a low dispersal rate and reaching a high density, was characteristically similar to II b in behavior patterns. I ld, I 2d, II 3d, and II 6d mice showed less fighting and were more aggressive than defensive in their fighting. II 3d and II 6d mice tended to show less exploratory behavior (10 and 1)4, respectively) than I id and I 2d (2)4 and 17, respectively). Ixploratory behavior for II b, second year non-dispersal population, was 9.5 which was low compared to other enclosures, except II 3d. The average behavior characteristics of adult male mice from Enclosures 1 to 7 and the Caledor.ta-6 plot from June, through December, 196)4, are given in Table IV. The fighting time averages for II Sd (12.6 seconds) and II 7 (11.5 seconds ) were longer than the over-all 1 O 7 FRC JUNE THROUGH OOBER, 1963. Total or I id I 2d I Sd I 7 U 3d II b II 6d Number Mice 3.3 23 16 L1. 22 6 25 119 Number Trials 18 32 21 22 31 9 32 165 Fighting Time' Seconds 2 Aggressive Fighting 10.3 11.7 15.9, 33.5 2.7 2.b .6 b.i 1. .7 Lb 2.6 2.? 1.2 1 .9 2.1 2.? Behavior Patterns Defensive Fighting2 Threats2 1 Retreats2 % Dinance % Subordinance cp1oratory2 Active2 InactIve2 .7 3.b Average lb.2 13.7 10.li. .7 Lb 1.5 2.6 .9 1.6 1.6 1.3 1.3 Lb .6 1.6 1.8 9.2 1 2 22 10 32 19 0 16 20 5 3.9 23 37 13 33 16 19 2b 17 13.5 15 10 lb 15 19 19 20 19 28 22 22 20 5 13 17 12 18 1? lb lb 3 9.5 Washing2 5.b b.3 2.9 b.7 S.b 5.6 1 lb.2 . 3.7 confidence interval at five percent significance 2 Indictes number of ten second intervals in trial displaying indicated activity b.b TABLE IV. AVERAGE CHARA.CTERISTICS OF ADULT MALE MICE FRC ENCLOSURES 1 To 7 AND THE CALEDONIA-6 PLOT FR( JUNE TO DECEM3ER, 196b. Behavior Patterns II id II 2d II 5d II 7 III 3d III 14. III 6d Total or Cal.-6 Average Number Aice 17 19 22 18 21 31 3h 165 17 Number Trials 25 26 32 29 142 143 143 2140 28 12.6 11.5 U .8 1.8 2.5 2.5 2.14. Fighting Timed- 9.5 7 9.L 9.1 '10.0 3.5 2.2 1.7 2.5 2.1 1.1 23 2 1.5 1.14 2 1 1 1 .8 1.3 2.3 1.6 1.2 1.7 Sec ond8 Aggressive Fightin2 2.6 Defensive Fighting2 2 Threats2 1.li. .8 2.1 2 Retreats2 1.5 1.2 1.6 1 Dominance 14.0 Ii. Subordinance 214. Exploratory2 Active2 % Inactive2 Washing2 1 10.0 + 2 .5 1.2 314. 38 33 26 14.2 33 7 38 28 21 31 16 23 26 18 23 20 19 16 20 18 20 20 19 21 21 21 23 20 20 21 21 20 6 13 8 11 11 13 10 11 11 2.8 2.8 3.2 2.5 1.14 2.6 2.5 3.14 1.5 Confidence Interval at five percent significance Indicates number of ten second intervals in trial displaying indicated activity 3.14 33 1961 Lighting time average (10.0 seconds). III 3d had the third , and longest Lighting time average, 11 seconds, then II id, III UI 6d followed with averages of 9.5, 9., and 9.1 seconds, respectively. U 2d and Caledonia-6 plot mice had short fighting time averages of 7.0 and 3.5 seconds, respectively. lU 3d was the only enclosure that exhibited more fighting in 196b than in 1963. Fighting time for III 1 was similar to dispersal ramp enclosures, except II Sd. III 3d, III respectively. In 196b, aggreaavo fighting generally increased for , II Sd, and III ád with 2.2, 1.7, 1.8, and 2.5, Aggressive fighting decreased fox' II 2d (.8) and (2.5) and did not change for U ld (2.6) in 1961i.. Both III ) II 7 and II 7 showed slightly less exploratory behavior (18 end 16, respective- ly) then the five diepeal ramp enclosures * Caledonia-6 plot mice from a low density and showing little fighting time (3.5 seconds), displayed less aggreesve (1.1) and defensive (1.0) fighting than the enclosure averages. These mice exhibited more threat (2.3) behavior in general than mice from the high density enclosures. In rnwmiary, the average fighting time for the first year populations was longer than that of the second year populations and still longer than that of the third year populations. The third year high density populations showed a slight increase of aggressive fighting over the previous year. Dominance behavior was identified more times in 196b populations than in 1963 populations. This suggests that mice of populations in 1961i 3L$ may have established better social relationships within their populations. Mice representing non-dispersal enclosures displayed more fighting behavior than mice from dispersal enclosures of their respective year phase, except in the third year phase. Exploratory behavior for the second and third year non-dispersal populations was less cc*muon than the respective dispersal populations of the same year phase. The low density plot population exhibited lees fighting behavior and more threat behavior than the high density enclosure populations. Non-dispersal Mice Compared to Dispersal Nice The average agonistic behavior characteristics for adult male non-dispersal mice and adult male dispersal mice from I id, I 2d, I 5d, II 3d, and II 6d from June to October, 1963, are given in Table V. The over-all averages of non-dispersal and dispersal trial mice show that fighting time for non-dispersal mice was significantly longer at 13.2 seconds than the average of 8.3 seconds for dispersal mice. The aggressive fighting (2.2) and defensive fighting (1.7) for non-dispersal mice were greater than the aggressive fighting (1.0) and defensive fighting (1.0) for' dispersal mice. The average percent dominance (26) for non- dispersal mice was larger than the nine percent average for dispersal mice, To conclude, the 1963 non-dispersal mice fought more, were more aggressive than defensive in their fighting, and displayed a higher percent of dominance than dispersal mice. TABLE V. AVER&GE AGONISTIC BEHAVIOR CHARACTERISTICS FOR AI1JLT MALE NON-DISPEIAL 5, 3, AND 6 RC1 J1JN THROUGH OCTOBER, 1963. AND DISPERSAL NICE 1RC}I ENCLOSURES L, Behavior Patterns Non-dispersa1 Nice Number Mice Number Trials Fighting Time1 Aggressive Fighting2 Defensive Fighting % Dominance % Subordinance Dispersal Mice Number Mice Number Trials Fighting Time3 Aggressive Fighting2 Deferive Fighting2 %Dominance % Subordinare I 2d I id 6 9 10.1 3.8 0 145 0 - 22 11.8 i7 10.5 1.3 L3 .8 22 11 30 10 II 6d Average or Total 114 12 514 2 214.2 10.14 .6 2.14 16 12.5 2.3 82 13.2 2.2 1.7 26 16 2 18 23 II 3d 12 8 114 9 10 11.7 2.2 7 9 I Sd 14.14 .9 1.14 17 33 26 9 31 13 8 8 9 8 13 16 14.14 5.5 8.14 .14 .5 .8 .6 .1 0 11 1.8 0 25 145 0 19 52 8.3 1 1 9 15 1 13.2 + 5.1 Confidence intervals at five percent significance Indictes number o± ten second intervals in trial displaying indicated activity 2 3 8.3+ 14.3 Confidence interva1 at five percent significance 'It 36 Non-dispersal and dispersal mice of I 14, I 2d, and I 54 varied in their agonistic behavior patterns. Fighting time in I 14 and I 2d far the two types of mice were similar, and I 24 dispersal mice displayed more aggressive fighting than nondispersal mice. fighting. I Sd non-dispersal mice had a very high rate of The non-dispersal mice in II 3d and II 64 were constatent in exhibiting more fighting, aggressiveness, and dominance than dispersal mice. The average agonistic behavior characteristics of adult male non-dispersal mice and adult male dispersal mice from II 14, II 24, U Sd, III 3d, and III 64 fran June to December, 19614, are given in Table VI. The over-all fighting time average for non-dispersal mice (10.7 seconds) was a little higher than the average (9.8 seconds) for dispersal mice. Aggressive fighting (2.1) was similar o defensive fighting (2) for non-dispersal mice. The aggressive fighting (1.9) for dispersal mice was slightly less than defensive fighting (2.1). Dominance (35 percent) was again higher for non- dispersal voles than the dominance (214 percent) for dispersal voles, The over-all 19614 agonistic behavior averages for non-dispersal and dispersal mice show that non-dispersal mice were slightly more aggressive, more dominant, and only fought slightly longer than dispersal mice. II 14, II 24, and II 5d characteristics for the non-dispersal and dispersal mice were inconsistent. II ld and II 24 dispersal mice had higher fighting time averages than non-dispersal mice. TABLE VI. AVERAGE AGONISTIC BEHAVIOR CHARACTERISTICS FOR ADULT MALE NON-DISPERSAL AND DISPERSAL NICE FROM ENCLOSURES 1, 2, 5, 3, AND 6 FROM JUNE THROUGH DECEMBER, 196)4. Behavioral Patterns Non-dispersal Mice Number Mice Number Trials Fighting Time1 Aggressive Fighting2 Defensive Fighting2 % Dominance % Subordinance Dispersal Nice Number Mice Number Trials Fighting Time3 Aggressive Fighting2 Defensive Fighting2 % Dominance % Subordivance 1 2 3 II id II 2d II Sd III 3d III 6d 10 11 13 5.4 18 21 37 24 12.6 1.7 11.5 2 1.9 10.2 2.7 1.7 47 17 6.9 1.9 1.5 35 24 7 8 14.9 3.6 2.9 So 25 .6 2.2 0 38 8 13 11.6 1 2.7 8 38 26 2.5 35 27 38 30 4 6 3 12.7 2.2 2.2 33 33 30 5 7.5 .2 Total or Average 84 123 10.7 2.1 2 27 35 28 10 13 7.3 2.2 32 45 9.8 1.9 .9 2.1 24 29 2.2 0 31 40 15 10.7+2.2 Confidence interval at five percent significance Indicates number of ten second intervals in trial displaying indicated activity 9.8+3.4 Confidence interval at five percent significance Essentially there was little difference in II d between the behavior of non-dispersal mice and dispersal mice, for both had equally high rates of fighting behavior. The fighting time of non-dispersal mice of II Id, II 2d, and II Sd decreased from higher averages in 1963, and fighting time for dispersal mice of II Id and II Sc! increased from lower averages in 1963. The III 3d and III 6d fighting for non-dispersal mice was greater than that for dispersal mice. In summary, the agoniatic behavior averages for 1963 nondispersal and dispersal mice show a greater difference and less variation than the averages for non-dispersal and dispersal mice of 19614. In general for 1963, non-dispersal mice exhibited a stronger tendency for fighting behavior than did dispersal mice. Solitary Behavior Patterns The exploratory, active, inactive, and washing and grooming solitary behavior characteristics for dispersal mice oi I id, I 2d, I Sd, II 3d, and II 6d and for non-dispersal mice from June through October, 1963, are presented in Table VII. Exploratory behavior for dispersal mice from ramps of I id, I 2d, and I Sc! was slightly higher at 38, 141, and 39, respectively, than that for II 3d and II 6d dispersal mice at 31 and 36,, respectively. Mice from the five dJ.spe a? :ap exhibited similar active behavior. Inactivity varied throughout the ramps with Ramp 1 at 0.14 and Ramp 3 at 9.0. Washing behavior was inconsistent * EXPLORATORY, ACTIVE, INACTIVE, AND WASHING AND GROOItENG SOLITARY BEHAVIOR CHARACTERISTICS FOR DISPERSAL 1IOE CAPTURED IN RAKPS 1, 2, 5, 3, AND 6 AND FOR NON-DISPERSAL MICE FROM THESE ENCLOSURES DURING JUNE THROUGH OCTOBER, 1963. TABLE VII. - Behavior Patterns Ramp I 1 Ramp I 2 Ramp I 5 Ramp II 3 Ramp II 6 Average Non-dispersal Exploratozy1 38 hi 39 31 36 36 25 Active1 i) 12 12 13 16 13.7 21 .5 3.1 1.8 9 2.5 8.2 3.6 7.7 7.3 3.9 inactive1 Washing1 Numuer Mice 1 3.0 10 6 16 6 5.9 16 7.3 22 Indicates number or ten second intervals in trial displaying indicated activity '0 The exploratory, active, inactive, and washing and grooming solitary behavior characteristics of dispersal mice of II id, II 2d, II Sd, III 3d, and III 6d from June to December, 196h, are presented in Table VIII. Voles from Ramps 1, 2, and continued to exhibit higher exploratory rates of 1l, hO, and 38, respectively, than Ramps 3 and 6 at rates of 27 and 36, respectively. Exploratory behavior patterns remained similar to the previous year's. Active behavior was similar among the ramps Non-dispersal mice obtained from these five dispersal enclosures in 1963 showed less exploratory (25) and more active (21) behavior than dispersal mice. A high number of inactive (6.0) and washing (7.3) behavior patterns were exhibited by non-dispersal mice. In summary, slightly more exploratory behavior was displayed by voles from Ramps 1, 2, and 5 in 1963 and 196h than in Ramps .3 and 6. Solitary behavior patterns for dispersal mice of II Id, II 2d, II Sd, and III 6d showed little change in 196h. The exploratory behavior patterns for dispersal mice showed no apparent relationship to population phases. Non-dispersal mice showed less exploratory behavior and more active behavior than dispersal mice. Population Characteristics Trapping Periods The populations were not trapped at the seine time, and to assist in analysis the trapping periods are given designations. TABLE VIII. EXPLORATOaT, ACTIVE, INACTIVE, AND WASHING AND GROOMING SOLITARY BEHAVIOR CHARACTERISTICS OF DISPERSAL MICE FROM RAMPS 1, 2, 5, 3, AND 6 THROUGH JUNE TO DECEMBER, 196)4, Behavior Patterns Ramp II 1 Ramp II 2 Ramp II 5 Ramp III 3 RampIII 6 Average Exploratory' lii hO 38 27 36 38 Active1 1)4 13 15 17 1)4 1)4 Inactive1 2.3 .8 2.2 9.2 )4.3 ashiQ- 2.8 6.2 b.8 6.8 5.8 liumber Nice 3. 18 12 11 6 12 Indicates, number of ten second interva1s in trial displaying indicated activity 3 Trapping periods for I id, I 24, and II 3d studied from June, 1963, through January, 19614, are shown in Table IL. In 1963, the trapping periods for I 5d and I 7 were similar to I Id and I 24, and trapping periods for II 14 and II 64 were similar to II 3d. Smith (19614) has presented the exact dates for II 14, I Sd, II 64, and In the second year of study the seven enclosures and the I 7. Caledonia-6 plot were trapped approximately once a month from I4ay to cernber, 1614. The trap months and dates are presented in Tables X and XI for II 14, II 2d, II d, and II 7, and for III 3d, III 14, III 64, and Caledonia-6 plot, respectively. Population Densities The total number of Microtus captured, the population estimate with two standard errors, the density per acre, and the number dead in the traps for each trap period of the 1963 and 19614 populations are given in Table XII and Table XIII, respectively. The comparison of populations are mainly based upon the population estimates. Population eat1inates for the seven enclosures in 1963 increased gradually from June to October and reached peak densities in November or December. 1 ld, I 2d, I Sd, and I 7 produced peak population estimates of 199, 230, 3145, and 1409 mice, respectively. The pop- ulations of II 3d, II 14, and II 64 reached peak estimates of 229, 269, and 295 mice, respectively. The average of peak population estimates for I id, I 24, I 54, and I 7 was 295, and the same average TABLE IX. TRAP PERIOD DESIGNATIONS FOR I Id, I 2d, AND II 3d FROM JUNE, 1963, THROUGH JANUARY, 196L Designation lid First Year High DensityI2d Second Year High Density 113d June June 25-29 June 25-29 June J-6 July July July 15-18 July 9-12 August 13-17 August 6-9 August - August I II August 13-17 August 27-29 September - 1 September - October I - 15-18 II September L-6 September )-6 September 23-27 September 23-27 October 15-17, 19 October 15-1?, 19 October - II September 17-21 October 9-12 October 29-31 November 2 November November 5, 7, 16 November December December 18-21 December 18-21 January 5, 7, 16 November 20-23 January 11v17 TABLE L TRAP PERIQD DESIJNATION FOR II 1d Designation II id May July Augzst September October II 2d, II Sd, AND II 7 FROM M1Y THROUGH DEOEM]3EF, 1961. Second Year High Density II 2d II Sd Nay 26-29 Nay 26-29 May 19-22 May 19-22 July 2i-2L. July 21-2Li. June 30 -July 3 July 17-18 Mgu.st 17-20 August 17-20 August i-7 August U-i! September 22-25 September 29-October 2 September 1-14 September 15-18 October 27-30 October 27-30 October 7-10 October 20-23 November 17-20 November 23-26 November December II 7 December 8-il December 8-11 TABLE XL TRAP PERIOD DESIGNATION FOR III 3d, III 14, III 6d, AND ThE CALEDONIA-6 PLOT FR NAT THROUGH DECEMBER, 19614. Designation III 3d Third Year High Density III 14 III 6d Low Density Galedonia-6 May Nay 26-29 May 19-22 Nay 19-22 July July 21-2 June 30 -July 3 July iS-18 July 1S-18 August August 17-20 August 14-7 August U-114 August 11-114 September September 29 -October 2 September 1-14 September 1-l8 September 22-2 October October 27-30 October 7-10 October 20-23 October 20-23 November 17-20 November 23-26 November 2L-26 December December 8-11 TABLE XII. 136 TOTAL NICE CAPTURED, POPULATION ESTIMATE, DENSITY PER ACRE, AND THE NUMBER DEAD LN TRAPS FOR ENCLOSURES 1, 2, 5, Trap Period Designation 7, 3, 13, AND 6 FROM JUNE, 1963, TO JANUARY, 19613. Total Captured Population Density Estimate Per Acre Dead in Traps I id Jme July 113. August 313 September-I September-Il October 9 November 53 92 107 136 December 9+1* 36 1 152 395 60 0 156 1 92+12 260 368 0 0 658 15l19 6013 0 19925 796 119 1131+18 5613 0 1 July 8 21 8+0 32 0 August 2l'3 84 1 36t 36j-5 1134 135 557 220 372 I 2d Jrne September-I September-lI October 83 November 130 169 December 92 93+12 156+20 230+29 9212 13 3 0 5 624 920 368 2 5 5 15d June July August September-I September-Il October November December January Li. June 13+13 153 52 113 2f$ 27+6 25 265 519 60 106 104 137 68 94 8i1812229 0 0 0 201j. 324 1386 . 0 0 0 3., 1 0 255 3135+54 34 137+22 5 5+0 190 20 76 39+7 156 0 3 1,380 186 0 July 19 August September-I 38 132 50113 200 0 September-lI October November December J 63 711S 9851 i85Th1 2813 2 76 134 2136 1309813 136 8252 392 2 1130 5 1,636 326 0 0 b7 TABLE XLI CONTINUE, Trap Period Designation Total Captured Population Density Estimate Per Acre Dead in. Traps II 3d Juno July 8 8+0 35 bOS 32 160 0 2 2 0 August-I August-Il 69 7b 8311 8110 332 September October-I October-lI November January 116 153 13317 202 22929 20426 532 76b 916 816 b28 1 0 173 88 191+21i. l07114 32h S 1 2 'lb July August September-I September-Il October November December January II 9 27 9+0 30'8 36 397 ho 57 hh+9 71 205 62+10 81+17 269Th3 59 618 36 0 120 156 176 0 0 218 2 1 32L1. 1,076 276 1 28 0 3 6 irluLy August September-I September-Il October 7 29 70 3717 h9 ib.8 2 0 Novsiber 1 80 117 123+17 165 December 19527 180 36b h96 780 210 83 295'53 1,180 0 b60 0 January * To standard errors 9118 1135 3 10 2 48 TABLE XIII. TOTAL ?aCE CAPTURED, POPULATION ESTIMkTE, DEflBITY PER ACRE, AND ThE NUMBER DEAD FOR ENCLOSURES 1 THROUGH 7 AND FOR CALEDONIA-6 PLOT FROM NAY THROUGH DECEMBER, 1964. IN 9S Trap Period Designation Tota]. Captured Population Density Per Acre Estimate Dead in Traps II ld May July August September October December II 48 111 111 80 68 70 6422* 13630 256 20839 834 103+28 82+21 544 1413 8622 329 345 0 0 0 6 0 0 2d 3012 1.0 0 102+24 122+24 407 488 10 73 65 98+18 426 392 92+17 367 C) 24 25+2 100 0 August September October 69 8221 327 S 147 121 886 11 760 0 90 22244 190+42 10722 427 2 November 47 6020 238 1 80 0 May July August September October December 24 88 101 84 10723 0 0 0 II 5d May July II 7 May July August September October November 18 117 153 119 83 56 20+5 15938 21249 161+30 9215 5810 638 S 848 646 368 230 22 0 0 0 TABLE XIII CONTINUED. Trap Period Designation Total Captured Population Density Estimate Per Acre Dead in Traps III May July August September October 19 59 65 71 i;i 6J!l2 76 0 2h1.. 2 70114, 280 2 87+2b 350 332 1 0 520 2 68 69 83 83+20 130+26 May 16 17+)4 July 63 136 118 December III August September October November 73 59 812. 325 0 0 2290 15123 915 13 603 Ii. 3 3 369 3 72 0 0 13 96+32 92+20 III 6d May 17 July August 106 123 September October November 103 80 71 18+L 1520 608 1h93!. 13+38 596 571j. 1 95+21 379 5820 230 1 0 12+18 60 L0 0 140 1 Gale donia -6 July 9 August September October 6 7 13 November 18 * Two standard errors 8+8 8+i 21+10 26+16 0 1014. 2 130 0 for II 3d, II lj, and II 6d was 26ii. Smith (196i) found that the peak density of I? (E 7) was significantly higher than that of II (E !t). For first year populations, peak densities were generally lower in enclosures with dispersal ramps than in I 7 which had none. In 19614, populations in II id, II 2d, II d, and II 7 rapidly increased in numbers from May through August when peak estimate8 reached 208, 122, 222, and 212 mice, respectively. The population estimates of these four enclosures gradually declined for the three trap periods following August. More variation existed in the peak density periods of III 3d, III 14, and III 6d. III 3d had a high population estimate of 130 mice in December while III 14 climbed to a high of 229 mice in August, and III 6d. reached a peak in July at 1S2 mice. The average of peak population estimates for II id, II 2d, II Sd, and II 7 was 191, and the same average for III 3d, III 14, and III 6d was 170. lU 3d and III 6d had lower densities than III i Caledonia-6 plot continually produced low population estimates ranging from 8 to 26 (140 to 130 per acre) with the high in November. In swmiary, high population estimates, in 1963, were reached by November or December in Enclosures I to 7. 1 ld, I 2d, I Sd, and I 7, in their first year of high densities, contained higher average densities than the populations in II 3d, II 14, and II 6d in their second year of high densities. The peak density of I 7 was significantly higher than that of II 14. Peak densities for 19614 5]- populations, except III 3d, were reached mainly in August, and afterwards numbers declined through late summer and fall. III 3d, III b, and III 64, in their third year of high densities, produced a lower average peak density and more variation in peak density periods than II id, II 24, II Sd, and IX 7 in their second year of high densities. Peak densities were generally lower where dispersion was allowed than where dispersion was not allowed, except in I]: Sd and Ii 64. Sex and Age Composition The sex and age composition for I 14, I 2d, and II 3d are presented in Table XIV. Sex and age data for II b, I Sd, II 64, and I 7 (E 1, E 5, E 6, and E 7) has been presented by Smith (1961i). Males were more numerous in II 1 and I 7 until October and September. In I 14, I 24, II 3d, I 54, and II 64, containing dispersal ramps, females were more numerous until November or December when males become more numerous. Higher percentages of adult mice, 75, 77, 70, and 73, were present in 1 24, II b, II 64, and I?, respectively, than in I id, II 3d, and I 54 'with )7, 65, and 62 percent, respectively. The percentage of adults, number of males and females, their means, and the sex ratios for Enclosures 1 to 7 and for the Ca].edonia6 plot from May through December, l96b, are shown in Table XV. II 24 and III 64 had higher numbers of females than males compared to the other five enclosures. The average percent of adults in the TkBLE XIV. 52 PERCENTAGE OF ADULTS, NUMBER OF MALES AND FEMALES, THEIR MEANS, AND THE SE.X RATIOS FOR I id, I 2d, AND II 3d, FROM JUNE, 1963, TO JANUARY, 196!. Trap Period Designation Percent Adults Number Males Number Females Sex Ratio lid June July August September-i September-Il October November December Mean 22 50 65 66 7S 62 53 6 14 5 11, 7 20 22 36 31 56 50 68 72 Li 7 7 66 147 80:100 100:100 70:100 71:100 614:100 88:100 103:100 153:100 36 I?d Juno July August September-I September-Il October November December Mean Ii 3ô June July August-I August-lI September October-I October-lI November January Mean 5 11 75 95 72 80 72 6 10 15 20 32 614 72 51 76 97 77 148 1414 7 31 1l 3 514 21 25 63 2 6 6: 13 35 33 22 58 59 51 63 53 77 98 65 149 71 95 85 314 67 79 107 88 1414 147 514 60:100 91:100 71:100 80:100 63:100 71:100 714:100 108:100 33:100 59:100 103:100 80:100 73:100 914:100 89:100 96:100 100:100 53 TABLE XV. PERCENTAGE OF ADULTS, NU)ER OF MALES AND FEMALES, THEIR MEANS, AND THE SEX RATIOS FOR ENCLOSuRES 1 TO 7 AND FOR CALEDONLt-6 PLOT FROM MAX TO DECEMBER, 19614. Trap Period Designation II id Ma July August September Percent Adults Number Males Number Females Sex Ratio 85:100 72:100 122:100 67 73 22 26 146 614 149 61 Ii). 147 50 33 82 98 39 December Mean 68 142 May July August September 75 69 16 8 32 56 148 147 514. 32 39 14$ October 614 1 32 Deceiiber 95 Mean 614 29 31i 35 Hay July August 87 75 8 16 35 85 314 1.41 September October 28 52 61 39 60 137:100 102:100 November Mean 83 61 Si 20 77:100 135:100 142 May July August September October November Mean 61 67 8 10 1414 60 93 73 60 80:100 60:100 155:100 145 614 1iS 1142:100 72 143 14.0 100 67 28 28 October 32 J2 1142:100 122:100 109:100 39 II 2d 27 147 62 1 200:100 57:100 87:100 87:100 128:100 80:100 50:100 103:100 106:100 100:100 514. TABLE XV CONTINUED. Trap Period Designation Percent Adults Number Males Number Females Sex Ratio III 3d May July August September October December Mean 100 66 38 72 9 26 38 140 714 36 51 33 914 6 714. 31 75 91 10 33 37 31 33 32 90:100 79:100 103:100 129:100 109:100 159:100 29 III 14 May July August September October November Mean 59 131 56 80 67 80 78 ).7 33 146 10 32 56 52 37 26 35 60:100 97:100 1143:100 150:100 127:100 127:100 ILL 6d May July August September October November Mean Caledonia-6 JUly August September October November Mean 76 75 60 36 59 91 66 78 16 100 69 89 70 60 11 60 63 149 514 33 35 38 147 6 146 6 2 14 3 5.6 76:100 95:100 91:100 70:100 97:100 15 7 2 7 8 514:100 14 6 10 5.0 350:100 50:100 133:100 116:100 80:100 55 enclosures, except III 3d, ranged frcei 61 to 68. The average percent of adults in III 3d was slightly higher at Th. The Oaledonia-6 plot popx1ation averaged 70 percent adults and showed considerable variation in the sex ratio. In summary, a larger number of females were present until November or December in the 1963 populations having a means to disperse. I 2d, II l, II 6d, and I 7 had higher percentages of adults than I id, II 3d, and I 5d. Males were slightly more numerous in the populations, except II 2d and III 64, for 196b. III d had slightly higher peràentages of adults than other 196I populations. Reproduction The number of sub-adult and adult females and percentages of these females perforate, pregnant, or with mammary glands large or lactating, and their means for Jnclosures 1 through 7 frc July to late November or December, 1963, are presented in Table XVI. The enclosure populations exhibited good reproductive rates through October. Scrotal malee were abundant throughout the enclosures frcm June to November. 1 id, I 2d, and II 3d showed the lower mean percentages forfemales perforate, 37, 38, and 36percent, respectively. The mean percentages of females pregnant for I id and II 3d, 21 and 16 percent, respectively, were similar to their corresponding mean percentages of females with large or lactating manunary glands, 20 and 17 percent, respectively. This suggests TABLE XVI. 56 NUMBER OF SUB-ADULT AND ADULT FEMALES AND PERCENTAGES OF THESE FEMALES PEPORATE, PREGNANT, OR WITH MAMMARY GLANDS LARGE OR LACTATING, AND THEIR MEANS FOR ENCLOSURES 1 THROUGH 7 FROM JULY TO NOVEMBER OR DECEMBER, 1963. Trap Period Designation Niber Females Females Perforate Females Pre$nant }famia Glands Large or Latating I id July August September-I September-Il October November December Mean Percent I 2d July August September-I September-Il October November December Mean Percent 7 29 19 57 28 67 51 61 hi 15 7 11 19 22 b9 71 92 29 21 35 31 25 6 2 29 16 11 31 11 0 20 37 21 6b b7 50 53 b6 36 21 31 20 20 0 0 13 18 19 18 lb 25 35 21 21 27 0 8 2 8 36 5 9 0 15d 8 August September-I September-lI October November December Mean Percent lb 16 33 b6 62 127 65 50 68 57 Sb 2 0 12 12 13 10 1 1 b7 19 ho 67 60 17 93 60 hI 25 6 8 20 13 22 8 18 l.7 LfJ 17 July August September-I September-Il October November December Mean Percent 5 12 23 3b U6 79 118 0 20 0 0 11 2 l0b 57 TABLE XVI CONTINUED. Trap Period Number Designation Females II 3d July August-I August-Il September October-I October-Il *November Mean Percent 21 32 37 51 75 99 88 Females Perforate 67 Females Pregnant 1)4 13 Nanmary Glands Large or Lact.ting 33 19 16 5)4 )40 L.9 35 2)4 21 7 8 0 0 16 19 6 6 36 1 17 II 1 July Aagust September-1 September-Il October November December 17 67 88 65 16 9)4 27 7)4 )43 7)4 88 17 69 0 30 0 13.5 5 80 75 80 87 60 29 0 31 32 33 20 20 8 4ean Percent II 6d July August September-.I September-Il October November December Mean Percent * Late November 33 13 0 33 38 30 50 29 28 3 16 25 39 55 83 9)4 7 60 6 0 17 13 22 1)4 19 0 13 29 12 7 1)4 58 that the reproduction that did occur within these two populations, I id and. II 3d, was successful. Smith (196ii) found that II h CE b) had better over-all reproductive rates than II 6d (E 6), I Sd (E 5), and I 7 CE 7). II 6d (E 6) showed slightly higher reproduction than I Sd (E 5) and I 7 CE 7) (Smith, 19614). II 14 The data of Table XVI indicates that and II 6d had higher mean percentages for perforate females, 68 and 60 percents, respectively, than did I 5d and I 7, both at II 147 percent. 14 females had the highest mean pregnancy rate (30 percent) for the enclosures. The mean percentages for pregnant females for I Sd, I 6d, arid I 7 were similar at 19, 18, and 17 percent, respectively. I Sd and, especially, II 14 mean percentages for females with large or lactating mamary glands, 6.8 and 13.5 percent, respectively, were lower than their respective mean percentages of females pregnant. This suggests that prenatal mortality was higher for these two enclosures than the other enclosures. Smith (19614) showed that prenatal mortality was more cmnon for II 14 (E 14) than I 5d, I 7, and II 6d (E 5, E 7, and E 6, respectivoly). The nwaber of sub-adult and adult females and percentages of these females perforate, pregnant, or with mammary glands larger or lactating, and their means for Enclosures 1 through 7 and the Caledonia-6 plot from May to December, 19614, are presented in Table XVII. The over-all reproductive performance for 19614 was 59 NUNBER. OF SUB-ADULT AND ADULT FEMALES AND PERCENTAGES OF THESE FEMALES PERFORATE, PREGNANT, OR WITH MA1MARI GLANDS LARGE OR LACTATING, AND THEIR MEANS FOR ENCLOSURES 1 T}LWUGH 7 AND THE CLAEDONIA-6 PLOT FROM NAY TO DECEMBER, 196b. TABLE XVII. Trap Period Designation II id May July August September October December Mean Percent Number Females 21 60 b5 33 32 32 Females Perforate 72 20 33 18 50 l Li.O Females Pregnant % Manunary Glands Large or Lactating 10 2b b8 0 0 0 0 0 2.2 3 3 0 12 II 2d May July August September October December Mean Percent 7 Sb b14 32 36 71 35 36 18 25 17 0 6 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 2 .3 II Sd May July 15 August September October November Mean Percent Sb 53 29 51 20 60 7b bo 20 27 b7 38 2 o 0 0 0 0 0 0 38 8 14 67 62 50 31 30 17 15 0 33 0 0 0 0 25 33 2 II 7 May July August September October November Naan Percent 6 68 52 45 40 28 lb 21 2 0 0 9 TABLE XVII CONTINUED. Trap Period Designation III 3d May July August September October December Mean Percent Number Females 10 32 314. 30 33 32 Females Perforate Females Pregnant 80 70 147 114 56 57 27 6 3 0 0 0 15 Nainary Glands Large or Lactating 50 28 12 13 27 22 II14 May July 10 August September October November Mean Percent 51 20 86 39 148 1414 28 10 39 0 20 29 25 8 0 0 0 140 8 10 90 SLi. 69 So 18 53 52 147 142 50 23 0 0 147 314. 2 14 36 16 0 12 13 37 26 143 2 3 0 13 III 6d Hay July August September October November Mean Percent Caledonia-6 July August September October Novembor Mean Percent 50 2 14 3 5 10 50 75 33 140 20 0 0 0 0 0 0 7 0 0 0 0 33 10 8.6 L 61 lower than that in 1963.. Scrotal males were numerous in all enclosures from May through October. Although poorer reproduction was evident, the percentages for females perforate indicated that the potential for reproduction exi.sted, The enclosures showed slight differences in reproductive percentages. the lowest reproductive rates II 2d exhibited Table XVII indicates that III 3d and III 6d had slightly better roproductioi than the other enclosures. III 3d and III 6d populations displayed the higher percentages of perforated females, l6 and O percent, respectively, and the higher percentages for pregnant females, ih and 12 percent, respectively. The occurrence of females with mammary glands large or lactating indicated that pregnancies were generally successful for the l961.s. enclosures. The Caledonia-6 plot continually possessed females in perforate condition, but pregnant females wore not found, arid only two females possessed large or lactating mammary glands. Scrotal males were caught on thts plot throughout the study year. In summary, voles in breeding condition were captured throughout th trapping periods for the 1963 and l96LL enclosures. success was good in I 14 and I 2d. than II 64, I d, and I 7. than that o± I Sd and I 7. Reproductive II 1 showed better reproduction II 64 reoduction was slightly better A high rate of prenatal mortality has been suggested to have occurred in the II 14 population. reproduction for the enclosures declined in August. During 19614, III 3d and III 64 showed slightly higher reproductive rates than other 19614 enclosures, and II 2d exhibited the lowest reproductive rates. No pregnant females were captured on the Qaledonia-6 plot. General Survival Survival rates of Mierotus captured in I ld, I 2d and II 3d frcei June through December, 1963, are given in Table XVIII. Smith (l961) has analyzed the survival rates of II i, I and I 7 (E 1, E S, E 6, and E 7) for 1963. d, II 6d, 1 ici had poorer survival through October than I 2d and II 3d, but the November and December cohorts among these enclosures were similar. better over-all survival than I d and I 7. II !. and II 6d had Survival data relationships between II 3d and first year populations were inconsistent. For January, II 3d had higher percent survival rates than I Sd or 17. Through November, I 5d and I 7 had better survival than II 3d, except May and June cohorts in I 7. Juveniles had generally good survival In all enclosures through August. Adults and sub-adults survived best frc*u August to December. Survival rates for the different age classes for each cohort of the seven enclosures frc*u May to December, 196L, are presented in Tables XXX through XXV. Survival was good in all enclosures, except II 2d, through August, but 8urvival rates declined from September through November or December. The means of the averages for the total percent surviving voles captured in August through November or December were calculated for each enclosure. The means, in descending TABLE XVIII. SURVI'TAL RATES OF ADULTS, SUB-ADULTS, AND JUVENILES COMBINED FOR I Id, I 2d, AND II 3d FROM JUNE THROUGH DECEMBER, 1963. Month No. % I ld June July August September-I September-Il October November 7100 July No. % 343 8 24 34 49 69 66 100 100 100 100 100 100 6 13 23 25 47 67 71 100 100 100 100 100 100 100 6 100 8 27 100 100 100 100 8 August,i Spt. I No. % No. % 229 0 6 2 0 25 10 142 75 Sept. 112 October No. Na. November December4 No. No. % 0 0 0 0 25 33 76 2 1 25 0 2 0 25 0 2 8 14 1 18 6 15 46 4 16 0 6 26 20 Lu 31 67 12d June July Aueust September-I September-il October November 6 8 100 62 3 6 12 50 46 52 2 33 2 6 1+6 18 48 72 5 ii 17 U 33 33 38 48 68 70 2 14 8 15 28 46 33 31 35 60 60 69 1 36 62 81 48 35 100 4 15 50 56 3 15 27 100 100 100 August-Il for II 3d, 2 20 17 29 26 0 0 0 0 13 3 2 8 6 9 13 13 34 24 113d June July August September October-I October-11 November 2 3 0 0 0 6 6 0 October-I for II 3d, 38 52 75 2 25 2 13 48 56 68 10 17 37 60 20 42 25 37 47 60 714 October-lI for II 3d, 14 2 25 1 9 12 32 43 23 3,3 S 33 5 52 53 47 17 13 19 14 22 27 27 17 5 35 14 January for II 3d TABLE XIX. S1J±VIV1UJ RATES OF ADULTS, SUB-ADULTS, MID JUVENILES IN II id FROM HAT TO DECJ1BER, 1964. July Month Age Adult Sub-adult Juvenile Total July Adult Sub-adult Juvenile Total August Adult Sub-adult Juvenile Total September Adult Sub-adult Juvenile Total October Adult Sub-adult Juvenile Total No. % No. % 17 100 100 100 100 10 59 75 60 61 4 5 26 38 14 7 9 6 20 6 32 100 100 100 100. 141 17 10 0 27 100 100 0 100 0 16 100 100 100 100 100 100 0 100 9 32 3 3 Auguat September October No. No. No. 2 2 5 12 So 0 19 14. 12 50 0 15 7 2 1 18 14 14 6 16 14 10 17 1 1 8 0 4 1 5 0 20 0 0 2 17 16 1 4 10 17 12 2 22 Iii 3 18 2 50 2 5 0 19 0 12 75 40 46 2 32 6 60 43 10 26 3 2 28 2 31 52 iS 21 28 25 7 3 2 C) 1 2 17 30 33 9 28 6 Decenfoer No. % 0 2 17 53 1 0 19 0 46 0 16 8 3 0 11 7 13 22 44 0 39 47 30 0 41 Heari of average monthly survival - 30.5 0 TABLE XX. SURVIVAL RATES OF ADULTS, SUB-ADULTS, AND JUVENILES IN II 2d FROM MAY TO DECEMBER, 19614. July Month Age No. Adult Sub-adult Juveiile Total 16 3 1 20 100 100 100 100 August September % October % December No. % No. 7 14J 5 0 31 0 0 1 100 30 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 26 70 15 140 10 27 6 2 2 1 33 20 58 0 13 0 30 0 13 0 9 1 214 140 0 0 12 21 10 17 0 1 0 100 8 140 6 No. No. 0 No. 0 0 0 0 % 0 0 0 0 July 37 Sub-adult Juvenile Total 15 5 57 August Adult Sub-adult Juvenile 7 27 22 Tota]. 56 September Adult Sub-adult Juvanile Total October Adult Sub-adult Juvenile Total Mean of av 6 143 1 50 9 11 0 20 age 100 lOO 100 100 100 100 100 100 100 100 100 100 100 100 0 100 7ionthly survival - 31,3 17 7 2 28 2 28 1 114 iS 5 55 ?3 10 5 37 23 3 2 11 9 22 39 27 14.8 6 ii 1 17 2 31 33 72 0 0 33 66 18 0 19 6 5 0 11 142 0 38 66 145 0 55 TABLE XXI. SURVIVAL RATES OF ADULTS, SUB-ADULTS, AND JIWENILEFJ IN III 3d FROM MAY TO DECEMBER, i96I. July August No. % September No. % October No % December No % Month Age No. % No. % Adult Sub-adult Juvenile Total 19 0 100 0 1 79 0 10 0 0 8 0 0 100 0 0 15 79 8 July Adult Sub-adult Juvenile Total 23 10 6 39 100 100 100 100 3 20 12 35 100 100 100 100 12 2!. 100 100 100 100 Subadult 16 12 100 100 Juvenile Total 2 100 6 1 30 100 13 August Adult Sub-adult Juvenile Total September Adult Sub-adult Juvenile Total October Adult 9 3 O Mean average monthly survival - 39.3 12 8 2 22 !.2 0 0 Li.2 52 80 33 56 8 0 0 !i2 8 !.2 7 6 30 60 33 38 2 15 0 0 7 66 80 58 25 71 2 16 6 0 0 6 31 0 0 31 1 0 0 1 2 9 1 5 50 33 2 2 23 5 0 50 58 L8 0 2 9 0 10 7 17 3 25 ! !i.h 3 10 100 12 6 L 10 3 1 1 5 6 S 0 0 5 ! 20 33 13 0 30 33 28 25 11 33 21 37 50 50 L3 TABLE XXII. SUThIIVAL RATES OF ADULTS, SUB-ADULTS, AND JUVENILES IN III L. FROM MAY TO DEGERNBER, 19614. August July Month Age Adult Sub-adult Juvenile Total July Adult Sub-adult Juvenile Total August Adult Sub-adult Juvenile Total September Jduit Sub-adult Juvenile Total October Adult Suii-adult Juvenile Total Moan o No. 15 1 0 16 30 8 7 145 33 21 16 70 16 142 17 75 10 30 0 140 % No. % 100 100 0 100 12 80 100 0 8 1 81 9 1 0 13 100 100 100 100 100 100 100 100 100 100 100 100 100 100 0 100 September -__i4o. 0 17 14 14 25 October No % November No. 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 143 5 2 1 1 17 12 14 50 28 114. 0 0 13 0 0 19 142 7 15 14 9 10 14 12 2 6 114 14 19 25 3 10 30 38 62 2 28 14.0 12 17 7 12 10 1 6 20 53 100 0 56 3 1 0 14 100 0 25 57 50 57 55 13 14 8 14 8 50 12 28 3 23 18 31 14. 9 2 12 7 9 14 3.4.0 II 0 15 37 O 38 average moth1y survival - 30.0 0' TABLE XXIII. Month Age Adult su:b-adult Juvenile Total July Adult Sub-adult Juvenile Total August Adult Sub-adult Juvenile Total September Adult Sub-adult Juvenile Total SURVIVAL OF ADULTS, SUB-ADULTS, AND JUVENILES IN II Sd FROM XIC( TO DECEMBER, 19614. No. No. 114 1 3 18 30 7 5 142 214 39 36 99 114 148 i14 76 100 100 100 100 $ 9 614 0 0 2 66 61 11 100 100 100 100 100 100 100 100 100 100 100 100 August No. % September October No. No. November No. % 5 0 0 0 0 2 7 1 3 1)4 0 0 0 140 1 1 21 Li. 1 2 14 6 0 0 6 143 1 7 1 7 0 0 0 0 1 0 0 1 0 0 33 0 0 5 114 147 5 17 3 143 28 3 60 2 2 20 148 9 20 9 6 25 2 8 :L 22 56 17 L5 147 13 12 33 33 3 6 LiS 27 27 10 3 21 17 5 35 36 25 33 0 U 3 114 0 0 0 0 0 8 17 10 0 23 21 18 October 6 Sub-adult Juvenile Total 214 0 30 Mean oZ average monthly survival 0 17 29 0 8 27 1 7 100 100 0 100 214.5 TABLE XXIV. SURVIVAL RATES OF ADULTS, SUB-ADULTS, AND JUVENILES IN III 6d FROM Mi1 TO DECEMBER, 1961.. July Noth Age Adult Sub-adult Juvenile Total July Adult Sub-adult Juvenile Total August Adult Sub-adult Juvenile Total September Adult Sub-adult Juvenile Total October Adult Sub-adult Juvenile Total No. % 10 l! 100 100 100 100 51 10 18 79 100 100 100 100 13 17 25 55 100 100 100 100 3 100 100 100 100 2 2 14.6 6 55 8 15 2 25 August No. % No. 8 1 80 6 0 100 1 2 9 2 11 7 100 100 100 100 Mean of average monthly survival - 35.5 Septenther No. % November No. % 60 50 100 1 1 0 10 1 0 0 0 0 10 0 0 0 0 10 0 0 61 2 1Li 1 7 1 7 13 1 6 1 2 25 10 11 2)4. 35 30 17 30 16 20 12 10 11 11 5 38 2 15 7 ]. iS 60 27 Li.9 32 63 18 L. Lt.O 6 33 53 3 3 14.2 October % No. 2 9 8 29 1h 1 5 9 33 15 27 0 30 66 33 29 11 18 1 0 0 0 22 138 114. 13 66 13 26 137 18 2 36 0 25 60 0 11 1414. 9 TABLE XXV. URVIVlth BATE3 OF ADULTS, SUB-ADULTS, AND JUVENILES IN II 7 FROM MAX TO DECEMBER, i96t. Month Age No. Adult Sub-adult Juvenile Total July Adult Sub-adult Juvenile Total 9 0 )4 13 52 32 7 91 August Adult Sub-adult Juvenile Total 35 33 19 September Adult Sub-adult Juvenile Total 56 October Adult Sub-adult Juvenile Total 87 9 0 65 I 8 0 12 % 100 0 100 100 July August No. No. 8 89 3 0 0 100 92 0 ! 12 100 100 100 100 100 100 100 100 100 100 0 100 100 100 0 100 Mean of average monthly survivaE - 31.5 2 5 31 16 b 51 September % No. October % No. 0 0 0 0 1 1 25 8 16 9 2 30 36 28 28 33 30 25 28 28 12 3!i. 7 51 21 38 11 33 0 50 38 1 11 0 1 0 25 2 15 60 Sb 57 56 19 17 1 33 8 2 26 It 22 5 31 0 36 November No. 0 0 0 0 % 0 0 0 0 12 23 5 16 0 17 0 19 11 15 20 33 21 25 1 55 55 0 55 15 0 18 33 27 0 28 1 5 0 6 62 0 50 14. 5 It 3 21 15 25 71 order, for III 3d, III 6d, II 7, II 2d, II id, III 1, and II Sd were III 3d 39.3, 3.3, 31.5, 31.3, 30.5, 30.0, and 2b.5, respectively. and III 6d had better survival throughout the study year. Survival for II 7, II 2d, II id, and III b was similar to one another, not as good as III 3d and III 6d, but better than the low rate for II Sd. III 1. had very good survival through August but showed poor rates afterwards. Juvenile survival was better in III 3d and III 6d. In general, survival of May and July juvenile cohorts rapidly declined in all enclosures after August. Adult survival rates dropped rapidly in August and/or September but afterwards leveled to a more gradual decline. In swrmiary, II 1 rates for 1963. and II 6d had the better over-all survival In 19614, juvenile survival dropped in August, and adult survival also declined sharply in August and September. III 3d and III 6d showed better over-all survival than II 7, II 2d, II id, and III 14, and II 5d had the lowest survival rates following August. Dispersal Rinpe The total mice caught, captures per ramp day, population estimate, and percent soars and wounds of voles captured in the dispersal ramps from July 2, 1963, to November 15, 1963, are given in Table XXVI. As the monthly population estimates increased, captures 72 TABLE XXVI. TOTAL MICE CAUGHT, CAPTURES PER RAMP DAY, POPTPATION ESTIMATE, MW PERCENT SCARS AND WOUNDS OF MICE CAPTURED IN THE DISPERSAL RAMPS FROM JULY 2, 1963, TO NOVBER 15, 1963. Total Caught Ramp I 1 July August September October November Total Average Ramp 1 2 July August September October November Total Average 3 6 12 15 20 Captures Per &mp Day .21 .29 .67 1.07 6.67 Population Estimate 15 39 92 151 199 % Wounds and Scars 0 17 25 20 25 56 .80 1 11 17 9 26 .07 .52 .50 21 36 93 1.86 156 7 2.33 230 0 9 22 19 L3 19 .77 Ramp 15 July August September October November Total verage Ramp II 3 July August September October November Total Average Ramp II 6 July August September October November Total Average 6 10 .21 .19 .33 .67 3 1.00 3 15 27 51 81 122 0 0 0 20 33 26 11 .37 5 11 13 .36 .52 .72 25 1.71 5.67 17 83 133 229 20J4 0 9 15 16 35 71 1.00 29 L. .29 7 9 20 .3 37 91 13 25 71 1.11 .87 .33 1.00 12b 25 22 25 31 195 29 73 per ramp day generally increased. Populations in their second year o± high densities dispersed more than first year high density populations. The average of 1.01 and 1.00 captures per ramp day for Ramp II 3 and Raap II 6, respectively, were higher than the .80, .77, and .37 averages, for Ramps I 1, I 2, and I 5, respectively. Wounding percentages increased with the increasing amount of dispersion. The total mice caught, captures per ramp day, population estimate, and percent scars and wounds of vales captured in the dispersal ramps from June 2h, 196l, to December 9, 196li, are presented in Table XXVII. More mice with higher percentages of wounds were captured in the ramps in 196!. The dispersal movemen was partially influenced by an earlier overutilization of grasses in the summer causing cover to decrease through the fall. Again, dispersal movent was usually high when the monthly population estimates were at high levels. However, the captures per ramp day were moderately high in November and December after populations had declined, except in Ramp III 3, when wounding rates were generally high. Ramp III 6 had the lowest average captures 'per ramp day (1.3) f or l96l.., but this average rate was higher than any ramp in 1963. Ramps II 1, II 2, and II 5 had averages that were similar. III 3d showed little dispersal movement until September when ramp captures increased sharply. Ramp III 3 had a 2.2 average for captures per ramp day which was highest. Ramp III 3 also had the highest 7L. TABLE XXVII. TOTAL MICE CAUGHT, CAPTURES PER RAMP DAY, 2(ULATI0N ESTIMATE, AND PERCENT SCARS AND WOUNDS OF NICE 196.L, CA1URED L THE DISPERSi RAMPS FROM JUNE TO DECEMBER 9, 19&. Total Caught Ramp II 1 June July August September October November December Total Average Ramp II 2 June July August 3eptember October November December Total Average Ramp II 5 June July August September October November December Total Average 10 26 13 0 12 9 2 Captures per Ramp Day Population Estimate % Wounds and Scars 2.5 307 136 1.9 208 0 103 82 20 23 38 0 33 86 100 1.7 1.0 1.0 61.+ 72 1.8 33 30+ 102 122 107 98 5 19 11 12 5 1.3 7 1.1. - .5 98 1 2.7 1.6 1.3 .7 60 26 36 50 80 29 100 60 1.5 1 20 20 9 3 3 5 61 .3 2.9 2.9 25+ 82 222 .6 190 107 60 2.5 - 1.1 .li 1.5 100 25 35 67 33 33 20 36 75 TABLE XXVII CONTINUED. Total Caught Captures per Raino Day ?opulation Estimate % Wounds and Scars Nainp III 3 June July August September October November December Total Average 0 0 19+ .6 0 20 26 22 0 2.5 3.7 14.14 61 70 87 63 - 8 14.0 130 0 0 0 35 142 62 63 80 2.2 149 Ramp 1116 June July August September October November December Total Average * 2 .5 16 2.6 1.3 9 5 14 .6 .6 8 1.6 14 2.0 18+ 152 So 1149 1143 1414 95 58 - 22 140 50 75 50 i.3 - Indicates no trap period 142 76 average percent wounding (19) which occurred during the period September to December when population numbers were increasing and vegetation was regressing. Ramps II 2 and III percent average wounding. Ramps I I and I 5 6 both bad 1i2 had 33 and 36 percent average wounding, respectively. Pir8t year high density enclosures (Ramps I 1, I 2, and I 5) had. lower average capture rates and wounding rates for dispersal mice than the second year dispersal enclosures. Averages of' captures and wounding for the l96L. second year high density Ramps II 1, II 2, and II S were higher than in 1963 and were more comparable to the 1963 second year high density Ramp II 6. The third year high density populations (Ramps III 3 and III 6) presented characteristics of' dispersion that deviated from other populations, except possibly Ramp II 2 in 196k. In Ramp III 3, a high dispersal rate (76 mice) nd a very high wounding rate from September through December. Ramp III (b9 percent) occurred 6 had a high wounding rate (b2 percent). More wounding occurred for dispersal mice from the populations with third year high densities and from II 2d. This suggests that more intraspecific strife occurred for dispersal mice of the second and third year population phase. In 1963, more males than females were captured in the ramps except in Ramp II 3 where females comprised 51 percent o± the total captured. Dispersing males were mainly of adult and sub-adult age groups with the adults being more numer'ous. Sub-adults were the dominant age class fox' females. 77 The sante ramps in l96t produced similar sex and age dispersal patterns. II Males were more numerous in Ramps XI 1, II 2, III 3, and comprising SB, 57, 63 and 67 percent of mice captured, respec- tively. III 6. Females comprised 63 percent of the total capture in Ramp Adults were the numerous age class for males in Ramps II 1, II 2, and III 6. Sub-adults were the numerous age class for males in Ramps Ifl 3 and II S. Sub-adults were the dominant age class for females in all l96L ramp captures. In summary, captures per ramp day generally increased with monthly population growth. Wounding had a tendency to increase as ramp captures increased even though the month to iso th relationships were inconsistent. Some dispersion occurred in 1963 even when little wounding was evident. Mice of second and third year high density populations had a greater tendency to disperse than mice of first year high density populations. In general, dispersal mice had a higher percentage of wounds when they were from populations experiencing more years at high density levels. More males than females were caught in dispersal ramps, except Ramp XI 3 in 1963 and Ramp III 6 in 196b. Adult arid sub-adult males and sub-adult females were the numerous age classes in ramp captures. Wounding The frequency of wounding occurring within a population is an indication of the amount of intraspecific strife. Examples of bead 78 and. rump wounds encountered in this study are pictured in Figures 7 and 8. The monthly percentages and mean annual percentages of scars and woundà found on ail voles captured in the seven from June, 1963,to January, 196)4., enclosures The are presented in Table XXVIII. mean annual percentages of wounding for I id, I 2d, I Sd, and I 7 were similar to II 3d, II (19 percent) b, and II 6d. II than the other enclosures. II b had more wounding 1 and 1 7 without dis- persal ramps had higher over-all percent wounding (19 and 15, respectively) than the enclosures with dispersal ramps. Monthly p'oentages and mean annual percentages of scars and wounds found on all voles captured from the seven enclosures and the Caledania-6 plot from May through December, 1961L, are represented in Table XXIX. III 3d bad more wounding (20 percent) than the other enclosures. II 7 had a slightly higher wounding percentage (17) than II ld, II 2d, and II Sd. III I bad a lower wounding percentage (15) than III 3d and IU 6th The Caledonia-6 plot mice exhibited very little wounding. In suxmnary, for 1963 II Ii. bad higher percent wounding than the other enclosures. Slightly more wounding occurred in II 1 and 1 7 than in the enclosures with dispersal slightly more wounding than the ramps. In l96I, other enclosures. plot mice showed little wounding. III 3d bad Caledonia-6 Figure 7. Head wounds of an adult vole. Figure 8. Rump wounds of an adult vole. 81 TAi3LE XXVIII. MOTHL EiTAGE P O1 WOUJ :i AiD }OAN ANiUAL ?ENTAOE.3 FOUND 0 MICE APTUR1!D, AND PXU1U FOft OLOSURE I ThROUO} 7 ?R.0 ;c.'J THE TOTAL JUNE, 1963, TO JANUARY, 19624. Trep Period Designation I id I 2d I 5d I 7 . - June July August september-I .eptemoer-II October Deoeiber Januaxy 9 0 124 0 13 240 7 124 124 3 21 36 14 2 145 12 12 13 3 130 169 92 9 13 25 25 S 11 25 19 324 3 8 53 15 92 17 107 12 136 18 119 -' ean Percent Period Desiguation 0 3 - 13 9 147 13 68 7 7 255 9 314, 914 8 No. June July August-I August-Il September-I eptember-II Octocer-I October-lI November December 3rrnazy 13 6 1 &sn Percent 8 5 9 No. 8 35 22 20 69 9 7 29 724 - 22 36 'LO 241 240 i24 153 202 173 35 12 20 19 57 9 - 80 117 71 11 205 59 10 12 - 11 30 3 - 116 - 10 10 No. 9 27 88 25 19 - - Indicates no trsp period 13 j 15 Tix 3( % 10 16 16 12 13 165 210 83 S 19 39 242 63 76 1314 2L8 146 TABLE XXIX. MONTHLY PERCENTAGES AND MEAN ANNUAL PERCENTAGES OF WOUNDS AND SCARS FOUND ON MICE CAPTURED, AND THE TOTAL CAPTURE FOR ENCLOSURES 1 THROUGH 7 AND CALEDONIA-6 PLOT FROM MI&Y TO DECEMBER, 196)4, Trap Pe±'iod II id Designation No. May July August September 12 October November December 25 Mean Percent 16 Trap Period Designation % 1)4 7 1)4 II Sd No. No. )48 8 2)4 111 111 80 68 10 88 9 101. 5 20 - 8)4 1)4 65 70 27 II 2d % 73 11 III 3d Nay July August September 0 13 6 III 1)47 11 121 90 13 )47 30 - 15 17 III 6d )4 73 59 80 71 - 22 28 - iS 16 9 7 Moan Percent 20 13 2)4 Indicates no trap period 29 * 17 106 123 103 35 - 9 6 iS 11 13 October November December 83 11 18 117 153 119 83 56 2)4 16 63 136 118 14)4 No. 69 No. 6 22 1 * 10 10 15 No. 19 59 65 71 25 21 26 11 II Cal -6 No. % - 0 9 0 0 6 8 13 18 5 2 7 DISCUSSION Agoniatio Behavior Versus Population Densities The average fighting times are compared to the peak density estimates for the seven enclosures during 1963 and 19614 in Table XXX. It was generally evident that mice of first and second year phase populations reaching higher estimated peak densities exhibited more agonistic behavior. Mice of the first year high density populations, I 5d and I 7, displayed more over-all Lighting time, Tables II, III, and IV, and higher estimated peak densities than mice of second year high density populations, II 14 mice exhibited the larger aiiount of fighting behavior, Tables III and XXVIII, for second year high density populations. In 19614, with one exception (III 3d), lower average fighting times and lower peak densities were observed in both the second and third year phase populations than were observed in 1963. II Mice in the second year high d, and II 7 density enclosures, II id, generally reached higher peak densities and exhibited slightly more fighting than those of the third year high density enclosures, III 3d and III 6d. These observed density and fighting patterns correspond to Clarke's (19%) study showing that more conflict existed in his higher populated group voles, The data suggest that a reduction in population densities and fighting behavior may be related with the year phase of high density populations. Variations in behavior from the above statement did TABLE XXX. AVERAGE FIGHTING TINES AND PEAK DENBITY ESTIMATES FOR MICE GF ENCLOSURES 1 THROUGH 7 DURING 1963 AND 196t. I Enclosure Year Phase Fighting Peak Time in Density II Year Phase Peak Fighting Time in Seconds Density Seconds 1963 III Year Phase Fighting Time in Pesk Density Seconds 1964 Id 10.3 199+25 9.5 208+39 2d 11.7 230+29 7.0 l22+21 5a 15.9 314.55L1. 12.6 222 7 33.5 LO9+8b 11.5 212L9 3d 9.2 229+29 11.0 130+26 b 13.7 269+13 9.L. 229+1.O 6d 1O.!. 29553 9.1 1S2+LO 1963 * Two iandsrd errors occur in 19614 for II 2d and III 3d. II 2d had the lowest density and exhibited the least amount of fighting for the enclosures. III 3d displayed more wounding, Table XXIX, and a low density until December. The factors that influence aggressive fighting characteristics are not Ailly understood. Even though less fighting occurred, the third year high density enclosures, III 3d, III Li, and III 6d displayed a slight increase in aggressive fighting, Tables II, III, and IV. I4ore mice eetablisbed the social characteristics at dominance and aubordinance in these older populations which could have been influenced by increased aggressive behavior. Chitty (1960) proposed that when populations of voles experience high densities, changes in characteristics occur through selection. Aggressive fighting behavior may be one of these changes, but Clark (1962) states that the type of fighting is learned, and that success in fighting re-enforces aggressive activity. Peak densities for first and third year phase dispersal enclosures were lower than their respective year phase non-dispersal enclosures, but this relationship did not entirely hold true for second phase populations, ar Table XXX. II 14, I 7, and II 7 were generally indicated to have more strife, wounding and fighting data of Tables III, IV, XXVIII and XXIX, than the dispersal enclosures of their respective year phase. The data suggests that dispersal movement, by temporarily lowering denaittes, may redee intraspecific strife tn the first and, to a lesser extent, second year phase high densities. The Caledonia-6 plot population is an example of a low density population. These mice exhibited very little fighting behavior and used threat tactics more than mice from higher densities, Tables IV and XXIX. Behavior Versus Sex and Age Composition Higher peràentages of adults were found in I 2d, II b, and I 7 than in other 1963 enclosures * III 3d had the higher percent of adults for the enclosures in 1961j. These above populations generally showed high fighting averages, Tables III and IV, and wounding was also higher for II ! and III 3d, Tables XXVIII and XXIX. Fighting behavior is suggested to have been encouraged by the older age structure in these populations. Clarke (195S) found that adults fight more than the young, and therefore aggressive behavior is most likely to occur more among sexually mature voles. In 1963, II 14 and I 7, non-dispersal enclosures previously discussed, were the only enclosures that had more males than females and displayed more fighting and slightly more wounding than their respective year phase dispersal enclosures. one exception, With males were more abundant in ramp captures than females in 1963 * In 19614, II 2d and III 6d were the only enclosures with more females than males, and both, particularly II 2d, displayed less fighting and wounding characteristics for the year. Bailey (19214) that male voles inflict more wounds on other voles states than do females. A higher ratio of males to females within these populations probably influenced an increase of agonistic behavior to occur. The low density Caledonia-6 population was characterized by similar numbers of males and female8 and a high percentage of adults. These mice displayed little strife, for they lacked the element of high density. Agonis tic Behavior Versus Reproduction Clarke (l9) found that voles of higher density populations had a shorter reproductIve season, and that an jncreaaed amount of fighting behavior was suggested to have severe effects on reproduction. This corresponds with the over-all findings for the following populations of this study. I Sd and I 7, with an in- dication of more fighting, Table III, had lower reproductive rates, Table XVI, than II L. and II 6d. II b had higher wounding rates, Table XXVIII, but did riot obtain as high a fighting time average as did I Sd and I 7. The reproductive rates for the enclosures were lower in l96l than in 1963. Second year pha8e enclosUres II Sd and II 7, with slightly higher estimated peak densities and longer average fighting time behavior, Table XXX, had slightly lower reproductive rates than the third year XVII. ase enclosures III 3d and III 6d, Table Although, III 3d mice showed higher wounding rates, Table XXII, which indicates that strife was significant for thIs population throughout the study year. The data suggest that increased strife among mice of populations exhibiting higher average righting tl.mea resulted in slightly lower reproductive perfonnanoe. Louch (1956) found that reproduction did not change with an increased population density, but tie did indicate that the litter survival was adversiy eLfected at higher densities. This study indicates that agonistic behavior, a form of intraspecific strife, at higher densities tends to alter reproduotion Chitty (1952) infers that intraspecific strife may increase prenatal mortality by affecting the physiology and behavior of pregnant females. Smith (196h) inferred that II 4 CE 4) exhibited more prenatal mortality than I Sd (E 5), I 7 (E 7), and II 6d CE 6). Table XVI indicates that fewer lactating females than pregnant females were caught in II 4 and, to a lesser extent, I Sd. II 4 exhibited the highest fighting and wounding rates for second year phase populations, Tables III, IV, XXVIII, and XXIX From this, it is suggested that prenatal mortality caused the II 4 peak density to remain at a lower level than the peak density of II 6d, Table XXX, which exhibited less fighting and wounding characteristics. Prenatal mortality, an indication o.f disrupted social structure which is correlated with intraspecific strife (Chitty, 1952; Clarke, 1955), has been suggested to have held down the peak density of II 4. Agonistic Behavior Versus General Survival Clarke (19%) found that survival of infant and juvenile voles was lees than adults. This was evident in the study. Juvenile survival was best through August, 1963, and thereafter the sub-adults Fran 196h, juvenile and adults survived best as densities increased. survival became poor in August as densities increased, and sub-adult This adds and adult survival declined in August and September. further support to Smith1s (196L) 8uggestion that juvenile survival decreases as densities Increase. In 1963, II ii. and II 6d had better survival and showed less fighting behavior, Table III, than I d and I 7. II 3d, with less fighting, had better survival in the Nay, June, and January cohorts than I 7. This correlation indicates that survival may have been adverely influenced by the higher fighting rates of I d and I 7. In l96i, III 3d and III 6d had better survival than II 7, II 2d, II ld, III !, or II 5d. all. II d had the lowest survival of Strife, indicated by fighting and wounding, was higher for The III 3d, II 5d, and II 7 than for III 6d, Table8 III and flIX. better survival for III 3d was probably influenced by the better cover condition in the fall. The better survival of III 6d was possibly influenced by less fighting behavior. II 7 survival was similar, in spite of the indication of more strife, to that of II 2d, II ld, and III b. Apparently, agonistic behavior had less of an influence on survival in 196h, With lower densities, than was indicated for 1963. Agonietic behavior is suggested to be more of a stress factor limiting survival when populations are at higher peak densities. Agonistic Behavior Versus Dispersal Movement In general, more males than females were caught in dispersal ramps and this reduction in males tended to reduce the strife in these enclosures. Adult and sub-adult males and sub-adult females were prodc*dnanty caught in the ramps. of Smith (196b). This follows the finding8 Dispersal movement increased as population den- sities and conflict, indicated by wounding, increased among the dispersal voles, Tables XXVI and XXVII. In solitary behavior observations it was found that dispersal mice displayed more exploratory behavcx' and less active behavior than non-dispersal mice, Tables VII and VIII. This suggests that exploratory behavior combined with conflIct promotes dispersion and movement. Barnett (1957) made similar deductions £ or Norway rats. Mice of the second year high density populations indicated a greater tendency to disperse than first year high density population mice, Tables XXVI and XXVII. Dispersal movements, indicated by captures per ramp day, increased in the second study year. Second and third year phase dispersal mice had a higher percent of wounding per ramp capture than first year phase dispersal mice, which implied that more fighting occurred before these mice dispersed. This 91 suggests that dispersion characteristics may be linked with the high density population year phase, and that more fighting occurs among mice before they disperse from second and third year populations. Also, dual trial observations indicated that fighting behavior increased among dispersal mice as the study aogresaed into the second year, Tables V and VI. In 1963, male dispersal mice displayed far less Lighting and more subnissive behavior than male nondispersal mice. First year high density I ld, I 2d, and I d, dis- persal males varied in their fighting characteristics as compared Dispersal males from III 3d and III 6d to non-dispersal males. consistently fought loss than non-dispersal males.. For l96b, dispersal males displayed slightly less over-all fighting and aggressive behaor than non-dispersal males. from II ld, II 2d, and II Dispersal males d varied again in their fighting, while both III 3d and III 6d dispersal males displayed slightly lower fighting rates than non-dispersal males. It is evident that the dispersal male fighting rates for the earlier populations increased to higher fighting rates in the older populations which were more similar to non-dispersal male fighting behavior. This implies that conflict is a significant factor influencing mice to disperse in older vole populations.. Where there was uq dispersal movement in second year populations, II b and II 7, exploratory behavior was less and fighting 92 occurred more, Tables III and IV. Fighting was higher for I 7 mice. This suggests that as densities increased, voles were forced together and with no nsans to disperse fighting increased. Scott (1962) discussed bis cause of fighting in deer mice. The evidence indicates that dispersion, influenced by fighting behavior and exploratory behavior, is an important factor in temporarily alleviating hostility and reducing densities in a vole population. Also, fighting appears to be a more influencial factor in promoting dispersion of mice from a population characterized by previous high den8ity years. From the forgoing discussion, it is suggested that agonistic behavior is a factor in regulating high densities of voles. Increased fighting tends to lower survival and, to a lesser extent, reproductive rates. In high density populations, vole numbers are suggested to be temporarily reduced, in part, by increased dispersal movement which apparently is connected with the high density year phase. CONCLUSIONS 1. Agonistic behavior reach high densities. is more apparent in vole populations that Dispersal movement reduces intraapec±fic strife by temporarily lowering densities of the fir8t and second year high density phases. This was not so evident in the third year high density populations. Voles of a low density population fighting behavior. utilize more threat behavior than contact 2. Agonis tic behavior is more pronounced in high density pop- ulations with a large number of adults. At high densitios, a larger ratio of males to females is suggested to increase agoniatic behavior * 3. increased in lower intraspecific strife is suggested to have resulted reproductive performance for populations exhibiting higher average fighting times. Prenatal mortality) induced by intra- specific strife, has been suggested to have held down the peak density level of a population ( II b) in this study. 14. Agonistic Juvenile survival decreases as densities increase, behavior is suggested to be a stress factor limiting survival of all age classes when populations reach the higher densities1 . Exloratory behavior combined with fighting promotes dispersion which increases with expanding population densities. Dispersion characteristics are suggested to be linked with the high density 9b year phase Non-dispersal males display more agonistic behavior than do dispersal males a! the earlier year phases of high densities. 6. This study indicates that agoniatic behavior, which in!luences dispersal movement, survival, and, to a lesser extent, reproduction is a factor in th regulation of high density vole populations. 95 1. Bailey, Vernon. Breeding, feeding, and other life habits of meadow mice (Microtue). Journal of Agriculture Research 27:523-535. E 2. Barnett, S. A. An analysis of social behavior in wild rats. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London 130:107-152. 1957. 3. Calhoun, John B. The ecology and sociology of the Norway rat. 1962. 286 p. (U.S. Public Realth Service. Publication no. 1008). b. Carter, Bernie E. Relationships between spring and fall populations of confined Microtus montanus (Peale). Master's thesis. Corvallis, Oregon Mate University, 1963. 50 numb. loaves, 5. Chitty, Dennis. Mortality among voles (Nicrotu8 agrestis) at Lake Vyrnwy, Montgomery-shire in 1936-9. 1osophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 236:505-552. 1952. 6. ______________. 7. ______________. Self-regulation of numbers through 1iges in viai1ity. Cold Spring Harbor S'mposia on Adverse effects of population density upon the viability of later generations. In The numbers of man and animals, ed. by J. B. Cragg and N. W. Pine. Edinburgh, Oliver and Boyd, 1955. p. 57-67. Quantitative Biology 22:277-280. 8. 9. 1957. ______________. Population processes in the vole and their relevance to general thex'opy. Canadian Journal of Zoology 38:99-113. 1960. Chitty, D. H. and. E. Phipps. Journal of Animal Ecology A declining vole population. 30:19O-1.9l. 1961. U. Clark, Lincoln I). Experimental studies of the behavior of an aggressive, predatory mouse, 0y1ya 1e*cogaater. In: Roots of Behavior, ed. by E. L. B1isi New York, Hoeber Medical Division, Harper & Brothers, 1962. p. 179-186. 11. Clarke, J. R. Influence of numbers on reproduction and survival in two experimental vole populations. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, lhh:68-85. 1955. 12. ____________. The aggressive behavior of the vole. Behavior 91-23. 1955. 13. Davis, David E. Manual for analysis of rodent populations. University Park, Penntiylvania. State University, 1962. 82 p. 114. An inquiry into the phylogeny of gangs. Roots of Behavior, Ed. by E. L. Bliss. New York, Hoeber Medical Division, Harper & Brothers, 1962. P. 316-320. In: 15. Ecke, Dean H. and Alva H. ICinnoy. Aging meadow mice, Nicrotus californicus, by observation of molt progession. JournaI of Mammalogy 37:249-2514. 1956. 16. Eisenberg, John F. 17. Elton, Charles. Voles, mice and lemmings, problems in population dynamics. Oxford, Clarendon Press, 1942. 1496 p. 18. Frank, Fritz. The causality of microtine cycles in Germany. The Journal of iild1ife Management 21:113-121. 1957. 19. Hamilton, W, J. The biology of microtine cycles. igricult.ure Research 514:779-790. 1937. 20. Hoffman, Robert S. The role of reproduction and mortality in population fluctuations of voles (Microtus). Ecological Monographs 28:79-109. l958. 21. Krebba, Charles J. Lemming cycle at Baker Lake, Canada, during 1939-1962. Science 1140:6714-676. May 10, 1963. 22. Louch, Charles D. Adreriocortical activity in relation to the density and dynamics of three confined populations of Nicrotua Ecology 37:701-713. 1956. 23. Scott, J. P. Hostility and aggression in animals. In: Boots of Behavior, ed. by E. L. Bliss. New York, kioeber Medical t1Msion, Harper & Brothers, 1962. p. 167-178. 24. Shillito, Elizabeth E. Exploratory behavior in the short-tailed vole Miorotus agreatia. Behaviour 21:145-1514. 1963. 25. Smith, Jerome S. Affects of dispersion and parent populations density on enclosed populations of Miorotus montanus (Peale). Master's Thesis. Corvallis, Oregon State University, 19614. 79 numb. leaves. Studies on the behavior of Peromyscus manioulatus 1ambelii and Peromjs cue calilornicus paaUus. Behaviour 19:177-207. 1962. Journal of 97 26. Tinbergen, N. Wiley and 27. U Social betiavior in animals. Inc., 1959. 150 p. S. Weather Bureaus 69:214-255. Sunnnary. C1iatological data. 1963. New York, John Oregon Annia1