Umnol. Oceanogr., 30(1), 1985, 202-212 © 1985, by the American Society of Limnology and Oceanography, Inc. The prediction of cladoceran grazing rate spectra1 Abstract—Reanalysis of published observa tions on the ingestion of artificial particles by cladocerans confirmed the statistical significance of particle size selection in situ. Three statistical approaches were tested to seek a framework for a general theory of cladoceran particle size selec tion: the algebraic continuous model, the alge braic discrete model, and the distributional mod- 1 A contribution to the Groupe d'Ecologie des Eaux douces ofl'Universite de Montreal and the Lake Memphremagog Project of McGill University. This work was supported by the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada, the Canadian National Sportsmen's Fund, the Lake Memphremagog Project, and the Department of Education of the province of Quebec. el. The first two approaches can predict the shape of grazing rate: particle size spectra but make biased predictions. The last approach, in which equations are used to predict the parameters of the negative binomial distribution which mimics grazing rate spectra, makes fairly precise (r2 = 0.67) and unbiased predictions. Improvement of this kind of model is suggested as a step toward quantification of the role played by zooplankton in the dynamics of phytoplankton communities. Particle size selection by zooplankton predators is considered responsible in part for the structure of phytoplankton com munities (Porter 1973; McCauley and Briand 1979). Lam and Frost (1976) sug- Notes 203 30- 20 SIZE OF 30 40 50 PARTICLE Ojiti dlam ) 0 5 10 15 20 25 30 Fig. 2. Size-frequency distribution of organic poly mer particles in grazing experiments conducted in Mikolajskie Lake during 1973. (Drawn from Gliwicz 1977: table 1.) SIZE OF PARTICLE (p; jjm dlam) Fig. 1. Grazing rate (mean and standard deviation of the sample) of Daphnia cucullata (Kp) on organic polymer particles of various sizes (p) during July, Au gust, and September 1973. (Redrawn from Gliwicz 1977: fig. 1.) Each of these three plots is here referred to as a "grazing rate spectrum." gested that the size composition of the phytoplankton influences the length and com plexity of planktonic food webs and thus that particle size selection by zooplankton should be included in simulation models of planktonic communities. The ability ofzooplankton to ingest various sizes of algae has also been thought to mold zooplankton community structure in lakes of different trophic status (Brooks and Dodson 1965; Makarewicz and Likens 1979; McCauley and Kalff 1981; McCauley 1983). Lehman (1976) even suggested that a model to pre dict the behavior of filter feeders is incom plete if it ignores the size-selective ingestion of food particles. Food particle size selec tion by zooplankton has been found in lab oratory experiments (e.g. Mullin 1963; Gli wicz 1970; McQueen 1970; Arnold 1971; DeMott 1982), and Peters and Downing (1984) have tried to quantify the general effect of food particle size on grazing rate. However there have been few attempts to measure in situ particle size selection, and therefore the literature lacks a general model or framework for future studies. The im portance of such a general model can be underscored by the recent proliferation of models of planktonic communities that make simple assumptions regarding particle size selection by zooplankton (e.g. Jernigan andTsokos 1980; Sjoberg 1980; Wulff 1980; Arnold and Voss 1981; Brown et al. 1982; Montague et al. 1982). Gliwicz (1977) examined in situ particle size selection by feeding the natural zoo plankton of Mikolajskie Lake beads of or ganic polymer of different sizes in addition to their natural food. Animals were re trieved after a short feeding period and the number of beads in each of 10 size classes was counted in their guts. Grazing rates 0*1 anim."1 h"1) for each size class were calcu lated by comparing the number of particles ingested to the number of particles of that size available per unit volume of lake water. Histograms with standard deviations were presented to show how grazing rate varied with particle size in five cladoceran species. 204 Notes The general form of the histograms (Fig. 1) suggests that positive selection is strongest for small particles (< 10 nm) and that neg ative selection is strongest against large nannoplankton (15-50 nm). Two types ofinvestigation should be made before we can use these results to theorize regarding the effects of cladocerans on phytoplankton communities. First, it is not clear how much of the variability in the grazing rates found by Gliwicz could have resulted from the experimental conditions rather than the behavior of the animals. The par ticle size distribution offered the animals was highly skewed (Fig. 2); thus, unless counts were pooled from hundreds of ani mals, the ingestion of large particles would be rare, even ifgrazing were independent of particle size. Ifthe concentration ofparticles of a given size is very low then the proba bility of random uptake of a single particle is small. Fractions of particles cannot be ingested. Therefore, if sample sizes are not large, grazing rates will be apparently zero, underestimating the more accurate small, positive value. This could account for some of the apparent selection against large par ticles shown by Gliwicz's histograms. In fact, his calculations show that grazing rates within species-date combinations are al most always significantly correlated (P < 0.05) with the density of plastic particles. Second, the data are presented as 25 sepa rate observations without general connec tion. Because these are the best field exper iments done on cladoceran particle size selection to date, it is tempting to use the data to produce a preliminary general the ory to predict zooplankton predation dam age to phytoplankton. At the very least, an analysis ofthese data might suggest a format for future studies. Our purpose here is to examine Gliwicz's data in greater detail, to determine the sig nificance of departures from random par ticle uptake, and to test a series of statistical approaches through which a more general theory might be produced. We thank M. Gliwicz for sharing his thoughts and data. Comments on the manuscript by R. Peters, M. Pace, J. Bence, and D. Smith are also appreciated. We originally intended to evaluate the size range through which grazing rates deter mined with small particles (1-4 jum) could be representative of the grazing rates on larger sized particles (5-35 pm) (see Down ing and Peters 1980; Downing 1981). The grazing rates on various sizes of particles (Vp, where p is the midpoint of the particle size range in nm, e.g. F25, V-,-5, etc.) and the bounds of the approximate 95% confidence intervals for replicate determinations were taken from figures 1-4 of Gliwicz (1977). Grazing rates (V, /xl anim."1 h"1) were de termined to within 0.1 fil anim."1 h~' with calipers and some photo-enlargements of these figures. Mean body lengths of various species in Gliwicz's experiments were taken from his table 2. The data so derived con sisted of grazing rates, confidence intervals, body lengths, and particle diameter mid point for Daphnia cucullata, Daphnia longispina, Bosmina coregoni, Diaphanosoma brachyurum, and Chydorus sphaericus during May, June, July, August, September, and October 1973. Some species-month cells are empty but we had a total of 25 different grazing rate: particle size distributions. We wanted to determine both the fre quency with which the grazing rate distri butions of Gliwicz would be expected to occur as a result of nonselective uptake of particles, and the particle sizes for which significant deviations occur. A x2 compar ison ofgrazing rate spectra with Fig. 2 would satisfy the first goal but would yield no in formation on the positive or negative selec tion for specific sizes of particles. Instead, we simulated the experimental protocol but used a random number generator instead of animals to ingest the particles. In the in situ experiments, each of the grazing rate: par ticle size curves was a result of 3-5 replicate determinations, each consisting of the par ticles ingested by 3-10 animals, during a period less than the gut passage time (about 10 min). Thus, we assumed that each rep licate arose from the particles ingested in 100 animal-min (10 animals feeding for 10 min). We used this estimate to back-cal culate the average number ofbeads that were ingested and counted in each replicate de termination for each of the 25 experiments. We then used the random number generator Notes (random at P < 0.01) to "ingest" this same number of beads from the size distribution in Fig. 2 for each of five replicates. Each single particle in Fig. 2 thus had an equal probability of being ingested. The grazing rate that would be obtained if selection were random (Vp) was then calculated from the number of particles of each size range in gested. If Gliwicz's results are simply an expression of random uptake, then in re peated simulations, 95% of the Vp should lie within the confidence interval found by Gliwicz(i.e. Vp ± f005SE).In nsimulations, the probability that the observed grazing rate results arose from random uptake of par ticles can be calculated: n/0.95n, where n is the number of simulations in which Vp fell within the confidence interval for Vp re ported by Gliwicz. Theoretically, n could be greater than 0.95«, indicating that ran dom uptake is a more precise descriptor of Gliwicz's data than the data themselves. This was not a common occurrence. We performed this simulation between 100 and 1,000 times for each of the 25 species-date combinations; more simulations would be prohibitively expensive and result in little increase in precision (J. Downing unpubl. data). Table 1 shows that some of the apparent particle size selection can be attributed to random uptake but that cladocerans are sig nificantly size-selective. Random uptake is especially prevalent at small and large par ticle sizes. The uptake of large particles ap pears random simply because the particle size distribution offered was highly skewed (see asterisks: Table 1). The cladocerans ingested very few (if any) particles >40 pm in diameter, but given the particles offered, the random number generator would en counter these particles with a probability of only 0.0009! Thus, it is obvious that the apparent size of the largest particle ingested by a filter-feeding animal (cf. Burns 1968a) will be a function of the shape of the food spectrum offered and the total number of particles censused. This is true even if there is no size selection. Random uptake at smaller particle sizes appears to be real but varies over the season, which suggests that animals are able to vary their uptake effi ciency seasonally, but this could be due to 205 O O O I I I I O O O t- + O I I I I I OOO + o I I I I loo© oooooooooo ool I I o o o o o OO I I I + o I I + O I I I I I + O I I I I I + O I I I I I I OOOO * # # # o o o o OOO » * # *© ♦ # # 00 OOO <n OOO <N ooo I I looooS OOl I I I ool I I I oooo 2 I I I I I I I oo O O O O o o o o o I ooloool I OOO I I I I ool I I I I I I I + o o w> O O <♦> oo loovo till O O O I O O O OO o O O O -< # * # v© • # <N o o o I oo o o oo Ol K N !>• 206 Notes behavioral or mechanical plasticity, or ge netic variability. Also notable is the rarity of positive selection, suggesting that cladocerans do not often seek out preferred par ticle sizes but reject those that are less ac ceptable. The dominating observation, however, is that cladocerans select against particles be tween 10 and 35 nm in diameter (Table 1). Some of the shape in Gliwicz's curves (e.g. Fig. 1) is thus due to behavioral or me chanical selectivity by cladocerans, not sim ply the statistical aspects of the size spec trum offered. The importance ofthis finding to the modeling of phytoplankton com munities is clear. If we assume that all nannoplankton (e.g. <35 mhi) is removed from the water by cladocerans at equal propor tional rates, then predation pressure should lead to the dominance of those algae that have superior rates of growth (i.e. small al gae; see Banse 1976; Friebele et al. 1978; Foy 1980; Malone 1980; Schlesinger et al. 1981; Smith and Kalff 1982). On the other hand, if predation pressure is proportionally lower on those algae that are growing most slowly, then the differential predation rate may allow coexistence of large and small algae or even dominance of slow growing forms under some conditions. It is thus im portant that we be able to predict differences in proportional predation rates on different size categories of phytoplankton. We here propose three different statistical approaches that could be used to make these predictions and compare them to find which yields the most accurate and precise pre dictions of grazing rate of cladocerans on algae of different sizes. We call these three approaches the algebraic continuous model, the algebraic discrete model, and the dis tributional model. The three types are cer tainly not exhaustive but include the most prevalent classes of analysis in ecology to day. The algebraic continuous model was sug gested by work showing that the grazing rate of a cladoceran is a continuous function of its body size and of the concentration of food offered to it (Downing and Peters 1980; Downing 1981; Peters 1984). The effect of body size (L; length, mm) is usually positive (Burns 1969; Egloffand Palmer 1971; Chis- holm et al. 1975; Geller 1975; Peterson et al. 1978), while the additional effect of food concentration is usually negative (McMahon and Rigler 1965; Burns 1968a; Chisholmetal. 1975; Geller 1975; Kersting and van der Leeuw 1976). The particle size (P) in question also determines the grazing rate of those cells, with lower values seen at higher P and perhaps at lower P as well (Burns 19686; Gliwicz 1969; Porter 1973; Peterson et al. 1978; Peters and Downing 1984). Thus, the grazing rate response in Gliwicz's data should be approximated by the simple function: Vp = a + b log L + cP - dP2 - eS (1) where S is the food concentration, P is the particle size (jim diam), and a, b, c, d, and e are fitted constants. Unfortunately, we do not know S for Gliwicz's experiments. The effect of food concentration can be account ed for, however, by using the grazing rate at 2.5 nm (V2.5) which should be negatively correlated with S, as an independent vari able to predict K75to F475. This is especially useful since in situ grazing rates are often approximated with particles ofthis size range (Haney 1973; Downing and Peters 1980). Thus, the expected algebraic equation to predict Vp (exclusive of V2 5, ofcourse) would be Vp = a + b log L + cP - dP2 + <?K2.5.(2) We fitted this equation to Gliwicz's data using least-squares regression (Draper and Smith 1966) and backwards elimination to find the best equation (Hocking 1976; Downing 1981). A highly significant regression equation could be derived by this technique. Over the small range of L, however, the logarith mic transformation of body length yielded no significant (P < 0.01) improvement over the linear form. In addition, there was no significant curvilinear effect of particle size (P2) on Vp. The best equation to predict Vp was therefore Vp = 4.897 + 0.021L - 0.958P + 0.415F,2.5- (3) This equation has an overall /''-value of 65 (n = 129, r2 = 0.61), and partial F-values of all regression coefficients are highly signif- Notes 207 Although this equation fits the data well, on average, there is one important failure in this approach. Figure 3 shows a plot of £ 20 40 60 OBSERVED GRAZING RATE Oil anlm.-'h"1) Fig. 3. The relationship between observed grazing rates and those predicted using the algebraic continu ous model. Predictions are from Eq. 3. The solid line indicates a 1:1 correspondence between observations and predictions. The model gives rise to negative graz ing rates, is positively biased between 5 and 30 fd anim."1 h~', and is negatively biased at high grazing rates (>40 nl anim."1 h~*)- icant (P <§: 0.01). We also tried logarithmic transformations of Fand P, but the result ing equations made poorer predictions of Vp. The effect of body size is positively lin ear, the effect of particle size is negatively linear, and F2.s acts as a positive scaling factor raising or lowering the response, probably corresponding to variations in food concentration. predictions vs. observations for these data. If Eq. 3 were an unbiased predictor of Vp (the grazing rate on the midpoint of size class p) then the observations would fall evenly along the "1:1" line. Throughout much of the range of Vp, however, Eq. 3 yields overestimates. It also fails in two oth er ways. First, predictions ofvery low values are poor and often negative; a negative val ue of Vp is impossible unless animals are producing algae. Second, Eq. 3 fails to pre dict very high observations of Vp. These failings result from the complex curvilinearity of the actual response and the linear ity of the fitted model. This is not to suggest that this approach cannot yield good pre dictions of grazing rate when new data are added, only that it is not sophisticated enough to yield an unbiased fit to these data. The algebraic discrete model involved the construction of a series of multivariate equations that describe how the grazing rate on independent size classes of prey varies as a function of cladoceran body size and the grazing rate on the 2.5-iim reference par ticle. This yielded a set of six multivariate equations (Table 2; part 1), one to predict grazing rates for each ofthe prey size classes. Significant (P < 0.01) relationships were Table 2. Equations to predict grazing rate spectra for cladocerans using either (part 1) discrete multivariate equations or (part 2) a model based on the negative binomial distribution. Vp is the grazing rate on prey size class p (jim diam), L is the body length in mm, X is the mean grazing rate over the grazing rate spectrum calculated on the basis of a size-class frequency distribution, and N is the definite integral of the grazing rate spectrum. 1. Discrete multivariate equations: Prey size class (p) b c 7.5 12.5 0.000704 0.00119 0.01557 0.01388 17.5 0.00172 22.5 27.5 32.5 0.00106 0.000105 -0.000103 0.01104 0.01451 0.00518 0.00431 2. Distributional terms: logI0* == 0.697 log10L - 0.164 logiOV2S - 0.80 S* == 2.779JE0-5 - 2.337 log,0AT == 0.0022L - 0.0408 V2i + 1.705 a 0.446 -0.034 -0.447 -0.427 -0.057 -0.011 r F 0.897 0.795 96.2 47.6 0.781 0.666 0.323 0.186 r 0.71 0.81 0.96 39.4 22.0 6.2 2.5 F 37.8 43.9 142.2 Notes 208 E 80- o 3 UJ • - • 60- • q: z 40- N - © 20- 0 UJ q. 0 20 40 60 OBSERVED GRAZING RATE (jul anim:1 tT1) Fig. 4. The relationship between observed grazing rates and those predicted using the algebraic discrete model. Predictions are from equations in part 1 of Ta ble 2. The solid line represents a 1:1 correspondence between observations and predictions. The relation ship between observed (Vp) and predicted (Vp) grazing rates can be characterized as Vp = 1.935 + 0.801 Vp (r* = 0.71; F= 385). The model is biased, especially for predicting low grazing rates (i.e. <10 /tl anim."1 h-). found for only four of the six size classes <35-fim diam (Table 2). Observations of grazing rates on particles >35 tim were nearly all zero and so were not examined further. Grazing rates on particles between 25 and 35 /-im were not functions of body size of cladocerans or of grazing rate on the reference particle. To examine the overall ability of the models to predict variation in grazing rates, we examined the relationship between predicted and observed values for the equations presented in Table 2 (Fig. 4). Regression analysis indicates that although the multivariate equations could account for more than 70% of the variance in grazing rate, the predictions were biased. The slope of the relationship between predicted and observed rates was significantly different from one (P < 0.005), and the intercept de viated significantly from zero (P < 0.005). 20 OBSERVED o 40 GRAZING 60 RATE (p\ aninvV) Fig. 5. The ability of the negative binomial distri bution to approximate Gliwicz's 25 grazing rate spec tra. The negative binomial distribution was fitted by expanding the individual terms of the negative bino mial distribution (Elliott 1977) based on mean, vari ance, k, and N calculated directly from each of the spectra. The solid line represents a 1:1 correspondence between calculated (Vp) and observed (Vp) grazing rates. The relationship can be characterized as V'p = 0.02 + 1.02 Vp (r2 = 0.88; F = 1,498). Grazing rate spectra can thus be fitted accurately by negative binomial distri butions. These equations overestimate grazing rates for values <10 yl anim."1 h"1. This bias could not be removed by further transfor mation of the independent and dependent variables. Because of this bias, we consid ered these models, too, to be inadequate for predictive purposes. The distributional model was based on the observation that the relationship be tween grazing rates and prey size for cladoc erans (Gliwicz 1977) resembles a negative binomial distribution. In this approach, the dependent variable is not a set of single val ues of grazing rate on particular particle size classes but rather an estimate of the param eters required to expand the individual terms of the negative binomial distribution to fit the grazing rate spectrum for each datespecies combination. The individual terms can be expanded in a recursive model (El- Notes 209 liott 1977), if we know the mean {x), vari ance (s2), total number of observations (N), and the expansion coefficient (k). There are two methods commonly used to calculate h, these are the product-moment and max imum likelihood techniques. The former method, where k = x/(s2 - x), (4) is recommended for distributions measured with small sample size, because it is the most efficient (Pieters et al. 1977). To find whether the observed relationship between grazing rate and prey size can be described by the negative binomial distri bution, we converted each grazing rate dis tribution to a frequency distribution and calculated the terms of the negative bino mial (x, s2, N, and k). N was calculated as the definite integral of the grazing rate spec trum. Expanding the terms yielded a differ ent fitted or calculated distribution for each ofthe observed distributions. By comparing the fitted value for each size class with the observed grazing rates, we can determine whether the negative binomial distribution can be used to describe individual filtering rate spectra. This comparison is shown in Fig. 5 for the 25 observed distributions. In general, the negative binomial distribution is a good descriptor ofthe observed variation in graz ing rates. Regression analysis shows that there is no bias introduced in fitting the dis tributions to the individual relationships. The slope is not significantly different from one (P < 0.01) and the intercept is not sig nificantly different from zero (P < 0.01). In addition, the F-value and correlation coef ficient indicate significant agreement be tween the distributions and the observed relationships. This only means that filtering rate spectra can be fitted using a negative binomial, not necessarily that it can be used for prediction. To attempt these predictions, we con structed empirical relationships between es timates of the parameters of the negative binomial distribution and the independent variables (body size and the grazing rate on the 2.5-/im reference particle). This analysis yielded three relationships (Table 2; part 2) that can be used to predict the variation in 0 20 OBSERVED 40 60 GRAZING RATE (jul animT'h"1) Fig. 6. The relationship between observed grazing rates and those predicted using the distributional mod el. The predictions are made by expanding a negative binomial distribution of grazing rate against particle size using the expansion coefficients predicted from the equations in part 2 of Table 2 (see text). The solid line represents a 1:1 correspondence between predicted (Vp) and observed (Vp) grazing rates. The actual relation ship can be characterized as Vp = 0.03 + 1.02F, (r2 = 0.67; F = 385). The distributional model makes un biased predictions of grazing rates on various particle sizes with body size of cladoceran and V2i as inde pendent variables. grazing rate on prey items of different sizes. The variables x and AT were predictable from body size and the grazing rate on the 2.5nm particle, while s2 was predictable from x. There was no significant relationship be tween k and the independent variables, but this parameter can be estimated from pre dicted estimates of x and s2 (Eq. 4). Figure 6 shows predicted and observed rates with the negative binomial relation ships. Predicted values are based on the ex pansion of the negative binomial distribu tion (Elliott 1977) using estimates of the required parameters from the regressions in part 2 of Table 2. Regression analysis in dicates no detectable bias with these rela tionships because the slope and intercept are not significantly different (P < 0.05) from one and zero. In addition, more than 210 Notes 67% of the variation in grazing rates can be explained with this approach. At very high grazing rates, predicted values deviate from the observed, although this deviation does not appear to be systematic. The analyses presented here represent a first attempt to construct empirical rela tionships that predict variations in grazing rates by cladoceran zooplankton on artifi cial prey items of different sizes. Our results complement other studies showing that the body size of zooplankton is useful for pre dicting grazing rates on single sized particles (e.g. Burns 19686; Downing and Peters 1980) by quantifying the relationship between body size and variation in grazing rates on prey particles of different sizes. The results also suggest a format for future studies to predict this variation. Empirical relation ships based on either discrete multivariate equations or transformed polynomial regressions, which have been used success fully to predict other aspects ofzooplankton feeding (Downing and Peters 1980; Down ing 1981; Peters and Downing 1984), were shown to be inadequate because they yield ed biased predictions of grazing rates. The distributional technique provides estimates that are not biased and that explain a similar or larger percentage of the variance. These techniques are therefore recommended over the other two methods. Our study was restricted by the avail ability of data. We found no other data that could be used to test the generality of this approach for cladocerans (e.g. Gliwicz 1969; Kersting and Holtermann 1973; Berman and Richman 1974; Neill 1975). Data for copepods are less rare but the danger of par ticle size modification {see Peters 1984) makes interpretation of some size classes difficult. The distribution of the grazing rates of copepods on different sized prey is either more symmetrical (Vanderploeg 1981) than that of cladocerans, or positively skewed (Richman et al. 1980) rather than negatively as it is for cladocerans. If the variation in the grazing rate distributions of copepods can be related to their body size, then it may be possible to produce a single empirical model for copepods and cladocerans through the use ofdummy variables (Gujarati 1978). This would yield a more general theory, de scribing the variation of grazing rates for crustaceans on prey items of different sizes. The models presented here could also be improved by incorporating the abundance of prey in different size categories as inde pendent variables. This would add infor mation concerning the functional response of zooplankton, which describes the varia tion ofingestion rates in response to changes in prey abundance (Holling 1959, 1965; Schoener 1971; Murdoch and Oaten 1975; Downing 1981). Unfortunately, Gliwicz (1977) did not report such data for the feed ing experiments with cladocerans so that surrogate data had to be used instead. Add ing prey concentration for various size classes as independent variables may ac count for some of the unexplained variance seen by this approximation. Comparisons should also be made with size selectivity of individuals, since lumping of data from dif ferent sized organisms could give rise to some of the apparent size selectivity. The synthesis of information about zoo plankton feeding through the use of empir ical relationships such as those presented here should eventually allow quantitative predictions of loss rates by algae of different sizes. Tests of these predictions against new observations ofgrazing rates on natural par ticles can lead to the improvement of the models. In addition, since these models use the body size of zooplankton as an inde pendent variable, shifts in the distribution of loss rates could be predicted from ob served changes in the size structure of zoo plankton communities. With these models, information from detailed studies offeeding behavior could be used to make quantita tive predictions about the role of zooplank ton in the dynamics of phytoplankton com munities. Edward McCauley2 Department of Biology McGill University 1205 Ave. Dr. Penfield Montreal, Quebec H3A 1B1 2 Present address: Department of Biological Sci ences, University of California, Santa Barbara 93106. Notes John A. Downing Departement de Sciences Biologiques Universite de Montreal C.P. 6128, Succursale 'A' Montreal, Quebec H3C 3J7 211 Foy, R. H. 1980. The influence of surface to volume ratio on the growth rates of planktonic blue-green algae. Br. Phycol. J. 15: 279-289. Friebele, E. S., D. L. Correll, and M. A. Faust. 1978. Relationship between phytoplankton cell size and the rate of orthophosphate uptake: In situ observations on an estuarine population. Mar. Biol. 45: 39-52. References Arnold, D. E. 1971. Ingestion, assimilation, surviv al and reproduction by Daphnia pulex fed seven species of blue-green algae. Limnol. Oceanogr. 16: 906-920. Arnold, E. M., and D. A. Voss. 1981. Numerical behavior of a zooplankton, phytoplankton and phosphorus system. Ecol. Model. 13: 183-193. Banse, K. 1976. Rates of growth, respiration, and photosynthesis of unicellular algae as related to cell size: A review. J. Phycol. 12: 135-140. Berman, M. S., and S. Richman. 1974. The feeding behavior ofDaphnia pulex from Lake Winnebago, Wisconsin. Limnol. Oceanogr. 19: 105-109. Brooks, J. L., and S. I. Dodson. 1965. Predation, body size, and composition of plankton. Science 150: 28-35. Brown, M. P., J. J. McLaughlin, J. M. O'Conner, and K. Wyman. 1982. A mathematical model ofPCB bioaccumulation in plankton. Ecol. Model. 15: 29-47. Burns, C. W. 1968a. Direct observations of mech anisms regulating feeding behavior of Daphnia in lake water. Int. Rev. Gesamten Hydrobiol. 53:83100. . 19686. The relationship between body size of filter-feeding Cladocera and the maximum size of particle ingested. Limnol. Oceanogr. 13: 675678. -. 1969. Relation between filtering rate, tem perature, and body size in four species of Daphnia. Limnol. Oceanogr. 14: 693-700. Chisholm, S. W., R. G. Stross, and P. A. Nobbs. 1975. Environmental and intrinsic control of fil tering and feeding rates in arctic Daphnia. J. Fish. Res. Bd. Can. 32: 219-226. DeMott, W. R. 1982. Feeding selectivities and rel ative ingestion rates of Daphnia and Bosmina. Limnol. Oceanogr. 27: 518-527. Downing, J. A. 1981. In situ foraging responses of three species of littoral cladocerans. Ecol. Monogr. 51: 85-103. , and R. H. Peters. 1980. The effect of body size and food concentration on the in situ filtering rate ofSida crystallina. Limnol. Oceanogr. 25:883895. Draper, N. R., and H. Smith. 1966. Applied regres sion analysis. Wiley. Egloff, D. A., and D. S. Palmer. 1971. Size rela tions of the filtering area of two Daphnia species. Limnol. Oceanogr. 16: 900-905. Elliott, J. M. 1977. Some methods for the statistical analysis of samples of benthic invertebrates. Freshwater Biol. Assoc. Sci. Publ. 25. 156 p. Geller, W. 1975. Die Nahrungsaufnahme von Daphnia pulex in AbhSngigkeit von der Futterkonzentration, der Temperatur, der Korpergrosse und dem Hungerzustand der Tiere. Arch. Hydro biol. Suppl. 48, p. 47-107. Gliwicz,Z.M. 1969. Studies on the feeding ofpelagic zooplankton in lakes with varying trophy. Ekol. Pol. 36: 663-705. . 1970. Calculations of food ration of zooplankton community as an example of using lab oratory data for field conditions. Pol. Arch. Hy drobiol. 17: 169-175. 1977. Food size selection and seasonal succession of filter feeding zooplankton in an eutrophic lake. Ekol. Pol. 25: 179-225. Gujarati, D. 1978. Basic econometrics. McGrawHill. Haney, J. F. 1973. An in situ examination of the grazing activities of natural zooplankton com munities. Arch. Hydrobiol. 72: 87-132. Hocking, R. R. 1976. The analysis and selection of variables in linear regression. Biometrics 32: 149. Holling, C. S. 1959. The components of predation as revealed by a study ofsmall mammal predation of the European Pine Sawfly. Can. Entomol. 91: 293-320. . 1965. The functional response of predators to prey density and its role in mimicry and pop ulation regulation. Mem. Entomol. Soc. Can. 45: 1-60. Jernigan, R. W., and C. P. Tsokos. 1980. A linear stochastic model for phytoplankton production in a marine ecosystem. Ecol. Model. 10: 1-12. Kersting, K., and W. Holtermann. 1973. The feeding of Daphnia magna, studied with the Coul ter Counter. Int. Ver. Theor. Angew. Limnol. Verh. 18: 1434-1440. , and W. van der Leeuw. 1976. The use of the Coulter Counter for measuring the feeding rates of Daphnia magna. Hydrobiologia 49: 233-237. Lam, R. K., and B. W. Frost. 1976. Model of copepod filtering response to changes in size and concentration offood. Limnol. Oceanogr. 21:490500. Lehman, J. T. 1976. The filter-feeder as an optimal forager, and the predicted shapes offeeding curves. Limnol. Oceanogr. 21: 501-516. McCauley, E. 1983. The impact of zooplankton on the dynamics of natural phytoplankton commu nities. Ph.D. thesis, McGill Univ. 139 p. , and F. Briand. 1979. Zooplankton grazing and phytoplankton species richness. Field tests of the predation hypothesis. Limnol. Oceanogr. 24: 243-252. , and J. Kalff. 1981. Empirical relationships 212 Notes between phytoplankton and zooplankton biomass in lakes. Can. J. Fish. Aquat. Sci. 38: 458-463. McMahon, J. W., and F. H. Rigler. 1965. Feeding rate of Daphnia magna Straus in different foods labeled with radioactive phosphorus. Limnol. Oceanogr. 10: 105-114. McQueen, D. J. 1970. Grazing rates and food selec tion in Diaptomus oregonensis (Copepoda) from Marion Lake, British Columbia. J. Fish. Res. Bd. Can. 27: 13-20. Makarewicz, J. C, and G. E. Likens. 1979. Struc ture and function of the zooplankton community of Mirror Lake, New Hampshire. Ecol. Monogr. 49: 109-127. Malone, T. C. 1980. Algal size, p. 433-463. In I. Morris [ed.], The physiological ecology of phyto plankton. Blackwell. Montague, C. L., W. R. Fey, and D. M. Gillespie. 1982. A causal hypothesis explaining predatorprey dynamics in Great Salt Lake, Utah. Ecol. Model. 17: 243-270. Mullin, M. M. 1963. Some factors affecting the feed ing of marine copepods ofthe genus Calanus. Lim nol. Oceanogr. 8: 239-251. Murdoch, W. W., and A. Oaten. 1975. Predation and population stability. Adv. Ecol. Res. 9:1-131. Neill, W. E. 1975. Resource partitioning by com peting microcrustaceans in stable laboratory mi crocosms. Int. Ver. Theor. Angew. Limnol. Verh. 19: 2885-2890. Peters, R. H. 1984. Methods for the study offeeding, filtering and assimilation by zooplankton, p. 336— 412. In J. A. Downing and F. H. Rigler [eds.], A manual on methods for the assessment of second ary productivity in fresh waters, 2nd ed. IBP Handbook 17. Blackwell. , and J. A. Downing. 1984. Empirical analysis ofzooplankton filtering and feeding rates. Limnol. Oceanogr. 29: 763-784. Peterson, B. J., J. E. Hobbie, and J. F. Haney. 1978. Daphnia grazing on natural bacteria. Limnol. Oceanogr. 23: 1039-1044. Pieters, E. P., C. E. Gates, J. H. Matis, and W. L. Sterling. 1977. Small sample comparisons of different estimators of negative binomial param eters. Biometrics 33: 718-723. Porter, K. G. 1973. Selective grazing and differential digestion of algae by zooplankton. Nature 244: 179-180. RlCHMAN, S., S. A. BOHON, AND S. E. ROBINS. 1980. Grazing interactions among freshwater calanoid copepods. Am. Soc. Limnol. Oceanogr. Spec. Symp. 3: 219-233. New England. SCHLESINGER, D. A., L. A. MOLOT, AND B. J. SHUTER. 1981. Specific growth rates of freshwater algae in relation to cell size and light intensity. Can. J. Fish. Aquat. Sci. 38: 1032-1058. Schoener, T. W. 1971. Theory of feeding strategies. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 2: 369-404. Sjoberg, S. 1980. Zooplankton feeding and queuing theory. Ecol. Model. 10: 215-225. Smith, R. E., and J. Kalff. 1982. Size dependent phosphorus uptake kinetics and cell quota in phy toplankton. J. Phycol. 18: 275-284. Vanderploeg, H. A. 1981. Seasonal particle-size se lection by Diaptomus sicilis in offshore Lake Mich igan. Can. J. Fish. Aquat. Sci. 38: 504-517. Wulff, F. W. 1980. Animal community structure and energy budget calculations of a Daphnia mag na (Straus) population in relation to the rock pool environment. Ecol. Model. 11: 179-225. Submitted: 24 October 1983 Accepted: 5 July 1984