HORMONAL CORRELATES BEHAVIOR OF PARENTAL IN COOPERATIVELY AND HELPING BREEDING HARRIS' HAWKS (PARABUTEO UNICINCTUS) CAROL MASTERSVLECK,• NORA A. MAYS,t JAMESW. DAWSON,2 AND ARTHUR g. GOLDSMITH 3 •Departmentof EcologyandEvolutionary Biology,Universityof Arizona,Tucson, Arizona85721 USA, 2School of Renewable Natural Resources, Universityof Arizona,Tucson, Arizona85721 USA, and 3Agricultural andFoodResearch Council,Research Groupon Photoperiodism andReproduction, Department of Zoology, University of Bristol,BristolBS8lUG, UnitedKingdom ABSTRACT.--We measuredplasmalevelsof progesteroneand prolactinin free-living adult breeders,adult-plumagedhelpers,and juvenal-plumagedhelpersof the cooperativelybreeding Harris' Hawk, Parabuteo unicinctus, at different stagesof the nestingcycle.Overall, progesteronelevels did not vary significantlywith sex,neststage,or behavioralrole, but they increasedsignificantlywith handling time. Progesterone levelswere highest(2-4 ng/ml) in breedingfemalesjust beforeegg laying. Prolactinlevelsdid not vary with handlingtime but were significantlyelevatedin breedingfemalesduring the nest-buildingstage(14.1ng/ ml) and in both breedingmales(8.4 ng/ml) and females(25.5 ng/ml) during incubation. Bothsexesof breedersincubatethe eggs,but the femalesdo the bulk of incubationand only they developa broodpatch.Prolactinlevelsin the breedersdeclinedimmediatelyafter the eggshatched,in contrastto the patternseenin many other altricial species.Prolactinlevels in the adult-plumagedmale helpersrosesignificantlyafter the eggshatched(9.1 ng/ml). At this time thesebirdsbring morefooditemsfrom groupkills to the nestlingsthan any other group members.The elevatedprolactinlevels in the adult-plumagedhelpersmay facilitate the helpingbehaviorthat they exhibittowardthe nestlingsand fledglings.Received 8 August 1991, acceptedI April 1991. HELPINGbehavior conventionally refers to parentlikebehaviordirectedtowardyoungthat are not the offspringof the bird displayingthe behavior. It has been one of the main foci of studies on cooperativelybreeding speciesbecausehelping behavior seemsto entail costs, often without clear-cutbenefits(seereviewsby Woolfenden and Fitzpatrick 1984, Brown 1987, Koenig and Mumme 1987). Investigatorshave soughtto explainnot only the presentselective advantagesof helping behavior, but also the ological level to augment the behavior. This would supportthe hypothesisthat helping behavior is adaptive. The socialsystemin the Harris' Hawk is com- plex and hasbeen describedin detail for populationsin Arizona (Dawsonand Mannan 1991a) and New Mexico (Bednarz 1987). In Arizona, evolutionaryoriginsof helper systems,and it groupsizerangesfrom a breedingpair to 7 birds (meangroupsize= 3.8).The breedingmaleand female (or alphabirds)build the nest,the female lays the eggs,both male and female incubate, shadeor brood the nestlings,and feed the nest- remains controversialwhether helping behavior is selectedfor, or is simply a result of mis- cubateapproximatelyone-fourthasmuch asfe- directed parental behavior (cf. Jamieson and Craig 1987, Jamieson 1989, Ligon and Stacy 1989). The proximate mechanismsthat control such parentlike behaviors,however, have not been investigated in any cooperativelybreed- lings and fledglings.Breedingmalesonly inmales (Dawson and Mannan 1991a), and the maledoesnot developa broodpatch(pets.obs.). The alpha female rarely leavesthe nest area during the nestingseasonand is providedfood fromgroupkillsby her mateandthe otherbirds ing species.We investigatedthe hormonalcor- in the group. relatesof parentalbehaviorin the cooperatively We call all other birds in the group helpers breeding Harris' Hawk (Parabuteounicinctus). even though the extent to which individual Evidencefor an endocrinesystemthat promotes hawks help differs (see Dawson and Mannan helping behavior in nonbreedingbirds would 1991a for details) and some of them breed suggestthat selectionhas operatedat a physi- ($heehyet al. MS).In particular,few groups(0638 The Auk 108:638-648. July 1991 July1991] Prolactin ina Cooperative Breeder 639 11%,depending on the year studied) contain and, in particular, we comparehormone levels another breeding female (termed an alpha-2fe- of breederswith thoseof helpers. We choseto male;Dawsonand Mannan 1991a),but this type examine the levels of these two hormones bewas absentfrom the groupswe studied. Two causethey (often in synergywith estrogenand classesof helpers have been described,distin- testosterone) are the hormones most often as- guished by behavior (Dawson and Mannan 1991a):betamalesand gammabirds,who may be sociatedwith parentalcare(incubationand care of young)in birds(reviewedin Balthazart1983, either males or females. Beta males are the most Goldsmith 1983,Silver and Cooper 1983,Brown common,are found in approx. 62% of groups, and are alwaysin adult plumage.Gammabirds are usuallyin juvenalplumage(1 yr old or less) and may be either male (66%)or female (34%). They are in adult plumage(> 1 yr old) approx. 32% of the time. Gamma birds are usually, but not always, offspring of the breeders in the group,whereasbetamalesare not relatedto the breeders. We did not divide our helpers into beta and gamma types becausewe did not always have enough information, but classified them as either in adult plumage or in juvenal 1985, Buntin 1985). Helpers rarely comeinto direct contactwith the eggsor young (Dawsonand Mannan 1991a). Betamalesare allowed to bring food to within 50 m of the nest before transferring it to one of the alpha birds, who delivers it to the incubating female or the nestlings.Gammabirds bring food to the nest areabut transfer it to the alpha birdsat least150m from the nest.Occasionally the beta males will incubate or bring food directly to the nest, and gamma birds will also occasionallyattempt to incubate and feed the young directly, but they are usuallysupplanted from the nestby the breeding pair. All birds in the group detect and harasspredators,defend the nesting territory from both predatorsand conspecifics(Dawson and Mannan 1991b),and take part in group hunting. Participationin cooperativehunting efforts(Mader 1975,Bednarz 1988, Bednarzand Ligon 1988) and delivering prey items to the nest area are probably the largest contributions that helpers make to the breeding effort. In the accompanyingpaper(Mays et al. 1991), were METHOD• Fieldmethods.--Thestudyareas,animals,blood collection, field measurements, and observations were describedin detailelsewhere(Mayset al. 1991).Briefly, we usedbal-chatritraps(Bergerand Mueller 1959) to capture 157 hawks north of Tucson,Arizona, in uplandSonoranDeserthabitatbetweenMay 1985and August 1987. Most hawks were trapped during their nestingseason(Februaryto July). Bloodsamplesfor hormonalanalyseswere takenfrom a wing vein, and plumage. Approximately80% of adult-plum- eachbird wasbandedwith a unique color-bandcomaged males were probably beta birds, and all binationand weighedbeforerelease.Approx.80%of adult-plumagedfemale helpersand all juvenal- bloodsampleswere collectedbetween0530and 1300, plumagedbirdswere probablygammabirds(see but becauseof the difficulty in trapping birds, we did some trapping in the afternoon and three samples details in Mays et al. 1991). we described how testosterone, estradiol-17•, collected after 1800. Most birds during the breeding seasonwere examined for evidence of a brood patch, which was scoredas 0 = absent:breastcompletely feathered; 1 = developing:breastbeginningto losefeathers;2 = present: breast fully defeathered, but not vascularized;3 = functional:breastfully defeatheredand vas- cularized;or 4 = regressing: breastdefeathered,but no longervascularizedand beginningto sproutnew feathers.In 1985we noticed reddening and swelling of the outercloacaof somefemalesduring the breeding seasonvery similar to that describedin Alpine Accentor (Prunella collaris) females (Nakamura 1990: fig. 2c).In 1986and 1987we recordedthe appearance of the cloacalopeningof all hawksas either dry and small, or red and swollen. Blood sampleswere stored on ice in the field and centrifugedlater the sameday.The plasmawasstored at -20øC until analyzed by radioimmunoassay.We recordedthe maximum possibletime the hawk was ensnaredon the trap and the time taken to obtain the bloodsample,and their sumis the maximumpossible handling time for that bird. The mean maximumhandling time was 70 min (+61 min SD). Maximum possiblehandlingtimeswerehighbecause we wereforced to remain away from the traps for long periodsof time to increasethe probabilityof capturinga bird. and luteinizing hormone (LH) levels are corWe identified hawks as either male or female, based relatedwith breeder/helperstatusand neststage on body mass,and as either in juvenal or adult plumin both males and females. Here we describe the correlationbetween progesteroneand prolactinand parentalbehaviorin individual birds age.Femalesare approx.30%larger,and there is essentially no overlap between the sexes(Hamerstrom and Hamerstrom1978).Birdsretainthe juvenalplum- 640 VrECK•r AL. age until after the next year's breeding season.We identified the behavioralrole (helper or breeder)and stage of the nesting cycle for 105 of the hawks for which we had bloodsamplesby observingthe hawks from enclosedblinds near the nests(details in Mays et al. 1991).The neststagewas calculatedrelative to the date of the first-laid egg. Samplescollected>50 daysbeforethe first-laidegg were consideredto be from the nonbreedingstageof the annual cycles(approx. August through early January),thosecollected during the 50 daysbeforethe first egg were from the nest-building and gamete-productionstage, those collectedduring the next 35 days were from the incubationstage(Mader 1975),and thosecollectedfrom 36 to 100 days after the first-laid egg were from the stagewhen young were being fed. The chicksremain in or near the nest for approx.45 daysafter hatching (Mader 1975),and group memberscontinuedto feed the young after fledging for several months (pets. obs.).The meandateof the first-laideggwas30 March (+17 days SD), and the range was from 21 February through 5 May. Radioimmunoassay of hormones.--Plasma samplesfor progesteroneanalysis(150-200 •1) were equilibrated overnight with approximately 1,000 cpm of 3H-progesteronefor determination of recoveryand then extracted in 4 ml hexane:ethyl acetate(98:2). The extractwasdried under nitrogen and then resuspended in phosphatebuffer. Duplicate samplesof the resuspensionwere analyzedby radioimmunoassay following the procedureof Wingfield and Farner (1975). The progesteroneantiserum used was Pl1-192 (En- [Auk, Vol. 108 categories(see Mays et al. 1991: table 1). We used three-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) on logtransformed data to test for the effects of sex, nest role, and handling time on hormone levels in each blood sample(data from all years combined).After separationby sex we used a two-way ANOVA to examine the effectsof nest role and handling time (Table 1A). For the prolactin data in which there wasno significanteffectof handling time, we then did oneway ANOVAs on eachbehavioralrole category,testing whether meansdiffered betweenneststagesfor each role (Table lB). One-way ANOVAS were also done for each nest stage,testing whether prolactin levels differed between behavioral roles during the sameneststage(Table 1C).Eachvariable that showed a significanteffect (P < 0.05) was further analyzed with Bonferroni multiple comparisontests(BON) to find which meansdiffered. We used Spearman'scoefficientof rank correlation(rs)to analyzebroodpatch scoresor cloacalswellingand hormonedata.For these testswe used only brood patch scoresbetween 0 and 3, indicative of progressingdevelopment. RESULTS Progesterone.--Progesteronelevels did not differ between males and females (three-way ANOVA, F = 1.29, P = 0.26, n = 97). In both sexesthere were significantdifferencesin progesteronelevels in different individuals, but the differencescanbe statisticallyaccountedfor docrine Sciences). The standard curve covered the by handling times (Fig. 1 and Table 1A). For rangefrom 1.96to 1,000pg and wasrun in triplicate. Lowestdetectableconcentrationwas 64 pg/ml plas- example, an exceptionallyhigh progesterone ma.Intrasssayand interassaycoefficientsof variation level in the plasmaof the onejuvenal-plumaged (SD/mean x 100) were 7.6% and 9.7%, respectively. male helper sampledduring the incubationpeMean recovery was 49%. riod was correlatedwith a long handling time Prolactin concentrationsin plasma from 102 birds (180 min) for this individual. Plasma progesfor which we knew the neststageand behavioralrole terone levels increasewith handling time in were measuredat the University of Bristol using a serially bled Harris' Hawks (Mays and Vleck heterologous radioimmunoassay systemdevelopedby 1987),probablydue to progesteronereleasefrom McNeilly et al. (1978)and modifiedby Goldsmithand the adrenal cortex where it is produced as a Hall (1980). After confirming that a plasma sample (Assenmacher 1973), from a Harris' Hawk assayedat five doubling dilu- precursorto corticosterone a hormone released in response to stress (Hartions gave a dilution curve parallel to that of the standard(NIADDK ovine prolactin PS-12), all sam- vey et al. 1984). In femalesduring the nest-building stage,the ples were assayedin duplicate 20 •1 volumes. Intraand interassaycoefficientsof variation were 5.2% (n mean progesteronelevel in the breeding birds = 10) and 12.3%(n = 10), respectively,and the lowest was about twice that found in adult or juvenaldetectableconcentrationwas 2.0 ng/ml. plumagedhelpers(Fig. 1). We do not think these Statistical analysis.--Foranalysiswe useda variable differencescan be accountedfor solely by difcalled nestroleto group the data. This grouping variferences in handling time becausethe mean able is a compositeof stageof the nest with which eachbird wasassociated(nonbreeding,nestbuilding, handling time during the nest-building stage incubatingeggs,or feedingyoung)and that individu- for the breeding femaleswith high progesteral'srole andage(adultbreeder,adult-plumagedhelp- one (62 min) wassimilar to that for the juvenaler, or juvenal-plumagedhelper). This compositevari- plumagedhelpers(59 min) and actuallylower ableseparatesthe valuesfor eachsexinto 12 possible than that for the adult helpers (90 min), al- July1991] Prolactin in a Cooperative Breeder 641 642 VLECK ETAL. Non 4 Neet Feedin Incube- Non Young Breedin; [Auk,Vol. 108 Neet Incube- BreedIn. • Buildingting Feeding Youn= ,, -150 -100 Time -50 relative 0 to first 50 egg 100 -100 (days} -50 o 50 100 Tlms relative to first egg (days) Fig. 1. Mean plasma progesteronelevels in Harris' Hawk males (left panel) and females (right panel) during each nest stage.Key: [] = adult breeder,ß = adult-plumagedhelper, and /x = juvenal-plumaged helper. Error bars indicate + 1 SE. Sample sizesand statisticalsignificanceare in Table 1. though the handling times do not differ significantlyamongthesegroups(one-wayANOVA, F = 0.05, P > 0.59). Prolactin.--Prolactin levels varied significantly with sexand nest role in the ANOVAs, but not with handling time (Fig. 2 and Table 1A). We could detect no tendencyin our data for prolactinlevels to increasewith handling time. A regressionof prolactinlevels from 97 birds plotted againstmaximumpossiblehandling time for eachsample(5-225 min) had a slope(0.001)not significantlydifferentfrom 0 (t = 0.14, P > 0.89). Overall, values were lower in males than in females (three-way ANOVA, F = 8.33, P < 0.005, n = 91). In the breeding were no significantchangesin prolactin with neststagein the juvenal-plumagedbirdsnor in the adult-plumagedfemale helpers (Table lB). We do not think the differences in prolactin levels between the breedersand helpers during incubation or care of young are due to an undetectedeffect of the long handling times for the different groupsbecausethe meanhandling times did not differ among the nest roles for both sexes(one-wayANOVA, F = 0.53,P > 0.74 for incubation;F = 0.81, P > 0.55 for feeding young). In addition, it seemsunlikely that differencesin the time of day sampleswere collected for the different nest roles could have biasedour interpretationbecauseall but one of birds, prolactin levels varied with nest stage 31 samplesfrombirdsin groupscaringfor young (Table lB), and rosesignificantlyduring incu- were collected before 1200. bation in both males (BON, t = 2.63, P < 0.05) Broodpatch and cloacalswelling.--Although and females(BON, t = 2.87, P < 0.05). The mean breedingmalesincubate,none of the 58 males level in female breedersduring incubationwas examined showed any sign of a brood patch significantlyhigherthanthatin helperfemales exceptone breeding male, who had slight, nonat this stage(Table1C;BON, t = 3.53,P < 0.05). vascularizeddefeathering of the breast at the Prolactinwasalsoelevatedin breeding females end of incubation(stage1). Only breeding feduring the nest-buildingstagecomparedwith malesdevelopedbrood patches(n = 23); none helperfemalesat the samestage(Table1C;BON, of the 16 female helpers examined before or t = 2.60, P < 0.05). during incubationhad a broodpatch.Breeding Levelsof prolactinfell in both breedingmales femalesbeganto showa developingbroodpatch and femalessoonafter hatchingof the eggs,but asearlyas55 daysbeforethe firstegg,and most there wasa significantelevationin prolactinin had a fully functionalbrood patchby the time the adult male helpers during care of young the eggs were laid, although two incubating (Table lB; BON, t = 2.05, P < 0.05). During this femalesshowedonly a stage-2brood patchdurstage, the prolactin levels in the adult male ing incubation. The brood patch began to rehelperswere significantlyhigher than thosein gresswithin a few daysafter hatching.In breedthe other males (Table 1C; BON, t = 2.05, P < ing females during the nest-building and 0.05), whereas the prolactin levels in the fe- incubating stages,there was a positive corremales did not differ from one another. There lation between the extent of development of July1991] Prolactin in a Cooperative Breeder Non Breeding Nest Building Incubiting Feeding Young 5O _= Non Nest Breeding Building ' Incubering Feeding Young 30- ß t....... 643 /_ _e • , Time relative to first egg (days) Time relative to firat egg (days) Fig. 2. Plasmaprolactinlevelsin Harris'Hawk males(left panel)and females(right panel)duringeach nest stage.Symbols(seeFig. 1 legend) representvaluesfrom individuals.The mean levels for individuals with the samebehavioralrole during eachneststageare connectedwith lines(solidline for adult breeders; dottedline for adult-plumaged helpers;dashedline for juvenal-plumaged helpers);errorbarsindicate_+1 SE. the brood patchand prolactinlevels (rs= 0.66, ing female Harris' Hawks were found in the P < 0.001,n = 14), althoughwe cannotdeter- week beforeegglaying (LH datain Mayset al. mine whether this relationshipis independent 1991),althoughthe effectsof handlingtimeand of the effectof time on brood-patch develop- probablediel rhythmsin hormonelevelsmake ment and prolactin levels over the sameintervalssincethe partial correlationbetweenbrood patchand prolactin,holdingthe effectof time constant,is lower (r = 0.47, P > 0.05). interpretation of the data difficult. In captivebreeding American Kestrels(Falcosparverious), peaksin progesteronereaching 2-4 ng/ml are closelyassociatedwith egg laying (Rehder et Cloacalswellingalsooccurredin breeding al. 1986). females at about the same time that the brood patchwas developing.Of 13 female breeders, 10showedcloacalswellingfrom57daysbefore the firsteggto 17daysafterthe firstegg.None showedany cloacalswellingafter the incubation period (n = 4). The presenceof cloacal swellingwaspositivelycorrelatedwith prolactin levels (rs = 0.58, P = 0.022, n = 15), but it wasnot significantlycorrelatedwith neststage or the levelsof luteinizing hormone,testosterone, estradiol(datafrom Mays et al. 1991)or with progesterone.Only 1 of 10 adult female helpersshowedany cloacalswelling,and no malesdid at any time (n = 41). Our data indicate no correlation between plasma progesterone and parental behaviors suchas incubationor careof young in Harris' Hawks. Plasmaprogesteroneis not elevatedin breedingmalehawksthat incubateand carefor young, although a changein progesterone-receptordensity,possiblymediatedby the previously elevatedtestosteronelevels (seeBrown 1985),might have altered the effectof proges- terone even if plasmalevels did not change. There doesnot seemto be an importantrole for progesteronein incubation in domestic fowl (Bedrak et al. 1981), but it does influence the timing of incubation bouts in African Turtle Doves (Streptopelia roseogrisea; Silver and NorDISCUSSION gren 1987).In the Harris' Hawk, progestrone couldbe involved in brood-patchformationas Progesterone.--In birds, progesterone is suggested by Jones(1971),possiblyin conjuncthought to be involved in ovulation. For ex- tion with elevated estradiol since only the ample,in the domesticfowl (Gallusgallus)there breeding femalesdevelop a brood patch, and isa positivefeedbackrelationshipbetweenpro- only they have elevatedestradiol(Mays et al. gesterone andLH thatdetermines theovulatory 1991) and progesteronebefore incubationbesequence;progesteronelevels usually peak 4gins. 6 h beforeovulation(reviewedin Kamiyoshi Prolactin.--Therolesof prolactinin avian reandTanaka1983).A similarrelationshipprob- productionand the mechanismsthat control its ably existsin Harris' Hawks. All elevatedlevels secretionare not well understoodand appear (2-4 ng/ml) of progesterone and LH in breed- to vary among species.Prolactin has been im- 644 VrECK ETAI.. plicatedin the developmentof the broodpatch (Jones1971),the developmentof photorefractoriness (Ebling et al. 1982, Goldsmith and Nicholls 1984), migration, molt, and fat deposition (Meier 1975)aswell asparentalbehavior (Balthazart 1983, Goldsmith 1983, Silver and Cooper 1983, Brown 1985, Buntin 1985). Elevationsin prolactin have been correlatedwith incubation and brooding in many species,as well aswith feeding of young in the European Starling (Sturnusvulgaris;Dawson and Goldsmith 1982) and columbiformbirds that producecropmilk (Goldsmithet al. 1981),andwith the displayof antipredatorybehaviorin Willow Ptarmigan(Lagopus I. lagopus; Pedersen1989). Elevatedprolactin is clearly associatedwith incubation behavior in birds. Whether or not prolactininitiatesparentalbehavioror simply maintains it, however, has been of interest. In addition, whether elevated prolactin levels are the cause or the result of incubation behavior is not always clear. In some species,artificial extensionor termination of the incubationperiod affectsprolactinlevels,which suggests that the act of incubation influences prolactin secretion (Silverin and Goldsmith 1984,Oring et [Auld, VoL108 involved in developmentof a functionalbrood patch and incubation in this species.Prolactin is alsoelevatedin breeding male Harris' Hawks during incubation,although not as much as in females.The significant elevation in prolactin we detected in all incubating hawks implies that our methods allow us to detect functional changesin plasma prolactin levels associated with the breedingcycles,even thoughthe long handling times for many of thesebirds might be expectedto have changedprolactin levels independentlyof the breedingcycle.Elevated prolactin levels may also be involved in the developmentof the swellingand reddeningof the cloacalregion in the females,but the functional significanceof this in Harris' Hawks is unknown.Nakamura(1990)suggested a display of cloacalswelling was used by female Alpine Accentorsto solicit copulationsfrom the male. Prolactinlevelsin the breedingHarris' Hawks decreaseimmediately after incubationto levels not different from those found in the nonbreed- ing season.In other altricial and semiprecocial species,prolactin levels usually remain relatively elevatedaslong asthe birdsare brooding nonthermoregulating young (Dawson and al. 1988).Similarly,manipulationof chickages Goldsmith 1982; Goldsmith 1982; Hector and caninfluenceprolactinlevelsin broodingPied Goldsmith1985;Oring et al. 1986,1988;Myers Flycatchers(Ficedulahypoleuca;Silverin and et al. 1989; Ketterson et al. 1990). Levels may Goldsmith1990).In otherspecies,prolactinlev- decrease at about the same time that the chicks els seem to be determined by an endogenous begin to thermoregulateand no longer require scheduleor by day length, relatively indepen- extensivebroodingby the parent(Silverinand dently of the bird's exposureto nest and eggs Goldsmith 1984, 1990;Oring et al. 1986, 1988), (Eblinget al. 1982,HectorandGoldsmith1985). but prolactinlevelsoften remainelevatedcomUsuallylevelsof prolactinarehigherin females pared with nonbreedinglevels as long as the than in males,whether or not the male partic- parentsare caring for young. Prolactin levels ipatesin incubation(Dawsonand Goldsmith remain elevated between broods in multiple1982,Hiatt et al. 1987).Prolactinlevelsare high- broodedWhite-crownedSparrows(Zonotrichia pugetensis; Hiatt et al. 1987)and Song er in malesthan in femalesin speciesin which leucophrys the male incubatesmore than the female, e.g. Sparrows (Melospizamelodia;Wingfield and the SpottedSandpiper(Actitismacularia; Oring Goldsmith 1990), although not in multipleet al. 1986) and Wilson's Phalarope (Phalaropus tricolor;Oring et al. 1988). On the other hand, prolactinlevelsneed not necessarilybe elevated for incubationbehaviorto be expressed.Turkeys(Meleagris gallopavo) deprivedof their nest show a decline in prolactin levels, but will in- brooded canaries (Serinus canaria; Goldsmith 1982).The pattern of prolactinchangeswe observedin breedingHarris'Hawksresembles that in precocialgalliform and anseriformspecies (reviewed in Goldsmith 1983) in which the young can thermoregulateand leave the nest soonafter hatching.In thesespecies,prolactin thoughprolactinlevelsdo not becomeelevated usually falls to nonbreeding levels as soon as the young are hatched. until later (Zadworny et al. 1985). The fall in prolactin in breeding Harris' In Harris' Hawks, prolactin levels are elevated in breeding femalesbefore egg laying and Hawks after the eggshatch, even though the during incubationin a mannersimilarto other altricial young remain in the nestapproximatebirds.Presumably,high levelsof prolactinare ly 45 days,suggeststhat the high levelsof procubate as soon as returned to their nest, even July1991] Prolactin in a Cooperative Breeder lactin associated with incubation need not be maintainedto ensurebroodingand feedingof the young.The chicksare altricialand the parentswill broodthe youngin the nestat night, but relativelylittle broodingoccursduringthe daybecause ambienttemperatures in May and June(the normal monthsfor hatchlingsto be in the nest)are relativelyhigh comparedwith higher-latitudespecies.In fact, the chicksare 645 ng/ml) was higher than that in either male (5.6 ng/ml) or female (7.3 ng/ml) breeders at the same time. This increase occurs at a time when the male beta helpers,if presentin a group, actually provide more food for the nestlings than either of the breedingbirds. For example, Dawson and Mannan (1991a) determined which individualsbroughtfoodto the nestareain four Harris'Hawk groupsof fiveindividualsmatched often shadedby the parentsrather than brood- for groupcomposition(a breedingpair, a beta ed, presumablyto keep chicksfrom overheat- male, and a male and female gammabird). In ing. In addition,becausefooditemsbroughtto 136 food deliveries, the adult beta male transthe nestlingsare large (e.g. parts of rabbits), ported the food 37% of the time, whereas the adultsmakerelativelyfew feedingtripsto the breeding male performed this task only apnest per day (Dawson and Mannan 1991a, refproximately 30% of the time, the breeding feerencestherein). Consequentlythe intensity of male only 3% of the time, and the two other parentalbehaviordisplayedby an individual gamma helpers combined, >30% of the time. Harris'Hawk (in termsof timespentin the nest We havefew samplesfromadultfemalehelpand feedingtrips to the nest)may be low com- ers during incubation and care of young, but paredwith altricialspecies in general,especial- their prolactin levelsdo not seemto be elevated ly thosewithout helpers,and could accountfor as in adult male helpers. This may not be surthe relatively low levelsof prolactinfound in prising becausefemale gamma helpers spend the parents at this time. the leastamount of time of any group member The contributionthat the helpersmake to- near the nest or feeding young, althoughthey ward provisioningthe youngmaydecreasethe participate in group hunting (Dawson and effort the breedingpair mustexpendto raise Mannan 1991a). The rise in prolactin in the adult-plumaged theyoung.In a surveyof feedingratesof young in 15 cooperativelybreeding species,Reyer male helper Harris' Hawks indicates that ele(1990)foundthat in speciesthatmakefew feed- vationin prolactinassociated with careof young ing trips (<10 per hour), the contribution the canbe uncoupledfrom the earlier prolactinelparentsmaketo feedingyoungisreducedwhen evation associatedwith nest building and inhelpersare present.In speciesthat make >20 cubationin thiscooperativelybreedingspecies. feedingtrips per hour, parentsdo not reduce We cannotrule out a relation between previtheir effortwhen helpersare present.If Harris' ously elevated sexsteroidsand the subsequent Hawkbreeders providelesscareforyoungwhen elevation in prolactin (see Goldsmith 1983 and helpersare present,one might expectan in- Buntin 1985), becausethese adult-plumaged earverserelationshipbetweengroupsizeandpro- helpershaveelevatedLH and testosterone lactin levels in the breedersduring the nest- lier in the nestingcycle(Mays et al. 1991).Luling/fledgling stage.We couldnot demonstrate teinizing hormones and testosteronedecrease sucha relationship(Fig. 3). In Pied Flycatchers in both the adult malehelpersand the breeders (Silverin and Goldsmith1984)and Dark-eyed during the incubation stage.This seemsto folJuncos(Juncohyemalis;Ketterson et al. 1990), low the pattern describedby Brown (1985) for there is alsono discernabledifferencein pro- many speciesin which both sexesshowparenlactinlevelsin femalesthat raiseyoungalone tal behavior. Elevated testosterone in males is and thosethat are aided by a mate. Thus the probablyassociated with defenseand courtship level of prolactinsecretiondoesnot seemto be earlyin the nestingseason,and it possiblyacts modulatedin any preciseway by the level of as a primer for parental behavior;but later as parental effort expended. the level of prolactinrises(possiblystimulated The mostsurprisingthing that we foundwas by the presenceof eggsor young),the testosthat prolactin levels rose significantlyin the teronelevel falls. The effectsof exogenousproadult male helpersduring the nestling/fledg- lactin on behaviorin nonbreedingindividuals ling stage;at the sametimeprolactindecreased have been demonstrated before (reviewed in in thebreedingbirds(Fig.2). In fact,the mean Goldsmith 1983,Buntin 1985),but we are aware level at thistime in the adultmalehelpers(9.1 of no previous report of a correlationbetween 646 VLEaC E'r^L. [Auk,Vol. 108 12 [] Females ß Males 10 ß [] ß [] ß ß ß ß [] ß 2 3 4 5 [] 6 7 8 Group Size Fig. 3. Plasmaprolactinlevelsin breedingmaleand femaleHarris' Hawksduring the stagewhen groups are caring for their young (36-100 daysafter the first-laid egg) plotted in relation to group sizes.Symbols represent values from individuals. endogenousprolactin levels and parentlike be- havior in a free-living, nonbreedingbird. Kettersonet al. (1990)measuredprolactinin DarkeyedJuncosduring the nestlingstagein natural fathersand in maleswho had replacedthe original male in a pair after the young hatched. They reported that prolactin levels in two replacementmales that were helping the female raise young were not different from those in replacementmalesthat were not feeding adopted young. The natural fathers who fed their own young had levels of prolactinhigher than thoseof the replacementmales.Interpretation of the data was confoundedby seasonaleffects sight or sound of young birds in the nest as suggestedby Woolfendenand Fitzpatrick1984), the elevated prolactin may increasethe probability that thesehelperswill exhibit parentlike behaviors even though they are not likely to be closelyrelated to the young they help. It would be interestingto know whether helpers display this rise in prolactin when caring for young in cooperativelybreedingspeciesin general, and if this differs from the responseof nonbreeding individuals to eggs or young in noncooperativelybreeding species.If this were the case,it is likely that selection has acted at a physiological level to alter behavior in coon prolactin levels. operativelybreedingspecies,althoughthis does If prolactin functions to facilitate provision- not allow us to saywith certainty whether care ing of the hatchlingsand fledglingsin Harris' of young by helpersis a uniquely evolved charHawks, we suggestthat there is a physiological acteristicof cooperativebreeders, nor specifibasisfor helping behavior in this cooperatively cally why helping behavioris adaptive.Experbreeding bird. Although we don't know what imental manipulation of prolactin levels in the environmental stimulus is for the rise in helpers may provide a tool to alter helping beprolactin in adult male helpers (it may be the havior and therebyallow direct testingof hy- July1991] Prolactin ina Cooperative Breeder 647 nusvulgaris)during the annual cycleand in re- pothesesconcerningcostsand benefitsof helping behavior. lation to nesting,incubationand rearing young. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol.48: 213-221. DAWSON,J. W., & R. W. MANNAN. 1991a. Dominance ACKNOWLEDGMENTS hierarchiesand helper contributionsin Harris' Hawks. We thank all those individuals who helped with the fieldwork, including Rick Bowers,Ellen Weintraub, Jeff White, Lynn Oliphant, Bill Mannan, Bob Scheibe and other members of the Tucson Audubon Society.We acknowledgethe loan of equipmentfrom OscarWard, Holly Hobart,Mable Mays,and Stephen Russell.Dave Vleck and Mike Moore critically read the manuscript, and Richard Straussoffered advice on statistics.Early in the study,fundswere provided by Arizona Wildlife Foundation,JamesR. Silliman Memorial Fund, Sigma Xi, and the Western Bird Banding Associationto Nora Mays. Major funding was provided by NSF grant BSN-8606548to Carol Vleck. Auk 108: 649-660. ,& . 1991b. The role of territoriality in the socialorganizationof Harris' Hawks.Auk 108: 661-672. EnLING,F. J.P., A. R. GOLDSMITH,• B. K. FOLLETT. 1982. Plasmaprolactinandluteinizinghormone during photoperiodically induced testicular growthand regression in starlings(Sturnus vulgaris).Gen.Comp.Endocrinol.48:485-490. GOLDSMITH,A. R. 1982. Plasma concentrations of prolactinduringincubation andparentalfeeding throughoutrepeatedbreedingcyclesin canaries (Serinus canarius). J. Endocrinol.94: 51-59. --. 1983. Prolactinin avian reproductivecycles. Pp.375-387inHormonesandbehaviourin higher vertebrates (J.Balthazart,E.Pr6ve,andR. Gilles, Eds.).Berlin, Springer-Verlag. LITERATURE CITED , C. EDWARDS, M. KOTRUCU,& R. SILVER. 1981. ASSEN•V•,CH•,I. Concentrations of prolactinand luteinizinghor- 1973. The peripheral endocrine monein plasmaof dovesin relation to incubation glands.Pp. 184-286 in Avian biology,vol. 3 (D. S. Farher,J. R. King, and K. C. Parkes,Eds.).New York, Academic Press. BALTHAZART, J. C. 1983. Hormonal correlates of be- havior. Pp. 221-365 in Avian biology,vol. 7 (D. S. Farher,J. R. King, and K. C. Parkes,Eds.).New York, Academic Press. BœDNARZ, J. C. 1987. Pair and group reproductive success,polyandry, and cooperativebreeding in and developmentof the crop gland.J. Endocrinol. 90: 437-443. --, & M. HALL. 1980. Prolactin concentrations in the pituitary gland and plasmaof Japanese quail in relation to photoperiodicallyinduced sexualmaturationand egg laying. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 42: 449-454. , T. J. NICHOLLS.1984. Prolactin is associated Hawks. Auk 104: 393-404. with the developmentof photorefractoriness in intact,castrated,and testosterone-implanted star1988. Cooperativehunting in Harris' Hawk (Parabuteounicinctus).Science239: 1525-1527. lings.Gen. Comp.Endocrinol.54:247-255. F., & F. HAMERSTROM.1978. External --, & J.D. LIGON. 1988. A studyof the ecological HAMERSTROM, sex characteristics of Harris' Hawks in winter. basesof cooperativebreedingin the Harris' Hawk. RaptorResearch12(1/2): 1-14. Ecology69: 1176-1187. Harris' BEDRAK,E., S. HARVEX, & A. CHADWICK. 1981. Con- centrationsof pituitary, gonadaland adrenalhormonesin serumof layingandbroodyWhite Rock Hens (Gallusdomesticus). J. Encocrinol. 89: 197204. BœRGœ•, D. D., & H. C. MUELLœ•. 1959. The bal-chatri: a trap for the birds of prey. Bird-banding30: 18- Stressand adrenal function. J. Exp. Zool. 232: 633-645. Hœc?oR,J. A. L., & A. R. GOLDSMrrH.1985. The role of prolactin during incubation: comparative studiesof three Diomedeaalbatrosses.Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 60: 236-243. HIATT, E. S., A. R. GOLDSMITH,& D. S. FA•NE•. 1987. 26. BI•owN,J.L. 1987. Helping and communalbreeding in birds. Princeton, Princeton Univ. Press. BI•owN,R.E. 1985. Hormonesand paternalbehavior in vertebrates. HARVEY,S., J. G. PHILLIPS,A. REES,& T. R. HALL. 1984. Am. Zool. 25: 895-910. Plasma levels of prolactin and gonadotropins duringthe reproductivecycleof White-crowned Sparrows(Zonotrichia leucophrys). Auk 104:208217. J•IESON, I. G. 1989. Behavioralheterochronyand BUNTIN,J. D. 1985. Role of prolactin in avian inthe evolution of birds' helping at the nest: an cubation behavior and care of young: Is there a unselectedconsequenceof communalbreeding? causalrelationship?Pp. 252-267in Reproduction: Am. Nat. 133(3): 394-406. a behavioral and neuroendocrine perspective(B. , & J. L. CRAIG. 1987. Dominance and mating R. Komisaruk,H. I. Siegel,M.-F. Cheng, and H. in a communalpolyandrousbird: cooperationor H. Feder, Eds.). New York, New York Acad. Sci. indifferencetowardsmating competitors? EtholDAWSON,A., & A. R. GOLDSMITH. 1982. Prolactin and ogy 75: 317-327. gonadotrophinsecretionin wild starlings(Stur- 648 VLECK ETAL. JONES, R.E. 1971. The incubationpatchof birds.Biol. Rev. 46: 315-339. KAMIYOSHI, M., & K. TANAKA. 1983. Endocrine con- trol of ovulatory sequencein domesticfowl. Pp. 167-177 in Arian endocrinology(S. Midami, K. Homma, and M. Wada, Eds.). Berlin, SpringerVerlag. [Auk,Vol. 108 PEDFJ•SEN, H.C. 1989. Effectsof exogenousprolactin on parentalbehaviour in free-living female Willow Ptarmigan LagopusI. Iagopus.Anim. Behav. 38: 926-934. REHDm•, N. B., D. M. BIRDß& P. C. LAGUE. 1986. Vari- ations in plasma corticosterone,estrone, estradiol-17-beta, and progesterone concentrations with forced renesting,moltßand body weight of captive female American Kestrels. Gen. Comp. KETTERSON,E. D., V. NOLAN JR., L. WOLF, & A. R. GOLDSMITH.1990. Effectof sex, stageof reproEndocrinol. 62(3): 386-393. duction, season,and mate removal on prolactin in Dark-eyed Juncos.Condor 92: 922-930. REYER, H.-U. 1990. Pied Kingfishers:ecologicalcausKOENIG,W. D., & R. L. MUMME. 1987. Population esand reproductiveconsequences of cooperative breeding. Pp. 529-557 in Cooperative breeding ecology of the cooperatively breeding Acorn Woodpecker.PrincetonßPrinceton Univ. Press. in birds (P. B. Staceyand W. D. Koenig, Eds.). New YorkßCambridgeUniv. Press. LIGONß J.D., & P. B. STACY.1989. On the significance of helping behavior in birds. Auk 106: 700-705. SILVER, R., & M. CooPm•. 1983. Arian behavioral endocrinology.BioSci.33: 567-572. MADER,W.J. 1975. Biology of the Harris' Hawk in --, & R. B.NORGREN JR. 1987. Circadianrhythms southernArizona. Living Bird 14: 59-85. MAYS,N. A., & C. M. VLECK.1987. Changesin plasma in arian reproduction.Pp. 121-147 in Psychobiology of reproductivebehavior,an evolutionary hormonelevelswith handling time in free-living Harris' Hawks (Parabuteounicinctus).Abstract. Am. perspective (D. CrewsßEd.). Englewood Cliffs, Zool. 27(4): 156A. New JerseyßPrentice-HallßInc. ß --, & J. W. DAWSON. 1991. Plasma lu- SILVERIN, B.,& A. R.GOLDSMITH. 19•4. Theeffects of modifying incubationand prolactin secretion in free-living Pied Flycatchers.Gen. Comp. En- teinizing hormoneßsteroidhormones,behavorial roleßand nest stage in cooperativelybreeding docrinol. Harris' Hawks (Parabuteo unicinctus). Auk 108:619637. --, McNEILLY, A. S., R. J. ETCHES,& H. G. FRIESEN. 1978. A heterologousradioimmunoassay for arian prolactin: applicationto the measurementof prolactin in the turkey. Acta Endocrinol.89: 60-69. MEIER,A. H. 1975. Chronophysiologyof prolactin in lower vertabrates. Am. Zool. WINGFIELDßJ. C., & D. S. FARNER. 1975. The deter- mination of five steroidsin arian plasmaby radioimmunoassayand competitive protein binding. Steroids 26: 311-327. 15: 905-916. MYERSßS. A., J. R. MILLAM, & M. E. EL HALAWANI. 1989. PlasmaLH and prolactin levelsduring the reproductivecycle of the cockatiel(Nymphicus hoIlandicus). Gen. Comp. Endocrinol.73: 85-91. 55: 239-244. & . 1990. Plasmaprolactin concentrations in breeding Pied Flycatchers(FiceduIahypoIeuca) with an experimentally prolonged brooding period. Horm. Behar. 24: 104-113. --, & A. R. GOLDSMITH. 1990. Plasma levels of prolactinand gonadalsteroidsin relation to multiple-broodingand renestingin free-living populationsof the SongSparrow,MeIospizameIodia. NAKAMU•',A,M. 1990. Cloacal protuberanceand copHorm. Behar. 24: 89-103. ulatory behaviorof the Alpine Accentor(PrunelIa WOOLeNDEN, G. E., & J. W. FITZPATPaCK.1984. The collaris).Auk 107: 284-295. OPaNG,L. W., A. J. FlVlZZANI, & M. E. EL HALAWANI. Florida ScrubJay:demographyof a cooperativebreeding bird. PrincetonßNew Jersey,Princeton 1986. Changesin plasmaprolactinassociated with Univ. Press. laying and hatching in the Spotted Sandpiper. Auk 103: 820-822. --, ZADWOPaWY, D., J. S. WALTONß& R. J. ETCHES. 1985. M. A. COLWELLß& M. E. EL HALAWANI. 19•8. Hormonal changes associated with natural and manipulated incubation in the sex-role reversed Wilson's Phalarope. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 72: 247-256. Effect of feed and water deprivation or forcefeeding on plasmaprolactinconcentrationin turkey hens.Biol. Reprod.32: 241-247.