Examining Differences in Attachment Styles Between Marriage Types (Love versus Arranged) in Asian Indians and Americans Melissa Chan, Ashley K. Randall, Deepti Duggi, Shanmukh V. Kamble & Emily A. Butler Introduction Methods Conclusions and Implications Attachment Theory Attachment theory originally focused on parent-child relationships and was later extended to address adult romantic relationships, describing the way that individuals experience emotion in the context of relationships (Ainsworth, 1982; Hazan & Shaver, 1987). The attachment system is thought to facilitate affiliation goals by motivating seeking and bonding activities with significant others, particularly under times of stress (Bowlby, 1982). However, the majority of attachment studies have been conducted in Western cultures, indicating the need for cross-cultural attachment research. Participants Participants included a total of 130 male/female dyads. U.S. Sample: 30 couples, age range: 19 - 64 years (M = 26 yrs, SD = 11.38 yrs) India Sample: 100 couples (collected from Karnatak University in Dharwad, India) age range: 22 73 years (M = 35 yrs, SD = 8.47 yrs). 50 couples in “love” marriages and 50 in arranged marriages. Procedure Participants from the U.S. completed an online survey. Asian Indian participants completed a paper survey. Measures Self-report attachment: 12-item Experience in Close Relationships Inventory – Short form (Wei et al., 2007) assessed differences in attachment anxiety and avoidance. Participants were asked a series of questions (e.g., “I try to avoid getting too close to my partner,” an avoidance-related item) and were instructed to indicate whether the relevant behaviors were generally characteristic of the self (1 = “strongly disagree”; 7 = “strongly agree”). Our findings suggest that culture plays an important role in the ways that individuals perceive and act in romantic relationships. Cultural Differences In the handful of cross-cultural attachment studies, individuals of collectivistic cultures tended to be higher in both attachment anxiety and avoidance than those of independent cultures (Cheng & Kwan, 2008; Schmitt et al., 2004; Wang & Mallinckrodt, 2006; Wei, Russell, Mallinckrodt & Zakalik, 2004). This phenomenon might be linked to collectivistic cultural values of social harmony and emotional restraint (Markus & Kitayama, 1991). Maintaining social harmony requires one to be highly considerate of others’ well-being, which predisposes one to be anxious. Similarly, practicing emotional restraint involves refraining from sharing about one’s personal wants and needs, which predisposes one to be avoidant. Marriage Types In independent cultures, couples are expected to have the final say in marriage. In contrast, in Asian Indian and other collectivistic cultures, arranged marriages are standard. Family approval and satisfaction are vital to marriages in Indian culture. Therefore, the emphasis on others’ opinions leads Indians in arranged marriages to be more prone to higher attachment anxiety levels. Furthermore, couples may need to restrain their personal wants and needs in order to appease the family, which can contribute to higher levels of attachment avoidance. Results Cultural Differences in Attachment There was a significant difference in attachment avoidance between U.S. and Asian Indian participants, with Asian Indian participants scoring higher in attachment avoidance (F(1, 254) = 8.63, p < .004). Contrary to our prediction, there was no significant difference for attachment anxiety by country (F(1, 256) = 0.68, n.s.) As predicted, results revealed higher attachment avoidance in Asian Indians, regardless of marriage type. Contrary to our hypothesis, this effect did not apply to attachment anxiety. When observing marriage types, we found a significant difference in both attachment avoidance and attachment anxiety. The couples in Indian arranged marriages reported the highest levels of attachment insecurity compared to those in U.S. marriages and Indian “love” marriages. These findings imply that adherence to collectivistic values renders an individual more prone to attachment avoidance and attachment anxiety. Following this logic, those in Indian arranged marriages seem to be the most collectivistic among those in Indian “love” marriages and U.S. marriages. Highly collectivistic individuals may value emotional restraint and social harmony more than those who are less collectivistic, thus predisposing them to higher levels of attachment avoidance and attachment anxiety, respectively. Thus, there may be a relationship between degree of adherence to collectivistic values and attachment insecurity. In an expanding, global society, understanding individuals of various cultures is crucial to crosscultural interactions and communication. Particularly, marital and family therapists may find this research useful for implementing more effective techniques when working with collectivistic couples. Limitations U.S. sample from Tucson, Arizona might not be an accurate representation of the United States. Our Asian Indian sample might not be an accurate representation because it was gathered at Karnatak University. Universities cannot accurately represent India’s population because the university population is generally more affluent and westernized than the overall population. Additionally, Karnatak University is located in Dharwad, a city in the state of Karnataka. This state is only one of 28 states in India, and may not be an accurate representation of India’s demographics. Hypothesis Based on previous research, we hypothesize the following: 1. Asian Indian participants will report higher levels of anxiety and avoidance than the American participants due to their collectivistic mindset. 2. Indians in arranged marriages will report the highest levels of anxiety and avoidance compared to those in Indian “love” marriages and U.S. marriages. Marriage Differences in Attachment There was a significant difference in attachment avoidance and anxiety among Indian arranged marriages, Indian “love” marriages, and U.S. marriages. Indian arranged marriages showed the highest avoidance (F(2, 126) = 7.19, p < .001) and anxiety levels (F(2, 255) = 6.63, p < .002). Although the ECR has shown high validity, participants’ responses to surveys might not accurately reflect their true thoughts and feelings. Moreover, the lower levels of alpha on the short form scale are not typical. Acknowledgements The authors acknowledge funding from the Frances McClelland Institute for Children, Youth, and Families.